Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 07 June 2018
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

The Role of Illness Perception and Its Association With Posttraumatic Stress at 3 Months Following Acute Myocardial Infarction

\r\nMary Princip*Mary Princip1*Christina GattlenChristina Gattlen1Rebecca E. Meister-LangrafRebecca E. Meister-Langraf2Ulrich SchnyderUlrich Schnyder3Hansjrg ZnojHansjörg Znoj4Jürgen BarthJürgen Barth5Jean-Paul SchmidJean-Paul Schmid6Roland von Knel,Roland von Känel7,8
  • 1Division of Cardiovascular Prevention, Rehabilitation and Sports Medicine, Department of Cardiology, Bern University Hospital, Inselspital, and University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
  • 2Clienia Schlössli AG, Oetwil am See, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 3University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 4Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
  • 5Complementary and Integrative Medicine, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 6Department of Cardiology, Clinic Barmelweid, Barmelweid, Switzerland
  • 7Department of Neurology, Bern University Hospital, Inselspital, and University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
  • 8Department of Consultation-Liaison Psychiatry and Psychosomatic Medicine, University Hospital Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

Background: The aim of this study was to investigate the relationship between illness perception and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms at three months following acute myocardial infarction (MI).

Methods: Patients (n = 96) were examined within 48 h and 3 months after the illness episode. The brief revised illness perception questionnaire (Brief-IPQ) was used to assess patients' cognitive representation of their MI. At 3-month follow-up, the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (PDS) and the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) were used to assess the level of PTSD symptoms.

Results: The subjective perception of the illness, including higher harmful consequences (r > 0.35, p < 0.01), higher illness concerns (r > 0.24, p < 0.05) and more emotional impairment (r > 0.23, p < 0.05), was associated with both self-rated and clinician-rated PTSD symptoms. Beliefs regarding harmful consequences after acute MI were independently associated with levels of PTSD symptoms assessed with both the self-rated PDS and CAPS interview (standardized β coefficient = 0.24; P < 0.05) adjusted for demographic factors, cognitive depressive symptoms, fear of dying during MI, factors related to study design, and illness severity.

Conclusions: The findings suggest that initial perception of acute MI is significantly associated with PTSD symptoms attributable to MI at 3 months follow-up.

Introduction

Myocardial infarction (MI) is one of the leading causes of death in the developed world (White and Chew, 2008). According to recent estimates, ~6 million people die annually due to cardiovascular diseases, including coronary heart disease (Leal et al., 2006). Patients usually perceive an acute MI as a sudden and life-threatening event, involving high intensity of fear of dying, helplessness and loss of control. Two reviews showed that ~15% of patients develop Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) after an acute MI (Gander and von Känel, 2006; Edmondson et al., 2012). It should be noted that this previous research applied Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-IV criteria for the assessment of PTSD (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), whereas PTSD diagnostic criteria were recently revised in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). According to the DSM-5 the diagnosis of PTSD requires several criteria: Criterion A stipulates exposure to a traumatic or stressful event, leading to Criterion B, re-experience of the event in form of e.g., nightmares and flashbacks; Criterion C, avoidance of event-related stimuli; Criterion D, persistence of negative feelings and thoughts, e.g., about oneself or the world that began or worsened after the event; and Criterion E, trauma-related arousal and reactivity that began or worsened after the trauma, such as hyperarousal, risky behavior, or aggression. Symptom criteria have to be present for at least 1 month (Criterion F) and must significantly interfere with the patients' daily functioning (Criterion G). Symptoms of PTSD after MI increase the risk of hospital readmission, recurrent MI and all-cause mortality (Edmondson et al., 2012) and are also associated with poor quality of life and general health, adverse health behaviors and medical comorbidities (Edmondson and von Kanel, 2017). In addition, PTSD is associated with poor adherence with cardiac rehabilitation cardiac medication (Kubzansky and Koenen, 2007; Kubzansky et al., 2007). Younger age, as well as helplessness, pain and fear of dying during MI (Hari et al., 2010), lower educational level (Ginzburg et al., 2002) and depressive symptoms (Whitehead et al., 2005) were shown to be predictive for PTSD symptoms in the first year after acute MI.

Following the self-regulatory model (Leventhal, 1984), illness recovery also depends on how patients view their illness and on their ability to cope with the new situation. Individuals are considered to master a threat to their health, such as acute MI, by developing their own appraisal, and determining subsequent coping procedures, which then modify illness outcome. The self-regulatory model integrates a constant feedback loop, where consequences of appraisal processes are fed back into the structure of illness perception and coping reactions. Illness perception has been described to consist of five cognitive components (causes of the illness, identity, consequences of the illness, time line and ways to control or cure the illness). Taken all five beliefs together, an illness schema is formed which determines how patients respond to their illness (Leventhal, 1984; Broadbent et al., 2006b; O'Connor et al., 2008). A number of studies have shown that illness perception predicts health behaviors (e.g., treatment adherence, functional outcomes) (Broadbent et al., 2004, 2009b; Reynolds et al., 2007), may increase the risk of PTSD symptoms (Oflaz, 2014) and affect recovery from MI (Petrie et al., 1996; Broadbent et al., 2006a; French et al., 2006). Petrie et al. showed that patients' perceptions of their MI had important effects on different aspects of recovery such as social interaction, recreational and sexual activity, resumption of work, and general domestic tasks (Petrie et al., 1996, 2002; Juergens et al., 2010). Furthermore, maladaptive negative cognitions about the consequences of acute MI, belief in a longer timeline of their condition, and lower cure and control beliefs, predicted depression (Dickens et al., 2008a,b). Recent research showed that MI patients' negative illness beliefs as well as anxiety in their spouses can be reduced by a brief illness perception intervention (Broadbent et al., 2009a). The time after an acute MI was shown to be a period of vulnerability after which patients may not be able to return to normal functioning if they fail to successfully cope with the traumatic experience (Daly et al., 2000; Chung et al., 2008; Lowe et al., 2010). There are two studies investigating the association between illness perception after an acute MI and PTSD symptoms (Sheldrick et al., 2006; Oflaz, 2014). In the present study we addressed this topic whereby focusing on patients with a high level of distress during MI, for whom adaptation to MI-related experiences may be particularly challenging. Furthermore, we measured illness perception shortly, i.e., within 48 h, after MI.

Our primary aim was to examine the predictive value of illness perception for the development of MI-induced PTSD symptoms at 3 months follow-up. We hypothesized that patients who perceive their MI with a high level of distress would show more self-rated and clinician-rated PTSD symptoms. The second aim was to identify the strongest associations of PTSD symptomatology with the different dimensions of illness perception. Finally, we aimed to examine if the association between illness perception and PTSD symptoms would be independent of demographic, psychological and medical variables as potential covariates.

Methods

Study Participants and Design

The data for the present analysis was collected between January 2013 and March 2015. A total of 130 patients with acute MI, admitted to the coronary Care unit (CCU) of a tertiary university center (Bern University Hospital, Inselspital), were included in the Myocardial Infarction Stress Prevention Intervention (MI-SPRINT) randomized controlled trial (Meister et al., 2013). The aim of MI-SPRINT was to reduce the incidence of PTSD symptoms by using an early behavioral intervention. Eligibility criteria for the trial were age 18 years or older, confirmed acute ST-segment elevation MI (STEMI) or non-ST-segment elevation MI (NSTEMI), stable hemodynamic conditions, sufficient knowledge of German, positive distress screening at admission, i.e., scores of at least 5 points on a numeric rating scale, ranging from 0 to 10, for chest pain and for fear of dying and/or helplessness, no serious comorbid diseases causing death within 1 year, no cognitive impairment, no current severe depression, no suicidal ideations. Patients participating in another trial at the department of cardiology, patients with suicidal intentions in the last 2 weeks and patients who underwent emergency coronary artery bypass grafting were excluded. After written informed consent had been obtained, patients' illness perception was assessed within 48 hours after the acute coronary event. Patients were then randomized to a 45-minute single-session intervention of either trauma-focused counseling (verum group), targeting potential MI-induced posttraumatic reactions, or an active control intervention (control group), targeting the general role of psychosocial stress in patients with heart disease. Three months after the counseling session, patients' PTSD symptoms were assessed. Of the 130 patients, 70 were in the verum group and 60 were in the control group. Patients' educational level was categorized across four groups: “Lower than apprenticeship or vocational school,” “Apprenticeship or vocational school,” “High school graduation,” and “University graduation (including applied sciences).”

Illness Severity

We obtained all the medical variables necessary to compute the prognostic Global Registry of Acute Coronary Events (GRACE) score (Elbarouni et al., 2009; Bhattacharyya et al., 2010). The score provides an objective prognostic marker of illness severity and was calculated with the GRACE ACS (Global Registry of Acute Coronary Events; Acute Coronary Syndrome) Risk and Mortality Calculator (http://www.mdcalc.com/grace-acs-risk-and-mortality-calculator). It comprises 8 variables (i.e., age, heart rate, systolic blood pressure, plasma creatinine concentration, Killip classification, cardiac arrest, and STEMI or NSTEMI) and is a scoring system to prospectively estimate the probability of death within 6 months after admission for an acute coronary syndrome. Although elevated creatinine kinase isoenzyme MB (CK-MB) levels are part of the GRACE score, we separately considered the absolute CK-MB peak value as a measure of cardiac injury.

Psychometric Assessment

Illness Perception

The brief German version of the self-rated Illness Perception Questionnaire (Brief IPQ) was used to assess patients' cognitive representations of their MI (Broadbent et al., 2006b). The Brief IPQ consists of 9 items. These dimensions are “consequences,” “time-line,” “personal control,” “treatment control,” “identity,” “illness concern,” “coherence,” and “emotional representation.” We applied the brief version of the IPQ to limit questionnaire burden for patients in the acute medical setting.

The ninth item is a causal item, where patients are asked to write down three self-assumed causes for their MI. This item was not used for the current analysis. Each item is scored between 0 and 10. Item 1 for example asks: “How much does your illness affect your life?” 0 means not at all, 10 means severely affects my life. The reliability was acceptable in this sample (Cronbach's α = 0.74).

Depressive Symptoms

The Beck Depression Inventory second edition (BDI-II) (Ahrari et al., 2013) is an instrument with two subscales, cognitive/affective and somatic/affective symptoms of depression as potential predictors of PTSD symptoms. We used the validated German version of the BDI-II (Kühner et al., 2007). Again, in order to limit questionnaire burden for patients, we only used the cognitive symptom subscale. The validated German version of the BDI-II (Kühner et al., 2007) cognitive subscale consists of 13 items. Each item is rated on a Likert scale ranging from 0 to 3 (total score 0–39). The BDI-II cognitive subscale has been applied to patients with medical conditions including MI (Poole et al., 2009). Ratings for symptom severity are 0–3 (no depressive symptoms), 4–6 (mild symptoms), 7–9 (moderate symptoms), > 10 (clinically relevant symptoms). The reliability of the cognitive symptom subscale was acceptable in this sample (Cronbach's α = 0.71).

Posttraumatic Stress

To assess PTSD symptoms at 3 months follow-up, we used the validated German version of the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) (Blake et al., 1995) and the self-rated Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (PDS) (Ehlers et al., 1996; Foa et al., 1997). The study was planned before the introduction of the DSM-5, so the CAPS and PDS both refer to DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for PTSD. The CAPS interview assesses six criteria of PTSD (A-F). The A Criterion designates exposure to a traumatic event, which involves risk or actual physical injury and emotional response to the event (intense fear, horror or helplessness). Re-experiencing symptoms (Criterion B), avoidance symptoms (Criterion C), and hyperarousal symptoms (Criterion D) together cover the 17 DSM-IV PTSD symptoms that are quantified on a 5-point Likert-type scale for frequency and intensity. The scale ranges from “never” (0) to “daily or almost daily” (4) and from “none” (0) to “extreme” (4). A particular symptom is present if frequency is scored with at least 1 point and intensity with at least 2 points. In total, a sum score of 136 can be reached. To diagnose PTSD, Criterion B requires 1 out of 5 symptoms, Criterion C 3 out of 7 symptoms and Criterion D 2 out of 5 symptoms all of which must have been lasted for at least 1 month (Criterion E). Impairment in daily functioning after the traumatic event is also required (Criterion F). Like Blanchard et al. (1995) we diagnosed sub-threshold PTSD when criteria B, E, and F plus either Criterion C or Criterion D were fulfilled. In the present sample, the CAPS sum score showed good internal consistency (Cronbach's α = 0.80), whereas consistency for the three subscales was poorer (re-experience, Cronbach's α = 0.70; avoidance, Cronbach's α = 0.68; arousal, Cronbach's α = 0.49).

The Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS) is a 17-item self-report questionnaire that is also based on DSM-IV criteria for PTSD (Foa et al., 1997). On a 4-point scale, participants rate the presence of symptoms in the previous month from “not at all” (0) to “very much” (3). We categorized symptom severity per the following cut-offs: 0 (no symptoms), 1–10 (mild symptoms), 11–20 (moderate symptoms), 21–35 (moderate-to-severe symptoms) and >36 (severe symptoms). To fulfill PTSD caseness criteria, 1 item of re-experiencing, 3 items of avoidance and 2 items of arousal must be present for at least 1 month. We used the validated German version of the PDS (Ehlers et al., 1996). Internal consistency of the scale was good in our sample (re-experiencing, Cronbach's α = 0.81; avoidance, α = 0.80; arousal, α = 0.82; sum score: α = 0.90).

Data Analysis

Statistical analysis was carried out with SPSS Version 22.0 statistical software package (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, USA). All tests were two-tailed with significance level at P < 0.05. Data were verified for normal distribution using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test. To approximate a normal distribution, values for the CAPS sum score were subject to square root transformation before analysis.

Group differences were calculated using Student's t-test for normally distributed variables and Mann-Whitney U test for non-parametric variables. Bivariate correlations between two variables were estimated with Pearson coefficients. We then applied hierarchical linear regression analysis with forced entry of covariates, added in five blocks, to compute the independent association of illness perception dimensions with sum scores of both clinician- and self-rated PTSD symptoms. Covariates were selected a priori based on previous literature on a potential association with PTSD symptoms (Edmondson et al., 2012); covariates included demographic factors (gender, age, educational level), peritraumatic stress (fear of dying during MI), depressive symptoms and illness severity (GRACE risk score, peak CK-MB level). In addition, as patients received a single psychological counseling intervention session (verum vs. control group), we firstly adjusted for the variable “intervention” (verum vs. control group) to investigate the association between illness perception and PTSD symptoms, independent of the type of intervention. Secondly, an interaction effect between “intervention” (verum vs. control group) and illness perceptions (“consequences”) was examined. The first block consisted of the demographic variables. The second block consisted of psychological variables (fear of dying during MI, depressive symptoms, intervention) and the third block of objective measures of MI severity. The fourth block consisted of the illness perception dimension “consequences” and the fifth and last block of the interaction effect between “intervention” and “consequences.” Effect sizes are reported as standardized B coefficients. Data output was verified for absence of multicollinearity by Durban Watson statistics and linearity of residuals was verified by means of scatter plots.

Results

Patient Characteristics

The mean age ± SD of participants was 60.3±10.4 years. All participants were Caucasian (n = 130) and the majority were men (81.5%). At the time of their MI, 43.8% worked full time, 12.3% worked part time and 43.8% were retired or unemployed; 11.5% of participants had been diagnosed with a previous MI. 63.8% were married, 16.9% were divorced, 5.4% were widowed and 13.8% were single. 28.5% of the participants lived alone and 71.5% were cohabiting. The majority of the patients had completed “vocational school” (n = 89) or an “apprenticeship.” The average hospital stay was 3.7 ± 2.1 days. Of the 130 patients, 47.9% (n = 92) had STEMI and 29.2% (n = 38) had NSTEMI. All of the patients had undergone coronary angioplasty; 92.3% (n = 120) were treated with at least one drug-coated stent. Thirty four patients did not attend the 3-month follow-up appointment because of lack of interest, death or change of contact information such that from the originally enrolled 130 patients, 96 could be assessed at follow-up (attrition rate 26.2%).

Prevalence of Clinically Relevant PTSD Symptomatology

Clinician ratings yielded a prevalence of 1% for full PTSD and of 14.4% for clinically relevant PTSD when also including subthreshold PTSD. The prevalence of clinically relevant PTSD symptom levels was18.8% based on self-ratings. Table 1 summarizes the scores of the psychological and physiological assessments. PTSD symptom levels did not significantly differ by gender and age regarding sum scores and scores for subscales for either the CAPS interview or PDS. Older patients reported fewer re-experiencing (r = −0.21; P < 0.05) and hyperarousal (r = −0.20; P < 0.05) symptoms in the CAPS interview, whereas there was no significant relation between age and the three PTSD symptom clusters of the PDS. No significant relationship between educational level and PTSD symptomatology was found. Patients who dropped out did not significantly differ from the final sample in terms of sex, education, previous MI, previous PTSD and illness perception dimensions. Also, there was no significant difference in PTSD symptoms between patients with and those without a history of previous MI.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean scores for psychological and physiological scores.

Brief IPQ Characteristics

There were gender differences in Brief IPQ items for the subscale “control over the illness,” with men reporting higher perceptions of control than women [t(97) = 2.75; P < 0.007]. Older patients had less illness concerns (r = 0.26; P < 0.01) and assumed that the illness would last for a shorter period of time (r = −0.25; P < 0.01) than younger ones. Patients with higher educational level reported lower personal control (r = −0.20; P < 0.05) and longer illness duration (r = 0.20; P < 0.05).

Significant correlations emerged amongst Brief IPQ subscales “consequences,” “illness concern” and “emotional representation.” Patients with a greater belief of serious consequences after MI also had more illness concerns (r = 0.56; P < 0.001) and felt more emotionally affected (r = 0.39; P < 0.001). The stronger the patients' beliefs of having personal control over their illness, the lower was the perceived severity of consequences (r = −0.28; P = 0.001).

Depressive Characteristics

Depressive symptom levels did not significantly differ depending on age, gender, educational level and illness severity measured by the GRACE score and peak levels of CK-MB. Of the 130 participants, 97 patients (74.7%) reported no depressive symptoms, 22 patients (16.8%) had mild depressive symptoms, 8 patients (6.1%) reported moderate symptoms, and 3 patients (2.4%) had severe depressive symptoms. Thus, 11 (8.5%) patients reported clinically relevant depressive symptoms at admission.

Bivariate Associations of Brief IPQ Factors With PTSD Symptoms

Table 2 shows that illness perception correlated with several clinician- and self-rated PTSD symptom dimensions at 3 months post-MI. Item 1 “consequences” showed significant associations with all PTSD scales. Illness concerns and emotional representation were significantly related to sum scores of both clinician- and self-rated PTSD symptoms. The CAPS sum score showed significant correlations with item 1 “consequences,” item 2 “timeline,” item 6 “illness concern” and item 8 “emotional representation,” whereas the PDS sum score significantly related to item 1 “consequences,” item 5 “identity,” item 6 “illness concern” and item 8 “emotional representation.” Personal control and treatment control as well as the coherence of the illness showed almost no significant relationship with PTSD symptoms. Table 3 shows the associations between illness perception dimensions and PTSD symptom sum scores for the verum and active control intervention groups separately. Similar results were found for the two groups as in the total sample, with, however, item 2 “timeline” no longer showing a significant correlation in either group.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Associations between illness perception dimensions at admission and PTSD symptoms (subscales and sum scores) at 3 months follow-up after acute myocardial infarction.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Associations between illness perception dimensions at admission and PTSD symptoms (sum scores) at 3 months follow-up separated for the verum and control intervention groups.

Associations of PTSD Symptoms With Other Variables

Fear of dying was related to the sum score of PTSD symptoms from the CAPS interview (r = 0.28; P = 0.007) and the PDS (r = 0.32; P = 0.001), whereas helplessness and pain intensity during MI showed no such significance. Moreover, CK-MB was significantly associated with the CAPS sum score (r = 0.21; P < 0.05), whereas the prognostic GRACE Score was not significantly associated with PTSD symptomatology. In addition, Table 4 shows significant direct correlations between depressive symptoms (BDI-II) and PTSD symptoms.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Associations between depressive symptoms at admission and PTSD symptoms (subscales and sum scores) at 3 months follow-up after acute myocardial infarction.

Association of Individual Brief IPQ Factors With PTSD Symptoms

Item 1 “consequences” was the only independent predictor for PTSD sum scores from the CAPS interview and the PDS. The adjusted R2 value for the model with the CAPS sum score was 11% for item 1 “consequences” and 10% for all Brief IPQ items together (Table 5; models a and b). Similar associations emerged for the PDS sum score (not shown in detail).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Association of illness perception dimensions with clinician-rated PTSD symptoms (sum score) at 3 months follow-up after myocardial infarction.

Comparison of Associations With Posttraumatic Stress at 3-Month Follow-Up

Table 6 shows the results of the hierarchical linear regression analysis with five models (a-e), in which five blocks of covariates were linked to the CAPS sum score. There was no significant difference between the verum and control group regarding associations with the level of clinician-rated PTSD symptoms 3 months after MI. Sociodemographic variables and depressive symptoms showed no significant association in any model, whereas fear of dying showed significance in the second model (b), but not in the additionally adjusted models (c-e). Also, measures of illness severity per CK-MB peak values and the GRACE risk score did not emerge as significant correlates of clinician-rated PTSD symptoms.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Correlates of clinician-rated PTSD symptoms (sum score) at 3 months follow-up after acute myocardial infarction.

Regarding illness perception, a greater sum score of clinician-rated PTSD symptoms at 3-month follow-up was significantly associated with item 1 “consequences” in the adjusted model (d). After taking into account all covariates, “consequences” explained 6 % of the total variance. The final model e showed no significant intervention-by-consequences interaction. Moreover, “consequences” was the only Brief IPQ dimension that showed a significant and independent association with PTSD symptoms.

Similar results were obtained with self-rated PTSD symptoms per the PDS sum score as the dependent variable (model output not shown). Specifically, demographic variables, fear of dying, and illness severity showed no significant associations in the final model (e). The intervention was not significantly linked to the PDS sum score in any model. Moreover, the intervention-by-consequences interaction did not emerge as a significant correlate of self-rated PTSD symptoms (model e). However, the level of depressive symptoms showed an independent and significant association with self-rated PTSD symptoms in all models, explaining 9% of the variance of the PDS sum score. Also, Item 1 “consequences” remained an independent correlate of the PDS sum score in the adjusted model (d), explaining 6% of the variance.

The adjusted R2 value for the adjusted models was 0.21 for the CAPS sum score and 0.28 for the PDS sum score, indicating that about 25% of the variance in clinician- and self-rated PTSD symptom sum scores was accounted for by the entire set of covariates.

Discussion

Significant associations were found between illness perceptions assessed within 48 h of an acute MI and PTSD symptoms assessed 3 months later. Seven out of eight general illness perception factors were significantly associated with the development of PTSD symptomatology. These findings are consistent with the report of Sheldrick et al. (2006) who found illness perception factors of identity, timeline (acute/chronic), consequences, and emotional representation to be significant predictors of self-reported PTSD symptoms 3 months after acute MI. Oflaz (2014) found that illness perception factors of consequences, identity, concern and emotions were positively correlated with a PTSD diagnosis. Thus, illness perception of MI seems to impact on the development of PTSD after acute MI.

The main finding of this study was that beliefs about “consequences” were the most reliable predictor amongst the Brief IPQ variables. The result is consistent with the finding of Petrie et al. (1996) showing that the belief about illness consequences had a major impact on functioning outside work in post-MI patients. Moreover, these authors showed that an in-hospital intervention after MI to change patients' illness perceptions resulted in better functional outcome. However, Sheldrick et al. (2006) reported that among the illness perception factors showing correlations with PTSD, identity, timeline (acute/chronic), consequences and emotional representation, the latter was the most significant predictor of PTSD after MI and subarachnoid hemorrhage. Thus, beliefs about physical impairments as a consequence of illness and emotional responses to illness are both related to functioning in the wake of recovering from acute MI.

Additionally, a number of variables assessed at admission (i.e., depressive symptoms, fear of dying and CK-MB) showed bivariate correlations with PTSD symptoms. In addition to consequences, depressive symptoms emerged as a significant predictor for the sum score of self-rated PTSD symptoms at 3-month follow-up; this is in agreement with previous findings (Frasure-Smith et al., 1993). Interestingly, depressive symptoms and consequences explained a similar amount of variance in self-rated PTSD symptoms, while consequences, but not depressive symptoms, were significantly related to clinician-rated PTSD symptoms. One explanation for this observation could be that the CAPS interview specifically asks for MI-triggered traumatic symptoms making it possible to distinguish symptoms due to the traumatic sequel of MI from those better explained by other diseases such as heart failure and depression. Overall, the results underscore the importance of illness perception as a significant correlate of PTSD symptoms at 3-month follow-up across self-rated and clinician-rated PTSD symptomatology in post-MI patients.

We further found that the illness perception of older patients differed from that of younger ones. Particularly, younger patients showed higher illness concerns and assumed a longer illness time-line than older ones. A possible explanation for these difference might be that older patients have greater awareness of the possibility of becoming sick in the life course and, consequently, are more likely to accept the illness. Younger patients instead might feel substantially impaired in resuming responsibilities in daily life such as related to work, taking care of children and plans for the future. However, these assumptions are hypothetical and would need to be tested.

The prevalence of clinically relevant MI-induced PTSD symptoms is about 16% when using self-rated questionnaires, whereas 4% of patients can be diagnosed with full PTSD when applying a clinical interview (Edmondson et al., 2012). We found a slightly higher prevalence of clinically relevant PTSD symptoms based on self-ratings (18.8%), but a clearly lower prevalence of clinician-rated full PTSD (1%). The latter seems remarkable, as we only enrolled patients with a substantial amount of distress indexed by pain, fear of dying, and helplessness during MI. A higher prevalence might have been prevented in the present study as a counseling intervention was implemented with the aim to reduce the incidence of PTSD symptoms after acute MI as previously discussed (von Känel et al., 2018). However, as shown in the present study, and elaborated elsewhere (von Känel et al., 2018), the trauma-focused intervention was not more effective in reducing clinician-rated PTSD symptoms than the active control intervention targeting the general role of psychosocial stress in heart disease. Nonetheless, psychotherapeutic interventions implementing illness perceptions have been shown to be predictive for better recovery in terms of anxiety and returning to work after acute MI (Broadbent et al., 2009a). Therefore, a counseling session specifically targeting illness perceptions could perhaps help to substantially reduce the development of PTSD symptoms post-MI, although this needs to be investigated in future studies.

We assessed objective markers of MI severity to explore potential associations with PTSD symptoms at three-month follow-up. Although peak CK-MB values (but not the prognostic GRACE score) were significantly associated with PTSD symptoms in the bivariate analysis, CK-MB and the GRACE score did not emerge as independent predictors in the final covariate-adjusted model. These results suggest that illness beliefs, rather than the extent of heart muscle damage as an objective measure of illness severity, affect psychological adaptation to acute MI. This reasoning is consistent with previous investigations showing objective markers of myocardial damage were not predictors of PTSD symptoms (Guler et al., 2009; Edmondson and von Kanel, 2017).

Limitations

Our study has several limitations. The sample size was small, which must be taken into account when interpreting the non-significant associations with several illness perception subscales. We only assessed patients who were referred to a tertiary university hospital and perceived high level of distress during MI, so the generalizability of our findings is limited. We assessed the Brief IPQ at admission only; that is very shortly after MI. Therefore, modification of illness perceptions over the course of the disease was not evaluated. Furthermore, a better comprehension of the role of illness representation would have been captured using the extended IPQ questionnaire. Exclusion of patients with severe depression likely resulted in a small variance of depressive symptoms at admission, a possible explanation for why depressive symptoms did not predict PTSD symptoms any stronger than illness perceptions. Also, it must be noted that the data analyzed for the present study were collected as part of a randomized behavioral intervention trial aimed at reducing the incidence of clinician rated PTSD symptoms (Meister et al., 2013). In addition, it is possible that another variable, such as social support, could account for some of the association between illness perception and PTSD symptoms.

Conclusions

This study highlights the importance of illness perception and its assessment as a valuable tool in identifying patients likely to develop clinically relevant PTSD symptoms post-MI. Our data suggest that illness perception may play a more important role in the manifestation of PTSD symptoms after MI than do objective markers of MI severity or demographic variables. Cognitive depressive symptoms were also strongly associated with self-rated PTSD symptoms and may enhance negative illness perception, although negative illness perception might also enhance depressive cognitions. Therefore, prevention strategies targeting illness perceptions, but also cognitive depressive symptoms after acute MI, might decrease the risk of developing clinically relevant PTSD symptoms. To reduce PTSD symptoms in patients after acute MI, we would recommend psychological interventions targeting negative illness perceptions while patients are still in the hospital. Medical staff awareness should be trained. Future trials are needed to test such interventions and their effects on PTSD symptoms as an important outcome after acute MI. Taken together, the present study showed that illness perceptions were independently associated with the development of PTSD symptoms attributable to acute MI.

Ethics Statement

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the State of Bern, Switzerland.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Author Contributions

RvK, J-PS, US, JB, and HZ contributed to the development of the study design. US, HZ, JB, RvK, RM-L, and MP contributed to the development of the verum and control intervention. MP and CG wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors critically revised and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grant 140960 from the Swiss National Science Foundation to RvK (PI), J-PS, US, HZ, and JB.

Abbreviations

BDI-II, The Beck Depression Inventory second edition; CAPS, Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale; CK-MB, Creatine kinase isoenzyme MB; CCU, Coronary Care Unit; DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders fourth edition; DSM-5, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders fifth edition; GRACE score, Global Registry of Acute Coronary Events (GRACE) score; Brief IPQ, Brief Illness Perception Questionnaire; MI, Myocardial infarction; MI-SPRINT, MI-SPRINT: Myocardial Infarction—Stress PRevention INTervention; NSTEMI, non-ST-segment elevation myocardial infarction; PDS, Self-report Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale; PTSD, Posttraumatic Stress Disorder; STEMI, ST-segment elevation myocardial infarction.

References

Ahrari, F., Salehi, S. H., Fatemi, M. J., Soltani, M., Taghavi, S., and Samimi, R. (2013). Severity of symptoms of depression among burned patients one week after injury, using Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II). Burns 39, 285–290. doi: 10.1016/j.burns.2012.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC.

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC.

Bhattacharyya, M. R., Perkins-Porras, L., Wikman, A., and Steptoe, A. (2010). The long-term effects of acute triggers of acute coronary syndromes on adaptation and quality of life. Int. J. Cardiol. 138, 246–252. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2008.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blake, D. D., Weathers, F. W., Nagy, L. M., Kaloupek, D. G., Gusman, F. D., Charney, D. S., et al. (1995). The development of a Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale. J. Trauma. Stress 8, 75–90. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490080106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, E. B., Hickling, E. J., Taylor, A. E., Forneris, C. A., Loos, W., and Jaccard, J. (1995). Effects of varying scoring rules of the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) for the diagnosis of post-traumatic stress disorder in motor vehicle accident victims. Behav. Res. Ther. 33, 471–475. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00064-Q

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broadbent, E., Ellis, C. J., Gamble, G., and Petrie, K. J. (2006a). Changes in patient drawings of the heart identify slow recovery after myocardial infarction. Psychosom. Med. 68, 910–913. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000242121.02571.10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broadbent, E., Ellis, C. J., Thomas, J., Gamble, G., and Petrie, K. J. (2009a). Can an illness perception intervention reduce illness anxiety in spouses of myocardial infarction patients? A randomized controlled trial. J. Psychosom. Res. 67, 11–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.11.006

CrossRef Full Text

Broadbent, E., Niederhoffer, K., Hague, T., Corter, A., and Reynolds, L. (2009b). Headache sufferers' drawings reflect distress, disability and illness perceptions. J. Psychosom. Res. 66, 465–470. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broadbent, E., Petrie, K. J., Ellis, C. J., Ying, J., and Gamble, G. (2004). A picture of health-myocardial infarction patients' drawings of their hearts and subsequent disability: a longitudinal study. J. Psychosom. Res. 57, 583–587. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2004.03.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broadbent, E., Petrie, K. J., Main, J., and Weinman, J. (2006b). The brief illness perception questionnaire. J. Psychosom. Res. 60, 631–637. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, M. C., Berger, Z., and Rudd, H. (2008). Coping with posttraumatic stress disorder and comorbidity after myocardial infarction. Compr. Psychiatry 49, 55–65. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2007.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Daly, J., Elliott, D., Cameron-Traub, E., Salamonson, Y., Davidson, P., Jackson, D., et al. (2000). Health status, perceptions of coping, and social support immediately after discharge of survivors of acute myocardial infarction. Am. J. Crit. Care 9, 62–69. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2648.2002.02301.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickens, C., McGowan, L., Percival, C., Tomenson, B., Cotter, L., Heagerty, A., et al. (2008a). Negative illness perceptions are associated with new-onset depression following myocardial infarction. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 30, 414–420. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2008.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickens, C., McGowan, L., Percival, C., Tomenson, B., Cotter, L., Heagerty, A., et al. (2008b). New onset depression following myocardial infarction predicts cardiac mortality. Psychosom. Med. 70, 450–455. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31816a74de

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edmondson, D., and von Kanel, R. (2017). Posttraumatic stress disorder and cardiovascular disease. Lancet Psychiatry 4, 320–329. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30377-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edmondson, D., Richardson, S., Falzon, L., Davidson, K. W., Mills, M. A., and Neria, Y. (2012). Posttraumatic stress disorder prevalence and risk of recurrence in acute coronary syndrome patients: a meta-analytic review. PLoS ONE 7:e38915. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehlers, A., Steil, R., Winter, H., and Foa, E. B. (1996). German Translation of the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale by Foa (1995). Oxford: Department of Psychiatry. 1996.

Elbarouni, B., Goodman, S. G., Yan, R. T., Welsh, R. C., Kornder, J. M., Deyoung, J. P., et al. (2009). Validation of the Global Registry of Acute Coronary Event (GRACE) risk score for in-hospital mortality in patients with acute coronary syndrome in Canada. Am. Heart J. 158, 392–399. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2009.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foa, E., Cashman, L., Jaycox, L., and Perry, K. (1997). The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: The posttraumatic diagnostic scale. Psychol. Assess. 9, 445–451.

Google Scholar

Frasure-Smith, N., Lesperance, F., and Talajic, M. (1993). Depression following myocardial infarction. Impact on 6-month survival. JAMA 270, 1819–1825. doi: 10.1001/jama.1993.03510150053029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

French, D. P., Cooper, A., and Weinman, J. (2006). Illness perceptions predict attendance at cardiac rehabilitation following acute myocardial infarction: a systematic review with meta-analysis. J. Psychosom. Res. 61, 757–767. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.07.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gander, M. L., and von Känel, R. (2006). Myocardial infarction and post-traumatic stress disorder: frequency, outcome, and atherosclerotic mechanisms. Eur. J. Cardiovasc. Prev. Rehabil. 13, 165–172. doi: 10.1097/01.hjr.0000214606.60995.46

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ginzburg, K., Solomon, Z., and Bleich, A. (2002). Repressive coping style, acute stress disorder, and posttraumatic stress disorder after myocardial infarction. Psychosom. Med. 64, 748–757. doi: 10.1097/01.PSY.0000021949.04969.2E

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guler, E., Schmid, J. P., Wiedemar, L., Saner, H., Schnyder, U., and von Kanel, R. (2009). Clinical diagnosis of posttraumatic stress disorder after myocardial infarction. Clin. Cardiol. 32, 125–129. doi: 10.1002/clc.20384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hari, R., Begre, S., Schmid, J. P., Saner, H., Gander, M. L., and von Kanel, R. (2010). Change over time in posttraumatic stress caused by myocardial infarction and predicting variables. J. Psychosom. Res. 69, 143–150. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2010.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juergens, M. C., Seekatz, B., Moosdorf, R. G., Petrie, K. J., and Rief, W. (2010). Illness beliefs before cardiac surgery predict disability, quality of life, and depression 3 months later. J. Psychosom. Res. 68, 553–560. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubzansky, L. D., and Koenen, K. C. (2007). Is post-traumatic stress disorder related to development of heart disease? Future Cardiol. 3, 153–156. doi: 10.2217/14796678.3.2.153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kubzansky, L. D., Koenen, K. C., Spiro, A. III., Vokonas, P. S., and Sparrow, D. (2007). Prospective study of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and coronary heart disease in the Normative Aging Study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64, 109–116. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.1.109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kühner, C., Burger, C., Keller, F., and Hautzinger, M. (2007). [Reliability and validity of the Revised Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II). Results from German samples]. Nervenarzt 78, 651–656. doi: 10.1007/s00115-006-2098-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Leal, J., Luengo-Fernandez, R., Gray, A., Petersen, S., and Rayner, M. (2006). Economic burden of cardiovascular diseases in the enlarged European Union. Eur. Heart J. 27, 1610–1619. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehi733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leventhal, E. A. (1984). Aging and the perception of illness. Res. Aging 6, 119–135. doi: 10.1177/0164027584006001007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowe, R., Norman, P., and Bennett, P. (2010). Coping, emotion and perceived health following myocardial infarction: concurrent and predictive associations. Br. J. Health Psychol. 5, 337–350. doi: 10.1348/135910700168964

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meister, R., Princip, M., Schmid, J. P., Schnyder, U., Barth, J., Znoj, H., et al. (2013). Myocardial Infarction - Stress PRevention INTervention (MI-SPRINT) to reduce the incidence of posttraumatic stress after acute myocardial infarction through trauma-focused psychological counseling: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 14:329. doi: 10.1186/1745-6215-14-329

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Connor, S. M., Jardine, A. G., and Millar, K. (2008). The prediction of self-care behaviors in end-stage renal disease patients using Leventhal's Self-Regulatory Model. J. Psychosom. Res. 65, 191–200. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oflaz, S. (2014). Does illness perception predict posttraumatic stress disorder in patients with acute myocardial infarction? Arch. Neuropsychiatry 51, 103–109. doi: 10.4274/npa.y6394

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrie, K. J., Cameron, L. D., Ellis, C. J., Buick, D., and Weinman, J. (2002). Changing illness perceptions after myocardial infarction: an early intervention randomized controlled trial. Psychosom. Med. 64, 580–586. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200207000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrie, K. J., Weinman, J., Sharpe, N., and Buckley, J. (1996). Role of patients' view of their illness in predicting return to work and functioning after myocardial infarction: longitudinal study. BMJ 312, 1191–1194.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Poole, H., Bramwell, R., and Murphy, P. (2009). The utility of the Beck Depression Inventory Fast Screen (BDI-FS) in a pain clinic population. Eur. J. Pain 13, 865–869. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2008.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, L., Broadbent, E., Ellis, C. J., Gamble, G., and Petrie, K. J. (2007). Patients' drawings illustrate psychological and functional status in heart failure. J. Psychosom. Res. 63, 525–532. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheldrick, R., Tarrier, N., Berry, E., and Kincey, J. (2006). Post-traumatic stress disorder and illness perceptions over time following myocardial infarction and subarachnoid haemorrhage. Br. J. Health Psychol. 11, 387–400. doi: 10.1348/135910705X71434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Känel, R., Barth, J., Princip, M., Meister-Langraf, R.-E., Schmid, J.-P., Znoj, H., et al. (2018). Early psychological counseling for the prevention of posttraumatic stress induced by acute coronary syndrome: the MI-SPRINT Randomized Controlled Trial. Psychother. Psychosom. 87, 75–84. doi: 10.1159/000486099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, H. D., and Chew, D. P. (2008). Acute myocardial infarction. Lancet 372, 570–584. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61237-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitehead, D. L., Strike, P., Perkins-Porras, L., and Steptoe, A. (2005). Frequency of distress and fear of dying during acute coronary syndromes and consequences for adaptation. Am. J. Cardiol. 96, 1512–1516. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2005.07.070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: llness perception, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), myocardial infarction patients, brief illness perception questionnaire, heart attack

Citation: Princip M, Gattlen C, Meister-Langraf RE, Schnyder U, Znoj H, Barth J, Schmid J-P and von Känel R (2018) The Role of Illness Perception and Its Association With Posttraumatic Stress at 3 Months Following Acute Myocardial Infarction. Front. Psychol. 9:941. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00941

Received: 02 February 2018; Accepted: 22 May 2018;
Published: 07 June 2018.

Edited by:

Francesco Pagnini, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Italy

Reviewed by:

Daniela Rabellino, Department of Psychiatry, University of Western Ontario, Canada
Elena Vegni, Dipartimento di Scienze della Salute, Università degli Studi di Milano, Italy

Copyright © 2018 Princip, Gattlen, Meister-Langraf, Schnyder, Znoj, Barth, Schmid and von Känel. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mary Princip, mary.princip@insel.ch

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.