Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 24 May 2018
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic On the Development of Space-Number Relations: Linguistic and Cognitive Determinants, Influences, and Associations View all 28 articles

Implicit and Explicit Number-Space Associations Differentially Relate to Interference Control in Young Adults With ADHD

  • 1Institute of Cognitive Science and Assessment, Research Unit Education, Culture, Cognition and Society, Faculty of Language and Literature, Humanities, Arts and Education, University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg, Luxembourg
  • 2Luxembourg Centre for Educational Testing, Faculty of Language and Literature, Humanities, Arts and Education, University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg, Luxembourg

Behavioral evidence for the link between numerical and spatial representations comes from the spatial-numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect, consisting in faster reaction times to small/large numbers with the left/right hand respectively. The SNARC effect is, however, characterized by considerable intra- and inter-individual variability. It depends not only on the explicit or implicit nature of the numerical task, but also relates to interference control. To determine whether the prevalence of the latter relation in the elderly could be ascribed to younger individuals’ ceiling performances on executive control tasks, we determined whether the SNARC effect related to Stroop and/or Flanker effects in 26 young adults with ADHD. We observed a divergent pattern of correlation depending on the type of numerical task used to assess the SNARC effect and the type of interference control measure involved in number-space associations. Namely, stronger number-space associations during parity judgments involving implicit magnitude processing related to weaker interference control in the Stroop but not Flanker task. Conversely, stronger number-space associations during explicit magnitude classifications tended to be associated with better interference control in the Flanker but not Stroop paradigm. The association of stronger parity and magnitude SNARC effects with weaker and better interference control respectively indicates that different mechanisms underlie these relations. Activation of the magnitude-associated spatial code is irrelevant and potentially interferes with parity judgments, but in contrast assists explicit magnitude classifications. Altogether, the present study confirms the contribution of interference control to number-space associations also in young adults. It suggests that magnitude-associated spatial codes in implicit and explicit tasks are monitored by different interference control mechanisms, thereby explaining task-related intra-individual differences in number-space associations.

Introduction

Numbers and space are closely associated in the human mind (e.g., Dehaene and Brannon, 2011). The most extensively studied and replicated behavioral evidence for this association is without a doubt the spatial-numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect (Dehaene et al., 1993). It describes the observation that individuals from Western societies are typically faster on their left/right hand-side for relatively small/large numbers respectively, when doing binary classifications on numbers. The SNARC effect was first documented in an experiment where numerical magnitude information was task-relevant (termed “the magnitude SNARC effect”) in that individuals judged whether a centrally displayed number was smaller or larger than a given standard (Dehaene et al., 1990). Subsequent experiments, however, demonstrated that numerical magnitude does not need to be task-relevant to observe the SNARC effect, since it was also evidenced during parity judgments (termed “the parity SNARC effect”; e.g., Dehaene et al., 1993).

Three spatial coding mechanisms were proposed to account for spatial-numerical interactions, including a visuospatial, verbal-spatial, and working memory (WM) account (for a review, see e.g., Fischer and Shaki, 2014). According to the dominant and most traditional visuospatial account, numbers are mentally represented along a continuous left-to-right-oriented spatial representational medium, also known as the mental number line (MNL), with small/large numbers located on its left/right respectively, at least in Western societies (Moyer and Landauer, 1967; Restle, 1970; Dehaene et al., 1993). An alternative view suggests that number-space associations arise from categorical verbal-spatial coding. The latter account is based on the polarity correspondence principle by Proctor and Cho (2006) and assumes that the SNARC effect results from the polar correspondence between the verbal categorical concepts “small” and “left” (both assigned to the same polarity) as well as “large” and “right” (both assigned to the opposing polarity). A final explanation for the link between numbers and space was provided by Fias et al. (2011), who argued that spatial-numerical interactions are task-specific associations established within WM (see also van Dijck and Fias, 2011; Abrahamse et al., 2016; Fias and van Dijck, 2016). More concretely, task-relevant numerical magnitudes are temporarily activated in their canonical order within a horizontal left-to-right oriented spatial sequence in WM. Spatial-numerical interactions then result from internal shifts of spatial attention within this encoded numerical sequence, with positions from the beginning/end of the sequence eliciting faster left-/right-sided responses respectively.

Inter-Individual Differences in Number-Space Associations

The strength of number-space associations considerably varies between individuals. For instance, variability is explained by inter-individual differences in mathematical skills. Participants with lower arithmetic performances featured stronger number-space associations in the parity judgment task (e.g., Georges et al., 2017; but see Cipora and Nuerk, 2013). Similarly, more pronounced parity SNARC effects were observed in humanities students with than without math difficulties (Hoffmann et al., 2014a), while the weakest number-space associations were evidenced in math professionals (Cipora et al., 2016). The parity SNARC effect also depends on math anxiety, with more anxious individuals displaying stronger number-space associations (Georges et al., 2016). Furthermore, it was shown to increase with age (Hoffmann et al., 2014b; Ninaus et al., 2017).

In addition to this, inter-individual variability in the parity SNARC effect has recently been shown to relate to differences in inhibitory control as indexed by the Stroop effect (Hoffmann et al., 2014b; for a review on the Stroop effect, see MacLeod, 1991; see also Stroop, 1935). Participants with weaker interference control in the Stroop paradigm featured stronger number-space associations in the parity judgment task. The relation between number-space associations during parity judgments and inhibitory control might be explained by the need to inhibit numerical magnitude and its associated spatial code to accurately respond based on the number’s parity status. It should, however, be noted that the relation between weaker interference control in the Stroop task and stronger parity SNARC effects was most pronounced in the elderly. It did not reach significance in young healthy individuals, which the authors ascribed to their near ceiling performances on the Stroop task.

Intra-Individual Differences in Number-Space Associations

Apart from inter-individual differences in the SNARC effect, number-space associations also vary intra-individually depending on the number processing task. For instance, Georges et al. (2017) observed no significant relation between the SNARC effects in a parity judgment and magnitude classification task (at least at the sample level – positive and negative correlations were evidenced in individuals with object and spatial visualization styles respectively). Moreover, verbal and visuospatial WM load selectively abolished the parity and magnitude SNARC effects respectively (Herrera et al., 2008; van Dijck et al., 2009). In addition, hemi-neglect patients were shown to display regular number-space associations in the parity judgment task, where access to numerical magnitude is implicit, but featured an atypical SNARC effect in the explicit magnitude classification task (Priftis et al., 2006; Zorzi et al., 2012). The SNARC effects in implicit and explicit tasks were also shown to associate with different cognitive factors. Namely, only the magnitude SNARC effect related to inter-individual differences in visualization cognitive styles (Georges et al., 2017; see also Kozhevnikov et al., 2005; Chabris et al., 2006). Furthermore, the relation between weaker arithmetic performances and stronger SNARC effects during parity judgments (e.g., Hoffmann et al., 2014a; Georges et al., 2017; but see Cipora and Nuerk, 2013) was not observed for number-space associations in the explicit magnitude classification task. Altogether, these findings suggest that numbers might be associated with qualitatively different spatial codes depending on the implicit or explicit nature of the numerical processing task.

Interference Control and Inter-/Intra-Individual Differences in Number-Space Associations

The present study aimed to (a) replicate the previously reported relationship between implicit number-space associations and inhibitory control (Hoffmann et al., 2014b) and (b) investigate whether this relationship extends to explicit magnitude processing.

While the “parity SNARC-Stroop” relation was significant in a group composed of young and elderly healthy participants, it was mainly driven by the elderly and did not reach significance in the young subgroup (Hoffmann et al., 2014b). We reasoned that this result pattern might be caused by the fact that young healthy adults achieved near ceiling performances on the Stroop task. In the current study, we therefore focussed on young individuals featuring atypical inhibitory control and included only participants formally diagnosed with ADHD and/or displaying symptoms consistent with ADHD according to the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (Kessler et al., 2005). These people not only feature weaker interference control (Walker et al., 2000; Rapport et al., 2001; Lansbergen et al., 2007), but their deficits are also highly variable (Lovejoy et al., 1999; Sergeant et al., 2002; Seidman, 2006). Such inter-individual variability in inhibitory control deficits should increase the statistical power of detecting significant relations with other continuous variables (e.g., Goodwin and Leech, 2006). This enables us to verify whether the previously reported null relation between the parity SNARC and Stroop effects in the younger healthy individuals (Hoffmann et al., 2014b) can indeed be ascribed to their near ceiling performances on the Stroop task. Finding evidence for a significant association between number-space associations in the parity judgment task and interference control in the Stroop paradigm in a relatively younger population would considerably strengthen the critical involvement of inhibitory control mechanisms in the spatial coding processes underlying the parity SNARC effect.

In addition to interference control in the Stroop task, the present study also determined whether executive control processes in the arrowhead version of the Flanker task (e.g., Stins et al., 2004; Davelaar and Stevens, 2009; for the original version, see Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974) might relate to inter-individual differences in number-space associations during parity judgments. Even though conflict occurs in both the Stroop and Flanker paradigms, its nature and processing likely differ depending on the executive control task. For instance, while elderly people were shown to display weaker interference control in the Stroop task than young adults (West and Alain, 2000; Van der Elst et al., 2006), inhibitory control in the Flanker task did not differ between younger and older participants (Falkenstein et al., 2001; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2002). Moreover, heritability of interference control was evidenced in the Stroop but not the Flanker task (Stins et al., 2004). In addition, interference control in the Stroop but not the Flanker task was related to WM capacity (Stins et al., 2005). Furthermore, relations could be evidenced neither between the time needed for conflict resolution nor between the interference scores in the Stroop and Flanker tasks (Stins et al., 2005). Conflict processing in the Flanker task was shown to relate to the activation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the insula (Zhu et al., 2010; Zmigrod et al., 2016). Conversely, neural responses reflecting the Stroop effect were measured in a broader network including not only the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, but also the posterior parietal, anterior cingulate and left premotor cortices (van Veen and Carter, 2005; Melcher and Gruber, 2009; Kim et al., 2011; for a meta-analysis, see Nee et al., 2007). These findings thus suggest that Stroop and Flanker effects likely reflect qualitatively different executive control processes. Consequently, contrasting their relations with number-space associations will allow for a better understanding of the specific inhibitory control mechanisms contributing to spatial-numerical interactions.

In a second step, we aimed to assess the relations between the SNARC effect during explicit magnitude classifications and inhibitory control indexed by the Stroop and Flanker effects, since number-space associations were previously shown to vary intra-individually depending on the implicit or explicit nature of the number processing task (van Dijck et al., 2009; Georges et al., 2017). This will inform us about the involvement of inhibitory control processes in the spatial coding processes underlying the magnitude SNARC effect and as such their role in intra-individual differences in number-space associations.

Materials and Methods

This study was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Review Panel (ERP) of the University of Luxembourg. All participants gave written informed consent and received a small monetary reward for their participation.

Participants

The study was advertised to the university students via their email addresses. Students could take part in the study if they were formally diagnosed with ADHD (Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder) and/or if they considered themselves as being easily distracted and unable to concentrate. A total of 42 students signed up for the study, of which 5 had a formal diagnosis of ADHD. Participants had various backgrounds with different mother tongues (e.g., English, Finnish, French, German, Greek, Russian, Spanish, etc.) and their study fields ranged from mathematics and physics over law to humanities. None of the participants suffered from any comorbid learning disabilities such as dyslexia or dyscalculia.

Procedure and Tasks

Before the start of the experiment, the 42 students that had signed up for the study completed the 6-item version of the World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale V 1.1 (ASRS) symptom checklist (Kessler et al., 2005; for psychometric properties, see Adler et al., 2006; Matza et al., 2011). This was to ensure that individuals not formally diagnosed with ADHD displayed symptoms consistent with this disorder. Participants without a formal diagnosis of ADHD that did not feature ADHD traits according to this self-report scale were excluded prior to the start of the study. This reduced the study sample to a total of 35 participants.

These participants completed the experimental tasks during two testing sessions that were run on separate days with an upper limit of 1 week apart. Following standard practice in individual differences research (e.g., Carlson and Moses, 2001), all participants performed the tests in the same order and trial sequences were identical for all participants in every task. On the first testing day, participants completed the speeded matching-to-sample task, the parity judgment task, the magnitude classification task and the Flanker task. These computerized tasks were programmed in E-prime (Version 1.2 or 2.0.8.79) and administered on a Windows computer. The classical verbal paper-and-pencil version of the Stroop task was implemented on the second testing day.

Prior to data analysis, 4 students were excluded from the sample since they did not complete all the tests. After removal of these participants, outliers were identified for each of the measures described below. A total of 5 participants had to be removed, since their performances fell 2.5 standard deviations (SD) below or above the mean group performances on at least one of the measures. All statistical analyses were thus conducted on data obtained from 26 individuals.

Parity Judgment and Magnitude Classification Tasks

The parity judgment task (adapted from Dehaene et al., 1993; see also Georges, 2017; see Figure 1A) was administered to determine number-space associations in a task with implicit numerical magnitude processing. The experiment consisted of 288 experimental trials divided equally across two blocks. Each experimental trial started with an empty black-bordered square (6.87° × 6.87°) on a white background. After 300 ms, one of eight possible stimuli (Arabic digits: 1, 2, 3, 4, 6, 7, 8, or 9; color: black; font: Arial; point size: 64) appeared in the center of the black-bordered square and remained until response. The inter-trial interval consisted of a blank screen of 1300 ms. In the first block, participants judged as quickly as possible whether the presented number was odd/even by pressing the “A”/“L” key on a QWERTZ keyboard respectively. This stimulus-response mapping was reversed for all participants in the second block. Each target number was displayed 18 times per block. The sequence in which the target stimuli appeared was pseudo-randomized in a way that no target number could appear twice in a row, and the correct response could not be on the same side more than three times consecutively. Each block started with 12–20 training trials, depending on response accuracy. Participants were given a small break half-way through each block.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Schematic representation of the different experimental tasks. Trial sequence in the computerized parity judgment and magnitude classification tasks (A). Trial sequence with a congruent target stimulus (C) in the computerized Flanker task (B). Incongruent (I) and neutral (N) target stimuli are displayed on the left (B). Word reading (WR), color naming (CN) and interference (I) conditions in the classical 100-item verbal paper-and-pencil version of the Stroop task (C).

The magnitude classification task (adapted from Bull et al., 2005; van Galen and Reitsma, 2008; see also Georges, 2017; see Figure 1A) was administered to determine number-space associations in a task with explicit numerical magnitude processing. The experiment was identical to the parity judgment task with the exception that it only consisted of 1441 trials and that participants had to judge whether the centrally presented single Arabic number was smaller/larger than five by pressing the “A”/“L” key respectively in the first block. This stimulus-response mapping was again reversed for all participants in the second block.

Data from the training sessions was not analyzed (for comparable data analysis, see Georges et al., 2017). The mean error rate on experimental trials was 2.52 and 2.56% in the parity judgment and magnitude classification task respectively [F(1,25) = 0.006; p = 0.94; ηp2 = 0.00]. Errors were not further analyzed. Reaction times (RTs) shorter or longer than 2.5 SD from the individual mean were considered as outliers and discarded prior to data analysis (2.86 and 3.19% of all correct trials in the parity judgment and magnitude classification task respectively, F(1,25) = 1.55; p = 0.23; ηp2 = 0.06).

SNARC effect regression slopes were computed using the individual regression equations method suggested by Fias et al. (1996). First, RTs were averaged separately for each number and each response side for every participant. Individual RT differences (dRTs) were then calculated by subtracting for each number the mean left-sided RT from the mean right-sided RT. The resulting dRTs were subsequently submitted to a regression analysis, using number magnitude as predictor variable. Unstandardized SNARC regression slopes were taken as a measure of the strength of the SNARC effect in terms of the inclination of the regression lines. Negative regression weights reflected SNARC effects in the expected direction (faster left-/right-sided RTs for small/large numbers respectively) with more negative regression slopes corresponding to stronger number-space associations.

Stroop Task

The English adaptation of the classical 100-item verbal paper-and-pencil version of the Stroop paradigm was used to determine Stroop-like interference control (Stroop, 1935). The task consisted of three conditions, each comprising 100 items that were displayed in a 10 × 10 matrix on an A4 sheet of paper (see Figure 1C). In the word reading condition (WR), participants had to read color words (“red,” “blue,” “green”) printed in black ink. In the color naming condition (CN), they named swatches of red, blue and green ink. In the interference condition (I), participants were required to indicate the color of the ink (red, blue, green) that a color word (“red,” “blue,” “green”) was written in without reading the color word (e.g., they had to indicate “red” for the color word “green” printed in red ink). Participants were instructed to name/read the different items in each condition as quickly and as accurately as possible going from left-to-right. The time needed to complete each of the three conditions was recorded in every participant using a stopwatch. The WR and CN conditions served as control conditions.

To get a single inhibitory control measure indexing each participant’s Stroop effect, we calculated RT differences between the interference and color naming conditions. This is one of the standard methods for quantifying Stroop interference control (Lansbergen et al., 2007). A greater RT difference is indicative of weaker interference control, as it reflects considerably slower RT in the interference than the color naming condition.

Flanker Task

The experiment was adapted from Eriksen and Eriksen (1974) and consisted of 48 trials (see Figure 1B). Each trial started with the display of a fixation cross (color: black; font: Arial; point size: 28) in the center of a white screen. After 400 ms, a horizontal black arrow (height: 0.69°; width: 2.06°) was presented on a white background until response or for a maximum of 1700 ms. On half of the trials, the central arrow pointed in the left direction, while on the remaining half its pointing direction was reversed. Two black horizontal flanker arrows appeared on each side of the central arrow and pointed either in the same direction than the central arrow (i.e., congruent condition, 16 trials) or in its opposite direction (i.e., incongruent condition, 16 trials). On the remaining neutral trials, the central arrow was flanked on both sides by two horizontal black bars. Participants were required to press the “A”/“L” key on a standard QWERTZ (Swiss-French) keyboard if the central arrow pointed in the left/right direction respectively. They were instructed to ignore the flanker arrows and bars. The inter-trial interval consisted of a blank screen of 500 ms. Trial sequence was identical for all participants and pseudo-randomized in a way that the correct response could not be the same more than 3 times consecutively. Moreover, the same target-distractor array did never successively appear. The actual experiment was preceded by 12 practice trials, consisting of 4 congruent, incongruent and neutral trials respectively. For each participant and every congruency condition, we computed error rates in percentages and averaged correct RTs that fell within 2.5 SD from the individual mean correct RT.

To incorporate error rates and RTs into a single performance measure, we computed inverse efficiency scores (IES) by dividing the means of congruent, incongruent or neutral correct RTs by their corresponding percentage accuracies for each participant (Bruyer and Brysbaert, 2011; Khng and Lee, 2014). IES thus adjusts RT performance for sacrifices in accuracy made in favor of response speed. Considering that faster responses together with fewer errors yield smaller IES, the smaller the IES is, the better the performance is.

To get a single inhibitory control measure indexing each participant’s Flanker effect, we calculated individual IES differences by subtracting congruent from incongruent IES. A greater IES difference is indicative of weaker inhibitory control, as it reflects considerably worse performance (i.e., slower RT and/or more errors) in the incongruent compared to the congruent condition.

Speeded Matching-to-Sample Task

The speeded matching-to-sample task was used to determine general processing speed (GPS) and described in detail by Hoffmann et al. (2014a; see also Georges et al., 2016). Each trial consisted of a centrally displayed target shape and two possible solution shapes, displayed below to the left and right. Participants had to identify the solution that was identical to the target as quickly as possible by clicking the “A”/“L” key on a QWERTZ keyboard if it appeared on the bottom left/right respectively. For each participant, we averaged correct RTs that fell within 2.5 SD from the individual mean correct RT.

Results

Descriptives

SNARC Effects

Split-half reliabilities were calculated for the parity and magnitude SNARC effect regression slopes using the odd–even method to control for systematic influences of practice or tiring within the tasks (see Cipora and Nuerk, 2013; Cipora et al., 2016; Georges et al., 2016, 2017; Ninaus et al., 2017). Trials were odd–even half-split (based on order of appearance) and two SNARC effect regression slopes were calculated separately for each participant and each task. The correlation coefficients were Spearman–Brown corrected to get a reliability estimate for the entire set of items. The Spearman-Brown corrected correlation coefficient was r = 0.56 in both the parity judgment and magnitude classification tasks.

To determine whether relatively low reliabilities could be caused by the influence of bivariate outliers, we performed linear regression analyses between odd and even SNARC effect regression slopes and identified influential data points based on the conventional Cook’s distances criterion of >4/N (Cook, 1979; Bollen and Jackman, 1985; see Viarouge et al., 2014 for application of this method in the SNARC context). Two separate analyses were performed – one for the parity judgment task and one for the magnitude classification task. For the parity judgment task, analysis revealed two influential data points with Cook’s distances greater than.154 (i.e., 4/26). After removal of these participants, the bivariate correlation between odd and even parity SNARC effect regression slopes remained similar (r = 0.35 for N = 24; r = 0.39 for N = 26; Fisher’s z for comparison of two correlations based on independent groups: z = 0.15; p = 0.88), yielding a Spearman-Brown corrected reliability estimate of r = 0.52. For the magnitude classification task, three influential cases were identified with Cook’s distances greater than.154 (i.e., 4/26). After removal of these three influential data points, the correlation between odd and even magnitude SNARC effect regression slopes improved from r = 0.39 (for N = 26) to r = 0.53 (for N = 23), yielding a Spearman-Brown corrected reliability estimate of r = 0.7. Influential cases were not removed in any of the following correlation analyses, where N = 26.

The mean SNARC effect regression slope across all participants was significantly negative in the parity judgment but not the magnitude classification task [parity SNARC effect regression slope = -11.71; SD = 13.36; t(25) = -4.47; p < 0.001; magnitude SNARC effect regression slope = -4.22; SD = 12.89; t(25) = -1.67; p = 0.11; see Figure 2]. A repeated-measures ANOVA on the SNARC effect regression slopes also revealed a main effect of task [F(1,25) = 4.59; p = 0.042; ηp2 = 0.16], indicating stronger number-space associations in the parity judgment than the magnitude classification task in terms of the inclination of the regression lines. Overall, a large proportion of the participants displayed a negative SNARC effect regression slope in both the parity judgment (20/26; 76.92%) and magnitude classification tasks (18/26; 69.23%).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Regressions of number magnitudes onto dRTs (i.e., differences between right and left RTs) in the parity judgment and magnitude classification tasks.

Stroop Effect

The mean RTs across all participants were 43.04 s (SD = 8.19) in the word reading, 64.19 s (SD = 10.08) in the color naming and 97.04 s (SD = 18.96) in the interference conditions. A repeated measures ANOVA on RT including condition as within-subject variable revealed a main effect of condition [F(2,50) = 222.93; p < 0.001; ηp2 = 0.9]. Participants performed significantly worse in the interference compared to the color naming [t(25) = -13.56; p < 0.001] and the word reading [t(25) = -16.13; p < 0.001] conditions. Performances were also significantly lower during color naming than word reading [t(25) = -12.51; p < 0.001].

The mean RT difference between the interference and color naming conditions (i.e., Stroop effect) across all participants was 32.85 s (SD = 12.35). Individual Stroop effects were used for the subsequent correlation analyses.

Flanker Effect

As for the two SNARC effects, reliability of the Flanker effect was determined using the split-half method (Greene et al., 2008; see also MacLeod et al., 2010). More concretely, congruent and incongruent trials were odd-even half-split (based on order of appearance) and Flanker effects (i.e., differences between incongruent and congruent IES) were computed separately for each half in every participant. The correlation between IES differences (i.e., Flanker effects) calculated on odd and even trials was Spearman-Brown corrected, yielding a reliability estimate of r = 0.63.

The mean error rates and RTs across all trials and participants were 1.2% (SD = 3.07) and 432 ms (SD = 72) in the congruent, 8.41% (SD = 11.17) and 490 ms (SD = 67) in the incongruent and 0.72% (SD = 2.04) and 440 ms (SD = 64) in the neutral conditions respectively. Error rates and RTs did not correlate in the congruent (r = 0.01; p = 0.96) and neutral (r = -0.23; p = 0.25) conditions, suggesting that these performance estimates provide different aspects of inhibitory control. Moreover, there was a speed-accuracy trade-off in the incongruent condition (r = -0.53; p = 0.006).

A repeated measures ANOVA on IES including congruency condition as within-subject variable revealed a main effect [F(2,50) = 47.00; p < 0.001; ηp2 = 0.65]. Participants performed significantly worse on incongruent (IES = 538.69 ms; SD = 67.57) compared to congruent [IES = 438.09 ms; SD = 75.49; t(25) = -7.33; p < 0.001] and neutral [IES = 443.17 ms; SD = 62.69; t(25) = 7.13; p < 0.001] trials. Performances did not differ between the congruent and neutral conditions.

The mean IES difference between incongruent and congruent trials (i.e., Flanker effect) across all participants was 100.59 ms (SD = 69.99). Individual Flanker effects were used for the subsequent correlation analyses.

General Processing Speed

The mean RT across all trials and participants in the speeded matching-to-sample task was 626 ms (SD = 242). RTs significantly positively correlated with RTs on the parity judgment (613 ms; SD = 85; r = 0.7; p = < 0.001), magnitude classification (536 ms; SD = 74; r = 0.46; p = 0.019), Stroop (68.09 s; SD = 11.11; r = 0.4; p = 0.045), and Flanker tasks (454 ms; SD = 66; r = 0.41; p = 0.036). It thus provided a valid index of general processing speed and can be used as a control measure in a partial correlation analysis to verify whether potentially significant correlations between number-space associations and any of the interference control measures might be reduced to inter-individual differences in general processing speed.

All descriptive information is displayed in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Descriptive information.

Correlation Analyses

All reported correlations are two-tailed, unless otherwise stated. Stronger parity SNARC effects were associated with weaker interference control in the Stroop task (r = -0.48; p = 0.012; Figure 3A). Conversely, no relation was observed between the parity SNARC effect and interference control in the Flanker task (r = 0.16; p = 0.44; Figure 3B). This difference between the relations of the parity SNARC effect with interference control in the Stroop and Flanker paradigms reached significance, as revealed by Pearson and Filon’s z (Pearson and Filon, 1898), assessing differences between two overlapping correlations based on dependent samples (z = -2.51; p = 0.006; one-tailed). As opposed to number-space associations in implicit tasks, stronger magnitude SNARC effects trended to be associated with better interference control in the Flanker task (r = 0.37; p = 0.06; Figure 3D). The magnitude SNARC effect was, however, unrelated to interference control in the Stroop task (r = -0.12; p = 0.58; Figure 3C). The difference between the correlations of the magnitude SNARC effect with Stroop and Flanker effects was significant (z = -1.80; p = 0.04; one-tailed). In line with previous findings, the parity and magnitude SNARC effects did not correlate (r = 0.08; p = 0.7). The difference between the relations of the Stroop effect with the parity and magnitude SNARC effects, however, only trended toward significance (z = -1.54; p = 0.06; one-tailed). Likewise, no significant difference could be observed between the correlations of the SNARC effects in implicit and explicit tasks with the Flanker effect (z = -0.87; p = 0.19; one-tailed). Performances on the Stroop and Flanker tasks did also not correlate (r = -0.14; p = 0.5), confirming qualitative differences between these interference measures. Finally, general processing speed did not relate to any of the SNARC effects or inhibitory control measures (all ps > 0.05).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Relations between number-space associations and executive control. Correlation of the parity SNARC effect with interference control in the Stroop (A) and Flanker (B) tasks. Correlation of the magnitude SNARC effect with interference control in the Stroop (C) and Flanker (D) tasks.

Considering the non-perfect reliabilities of the SNARC effect regression slopes, we corrected bivariate correlations for attenuation using Spearman’s correction for attenuation formula, corresponding to rxy /sqrt(rxxryy), with rxx and ryy coding for the reliabilities of X and Y respectively (Spearman, 1904, 1910; Muchinsky, 1996; see also Cipora and Nuerk, 2013; Gloria et al., 2016; Georges et al., 2017, for a comparable application of this correction for attenuation method). This procedure determines the correlation between two variables if they were perfectly reliable, and therefore provides for a more accurate estimate of the correlation between two parameters. Attenuated and disattenuated correlation coefficients are shown in the upper and lower part of Table 2 respectively.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Correlation analysis.

All the above relations remained similar when controlling for general processing speed in a partial correlation analysis (see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Partial correlation analysis controlling for general processing speed.

Discussion

Inter-Individual Differences in Number-Space Associations During Parity Judgments Relate to Interference Control in the Stroop Task

Stronger number-space associations in the parity judgment task correlated with weaker interference control in the Stroop task in young adults with diagnosed or self-reported ADHD. This relation remained significant even after controlling for general processing speed, previously implicated in both the parity SNARC (e.g., Wood et al., 2008; Cipora and Nuerk, 2013; Hoffmann et al., 2014b) and Stroop effects (e.g., Bugg et al., 2007; Hoffmann et al., 2014b). The present findings extend the recently reported relation between stronger parity SNARC effects and weaker Stroop inhibitory control in the elderly and confirm the hypothesis that the null relation in young healthy participants can be explained by their near ceiling performances on the Stroop task (Hoffmann et al., 2014b).

In contrast, number-space associations during parity judgments were not related to interference control in the Flanker task in the present population. It is unlikely that this null relation can be explained by insufficient variance in the Flanker effect due to near ceiling task performances, considering the tendency for a positive relation between interference control in the Flanker task and number-space associations in the magnitude classification task (discussed in the next section). Moreover, individuals with ADHD were previously shown to feature abnormal inhibitory control in both the Stroop (Nigg et al., 2005; Walker et al., 2000; King et al., 2007) and Flanker paradigms (Lundervold et al., 2011). The spatial coding mechanisms underlying the parity SNARC effect thus depend on those inhibitory control processes indexed by the Stroop but not the Flanker effect. Overall, this provides valuable information regarding the type of conflict encountered during parity judgments, thereby advancing our understanding of the spatial coding processes underlying the parity SNARC effect.

To characterize the coding mechanisms accounting for the parity SNARC effect, it is important to firstly understand the cognitive processes underlying interference control in the Stroop and Flanker tasks. Interference in the Stroop paradigm originates at the semantic level from an attribute that is intrinsic to the target stimulus (i.e., the meaning of the color word conflicts with the semantic representation of the ink color, e.g., Klein, 1964; La Heij, 1988). Moreover, the distracting color word meaning is highly salient, considering that literate individuals are primed to automatically access a word’s meaning upon sight prior to processing any additional features (Ashcraft and Radvansky, 2010). Conversely, interference in the Flanker paradigm occurs spatially instead of semantically from lateral arrows that are drawn from the same set of stimuli than the target stimulus (Eriksen and Schultz, 1979). The relation between the parity SNARC and Stroop (but not Flanker) effects thus suggests that the spatial code associated with numerical magnitude during parity judgments is semantic in nature and/or intrinsic to the target stimulus (see Table 4). Since the Stroop as opposed to the Flanker task yields basically no perceptual interference (Valle-Inclán, 1996), the conflict in the parity judgment task is also unlikely of perceptual nature. This outcome is in line with the parity judgment paradigm, where the task-relevant parity status and the conflicting spatial code associated with the automatically activated yet task-irrelevant magnitude information reflect distinct semantic properties of the same target number.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Characteristics of the spatial code associated with numerical magnitude during parity judgments and magnitude classifications.

While distraction in the Flanker task is provided by externally available visuospatial information (i.e., the flanking arrows), the distracting color word meaning in the Stroop paradigm is rather verbal in nature. The Stroop task is highly left lateralized, most prominently in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and inferior frontal areas, previously implicated in the resolution of verbal conflict (Jonides et al., 1998; Leung et al., 2000; Jonides and Nee, 2006). In the present Stroop paradigm, responses were also given verbally, thereby adding to the already rather verbal nature of the Stroop task. The strong relation between the parity SNARC and Stroop effects thus suggests that the distracting spatial code associated with numerical magnitude in the parity judgment task might also be verbal in nature (see Table 4). In line with previous claims, this suggests that the parity SNARC effect predominantly results from verbal-spatial polarity coding as opposed to arising from the spatial coding of numerical magnitudes on a horizontally oriented MNL (Gevers et al., 2010; Georges et al., 2017).

According to the dimensional overlap model by Kornblum et al. (1990; see also Kornblum and Lee, 1995; Zhang et al., 1999), interference in the Flanker task mainly reflects a stimulus-stimulus conflict, where the pointing directions of the task-irrelevant flanking arrows interfere with that of the targeted central arrow at the early stage of stimulus encoding. Such interference is likely resolved via the spatial filtering of the perceptual distractors and the narrowing of the attentional focus to the task-relevant central arrow location (Wendt et al., 2012). Conversely, conflict in the Stroop paradigm occurs at multiple stages of stimulus processing (Zhang and Kornblum, 1998; Milham et al., 2001; De Houwer, 2003). In addition to the semantic stimulus-stimulus conflict at earlier processing stages (e.g., Klein, 1964; Kornblum et al., 1990; Sharma and McKenna, 1998; Schmidt and Cheesman, 2005; Goldfarb and Henik, 2007), stimulus-response conflict arises during response selection (e.g., Cohen et al., 1990; MacLeod, 1991; van Veen and Carter, 2005; Szucs and Soltész, 2010), when the task-relevant ink color and the irrelevant meaning of the color word activate competing responses. Such stimulus-response conflict is then probably resolved via biasing units reflecting the task-relevant semantic dimension (i.e., the ink color of the color word; Szucs et al., 2009). The relation between number-space associations in the parity judgment task and interference control in the Stroop paradigm thus suggests that the parity SNARC effect also mainly originates at later processing stages during response selection (see Table 4). Accordingly, the response provoked by the task-irrelevant numerical magnitude-associated spatial code competes/conflicts with that induced by the task-relevant parity status prior to response execution. Such competition is likely resolved via biasing units coding the response associated with the task-relevant parity status (see Table 4). Considering the absence of a relation between the parity SNARC and Flanker effects, interference in the parity judgment task is unlikely controlled by filtering mechanisms already at the early stage of number encoding. This outcome is in line with previous models proposed to account for the parity SNARC effect (Keus et al., 2005; Gevers et al., 2006). According to Gevers et al. (2006), the parity SNARC effect results from the interference of two processing routes operating in parallel. The conditional route links task-relevant parity information with response keys based on task instructions, while the unconditional route conveys the automatic association between numerical magnitude and space. On congruent trials, both routes activate the same response location, while on incongruent trials responses are slowed down and more error-prone since the two routes activate competing outcomes.

Evidence for such parallel processing of task-relevant and irrelevant information and of conflict resolution mainly at the response selection stage during parity judgments has also been provided by EEG studies. Namely, congruency effects were previously reported on the latency of the lateralized readiness potential (Keus et al., 2005; Gevers et al., 2006), an EEG component considered to be the output of response selection stages (Gratton et al., 1988; Coles, 1989; for a review, see also Leuthold et al., 2004). In addition and in line with observations regarding the Stroop effect (Ilan and Polich, 1999; Zurrón et al., 2009; for a review, see Sahinoglu and Dogan, 2016), the P300 peak latency did not show an onset difference between congruent and incongruent trials in the parity judgment task (Gevers et al., 2006), indicating that the conflict indexed by the parity SNARC effect is unlikely detected at early perceptual stages.

The assumption that the conflict indexed by the parity SNARC effect originates at later processing stages during response selection also agrees with findings regarding stronger parity SNARC effects in the elderly compared to young healthy individuals (Hoffmann et al., 2014b; Ninaus et al., 2017). Elderly persons featured weaker interference control in the Stroop paradigm than young controls (West and Alain, 2000; Van der Elst et al., 2006), suggesting an age-associated decline in conflict resolution particularly at later response selection stages. In contrast, the resolution of stimulus-stimulus conflict at earlier processing stages in the Flanker task did not differ between younger and older participants as reflected by similar behavioral performances of both age groups (Falkenstein et al., 2001; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2002).

Inter-Individual Differences in Number-Space Associations During Magnitude Classifications Relate to Interference Control in the Flanker Task

Inter-individual variability in the strength of number-space associations during explicit magnitude classifications did not relate to inter-individual differences in the Stroop effect. Conversely, stronger magnitude SNARC effects were associated with better interference control in the Flanker task. However, it should be noted that this correlation did not reach significance, also not prior to partialling out the effects of general processing speed. Nonetheless, the relation between more pronounced number-space associations during explicit magnitude classifications and better interference control in the Flanker paradigm was significantly different from the null correlation between the magnitude SNARC and Stroop effects.

The latter null relation might suggest that the spatial code associated with numerical magnitude during explicit classifications is not of verbal nature, akin to the verbal interference encountered in the Stroop paradigm (Jonides et al., 1998; Leung et al., 2000; Jonides and Nee, 2006) and probably also during parity judgments. This lines up with previous findings indicating that the magnitude SNARC effect was selectively abolished by a visuospatial but not verbal WM load, highlighting the importance of visuospatial coding mechanisms (van Dijck et al., 2009). Moreover, Georges et al. (2017) reported a relation between stronger magnitude SNARC effects and greater preferences for spatial as opposed to object visualization. Number-space associations during explicit magnitude classifications thus likely predominantly depend on visuospatial processing resources in the right parietal cortex associated with spatial visualization (Lamm et al., 1999; see Table 4). The absence of a correlation between number-space associations in the magnitude classification task and interference control in the Stroop paradigm might also indicate that the magnitude SNARC effect differs from conflict that originates from a semantic feature intrinsic to the target stimulus (i.e., the central number). Furthermore, interference in the magnitude classification task might diverge from conflict that is mainly resolved at the response selection stage, such as the conflict induced by the irrelevant color word meaning in the Stroop paradigm. The null relation between the magnitude SNARC and Stroop effects could, however, also simply suggest that no conflict arises from the spatial code associated with numerical magnitude during explicit classifications.

When considering the tendency for an association between the magnitude SNARC and Flanker effects, it might suggest that the potential interference during explicit classifications originates from irrelevant visuospatial information extrinsic to the target stimulus (see Table 4, alternative a). Additionally, it could indicate conflict resolution directly at the early stage of stimulus encoding via spatial filtering (see Table 4, alternative a). At first, this idea seems difficult to reconcile with the magnitude classification paradigm, considering that it only comprises a single task-relevant centrally displayed number. If extrinsic distraction might be encountered during magnitude classifications, it can only originate internally. One possibility is for instance that interference arises from task-irrelevant numerical magnitudes represented adjacently to the target number on a horizontal MNL (or sequence within WM; see e.g., Fias et al., 2011). Indirect support for such an interplay between the externally available task-relevant number and internally represented task-irrelevant numerical magnitudes was provided by Nuerk et al. (2005). Their findings suggested that the representation of closely related task-irrelevant numbers can interfere with task-relevant numerical magnitude classifications at least when these distracting numbers are externally available. Of course, the assumption of such interference by internally represented task-irrelevant numerical magnitudes is only valid if the spatial code associated with numerical magnitude during explicit classifications is indeed visual instead of verbal in nature. A greater ability to suppress such task-irrelevant spatial-numerical activations at earlier processing stages (akin to the spatial filtering of distractors in the Flanker task) might then facilitate the processing of the task-relevant numerical magnitude together with its associated spatial code, manifesting in stronger magnitude SNARC effects. This explanation could then account for the positive relation between stronger magnitude SNARC effects and better interference control in the Flanker task.

Alternatively, the trend for a relation between stronger magnitude SNARC effects and better inhibitory control in the Flanker task might indicate that a greater ability to selectively focus attention on task-relevant information (as indexed by better interference control in the Flanker task; see Wendt et al., 2012) is associated with stronger number-space associations during explicit magnitude classifications. Of course, this entails that the spatial code associated with the task-relevant numerical magnitude is also relevant rather than distracting for successful resolution of the magnitude classification task (see Table 4, alternative b). The relevance of spatial-numerical mappings during explicit magnitude classifications could then also account for the lack of a correlation between the magnitude SNARC and Stroop effects. Moreover, it seems likely considering that coding small/large numerical magnitudes as left/right on the MNL (or within WM) might assist left-/right-sided numerical magnitude classifications. It would also provide an explanation for the observation that stronger magnitude SNARC effects are not related to weaker arithmetic performances (Georges et al., 2017), contrary to the parity SNARC effect (e.g., Cipora et al., 2016; Georges et al., 2017; but see Cipora and Nuerk, 2013). In general, more linear spatial representations of numerical magnitudes, as assessed using number line estimations, are commonly associated with better magnitude comparison performances (Laski and Siegler, 2007) as well as higher math skills (Link et al., 2014). These findings thus highlight the importance/relevance of spatial-numerical representations for arithmetic performances.

Intra-Individual Differences in Number-Space Associations and Task-Dependent Differences in the Relation to Interference Control

The present results provide further evidence for the previously reported intra-individual variability in number-space associations depending on the implicit or explicit nature of numerical magnitude processing (van Dijck et al., 2009; Georges et al., 2017). More concretely, parity and magnitude SNARC effects were uncorrelated and related (or at least tended to relate) inversely to distinct inhibitory control measures, namely negatively with the Stroop and positively with the Flanker effects respectively. This heterogeneity in the cognitive processes underlying the SNARC effect generally agrees with studies indicating that both long-term spatial coding mechanisms such as the spatial representation of numerical magnitudes on a MNL and temporary associations between the ordinal position of numerical magnitudes and space in WM might exist in parallel (Ginsburg and Gevers, 2015; Huber et al., 2016; but see Abrahamse et al., 2016).

Previous explanations for such intra-individual variations in number-space associations depending on the number processing task suggested task-related differences in the nature of the numerical magnitude-associated spatial code, with verbal- and visuospatial coding processes probably underlying the parity and magnitude SNARC effects respectively (van Dijck et al., 2009; Gevers et al., 2010; Georges et al., 2017). This assumption might further be supported by the present findings. Namely, only the parity SNARC effect correlated with interference control in the Stroop paradigm, reflecting the suppression of task-irrelevant verbal information (i.e., the color word meaning).

The current results, however, allow for an additional (or even alternative) explanation regarding intra-individual differences in number-space associations depending on the number processing task. Namely, as already discussed above, the relations of the parity and magnitude SNARC effects with stimulus–response and stimulus–stimulus conflict resolution in the Stroop and Flanker paradigms respectively suggests that the task-dependency of number-space associations might result from task-related differences in the processing stages of the spatial code associated with numerical magnitude, irrespective of its visual or verbal nature. While the conflict provided by the numerical magnitude-associated spatial code during parity judgments might predominantly be resolved at the response selection stage via biasing units coding the task-relevant response location (see Table 4), the potential conflict during explicit magnitude classifications probably rather originates from extrinsic distractors and is resolved via their spatial filtering at earlier processing stages (see Table 4, alternative a). The conflicts indexed by the parity and magnitude SNARC effects would thus have distinct origins and be resolved via different mechanisms at different processing stages, thereby potentially explaining the task-dependency of number-space associations.

Alternatively, as already mentioned before, differences in the relevance of the spatial code associated with numerical magnitude during parity judgments and magnitude classifications and consequently in its processing (inhibition vs. activation respectively) could probably underlie the task-dependency of number-space associations (see Table 4).

Limitations and Future Directions

First, it should be reminded that split-half reliabilities for both the parity and magnitude SNARC effect regression slopes were relatively low. Lower reliabilities are, however, not unusual in SNARC-related studies. Comparably low reliabilities were also reported in previous studies by means of both internal consistency (Cipora and Nuerk, 2013; Viarouge et al., 2014; Georges et al., 2016, 2017; Cipora et al., 2018) as well as test–retest stability (Viarouge et al., 2014).

To increase reliability estimates, the length of the parity judgment task was increased, which was shown to yield better split-half reliability estimates (Cipora and Wood, 2012, 2017; see also Cipora and Nuerk, 2013; Cipora et al., 2016). Nonetheless, the Spearman–Brown corrected correlation coefficient in the present parity judgment task was comparable to that in Georges et al. (2017) using a task that included only half of the number of trials. Moreover, similar split-half reliability estimates were obtained for the parity and magnitude SNARC effects, albeit the parity judgment task had twice the length of the magnitude classification paradigm. Increasing the number of repetitions per stimulus in the parity judgment task did thus not seem to enhance split-half reliability in the current study. It should, however, be noted that the present study only included individuals with diagnosed or self-reported ADHD, generally featuring relatively high intra-individual variability in RTs (Castellanos et al., 2005; Vaurio et al., 2009). This might thus have generally accounted for the lower reliabilities, despite the increase in some of the task lengths.

Importantly, the relatively poor reliabilities of the parity and magnitude SNARC effect regression slopes could have negatively impacted the correlations reported in the current study. Namely, the upper bound of a correlation between two parameters depends on their reliabilities in that the highest correlation between two variables equals the square root of the product of their reliabilities [i.e., sqrt(rxxryy), with rxx and ryy coding for the reliabilities of X and Y respectively]. The correlation between two variables is thus weakened by measurement error, such that true correlations between measures with poor reliability might be overlooked (Osborne and Waters, 2002, p. 2). Consequently, we need to be careful when drawing conclusions about (the absence of) relations between number-space associations and interference control from the present findings. Nevertheless, any task-related differences in the relations between number-space associations and the different interference control measures cannot be ascribed to low measurement reliability, since split-half reliability estimates for the parity and magnitude SNARC effect regression slopes were equally low.

Another drawback of the present study could be the relatively small sample size of N = 26. A post hoc power analysis based on effect size, conventional alpha level, and sample size (i.e., N = 26) using the program GPower (Faul et al., 2007, 2009) revealed that the probability of rejecting a false null hypothesis was 81% for large (r = 0.5), 34% for medium (r = 0.3) and 8% for small (r = 0.1) effect sizes. The present study had thus sufficient power to detect a significant relation between the SNARC effect and inhibitory control at the large effect size level. Conversely, less than adequate statistical power was obtained at the small to medium effect size level to reject an incorrect null hypothesis. The lack of sufficient power for detecting small to medium effect sizes could potentially account for the non-significant relation between stronger number-space associations during magnitude classifications and better interference control in the Flanker task in the current sample.

Future studies should also consider the inclusion of control variables. Especially the involvement of verbal and visuospatial WM could be assessed in greater detail. Relations between number-space associations and inhibitory control might indeed be (partially) confounded by WM processes. WM is not only implicated in the Stroop (Long and Prat, 2002; Kane and Engle, 2003; Hutchison, 2011) as well as Flanker effects (Redick and Engle, 2006; Heitz and Engle, 2007), but also likely contributes to number-space associations (van Dijck et al., 2009). Nonetheless, it should be noted that Hoffmann et al. (2014b) controlled for the influence of verbal WM in their study, thereby excluding the possibility that the relation between stronger parity SNARC effects and weaker interference control in the Stroop paradigm might be confounded by verbal WM.

Future research could also elaborate on the assumption that no interference originates from the spatial code associated with numerical magnitude during explicit classifications by investigating whether the strength of the magnitude SNARC effect varies with age, similarly to the age-associated increase in number-space associations during parity judgments (Hoffmann et al., 2014b; Ninaus et al., 2017). Inhibitory control declines with age (see Glisky, 2007) mostly regarding conflict resolution at later response selection stages (Falkenstein et al., 2001; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2002; Van der Elst et al., 2006), while target selection processes usually remain intact even in the elderly (West and Alain, 2000). Consequently, if the magnitude SNARC effect indeed does not index interference control, its strength should not be altered by aging.

Conclusion

Stronger parity SNARC effects were associated with weaker interference control in the Stroop but not Flanker task in young adults with diagnosed or self-reported ADHD. Number-space associations in the parity judgment task thus index conflict resolution akin to the Stroop effect. In other terms, the parity SNARC effect likely reflects interference between the (probably) verbal-spatial code associated with numerical magnitude and the spatial location of the response associated with parity status at later processing stages during response selection (see Table 4). Conversely, the magnitude SNARC effect was not related to interference control in the Stroop paradigm. Stronger number-space associations during explicit magnitude classifications, however, tended to be associated with better conflict resolution in the Flanker task. The (probably) visuospatial code associated with numerical magnitude is thus likely relevant during explicit magnitude classifications, with its activation at the early stage of stimulus encoding underlying the magnitude SNARC effect (see Table 4, alternative b). Overall, the present findings suggest that the relevance/importance of number-space associations for numerical judgments depends on the implicit or explicit nature of the number processing task. While the spatial code associated with numerical magnitude seems to assist explicit magnitude classifications (and is therefore activated at the encoding stage), it seems to interfere with parity judgments (and is therefore suppressed at the response selection stage). Such differences in the relevance of the numerical magnitude-associated spatial code during parity judgments and magnitude classifications and in the related executive control mechanisms monitoring its processing (suppression vs. activation respectively) might account for the previously reported task-dependency of number-space associations.

Author Contributions

CG, DH, and CS: conceived and designed the experiments. CG: analyzed the data. CG, DH, and CS: wrote the paper.

Funding

The current research was supported by the National Research Fund Luxembourg (FNR; www.fnr.lu) under Grant AFR PHD-2012-2/4641711.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Some content in the introduction and method sections has been taken and/or adapted from Georges’ dissertation thesis, defended on 17th February 2017 in Luxembourg. A full reference to the thesis is provided in the reference list.

Footnotes

  1. ^Since previous research indicated that split-half reliability was significantly lower for the parity than the magnitude SNARC effect (Georges et al., 2017), we decided to double the number of trials in the parity judgment task, as this was suggested to considerably enhance reliability estimates (Cipora and Wood, 2012; see also Cipora and Nuerk, 2013; Cipora et al., 2016). Due to time constraints, we did, however, not increase the length of the magnitude classification task.

References

Abrahamse, E., van Dijck, J.-P., and Fias, W. (2016). How does working memory enable number-induced spatial biases? Front. Psychol. 7:977. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adler, L. A., Spencer, T., Faraone, S. V., Kessler, R. C., Howes, M. J., Biederman, J., et al. (2006). Validity of pilot adult ADHD self- report scale (ASRS) to rate adult ADHD symptoms. Ann. Clin. Psychiatry 18, 145–148. doi: 10.1080/10401230600801077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashcraft, M. H., and Radvansky, G. A. (2010). Cognition, 5th Edn. Boston, MA: Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Bollen, K. A., and Jackman, R. W. (1985). Regression diagnostics: an expository treatment of outliers and influential cases. Sociol. Methods Res. 13, 510–542. doi: 10.1177/0049124185013004004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruyer, R., and Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)? Psychol. Belg. 51, 5–13. doi: 10.5334/pb-51-1-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bugg, J. M., DeLosh, E. L., Davalos, D. B., and Davis, H. P. (2007). Age differences in Stroop interference: contributions of general slowing and task-specific deficits. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 14, 155–167. doi: 10.1080/138255891007065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bull, R., Marschark, M., and Blatto-Valle, G. (2005). SNARC hunting: examining number representation in deaf students. Learn. Individ. Dif. 15, 223–236. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2005.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, S. M., and Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Dev. 72, 1032–1053. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Scheres, A., Di Martino, A., Hyde, C., and Walters, J. R. (2005). Varieties of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-related intra-individual variability. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1416–1423. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chabris, C. F., Jerde, T. E., Woolley, A. W., Gerbasi, M. E., Schuldt, J. P., Bennett, S. L., et al. (2006). Spatial and Object Visualization Cognitive Styles: Validation Studies In 3800 Individuals. Technical Report No. 2. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.

Google Scholar

Cipora, K., Hohol, M., Nuerk, H.-C., Willmes, K., Brożek, B., Kucharzyk, B., et al. (2016). Professional mathematicians differ from controls in their spatial-numerical associations. Psychol. Res. 80, 710–726. doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0677-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cipora, K., and Nuerk, H.-C. (2013). Is the SNARC effect related to the level of mathematics? No systematic relationship observed despite more power, more repetitions, and more direct assessment of arithmetic skill. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 66, 1974–1991. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.772215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cipora, K., van Dijck, J.-P., Georges, C., Masson, N., Schiltz, C., Pesenti, M., et al. (2018). “Minority pulls the sample mean: only about 35% of individuals reveal a consistent SNARC effect,” in Proceedings of the European Workshop on Cognitive Neuropsychology, Brixen.

Cipora, K., and Wood, G. (2012). Optimal power to detect (between group differences) in SNARC–Monte Carlo study. Poster Presented on XXXth European Workshop on Cognitive Neuropsychology, Brixen.

Google Scholar

Cipora, K., and Wood, G. (2017). Finding the SNARC instead of hunting it: a 2020 Monte Carlo investigation. Front. Psychol. 8:1194. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., and McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychol. Rev. 97, 332–361. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.332

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coles, M. G. (1989). Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. Psychophysiology 26, 251–269. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1989.tb01916.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, R. D. (1979). Influential observations in linear regression. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 74, 169–174. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1979.10481634

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davelaar, E. J., and Stevens, J. (2009). Sequential dependencies in the Eriksen flanker task: a direct comparison of two competing accounts. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 16, 121–126. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.1.121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Houwer, J. (2003). On the role of stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus compatibility in the Stroop effect. Mem. Cogn. 31, 353–359. doi: 10.3758/BF03194393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., and Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 122, 371–396. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., and Brannon, E. (eds) (2011). Space, Time and Number in the Brain. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., and Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-number number comparison. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 16, 626–641. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.16.3.626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksen, B. A., and Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophys. 16, 143–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03203267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksen, C. W., and Schultz, D. W. (1979). Information processing in visual search: a continuous flow conception and experimental results. Percept. Psychophys. 25, 249–263. doi: 10.3758/BF03198804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falkenstein, M., Hielscher, H., Dziobek, I., Schwarzenau, P., Hoormann, J., Sunderman, B., et al. (2001). Action monitoring, error detection, and the basal ganglia: an ERP study. Neuroreport 12, 157–161. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200101220-00039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., and Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using GPower 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). GPower 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., and d’Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: evidence from the SNARC effect. Math. Cogn. 2, 95–110. doi: 10.1080/135467996387552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fias, W., and van Dijck, J.-P. (2016). The temporary nature of number—space interactions. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 70, 33–40. doi: 10.1037/cep0000071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fias, W., van Dijck, J.-P., and Gevers, W. (2011). “How is number associated with space? The role of working memory,” in Space, Time and Number in the Brain, eds S. Dehaene and E. Brannon (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 133–148.

Google Scholar

Fischer, M. H., and Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition—From single digits to arithmetic. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 67, 1461–1483. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.927515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georges, C. (2017). Number-Space Associations as Indexed by the SNARC Effect – Their Relations to Mathematical Abilities and Anxiety & Their Underlying Cognitive Mechanisms. Doctoral thesis, University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg.

Google Scholar

Georges, C., Hoffmann, D., and Schiltz, C. (2016). How math anxiety relates to number–space associations. Front. Psychol. 7:1401. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georges, C., Hoffmann, D., and Schiltz, C. (2017). How and why do number-space associations co-vary in implicit and explicit magnitude processing tasks? J. Numer. Cogn. 3, 182–211. doi: 10.5964/jnc.v3i2.46

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gevers, W., Ratinckx, E., De Baene, W., and Fias, W. (2006). Further evidence that the SNARC effect is processed along a dual-route architecture: evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. Exp. Psychol. 53, 58–68. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.53.1.58

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gevers, W., Santens, S., Dhooge, E., Chen, Q., Van den Bossche, L., Fias, W., et al. (2010). Verbal-spatial and visuospatial coding of number-space interactions. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 139, 180–190. doi: 10.1037/a0017688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ginsburg, V., and Gevers, W. (2015). Spatial coding of ordinal information in short- and long-term memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:8. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glisky, E. L. (2007). “Changes in cognitive function in human aging,” in Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms, ed. D. R. Riddle (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press).

Google Scholar

Gloria, A. M., Castellanos, J., and Herrera, N. (2016). The reliability and validity of the cultural congruity and university environment scales with Chicana/o community college students. Community Coll. J. Res. Pract. 40, 426–438. doi: 10.1080/10668926.2015.1066276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldfarb, L., and Henik, A. (2007). Evidence for task conflict in the Stroop effect. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 33, 1170–1176. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.5.1170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodwin, L. D., and Leech, N. L. (2006). Understanding correlation: factors that affect the size of r. J. Exp. Educ. 74, 249–266. doi: 10.3200/JEXE.74.3.249-266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., Sirevaag, E. J., Eriksen, C. W., and Donchin, E. (1988). Pre- and post-stimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 14, 331–344. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.14.3.331

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, D. J., Barnea, A., Herzberg, K., Rassis, A., Neta, M., Raz, A., et al. (2008). Measuring attention in the hemispheres: the lateralized attention network test (LANT). Brain Cogn. 66, 21–31. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heitz, R. P., and Engle, R. W. (2007). Focusing the spotlight: individual differences in visual attention control. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 136, 217–240. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrera, A., Macizo, P., and Semenza, C. (2008). The role of working memory in the association between number magnitude and space. Acta Psychol. 128, 225–237. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffmann, D., Mussolin, C., Martin, R., and Schiltz, C. (2014a). The impact of mathematical proficiency on the number-space association. PLoS One 9:e85048. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffmann, D., Pigat, D., and Schiltz, C. (2014b). The impact of inhibition capacities and age on number–space associations. Cogn. Process. 15, 329–342. doi: 10.1007/s10339-014-0601-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, S., Klein, E., Moeller, K., and Willmes, K. (2016). Spatial–numerical and ordinal positional associations coexist in parallel. Front. Psychol. 7:438. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutchison, K. A. (2011). The interactive effects of listwide control, item-based control, and working memory capacity on Stroop performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 37, 851–860. doi: 10.1037/a0023437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ilan, A. B., and Polich, J. (1999). P300 and response time from a manual Stroop task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 367–373. doi: 10.1016/S0168-5597(98)00053-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jonides, J., and Nee, D. E. (2006). Brain mechanisms of proactive interference in working memory. Neuroscience 139, 181–193. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Marshuetz, C., Koeppe, R. A., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (1998). Inhibition in verbal working memory revealed by brain activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 8410–8413. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.14.8410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kane, M. J., and Engle, R. W. (2003). Working memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 132, 47–70. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Adler, L., Ames, M., Demler, O., Faraone, S., Hiripi, E., et al. (2005). The World Health Organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol. Med. 35, 245–256. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keus, I. M., Jenks, K. M., and Schwarz, W. (2005). Psychophysiological evidence that the SNARC effect has its functional locus in a response selection stage. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 48–56. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khng, K. H., and Lee, K. (2014). The relationship between Stroop and stop-signal measures of inhibition in adolescents: influences from variations in context and measure estimation. PLoS One 9:e101356. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, C., Kroger, J. K., and Kim, J. (2011). A functional dissociation of conflict processing within anterior cingulate cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 304–312. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, J. A., Colla, M., Bras, M., Heuser, I., and Von Cramon, D. Y. (2007). Inefficient cognitive control in adult ADHD: evidence from trial-by-trial Stroop test and cued task switching performance. Behav. Brain Funct. 3:42. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-3-42

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, G. S. (1964). Semantic power measured through the interference of words with color-naming. Am. J. Psychol. 77, 576–588. doi: 10.2307/1420768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., and Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility–a model and taxonomy. Psychol. Rev. 97, 253–270. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.2.253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kornblum, S., and Lee, J.-W. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 21, 855–875. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.21.4.855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kozhevnikov, M., Kosslyn, S., and Shephard, J. (2005). Spatial versus object visualizers: a new characterization of visual cognitive style. Mem. Cogn. 33, 710–726. doi: 10.3758/BF03195337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

La Heij, W. (1988). Components of Stroop-like interference in picture naming. Mem. Cogn. 16, 400–410. doi: 10.3758/BF03214220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamm, C., Bauer, H., Vitouch, O., and Gstättner, R. (1999). Differences in the ability to process a visuo-spatial task are reflected in event-related slow cortical potentials of human subjects. Neurosci. Lett. 269, 137–140. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00441-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lansbergen, M. M., Kenemans, J. L., and van Engeland, H. (2007). Stroop interference and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology 21, 251–262. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.2.251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laski, E. V., and Siegler, R. S. (2007). Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. Child Dev. 78, 1723–1743. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01087.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, H. C., Skudlarski, P., Gatenby, J. C., Peterson, B. S., and Gore, J. C. (2000). An event-related functional MRI study of the Stroop color word interference task. Cereb. Cortex 10, 552–560. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.6.552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leuthold, H., Sommer, W., and Ulrich, R. (2004). Preparing for action: inferences from CNV and LRP. J. Psychophysiol. 18, 77–88. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.18.23.77

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Link, T., Nuerk, H.-C., and Moeller, K. (2014). On the relation between the mental number line and arithmetic competencies. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 67, 1597–1613. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.892517

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, D. L., and Prat, C. S. (2002). Working memory and Stroop interference: an individual differences investigation. Mem. Cogn. 30, 294–301. doi: 10.3758/BF03195290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovejoy, D. W., Ball, J. D., Keats, M., Stutts, M. L., Spain, E., Janda, L., et al. (1999). Neuropsychological performance of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): diagnostic classification estimates for measures of frontal lobe/executive functioning. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 5, 222–233. doi: 10.1017/S1355617799533055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundervold, A. J., Adolfsdottir, S., Halleland, H., Halmøy, A., Plessen, K., and Haavik, J. (2011). Attention network test in adults with ADHD — the impact of affective fluctuations. Behav. Brain Funct. 7, 27–34. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-7-27

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 109, 163–203. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLeod, J., Lawrence, M., Mcconnell, M., Eskes, G., Klein, R., and Shore, D. (2010). Appraising the ANT: psychometric and theoretical considerations of the attention network test. Neuropsychology 24, 637–651. doi: 10.1037/a0019803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matza, L. S., Van Brunt, D. L., Cates, C., and Murray, L. T. (2011). Test-retest reliability of two patient-report measures for use in adults with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 15, 557–563. doi: 10.1177/1087054710372488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melcher, T., and Gruber, O. (2009). Decomposing interference during Stroop performance into different conflict factors: an event-related fMRI study. Cortex 45, 189–200. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milham, M. P., Banich, M. T., Webb, A., Barad, V., Cohen, N. J., Wszalek, T., et al. (2001). The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 12, 467–473. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00076-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moyer, R. S., and Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature 215, 1519–1520. doi: 10.1038/2151519a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muchinsky, P. M. (1996). The correction for attenuation. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 56, 63–75. doi: 10.1177/0013164496056001004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nee, D. E., Wager, T. D., and Jonides, J. (2007). Interference resolution: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 1–17. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Talsma, D., Coles, M. G., Holroyd, C. B., Kok, A., et al. (2002). A computational account of altered error processing in older age: dopamine and the error-related negativity. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2, 19–36. doi: 10.3758/CABN.2.1.19

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigg, J. T., Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., and Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2005). Causal heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Do we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1224–1230. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ninaus, M., Moeller, K., Kaufmann, L., Fischer, M. H., Nuerk, H.-C., and Wood, G. (2017). Cognitive mechanisms underlying directional and non-directional spatial-numerical associations across the lifespan. Front. Psychol. 8:1421. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nuerk, H., Bauer, F., Krummenacher, J., Heller, D., and Willmes, K. (2005). The power of the mental number line: how the magnitude of unattended numbers affects performance in an Eriksen task. Psychol. Sci. 47, 34–50.

Google Scholar

Osborne, J. W., and Waters, E. (2002). Four assumptions of multiple regression that researchers should always test. Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 8, 1–5.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Pearson, K., and Filon, L. N. G. (1898). Mathematical contributions to theory of evolution: IV. On the probable errors of frequency constants and on the influence of random selection and correlation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. A 191, 229–311. doi: 10.1098/rsta.1898.0007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Priftis, K., Zorzi, M., Meneghello, F., Marenzi, R., and Umilta, C. (2006). Explicit versus implicit processing of representational space in neglect: dissociations in accessing the mental number line. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 680–688. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proctor, R. W., and Cho, Y. S. (2006). Polarity correspondence: a general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychol. Bull. 132, 416–442. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapport, L. J., Van Voorhis, A., Tzelepis, A., and Friedman, S. R. (2001). Executive functioning in adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Neuropsychol. 15, 479–491. doi: 10.1076/clin.15.4.479.1878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Redick, T. S., and Engle, R. W. (2006). Working memory capacity and Attention Network Test performance. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 20, 713–721. doi: 10.1002/acp.1224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Restle, F. (1970). Speed of adding and comparing numbers. J. Exp. Psychol. 83(2 Pt 1), 274–278. doi: 10.1037/h0028573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahinoglu, B., and Dogan, G. (2016). Event-related potentials and the Stroop effect. Eurasian J. Med. 48, 53–57. doi: 10.5152/eurasianjmed.2016.16012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, J. R., and Cheesman, J. (2005). Dissociating stimulus–stimulus and response–response effects in the Stroop task. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 59, 132–138. doi: 10.1037/h0087468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidman, L. J. (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in people with ADHD across the lifespan. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 26, 466–485. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., and Oosterlaan, J. (2002). How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Behav. Brain Res. 130, 3–28.

Google Scholar

Sharma, D., and McKenna, F. P. (1998). Differential components of the manual and vocal Stroop tasks. Mem. Cogn. 26, 1033–1040. doi: 10.3758/BF03201181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spearman, C. (1904). The proof and measurement of association between two things. Am. J. Psychol. 15, 72–101. doi: 10.2307/1412159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spearman, C. (1910). Correlation calculated from faulty data. Br. J. Psychol. 3, 271–295. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1910.tb00206.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stins, J. F., Polderman, J. C., Boomsma, D. I., and de Geus, E. J. (2005). Response interference and working memory in 12-year-old children. Child Neuropsychol. 11, 191–201. doi: 10.1080/092970490911351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stins, J. F., van Baal, G. C. M., Polderman, T. J., Verhulst, F. C., and Boomsma, D. I. (2004). Heritability of Stroop and flanker performance in 12-year old children. BMC Neurosci. 5:49. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-5-49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szucs, D., and Soltész, F. (2010). Stimulus and response conflict in the color–word Stroop task: a combined electro-myography and event-related potential study. Brain Res. 1325, 63–76. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szucs, D., Soltész, F., Bryce, D., and Whitebread, D. (2009). Real-time tracking of motor response activation and response competition in a Stroop task in young children: a lateralized readiness potential study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 2195–2206. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valle-Inclán, F. (1996). The locus of interference in the Simon effect: an ERP study. Biol. Psychol. 43, 147–162. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05181-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Van Breukelen, G. J. P., and Jolles, J. (2006). The Stroop color-word test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment 13, 62–79. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dijck, J.-P., and Fias, W. (2011). A working memory account for spatial–numerical associations. Cognition 119, 114–119. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dijck, J.-P., Gevers, W., and Fias, W. (2009). Numbers are associated with different types of spatial information depending on the task. Cognition 113, 248–253. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Galen, M. S., and Reitsma, P. (2008). Developing access to number magnitude: a study of the SNARC effect in 7- to 9-year-olds. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 101, 99–113. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Veen, V., and Carter, C. S. (2005). Separating semantic conflict and response conflict in the Stroop task: a functional MRI study. Neuroimage 27, 497–504. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaurio, R. G., Simmonds, D. J., and Mostofsky, S. H. (2009). Increased intra-individual reaction time variability in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder across response inhibition tasks with different cognitive demands. Neuropsychologia 47, 2389–2396. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viarouge, A., Hubbard, E. M., and McCandliss, B. D. (2014). The cognitive mechanisms of the SNARC effect: an individual differences approach. PLoS One 9:e95756. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, A. J., Shores, E. A., Trollor, J. N., Lee, T., and Sachdev, P. S. (2000). Neuropsychological functioning of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 22, 115–124. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200002)22:1;1-8;FT115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wendt, M., Luna-Rodriguez, A., and Jacobsen, T. (2012). Conflict-induced perceptual filtering. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 38, 675–686. doi: 10.1037/a0025902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, R., and Alain, C. (2000). Age-related decline in inhibitory control contributes to the increased Stroop effect observed in older adults. Psychophysiology 37, 179–189. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3720179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, G., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H.-C., and Fischer, M. (2008). On the cognitive link between space and number: a meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychol. Sci. Q. 50, 489–525.

Google Scholar

Zhang, H., and Kornblum, S. (1998). The effects of stimulus-response mapping and irrelevant stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus overlap in four-choice Stroop tasks with single-carrier stimuli. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24, 3–19. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, H. H., Zhang, J., and Kornblum, S. (1999). A parallel distributed processing model of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response compatibility. Cogn. Psychol. 38, 386–432. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, D. C., Zacks, R. T., and Slade, J. M. (2010). Brain activation during interference resolution in young and older adults: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 50, 810–817. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zmigrod, S., Zmigrod, L., and Hommel, B. (2016). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affects stimulus conflict but not response conflict. Neuroscience 322, 320–325. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.02.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zorzi, M., Bonato, M., Treccani, B., Scalambrin, G., Marenzi, R., and Priftis, K. (2012). Neglect impairs explicit processing of the mental number line. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:125. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zurrón, M., Pouso, M., Lindín, M., Galdo, S., and Díaz, F. (2009). Event-related potentials with the Stroop colour-word task: timing of semantic conflict. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 72, 246–252. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: SNARC effect, magnitude processing, interference control, Stroop effect, Flanker effect, individual differences

Citation: Georges C, Hoffmann D and Schiltz C (2018) Implicit and Explicit Number-Space Associations Differentially Relate to Interference Control in Young Adults With ADHD. Front. Psychol. 9:775. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00775

Received: 13 October 2017; Accepted: 01 May 2018;
Published: 24 May 2018.

Edited by:

Maciej Haman, University of Warsaw, Poland

Reviewed by:

Thomas Dresler, Universität Tübingen, Germany
Małgorzata Gut, Nicolaus Copernicus University in Toruń, Poland

Copyright © 2018 Georges, Hoffmann and Schiltz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carrie Georges, Y2FycmllLmdlb3JnZXNAdW5pLmx1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.