The Mental Lexicon, Blueprint of the Dictionaries of Tomorrow: Cognitive Aspects of the Lexicon

  • 4,533

    Total downloads

  • 27k

    Total views and downloads

About this Research Topic

Submission closed

Background

Whenever we speak, read or write we always use words which we had to acquire and organize to prevent their forgetting. Yet, none of this guarantees word finding, which is why lexicographers have built dictionaries. Alas, most of them list words in alphabetical order, which generally suites the reader, but hardly ever the writer. Indeed, how can they find in such a resource the form ‘panda’ while their mind sees only a ‘fluffy white bear with black patches around his eyes’?

To address this kind of problem it would be nice to have a lexicon built with the speaker/writer in mind. To achieve this goal, we need to integrate the author into the development cycle right from the start, and to account for diverse research findings regarding:

the human brain (modularity, parallelism, dynamism, high connectivity).
the mental lexicon: While meanings and forms are presented next to each other in symbolic form in paper dictionaries, they are decomposed and represented subsymbolically in our brain. The word’s critical parts (meaning, form, sound) are distributed across various layers, and their organization at the various levels is by affinity rather than by alphabetical order. These conclusions are clearly supported by empirical data (speech errors, brain scans, etc.). Given these different views concerning words and their organization it is not surprising to see that in one case (traditional dictionaries) words are retrieved as holistic entities, while in the other (brain) they are gradually activated.
performance factors affecting online and off-line processing, that is, word access in real time (incremental synthesis of word form), and deliberate search in a lexical resource (navigation, retrieval).
meta-knowledge: knowledge concerning the organization of the mental lexicon: proximity of source and target word; type of relation between the two, etc.)— and
knowledge states, i.e., the knowledge a user has at the onset of the search. Studies concerning the Tip-of-the-Tongue problem have shown that a user being in this state has only access to fragments of the final form, and that these fragments generally pertain to different levels (conceptual, syntactic, phonological).

To be more concrete, we ask how a deeper and broader understanding of the human mind and its search strategies might enable us to develop resources that allow authors to access a desired lexical form via fragmentary input, available at the moment of consultation, though probably activated at different points in time, hence pertaining to different levels (meaning, sound, associated words).

Indeed, lexicographers have already built resources corresponding to the various stages or layers described by psychologists. Yet these resources are not only incomplete, they are also static and not connected to each other. For example, it is extremely rare to see a resource allowing access via meaning ‘and’ sound. Yet, integrating into a single resource information coming from different levels (meaning, form, sound) would allow not only reduce the search space, but also to emulate parallelism. Knowledge of data of one level (say, meaning) may allow us to complete the missing parts of another (sound), hence contribute to the completion of the entire puzzle/picture.

This goal is ambitious, as it poses many problems not addressed in the past. For example, how to reconcile the fact that words are represented holistically in books and computers (dictionaries, electronic resources), while they are decomposed and subsymbolic in the human mind? No doubt, people need symbols at the interface level (input/output of dictionary consultation), and, they are often able to provide only incomplete data (fragments, and data lacking precision), while the machine (or the dictionary) generally expects entire words and precise information.

The scope of this Research Topic is thus two-fold:
1. Consider the lexicon from a cognitive point of view by and large. This is a broad view akin to CogALex (https://sites.google.com/view/cogalex-2020/home) standing for Cognitive Aspects of the Lexicon;
2. Consider the development of an ecosystem to support word finding (word access at the moment of speaking or writing)

This being so, we welcome articles addressing the problems raised in the CogALex workshop series as well as the following points:

(a) Given our goal, and to be fruitful for our discussion, what shall we put behind the term ‘mental lexicon’?
(b) How can we reconcile the fact that words are decomposed, hence subsymbolic, in the human brain, while humans can only provide and interpret symbolic information, which is why we have words in the lexicon?
(c) How to build a representative map of the mental lexicon for a given user group?
(d) Which corpora (knowledge graphs, BabelNet, ConceptNet) and which combinations have the best potential to yield a representative encyclopedic lexicon?
(e) How to achieve the right mix between encyclopedic and (collective) episodic knowledge?
(f) How to ‘dynamize’ the lexicon? The weight of the links between words is not frozen. During communication our focus shifts all the time. Likewise, any event (news) may have an impact on the evocation potential of a given word.
(g) Which lexical resources are relevant to support word finding? What layers of Levelt’s lexical access model are relevant, and to what extent do they need to be worked out? While we have a good understanding of the time course of lexical access, we still do lack details concerning the components at the various levels. For example, what are the specificities at the conceptual level? Which features typically get activated to evoke concepts like ‘justice’, ‘nationality’, ‘panda’?
(h) How to combine existing lexical resources to allow for their joined usage?

In sum, both theoretical/conceptual and empirical contributions are welcome.

Keywords: lexical resources, mental lexicon, lexical organization, word access, navigation, puzzle completion, meta-knowledge, knowledge states, tip of the tongue problem

Important note: All contributions to this Research Topic must be within the scope of the section and journal to which they are submitted, as defined in their mission statements. Frontiers reserves the right to guide an out-of-scope manuscript to a more suitable section or journal at any stage of peer review.

Frequently asked questions

  • Frontiers' Research Topics are collaborative hubs built around an emerging theme.Defined, managed, and led by renowned researchers, they bring communities together around a shared area of interest to stimulate collaboration and innovation.

    Unlike section journals, which serve established specialty communities, Research Topics are pioneer hubs, responding to the evolving scientific landscape and catering to new communities.

  • The goal of Frontiers' publishing program is to empower research communities to actively steer the course of scientific publishing. Our program was implemented as a three-part unit with fixed field journals, flexible specialty sections, and dynamically emerging Research Topics, connecting communities of different sizes and maturity.

    Research Topics originate from the scientific community. Many of our Research Topics are suggested by existing editorial board members who have identified critical challenges or areas of interest in their field.

  • As an editor, Research Topics will help you build your journal, as well as your community, around emerging, cutting-edge research. As research trailblazers, Research Topics attract high-quality submissions from leading experts all over the world.

    A thriving Research Topic can potentially evolve into a new specialty section if there is sustained interest and a growing community around it.

  • Each Research Topic must be approved by the specialty chief editor, and it falls under the editorial oversight of our editorial boards, supported by our in-house research integrity team. The same standards and rigorous peer review processes apply to articles published as part of a Research Topic as for any other article we publish.

    In 2023, 80% of the Research Topics we published were edited or co-edited by our editorial board members, who are already familiar with their journal's scope, ethos, and publishing model. All other topics are guest edited by leaders in their field, each vetted and formally approved by the specialty chief editor.

  • Publishing your article within a Research Topic with other related articles increases its discoverability and visibility, which can lead to more views, downloads, and citations. Research Topics grow dynamically as more published articles are added, causing frequent revisiting, and further visibility.

    As Research Topics are multidisciplinary, they are cross-listed in several fields and section journals – increasing your reach even more and giving you the chance to expand your network and collaborate with researchers in different fields, all focusing on expanding knowledge around the same important topic.

    Our larger Research Topics are also converted into ebooks and receive social media promotion from our digital marketing team.

  • Frontiers offers multiple article types, but it will depend on the field and section journals in which the Research Topic will be featured. The available article types for a Research Topic will appear in the drop-down menu during the submission process.

    Check available article types here 

  • Yes, we would love to hear your ideas for a topic. Most of our Research Topics are community-led and suggested by researchers in the field. Our in-house editorial team will contact you to talk about your idea and whether you’d like to edit the topic. If you’re an early-stage researcher, we will offer you the opportunity to coordinate your topic, with the support of a senior researcher as the topic editor. 

    Suggest your topic here 

  • A team of guest editors (called topic editors) lead their Research Topic. This editorial team oversees the entire process, from the initial topic proposal to calls for participation, the peer review, and final publications.

    The team may also include topic coordinators, who help the topic editors send calls for participation, liaise with topic editors on abstracts, and support contributing authors. In some cases, they can also be assigned as reviewers.

  • As a topic editor (TE), you will take the lead on all editorial decisions for the Research Topic, starting with defining its scope. This allows you to curate research around a topic that interests you, bring together different perspectives from leading researchers across different fields and shape the future of your field. 

    You will choose your team of co-editors, curate a list of potential authors, send calls for participation and oversee the peer review process, accepting or recommending rejection for each manuscript submitted.

  • As a topic editor, you're supported at every stage by our in-house team. You will be assigned a single point of contact to help you on both editorial and technical matters. Your topic is managed through our user-friendly online platform, and the peer review process is supported by our industry-first AI review assistant (AIRA).

  • If you’re an early-stage researcher, we will offer you the opportunity to coordinate your topic, with the support of a senior researcher as the topic editor. This provides you with valuable editorial experience, improving your ability to critically evaluate research articles and enhancing your understanding of the quality standards and requirements for scientific publishing, as well as the opportunity to discover new research in your field, and expand your professional network.

  • Yes, certificates can be issued on request. We are happy to provide a certificate for your contribution to editing a successful Research Topic.

  • Research Topics thrive on collaboration and their multi-disciplinary approach around emerging, cutting-edge themes, attract leading researchers from all over the world.

  • As a topic editor, you can set the timeline for your Research Topic, and we will work with you at your pace. Typically, Research Topics are online and open for submissions within a few weeks and remain open for participation for 6 – 12 months. Individual articles within a Research Topic are published as soon as they are ready.

    Find out more about our Research Topics

  • Our fee support program ensures that all articles that pass peer review, including those published in Research Topics, can benefit from open access – regardless of the author's field or funding situation.

    Authors and institutions with insufficient funding can apply for a discount on their publishing fees. A fee support application form is available on our website.

  • In line with our mission to promote healthy lives on a healthy planet, we do not provide printed materials. All our articles and ebooks are available under a CC-BY license, so you can share and print copies.