Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Virtual Real., 14 September 2021
Sec. Virtual Reality and Human Behaviour
This article is part of the Research Topic Simulating Virtual Humans and Crowds for Virtual Reality View all 4 articles

Creepy, but Persuasive: In a Virtual Consultation, Physician Bedside Manner, Rather than the Uncanny Valley, Predicts Adherence

  • Department of Human-Centered Computing, School of Informatics and Computing, Indiana University, Indianapolis, IN, United States

Care for chronic disease requires patient adherence to treatment advice. Nonadherence worsens health outcomes and increases healthcare costs. When healthcare professionals are in short supply, a virtual physician could serve as a persuasive technology to promote adherence. However, acceptance of advice may be hampered by the uncanny valley effect—a feeling of eeriness elicited by human simulations. In a hypothetical virtual doctor consultation, 441 participants assumed the patient’s role. Variables from the stereotype content model and the heuristic–systematic model were used to predict adherence intention and behavior change. This 2 × 5 between-groups experiment manipulated the doctor’s bedside manner—either good or poor—and virtual depiction at five levels of realism. These independent variables were designed to manipulate the doctor’s level of warmth and eeriness. In hypothesis testing, depiction had a nonsignificant effect on adherence intention and diet and exercise change, even though the 3-D computer-animated versions of the doctor (i.e., animation, swapped, and bigeye) were perceived as eerier than the others (i.e., real and cartoon). The low-warmth, high-eeriness doctor prompted heuristic processing of information, while the high-warmth doctor prompted systematic processing. This pattern contradicts evidence reported in the persuasion literature. For the stereotype content model, a path analysis found that good bedside manner increased the doctor’s perceived warmth significantly, which indirectly increased physical activity. For the heuristic–systematic model, the doctor’s eeriness, measured in a pretest, had no significant effect on adherence intention and physical activity, while good bedside manner increased both significantly. Surprisingly, cognitive perspective-taking was a stronger predictor of change in physical activity than adherence intention. Although virtual characters can elicit the uncanny valley effect, their effect on adherence intention and physical activity was comparable to a video of a real person. This finding supports the development of virtual consultations.

Introduction

There is growing interest in adopting virtual characters to persuade people to change their health-related behaviors. Strategies that people adopt to persuade others, such as arguments and social cues, could also be employed in virtual characters (André et al., 2011). This could offer a novel and efficient way to increase adherence to treatment advice and improve health literacy more generally. Consulting with conversational agents has been found to increase users’ physical activity (Yin et al., 2010) and guide older adults toward healthy behaviors (Looije et al., 2010). However, virtual characters, depending on their level of realism, could appear eerie—an effect called the uncanny valley (Mori, 2012). Thus, we must consider how eeriness influences persuasion in virtual characters.

Persuasion can be used socially to encourage behaviors that enhance health and discourage those that harm it (Umberson, 1987; Lewis and Rook, 1999). Adherence measures how much a patient’s behavior follows the program they have established with their healthcare provider, such as taking prescribed medications on time and making recommended lifestyle changes (McDonald et al., 2002). Adherence intention is the patient’s strength of determination to follow the program.

This work adopts as its use case scenario a virtual consultation involving a possible diabetes diagnosis. Diabetes and other chronic diseases pose a growing threat to global health. By 2030, the number of people with type II diabetes is projected to hit 578 million (Saeedi et al., 2019). Diabetes reduces life expectancy by 5 years, and its complications reduce quality of life (Zhuo et al., 2013). To stay healthy, diabetes patients must follow their doctors’ diet and exercise advice (Nelson et al., 2002). Although a sedentary lifestyle and poor dietary habits are risk factors, a healthy diet and regular exercise can delay or prevent type II diabetes and its associated health complications (Eriksson and Lindgärde, 1991; Tuomilehto et al., 2001; Rejeski et al., 2012; Sami et al., 2017).

Increasing adherence is a major public health challenge. In the United States, nonadherence to medication affects 40–50% of patients with chronic diseases like diabetes and hypertension, leading to 100,000 preventable deaths each year (Kleinsinger, 2018). For diabetic patients, nonadherence to medication increases adverse health outcomes, hospitalization, and mortality (Ho et al., 2006). The rate of nonadherence can exceed 70% when the physician advises significant or complex changes in behavior (Martin et al., 2005). Nonadherence results in increased hospitalization and mortality rates, especially in older adults (Walsh et al., 2019).

The cost of nonadherence to healthcare systems is staggering. In the United States, estimates ranged from $100 billion to $300 billion per year (Chisholm-Burns and Spivey, 2012; McGuire and Iuga, 2014; Morello and Hirsh 2017). Per patient, they ranged from $949 to $44,190 in 2015, depending on the disease (Cutler et al., 2018).

Physician bedside manner influences the physician–patient relationship, which, in turn, influences adherence (Miller, 1997; Safran et al., 1998; Ettner, 1999). For example, a poor physician–patient relationship lowered long-term adherence in patients with hypertension (Waeber et al., 2000). However, a European study found that a good physician–patient relationship encouraged patients to follow recommendations (Stavropoulou, 2011). A good relationship also improved adherence for HIV-positive patients (Schneider et al., 2004).

The United States faces a shortage of clinicians and other healthcare professionals (McKechnie, 2016; Kirch and Petelle, 2017; Marć et al., 2019). Researchers estimate that nearly 52,000 more primary care physicians will be needed by 2025 (Petterson et al., 2012). A virtual clinical consultation could address the scarcity of healthcare experts. However, creating credible virtual characters remains a challenge for researchers and designers, in part because factors like the uncanny valley could negatively influence the persuasiveness of their advice (McDonnell and Breidt, 2010; Wang et al., 2013).

The uncanny valley effect, proposed in 1970 by Mori (2012), is a negative affective reaction toward objects that imperfectly resemble human beings, such as android robots or computer-animated characters. A large meta-analysis has confirmed the effect (Diel et al., 2022), which could hinder the acceptance of virtual characters and their advice. The uncanny valley effect can occur when a character’s features are atypical, appear less real than others, or deviate from a familiar configuration (Chattopadhyay and MacDorman, 2016; Diel and MacDorman, 2021). This can elicit in viewers a feeling of eeriness and revulsion. For example, eeriness could be elicited by atypical proportions in a photorealistic human face (Green et al., 2008), such as enlarged eyes, or inconsistent realism among facial features (MacDorman et al., 2009; Stein and Ohler, 2018). Eeriness has been considered detrimental to persuasion because of its negative effect on a virtual character’s credibility (Patel and MacDorman, 2015). Increasing a human character’s overall realism makes subtle nonhuman imperfections in its features more noticeable and disturbing (McDonnell et al., 2012). Different stylizations of computer-generated characters could affect the perception of eeriness, trustworthiness, attractiveness, and realism (Zell et al., 2015; Schindler et al., 2017).

MacDorman (2019) proposed that the uncanny valley effect could disrupt feelings of empathy for a virtual character. It could, therefore, have a greater effect on a high-warmth character than a low-warmth character because a high-warmth character elicits more empathy. Thus, in a virtual doctor’s consultation, it would be useful to examine both realism and bedside manner to check for interaction effects.

Hypotheses

Virtual characters have been found to be at least as persuasive as real people (Bickmore et al., 2009; Dai and MacDorman, 2018; Ogawa et al., 2018). Their ability to persuade has been used to improve patient adherence (Brown et al., 2016; Richards and Caldwell, 2016). Thus, we hypothesize that (H1) the depiction of the virtual physician affects adherence. In our previous results, the animated physician was more persuasive than a video recording of a real person because interacting with the animated character was more enjoyable (Dai and MacDorman, 2018).

In the literature on persuasion, the heuristic–systematic model (HSM) explains how people process information (Chaiken, 1980; Chaiken and Eagly, 1983; Chen and Chaiken, 1999; Todorov, Chaiken et al., 2002). Systematic processing demands cognitive effort to understand and evaluate the content of a message. Thus, for systematic processing, message-related thoughts mediate persuasion. By contrast, heuristic processing relies on rules of thumb or peripheral cues, which reduce cognitive effort. For heuristic processing, persuasion is mediated by thoughts unrelated to the message, such as features of the message’s source. These could include, for example, a speaker’s interpersonal warmth, physical attractiveness, novelty, and realism (Miller et al., 1976; Chaiken, 1980).

People may pay more attention to a virtual character than a real human because its visual novelty stimulates curiosity, discovery, and learning (Sokolov, 1963; Patel et al., 2014). For example, people stared longer at the faces of eerie digital human characters than real humans (Carter et al., 2013). Virtual environments have used behavioral measures like proximity (i.e., the minimum distance between a person and a virtual character). People moved closer to eerie, zombielike virtual characters and paid more attention to them than to more realistic characters (Zibrek et al., 2018). Thus, we hypothesize that by increasing the heuristic processing of information, (H2a) the high-eeriness virtual physician elicits more thoughts related to the message source than the low-eeriness virtual physician and that (H2b) the low-eeriness virtual physician elicits more thoughts related to the message itself than the high-eeriness physician.

Fiske and colleagues proposed the stereotype content model (SCM), which describes the formation of interpersonal impressions and group stereotypes on two dimensions: warmth and competence (Fiske et al., 2002). Warmth encompasses traits indicating whether a person intends to help or harm, such as the presence or absence of friendliness, helpfulness, morality, sincerity, and trustworthiness. Competence encompasses traits indicating the ability or inability to act on that intention, such as the presence or absence of creativity, efficacy, intelligence, and skill (Fiske et al., 2002, 2007). A person viewed as warm and competent elicits positive emotions and behaviors, whereas a person viewed as lacking these traits elicits negative emotions and behaviors. These responses confer selective advantage to individuals and groups. Warmth is the primary dimension of interpersonal perception. It accounts for 53% of the variance in global impressions, while competence accounts for 29% (Wojciszke et al., 1998). Warmth and competence have been used to explain interpersonal and intergroup social cognition, especially as related to competition and status.

The physician’s warmth helps support the physician–patient relationship (Buller and Street, 1992). In clinical settings, it is exhibited by such affiliative behavior as attention to patients, empathy, and kindness. Affiliative behavior motivates patient adherence in real (Willson and McNamara, 1982; Kim et al., 2004) and virtual contexts (Dai and MacDorman, 2018). However, affiliative behavior does not ensure that patients will continue to follow the recommended treatment (O’Hair, 1986; Street, 1990; Buller and Street, 1992). We hypothesize (H3) greater adherence to the advice of a high-warmth physician than a low-warmth physician.

Warmth and competence are positively correlated when patients interact with physicians in clinical settings (Kraft-Todd et al., 2017). A rude, unfriendly physician may attract more attention than a polite, friendly one. Moreover, sarcasm and dark humor could make communication more entertaining and memorable (Ziegele and Jost, 2020). We hypothesize that (H4a) the low-warmth virtual physician elicits more source-related thoughts than the high-warmth virtual physician by encouraging the heuristic processing of information, and (H4b) the high-warmth virtual physician elicits more message-related thoughts than the low-warmth virtual physician by encouraging the systematic processing of information.

Our assumption may appear to run counter to findings in the persuasion literature, namely that warm, competent, or attractive sources of information elicit more heuristic processing than cold, incompetent, or unattractive ones (Chaiken, 1980, 1987; DeBono and Harnish, 1988; Wood and Kallgren, 1988). According to HSM, if a source is judged positively, less cognitive effort must be expended to evaluate the message. We justify our assumption based on the context of the clinical scenario: Patients expect their doctor to be warm and competent by default. This expectation is implied by the Hippocratic Oath. Behavior deviating from it is surprising, thus drawing attention away from the message to its source.

People’s attitudes shape their intentions, which, in turn, predict behavior change (Austin and Vancouver, 1996; Abraham et al., 1998; Webb and Sheeran, 2006; Montano and Kasprzyk, 2015). In general, a shift in attitudes resulting solely from heuristic processing will be less stable and less resistant to counterarguments and have less of an effect on behavior change than a shift resulting from systematic processing (Chaiken, 1980). Prochaska and DiClemente (1983) proposed the transtheoretical model of behavior change, which has been used to increase physical activity (Prochaska et al., 1994; Prochaska et al., 2015; Stonerock and Blumenthal, 2017). In their model, adherence intention is a major predictor of behavior change (Austin and Vancouver, 1996; Abraham et al., 1998; Webb and Sheeran, 2006; Montano and Kasprzyk, 2015).

This study investigates the effect of bedside manner on people’s behavior change in diet and physical activity. Good bedside manner in a virtual physician has been found to increase patients’ adherence and satisfaction (Schmid Mast et al., 2007; Dai and MacDorman, 2018). Based on these findings, we hypothesize that (H5a) the consultation with the high-warmth physician will improve diet and (H5b) physical activity more than the consultation with the low-warmth physician. Based on the literature on adherence, we hypothesize that (H6a) those reporting greater adherence intention after the virtual consultation will improve their diet and (H6b) increase their physical activity more than others.

To the best of our knowledge, the effect of a virtual character on persuasion has not yet been fully explored in a virtual clinical setting. In particular, the extent to which the physician’s warmth and eeriness affect heuristic and systematic processes and how these processes affect adherence intention and behavior change has not been considered. Our experiment manipulated a virtual doctor’s bedside manner and depiction to determine how these variables influence perceived warmth and eeriness, heuristic and systematic processing, adherence intention, and behavior change. We further explored these variables of the stereotype content model and the heuristic–systematic model in a path analysis to predict adherence intention and behavior change. Thus, our overarching research question is this: How does a physician’s bedside manner and depiction influence adherence intention and behavior change through the stereotype content model and heuristic–systematic model?

Addressing Threats to Validity

Our previous study examined how bedside manner, outcome (i.e., being awarded a fellowship or sued for malpractice), and virtual depiction affected adherence intention in a virtual clinical scenario (Dai and MacDorman, 2018). Its findings revealed two main threats to interval validity resulting from the experimental design. First, poor bedside manner caused the doctor to be rated significantly higher in eeriness than good bedside manner. The eeriness index was not designed as a posttest for comparing characters whose responses differ greatly in warmth or other traits between scenarios. To eliminate the effect of bedside manner on eeriness, this study measured the doctor’s eeriness in a neutral setting as a pretest (see Procedure). The second threat to internal validity was that our previous study failed to separate the computer-animated doctor’s positive effect on consultation enjoyment and adherence intention from the uncanny valley’s negative effect. To address this threat, this study used multiple versions of the computer-animated doctor to measure eeriness. Finally, our previous study did not measure behavior change. This study measured dietary and exercise change at least 1 week after the virtual consultation.

Methods

The experiment centered on an online virtual consultation. Participants took on the role of a patient in a doctor’s consultation regarding a possible diabetes diagnosis. The virtual doctor was manipulated by bedside manner and depiction. Bedside manner had two stimulus conditions: either good or poor. Depiction had five stimulus conditions that varied in their level of realism: cartoon, bigeye, swapped, animation, and real (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. The doctor in the experiment was presented with five styles of depiction. These were, from left to right, (A) cartoon, (B) bigeye, (C) swapped, (D) animation, and (E) real. Real was a video of a real person. Animation was a 3-D computer animation. Swapped was a face replacement of animation using deep learning. Bigeye was also a face replacement but with eyes enlarged 50%. Cartoon was a cartoon version of animation. The props and background for bigeye and swapped were identical to animation.

These five conditions were selected to manipulate the virtual physician’s level of eeriness to measure the effect of eeriness on adherence intention and behavior change, as mediated by the stereotype content model and the heuristic–systematic model. The real condition used a video of a male actor playing the part of the doctor. This video was used as a reference by a professional animator to create the animation condition. A realistic computer model was first developed from high-resolution reference photos of the human actor. In a previous study, the same model had been rated significantly eerier than the real actor (Patel and MacDorman, 2015).

The animation condition served as a reference for the remaining three conditions. We created a cartoon character as a low-eeriness condition with flat shading and fewer textural features. This approach has been used to avoid the uncanny valley in game development (McDonnell and Breidt, 2010; McDonnell et al., 2012). FaceSwap, a deep learning neural network program, was used to generate characters with a swapped face (swapped) and a swapped face with eyes enlarged 50% (bigeye). The bigeye condition was motivated by the fact that viewers are prone to be sensitive to atypical features in human faces, especially when they involve the eyes (Kätsyri et al., 2015; Schein and Gray, 2015). Increasing the size of the eyes or making their level of realism inconsistent with other facial features causes the face to appear eerier (MacDorman et al., 2009; Seyama and Nagayama, 2009; MacDorman and Chattopadhyay, 2016; Feng et al., 2018).

Participants

Undergraduate and graduate students randomly selected from a public university system in the Midwestern United States comprised the sample. Each was randomly assigned to one of 10 groups.

A total of 441 participants completed the experiment and first survey (72% female, n = 318) with between 36 and 52 in each group. Among those participants, 329 completed a second follow-up survey, with between 28 and 38 in each group (73% female, n = 244). One participant’s responses were removed for not completing the thought-listing task. Participants ranged in age from 18 to 70 (Mdn = 22, IQR = [19, 28]), and most had grown up in the United States (89%, n = 394).

Power Analysis

The power analysis aimed to determine how many participants each condition would require for the effect of depiction on pretest Eeriness to be sufficient to have a significant effect on Adherence Intention. In MacDorman et al. (2009), a 50% increase in eye size significantly increased eeriness, m1 = −1.60, SD1 = 1.22, m2 = 1.37, SD2 = 1.23, ηp2 ≈ 0.65. To consider related constructs, Feng et al., (2018) found that a 25 and 50% increase in eye size significantly decreased how often a face was preferred, ηp2 = 0.35. Seyama and Nagayama (2009) found that a 50% increase in eye size decreased pleasantness significantly, F (5, 195) = 49.73, ηp2 = 0.56, p < 0.001.

In preparation for this experiment, 30 participants rated a virtual physician on Eeriness. The virtual physician was presented at three levels of realism in 20-s video clips. At the lowest level, the eyes were enlarged 50%. The effect of realism on Eeriness was significant and large, F (2, 58) = 15.34, ηp2 = 0.35, p < 0.001. In Dai and MacDorman (2018), bedside manner had a significant effect on Warmth, F (1, 214) = 193.99, ηp2 = 0.48, p < 0.001, and a significant effect on Adherence Intention, F (1, 730) = 282.86, ηp2 = 0.28, p < 0.001. Based on these results, we assumed that the effect of increasing eye size 50% on pretest Eeriness and Adherence Intention would be similar in magnitude to the effect of bedside manner on Warmth and Adherence Intention. For a 0.05 alpha level and an effect size set conservatively at ηp2 = 0.15, a two-way between-subjects ANOVA with two levels for the first factor and five for the second factor would require 36 participants in each of the 10 groups to achieve power of at least 0.90 (bedside manner: n = 180, power > 0.99, depiction: n = 72, power > 0.91).

Research Design

The experiment adopted a 2 × 5 between-groups posttest design, although the doctor’s perceived Eeriness was also pretested. The 10 treatment groups had five different stylizations of the virtual doctor, who had either good or poor bedside manner.

Procedure

Participants first completed informed consent. To measure the eeriness of the virtual physician, Dr. Richards, independently from the bedside manner manipulation, participants rated the doctor on Eeriness in a short, 7-s clip. Dr. Richards was presented on the screen holding a clipboard. He wore a white lab coat over a white shirt with a red tie.

Participants were then introduced to the doctor–patient scenario and their role as patient. They began the consultation with a test result indicating higher-than-normal blood sugar.

Eight hypothetical doctor–patient interactions comprised the virtual consultation. For all but the last, the participant chose a preferred question from four options. For experimental control, the doctor provided the same response, regardless of the question selected, and the questions and responses were designed to create the impression of a logically flowing conversation. The doctor closed the consultation by providing the patient with a recommendation concerning diet and exercise in audio and text.

Next, the participant completed the indices (see Dependent variables) and a demographics questionnaire. The indices were presented in the following order: a thought-listing task; a fully randomized block of scales composed of human realism, eeriness, warmth, and competence; cognitive perspective-taking; adherence intention; diet; and physical activity. After at least 1 week, the diet and physical activity indices were administered a second time to measure behavior change.

The experiment was conducted from October 10 to November 8, 2019. The median duration of the experiment and first survey was 21 min 39 s (25th percentile = 18 min 2 s, 75th percentile = 29 min 35 s). For the virtual consultation, the duration of the physician’s part of the physician–patient interactions was 3 min 26 s for the physician with good bedside manner and 2 seconds longer for the physician with poor bedside manner. The average duration of the user’s part was 2 min 18 s for a total virtual consultation time of 5 min 45 s.

Independent Variables

The independent variables were bedside manner (good or poor) and depiction (five levels of realism). Depiction was recoded as real (two levels) and 3-D animation (two levels) for planned contrasts. A transcript of the interactions including the bedside manner manipulation and diet and exercise recommendation is provided in Supplementary Material. All videos including the bedside manner and depiction manipulations are available at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.13337267.

Bedside Manner

Bedside manner had two conditions (i.e., good or poor), which were designed to exhibit high warmth or low warmth, respectively. The doctor’s bedside manner was depicted mainly via dialogue (40% of the total), nonverbal expressions, and gestures. The high-warmth doctor treated the patient with care, empathy, and patience, responded to questions positively, provided reassurance and emotional support, and expressed a willingness to be available. By contrast, the low-warmth doctor was rude and impatient, joked about the patient’s diagnosis, and tried to end the consultation early. The low-warmth doctor showed little empathy for the patient, interacted paternalistically, and implied that the patient’s questions were ill-informed. These are the same qualities encountered in real physicians with good and poor bedside manner (Hickson, 1994; Levinson et al., 1997; Schmid Mast et al., 2007).

Depiction

The depiction of the character was presented in five different rendering styles: cartoon, bigeye, swapped, animation, and real. The five styles were a video of a real person, a 3-D computer animation modeled on the video using Maya and ZBrush, a software-generated cartoon version of the animation, and face replacements of the animation generated by FaceSwap, which contained two versions (i.e., the face replacement with an animated face, and the same face with eyes enlarged 50%). Apart from character stylization, all five high-warmth conditions had the same narrative content, and all five low-warmth conditions had the same narrative content. The doctor’s mouth movements were synchronized to his voice.

Dependent Variables

Indices were used to measure the dependent variables. They are discussed below and listed in the Supplementary Material. Source- and message-related thoughts were determined by two coders who labeled and counted each participant’s thoughts. Dietary and exercise change were quantified by the participant who counted servings of food or hours of activity during the prior week. All other indices were composed of semantic differential scales (e.g., strongly agreestrongly disagree or creepy–ordinary). Each scale was depicted with a horizontal bar with a term on one end and its antonym on the other (see Reips and Funke, 2008; Funke and Reips, 2012). For each scale, the participant used a slider to position an indicator on the bar. The indicator’s position was recorded as a decimal value between –1.00 and 1.00.

Human Realism and Eeriness

To evaluate the appearance of the message source, Dr. Richards’ human realism was measured using the combined scales of the realism and humanness indices; eeriness was measured using the eeriness index’s eerie subindex (Ho and MacDorman, 2017).

Warmth and Competence

Dr. Richards’ perceived warmth and competence were measured using a warmth index (Ho and MacDorman, 2010) and a competence index (McCroskey and Teven, 1999).

Source- and Message-Related Thoughts

A thought-listing task was used to analyze the heuristic and systematic processing of information for persuasion (Chaiken, 1980; Pallak et al., 1983; Koh and Sundar, 2010). Chaiken and Maheswaran (1994) used the number of source-related thoughts to measure heuristic processing and the number of message-related thoughts to measure systematic processing. In the experiment, participants listed their thoughts about consultation content and Dr. Richards’ appearance. They were given 3 min and 10 text input boxes to record their thoughts. Two coders, blind to the conditions, independently assigned each thought to one of seven categories: positive, neutral, or negative source-related, positive, neutral, or negative health-related, or other. An index for source-related thoughts was calculated by adding up the number of thoughts in this category. The process was repeated for message-related thoughts, which were further divided into negative and nonnegative thoughts.

Cognitive Perspective-Taking

Participants’ ability to imagine Dr. Richards’ mental state (Davis, 1983; Leslie, 1987; Frith, 2001) was measured using a cognitive perspective-taking index designed for the virtual consultation’s specific clinical scenario (Dai and MacDorman, 2018). The index was composed of statements about Dr. Richards’ mental state paired with the semantic differential scale strongly agreestrongly disagree. Either strongly agree or strongly disagree corresponded to the correct answer. In that sense, the index was a disguised test. This variable was included as a second measure of information processing (DeBono and Harnish, 1988).

Adherence Intention

The intention to adhere to the doctor’s advice was measured using an adherence intention index designed for the clinical scenario (Dai and MacDorman, 2018).

Dietary Change and Exercise Change

Behavior change was measured by a survey on diet and physical activities, inspired by a lifestyle change survey for diabetic patients (Chong et al., 2017). Participants reported their consumption of vegetables, fruits, fatty and processed foods, and foods with added sugar for the prior week. These four items comprised a diet index. They also reported how many hours they spent walking and doing moderate and vigorous physical activities for the prior week. These three items comprised an index on physical activity. The indices were administered immediately after the experiment and at least 1 week later. The difference between their first and second administration constitutes the dietary change and exercise change indices.

Data Analysis

For reliability thresholds, Cronbach’s α was interpreted with acceptable = 0.7, good = 0.8, and excellent = 0.9. For effect size thresholds, Cohen’s κ was interpreted with fair = 0.21, moderate = 0.41, substantial = 0.61, and perfect = 0.81 (Cohen, 1960; Landis and Koch, 1977), and partial eta squared (ηp2) with small = 0.01, medium = 0.06, and large = 0.14 (Cohen, 1973).

The cognitive perspective-taking index was an unweighted average of its scales. To maximize variance explained, all other indices were averages of their respective scales weighted by their component loadings.

Test statistics were interpreted at the significance level p ≤ 0.05. For the manipulation checks and directional hypotheses, planned contrasts were one-tailed. All other tests were two-tailed. Linear mixed-effects models were fitted by maximum-likelihood estimation. Mixed-effects models were used instead of ANOVA and ANCOVA because they account for variability among groups. Planned contrasts were orthogonal and used type III sum of squares.

For path models, the standard for acceptable global fit required meeting the following criteria: p > 0.05 for model χ2, the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) ε^ ≤ 0.08 (MacCallum et al., 1996), the lower bound of its confidence interval ε^L = 0, the comparative fit index (CFI) ≥ 0.95, and the standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR) ≤ 0.08 (Hu and Bentler, 1999). The standard for acceptable local fit was a significant p-value for a parameter estimate representing a direct effect between variables.

Results

Reliability and Psychometrics of Indices

All indices were reliable (0.72 ≤ α ≤ 0.93). Item 7 was removed from the adherence intention index to increase its reliability.

In the thought-listing task, the two coders had substantial agreement in assigning one of the seven labels to each thought, κ = 0.73, agreeing on the labels for 75% of the thoughts. Labeling discrepancies were resolved through discussion.

The psychometric properties of the final indices are listed in Table 1. The initial indices and their factor loadings appear in the Supplementary Material. Correlations among independent and dependent variables (indices) are listed in Figure 2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Psychometric properties of the dependent variables.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Pearson’s correlation between the dependent and independent variables. All correlations were significant except those indicated by white text on a gray background.

Manipulation Checks

A two-way MANOVA indicated a significant effect of bedside manner, Pillai’s trace = 0.49, F (4, 432) = 105.00, p < 0.001, and depiction, V = 0.19, F (16, 1740) = 5.28, p < 0.001, on the manipulation check variables. Two-way mixed-effects models confirmed that bedside manner × depiction had only nonsignificant interaction effects on Human Realism, pretest Eeriness, Eeriness, Warmth, and Competence. Mixed-effects models with planned contrasts were used for the manipulation checks.

After controlling for the effect of depiction, bedside manner had a nonsignificant effect on Human Realism and pretest Eeriness, but a significant effect on posttest Eeriness, Warmth, and Competence (Table 2, n = 441). Planned contrasts revealed that the doctor with good bedside manner was rated significantly less eerie, warmer, and more competent than the doctor with poor bedside manner.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Mixed-effects model of bedside manner and depiction on Human Realism, pretest Eeriness, Eeriness, Warmth, and Competence with planned contrasts.

After controlling for the effect of bedside manner, depiction had a significant effect on Human Realism and pretest Eeriness, but a nonsignificant effect on posttest Eeriness, Warmth, and Competence. The fact that bedside manner, not depiction, had a significant effect on posttest Eeriness indicates that not only is bedside manner a confounding variable, but that it renders nonsignificant the effect of depiction.

A planned contrast revealed that the real human doctor (i.e., real) was rated significantly higher in Human Realism than the others (i.e., cartoon, bigeye, swapped, and animation). Another planned contrast revealed that 3-D animation (i.e., bigeye, swapped, and animation) was rated significantly higher in pretest Eeriness than the others (i.e., cartoon, real). Figure 3 plots pretest Eeriness by depiction. Increasing levels of realism reproduced the characteristic U-shape of the uncanny valley (Mori, 2012; Ho and MacDorman, 2017).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Mean and 95% confidence interval of pretest Eeriness (reverse scaled) by depiction.

Women have been found to be more responsive than men to specific persuasion strategies and scenarios (MacDorman et al., 2009; Orji et al., 2015). Their satisfaction is also more likely to increase when the physician exhibits good bedside manner (Schmid Mast et al., 2007). Thus, to ensure the generalizability of our results given that the participants were 72% female, we tested the effect of participant gender on all dependent variables used in the manipulation checks and in hypothesis testing. A MANOVA revealed that the effect of gender on these variables was nonsignificant.

Hypothesis Testing

A two-way mixed-effects model confirmed that bedside manner × depiction had only nonsignificant interaction effects on Adherence Intention, number of source- and message-related thoughts, Dietary Change, and Exercise Change. Mixed-effects models with planned contrasts were used in hypothesis testing. The results are shown in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Mixed-effects model of bedside manner and depiction on Adherence Intention, number of source- and message-related thoughts, Dietary Change, and Exercise Change with planned contrasts.

H1 states that the physician’s depiction affects the intention to adhere to his advice. After controlling for the effect of bedside manner, depiction had a nonsignificant effect on Adherence Intention. Thus, H1 was not supported.

H2a states that the high-eeriness physician elicits more source-related thoughts than the low-eeriness physician. High eeriness is operationalized as 3-D animation (i.e., bigeye, swapped, and animation) and low eeriness as cartoon and real. After controlling for the effect of bedside manner, depiction had a significant effect on the number of source-related thoughts. A planned contrast revealed that 3-D animation elicited significantly more source-related thoughts than cartoon and real (Table 3; Figure 4). Thus, H2a was supported.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Mean and 95% confidence interval of the number of source-related thoughts and number of message-related thoughts by bedside manner and depiction.

H2b states that the low-eeriness physician elicits more message-related thoughts than the high-eeriness physician. After controlling for the effect of bedside manner, depiction had a nonsignificant effect on the number of message-related thoughts. Thus, H2b was unsupported.

H3 states that patients would be more likely to adhere to the advice of the high-warmth physician than the low-warmth physician. After controlling for the effect of depiction, bedside manner had a significant effect on Adherence Intention. A planned contrast revealed that Adherence Intention was significantly higher for the doctor with good bedside manner (Table 3; Figure 5). Thus, H3 was supported.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Mean and 95% confidence interval of Adherence Intention by beside manner and depiction.

H4a states that the low-warmth physician elicits more source-related thoughts than the high-warmth physician. After controlling for the effect of depiction, bedside manner had a significant effect on the number of source-related thoughts. A planned contrast revealed that the doctor with poor bedside manner elicited significantly more source-related thoughts. Thus, H4a was supported.

H4b states that the high-warmth physician elicits more message-related thoughts than a low-warmth physician. After controlling for the effect of depiction, bedside manner had a significant effect on the number of message-related thoughts. A planned contrast revealed that the doctor with good bedside manner elicited significantly more message-related thoughts. Thus, H4b was supported.

H5a states that the consultation with the high-warmth physician will improve diet more than the consultation with the low-warmth physician, and H5b states that it will also improve physical activity more than the consultation with the low-warmth physician. To operationalize behavior change, diet and exercise activity were measured immediately after the consultation and at least 1 week later. After controlling for the effect of depiction, bedside manner had a nonsignificant effect on Dietary Change (Table 3). However, after controlling for the effect of depiction, bedside manner had a significant effect on Exercise Change. A planned contrast revealed that good bedside manner increased Exercise Change more than poor bedside manner. Thus, although H5a was not supported, H5b was supported.

H6a states that those reporting greater adherence intention after the virtual consultation will improve their diet more than others, and H6b states that they will also increase their physical activity more than others. After controlling for the effect of bedside manner and depiction, Adherence Intention had a nonsignificant effect on Dietary Change and Exercise Change. Thus, neither H6a nor H6b were supported.

Regression Analysis

A regression analysis revealed that 3-D animation was a nonsignificant predictor of Adherence Intention and Exercise Change. Bedside manner was a significant predictor of Adherence Intention with a medium-to-large effect size, β = 0.48, t (439) = 11.39, p < 0.001, and explained a significant proportion of the variance, R2 = 0.23, adj. R2 = 0.23, F (1, 439) = 129.67. Bedside manner was a significant predictor of Exercise Change with a small effect size, β = 0.11, t (326) = 2.05, p = 0.042, and explained a significant proportion of the variance, R2 = 0.01, adj. R2 = 0.01, F (1, 326) = 4.18.

Path Analysis

We next develop path models to address our overarching research question: How does a physician’s bedside manner and depiction influence adherence intention and behavior change through the stereotype content model and heuristic–systematic model?

The stereotype content model predicts that adherence intention and behavior change will be greater for the doctor perceived as having more interpersonal warmth because warmth indicates a willingness to help. Moreover, the uncanny valley effect predicts that a 3-D animated doctor elicits the perception of greater eeriness and less warmth than a real or cartoon doctor, which could decrease adherence intention and behavior change, owing to an aversive response. Hypothesis testing confirmed that over the course of a week, the doctor with good bedside manner increased participant’s physical activity significantly more than the doctor with poor bedside manner.

In a path model, a direct effect is a hypothesized directional relation between two variables, represented by an arrow. The relation’s strength is a parameter estimated by model identification. If no relation is hypothesized, there is no arrow, and the parameter is fixed to zero. By the parsimony principle, model specification begins with the simplest model with the highest priority effect and proceeds by adding the next highest priority effects incrementally (Kline, 2016).

We start with Exercise Change—the effect we seek to predict. Among the dependent variables, only cognitive perspective-taking had a significant direct effect on Exercise Change. Adherence Intention and Warmth were added in Model 1, followed by Competence in Model 2, and Warmth’s direct effect on Competence in Model 3. In Model 4—our final SCM model—the independent variables bedside manner and 3-D animation had significant direct effects on Warmth (Figure 6).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 6. Model 4: Path model of Exercise Change with SCM variables. The blue arrow indicates a positive direct effect, and the red arrow indicates a negative direct effect. Next to each arrow, the standardized estimate of the parameter (β, γ, ζ, φ, ψ) indicates the strength of the direct effect, and the number of asterisks indicates its significance level: *p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01, and ***p ≤ 0.001. The independent variables bedside manner (good or poor) and 3-D animation (two levels: either bigeye, swapped, and animation or cartoon and real) are the exogenous variables. The model had acceptable local and global fit (Table 4).

Models 1–4 had acceptable local fit (p ≤ 0.05 for β's and γ's). Table 4 shows that Models 1–4 also had acceptable global fit (p > 0.05 for model χ2, RMSEA ε^ ≤ 0.08 and ε^L = 0, CFI ≥ 0.95, and SRMR ≤ 0.08). Model 4 shows that bedside manner had a much stronger direct effect on Warmth than 3-D animation. An alternative model, Model 4a, inserts pretest Eeriness between 3-D animation and Warmth (Figure 7). Pretest Eeriness had a significant but negligible direct effect on Warmth.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Global fit statistics for path models of Exercise Change.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 7. Model 4a: Path model of Exercise Change with SCM variables and pretest Eeriness. Pretest Eeriness was incorporated into Model 4. The model had acceptable local and global fit (Table 4).

The heuristic–systematic model predicts that the high-eeriness doctor is processed more heuristically, eliciting more source-related thoughts owing to its visual novelty. By contrast, the low-eeriness doctor is processed more systematically, eliciting more message-related thoughts. Thus, the low-eeriness doctor is predicted to promote adherence more because persuasion increases more and endures longer with the number and favorability of message-related thoughts (Greenwald, 1968). As before, we proceed by adding significant direct effects, this time to Model 4. Model 7—our final hybrid model—had acceptable local and global fit (Figure 8; Table 4).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 8. Model 7: Path model of Exercise Change with both SCM and HSM variables. The number of source-related thoughts and message-related thoughts were incorporated into Model 4.

Based on the heuristic–systematic model, we found that number of source-related thoughts had a negative direct effect on Adherence Intention. We wanted to explore these effects in more detail but could not add more free parameters given the sample size. The N:q rule stipulates that the sample size should exceed the number of free parameters by a factor of at least 20 (Jackson, 2003). Given 441 participants, this set the upper bound on the number of free parameters at 22. So, we removed variables related to the stereotype content model, and instead focused on the heuristic–systematic model, dividing source- and message-related thoughts into negative or nonnegative ones. Model 11 was our final model using HSM variables (Figure 9). The model had acceptable local and global fit (Table 4). Model 11 shows that good bedside manner had a strong negative direct effect on number of negative source-related thoughts, which had a negative direct effect on Adherence Intention.

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 9. Model 11: Path model of Exercise Change with HSM variables. The model includes the number of negative and nonnegative source-related thoughts and the number of nonnegative message-related thoughts.

Discussion

This research investigates how a character’s warmth and eeriness in a virtual clinical scenario impact adherence intention and behavior change. The 3-D computer-animated versions of the doctor in the virtual consultation had a nonsignificant effect on adherence intention (H1) and change in physical activity as compared with the real and cartoon versions; however, the doctor’s bedside manner significantly increased adherence intention (H3) and physical activity (H5b). These results indicate that a good bedside manner, which is perceived as warm and competent, has a much stronger effect on persuasion than the uncanny valley effect.

In the path analysis of Model 4, derived from the stereotype content model’s variables, 3-D animation slightly decreased warmth, while good bedside manner increased warmth significantly with a large effect size. This in turn increased perceived competence, adherence intention, cognitive perspective-taking, and physical activity (Figure 6). In Model 11, derived from the heuristic–systematic model’s variables, 3-D animation significantly increased negative source-related thoughts with a small effect size, which significantly decreased adherence intention, while good bedside manner significantly increased adherence intention (Figure 9).

We found roughly comparable adherence intention and exercise change in a virtual consultation using a 3-D computer animation as using a cartoon or real human, even though the 3-D rendering styles elicited the uncanny valley effect. The effect of the doctor’s bedside manner washed out that of the uncanny valley to such an extent that the effect of depiction on eeriness was nonsignificant in the posttest. This bodes well for the acceptance of virtual characters in clinical settings as compared with videos of real humans. Virtual characters have the added advantage of efficiency and controllability (Johnson et al., 2000; Kenny et al., 2007; Bickmore et al., 2010b). Interactions can be scripted and subsequently enhanced by artificial intelligence (AI) systems like IBM Watson Health. It would be worthwhile to determine a virtual character’s effect on persuasion in other clinical scenarios.

Adherence intention, however, was not an ideal predictor of exercise change. We found that cognitive perspective-taking instead directly predicted exercise change. Cognitive perspective-taking measured participants’ ability to infer the doctor’s mental state, including his thoughts, feelings, and beliefs. Most behavior change theories, including the most popular ones, share the idea that intention determines behavior. These include the theory of planned behavior, the theory of reasoned action, theories of attitude–behavior relations, goal theories, models of health behavior, and the technology acceptance model and its extensions (Ajzen, 1991; Eagly and Chaiken, 1993; Austin and Vancouver, 1996; Conner and Norman, 1996; Gollwitzer and Moskowitz, 1996; Abraham et al., 1998; Maddux, 1999; Hale et al., 2002; Webb and Sheeran, 2006; Tamilmani et al., 2020). In the path models, however, adherence intention only predicted exercise change indirectly, mediated by cognitive perspective-taking.

SCM variables like warmth and competence were far more predictive of adherence intention than HSM variables like the number of source- and message-related thoughts. This is surprising because SCM is not a theory of persuasion.

Source credibility (McGuire, 1978), which is determined by the source’s trustworthiness and competence (Kelman and Hovland, 1953), is crucial for persuasion (Petty and Wegener, 1998; Pornpitakpan, 2004; Patel and MacDorman, 2015). Positive and neutral thoughts increase persuasion, while negative thoughts reduce it (Greenwald, 1968; Petty et al., 2014). Thus, source- and message-related thoughts should increase persuasion as long as they are not negative (Chaiken, 1980; Chen and Chaiken, 1999; Griskevicius et al., 2010). Model 11 aligns with this characterization (Figure 9).

The doctor with poor bedside manner elicited significantly more source-related thoughts than the doctor with good bedside manner (H4a), while the doctor with good bedside manner elicited significantly more message-related thoughts (H4b, Table 3). This pattern also emerged in the path analysis (Figures 8, 9; Table 4). These results run counter to findings in the persuasion literature. HSM predicts that interpersonal warmth prompts heuristic processing, indicated by an increase in source-related thoughts (Chaiken, 1980, 1987; DeBono and Harnish, 1988; Wood and Kallgren, 1988; Horcajo et al., 2010) and that less warm, attractive, and competent sources prompt systematic processing (Chaiken, 1979; Pallak et al., 1983; Brownlow, 1992; Messner et al., 2008; Koh and Sundar, 2010). However, in a clinical setting, patients may expect good bedside manner; thus, a doctor who breaks this expectation could draw attention. Likewise, patients may not expect the doctor to appear eerie; thus, an eerie appearance could also draw attention.

The doctor’s interpersonal warmth increased his perceived competence and shifted thoughts from the source to the message, contrary to most HSM studies. In HSM, both warmth and competence are deemed heuristic cues, which prompt people to make decisions by rules of thumb learned from experience, such as trusting a source that looks and sounds authoritative (Abelson, 1976; Lin et al., 2016; Lin and Spence, 2018). The effects of heuristic cues tend to be fleeting because they are unlikely to elicit systematic processing of the message. They may increase adherence intention temporarily but without a lasting effect on behavior change. This aligns with Model 11, which shows that message-related thoughts have a stronger effect on adherence intention and exercise change than source-related thoughts.

In the medical literature, however, there is a close link between warmth and competence. When patients perceive their physicians as friendly and helpful, they also perceive them as competent, and they are less likely to sue them for malpractice (Charles et al., 1985; Shapiro et al., 1989; Hickson, 1994; Levinson et al., 1997). The physician’s warmth has a major influence on the patient–physician relationship. It prevents patient dissatisfaction, mistrust, and nonadherence (Cousin et al., 2013). Determining how SCM variables influence heuristic and systematic processing is crucial to the study of adherence because systematic processing tends to result in more enduring changes in attitude and intention—as well as changes in behavior that are more resistant to relapse (Cacioppo and Petty, 1984).

The results confirmed our earlier suspicion that interpersonal warmth has a confounding effect on measuring eeriness (Dai and MacDorman, 2018). Depiction affected pretest eeriness, but not posttest eeriness, which was instead affected by the doctor’s bedside manner, even when controlling for this variable. The manipulation check of pretest eeriness revealed that the 3-D computer-animated doctor, rendered in three different styles, was eerier than the real and cartoon doctors. Thus, eeriness should be measured in a pretest before an interactive scenario that manipulates warmth instead of in a posttest with other dependent variables.

This study builds on past research on how a relational agent’s characteristics influence credibility and trust in health counseling (Bickmore et al., 2005; Bickmore et al., 2010a; Bickmore and Gruber, 2010; Sillice et al., 2018) and pedagogy (Baylor and Kim, 2005). However, we varied warmth and realism systematically by using the same character interacting in the same way while controlling for other aspects of the scenario. By eliminating the effect of using virtual agents based on different characters, we could isolate the effect of warmth and realism on adherence.

Virtual consultations offer a cost-effective way to improve patient adherence and health literacy (Bickmore et al., 2009b; 2010c). Recent advances in AI, computer modeling and animation, and immersive games will enable the rapid development and deployment of virtual consultations via the Internet to computers and mobile devices. The advantages of AI-enabled virtual consultations include the ease of applying them to different conditions and treatments, speed of revision, and internationalization and localization for language, culture, and demographic group (DeSmet et al., 2014).

Limitations and Future Work

Concerning the effect of depiction on Adherence Intention, the study was underpowered. MacDorman et al. (2009) found that a 50% increase in eye size significantly increased Eeriness with a large effect size, ηp2 ≈ 0.65. However, in this study, the manipulation only had a small, though significant, effect on pretest Eeriness, p = 0.019, ηp2 = 0.03. For a 0.05 alpha level and this study’s effect size of ηp2 = 0.03, depiction: power = 0.08. With such a small effect size, just to achieve a power of 0.80 would require 670 participants in each of the 10 groups. As noted in Methods, the effect size was large when participants rated the videos in preparation for this study. One explanation is that they were aware of comparing five different styles of depictions, whereas in the present study, participants were only aware of their own condition.

Cognitive perspective-taking was a better predictor of exercise change than adherence intention. It is unclear why this should be the case. Was understanding the situation from the doctor’s perspective predictive, or was this variable simply a correlate for understanding the message? Further research is needed. In addition, behavior change only occurred for physical activity, not diet. This pattern, however, is not uncommon in the literature (e.g., Anderson et al., 2010).

The use of self-reported scales to measure behavior change in participants could be inaccurate for various reasons, such as the fabricated purpose of the virtual consultation, the participants’ poor recollection of it, or their tendency to respond in a socially desirable way. Exercise change can be measured more accurately by taking a different approach, such as using a monitoring device for physical activity. These include accelerometers, pedometers, and heart rate monitors (Bravata et al., 2007; Rogers et al., 2014; Patel et al., 2015; Stonerock and Blumenthal, 2017; Jelen et al., 2020).

Although patients’ emotional responses to their health conditions can influence their attitudes and intention to follow health-related advice and their actual behavior change (Dillard and Shen, 2005; Krakow et al., 2018), variables like fear and anger were not measured in the current study. Emotions like fear, anxiety, and disgust are also known to correlate with the uncanny valley effect (Ho et al., 2008). Future research could examine how these variables affect the persuasiveness of virtual agents.

Contributions to the Field

A major finding in the literature on persuasion is that a message will be less convincing if its source is evaluated negatively. Likewise, a major finding of the literature on the uncanny valley effect is that virtual characters that resemble humans will be evaluated negatively because of their eerie appearance. Therefore, it stands to reason that a human-looking virtual character should be less convincing than a real human. However, this study found no significant difference. Instead, it found that, in the context of a virtual doctor’s consultation, good bedside manner increased the intention to adhere to health advice and actual physical activity. The study also showed how bedside manner—a form of interpersonal warmth—had such an overriding effect on perceptions of the virtual character that a standard measure of eeriness was rendered invalid for use in a posttest. Although in the persuasion literature warmth typically elicits more thoughts about the message source, in the virtual consultation, the doctor with poor bedside manner or an eerie depiction elicited more source-related thoughts. Finally, contrary to the literature on behavior change, adherence intention was a poor predictor of exercise change compared with the ability to take the doctor’s perspective.

Conclusion

In 1970, Mori (2012) proposed that human replicas—like android robots—could appear eerie. This effect, known as the uncanny valley, has been confirmed by a meta-analysis (Diel et al., 2022). The present study reproduced this effect by using a 3-D computer-animated doctor in a virtual consultation with three different rendering styles. However, the impact of the uncanny valley on adherence intention and exercise change was negligible compared with bedside manner. Bedside manner also had a pronounced effect on two SCM variables, warmth and competence, and on the HSM variables number and valence of source- and message-related thoughts. Thus, although virtual characters can elicit the uncanny valley effect, in this study they were comparable to a video of a real human in their effect on adherence intention and exercise change.

The present study provides empirical findings that extend the theory and practice of persuasion to virtual clinical settings. SCM variables like warmth and competence were found to be important dimensions in designing persuasive computer-animated characters. Contrary to the literature, a physician with good bedside manner prompted greater systematic processing in HSM, which increased adherence intention and exercise change. Physician warmth predicted competence. Surprisingly, both warmth and competence were better predictors of adherence intention than HSM variables. Cognitive perspective-taking performed better in predicting exercise change than adherence intention. Depiction had much less of an impact on persuasion than the physician’s warmth and competence.

Data Availability Statement

The analysis was performed in the R statistical computing environment (packages: effectsize, jmv, lavaan, lavaanPlot, psy, psych, pwr2, multcomp, nlme, performance). The dataset and R scripts for the analyses are available as Supplementary Material.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Indiana University’s Office of Research Administration. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

ZD and KM conceptualized and designed the experiments, and ZD conducted them. ZD and KM analyzed the data, prepared the tables and figures, and wrote and revised the manuscript. ZD reviewed the literature on persuasion and adherence. KM wrote the final R scripts and supervised the research.

Funding

The study was supported by the National Institutes of Health (P20 GM066402/GM/NIGMS NIH HHS/United States), an IUPUI Signature Center grant, Research Investment Funds grant, and Open Access Fund.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Himalaya Patel, Lino Stephen, and Zebulun M. Wood for their help with producing the virtual consultation: Patel filmed and edited the consultation; Stephen modeled the doctor, his office and props and animated the doctor; and Wood shot reference photographs in preparation for computer modeling. We would also like to thank Danni Yang for coding data from the thought-listing task.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/frvir.2021.739038/full#supplementary-material

References

Abelson, R. P. (1976). “Script Processing in Attitude Formation and Decision Making,” in Cognition and Social Behavior (Oxford, England: Lawrence Erlbaum), 31–45.

Google Scholar

Abraham, C., Sheeran, P., and Johnston, M. (1998). From Health Beliefs to Self-Regulation: Theoretical Advances in the Psychology of Action Control. Psychol. Health 13, 569–591. doi:10.1080/08870449808407420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajzen, I. (1991). The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50, 179–211. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, R., Anderson, D., and Hurst, C. (2010). Modeling Factors that Influence Exercise and Dietary Change Among Midlife Australian Women: Results from the Healthy Aging of Women Study. Maturitas 67, 151–158. doi:10.1016/j.maturitas.2010.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

André, E., Bevacqua, E., Heylen, D., Niewiadomski, R., Pelachaud, C., Peters, C., et al. (2011). “Non-verbal Persuasion and Communication in an Affective Agent,” in Emotion-Oriented Systems: The Humaine Handbook. Editors R. Cowie, C. Pelachaud, and P. Petta (Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer), 585–608. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-15184-2_30

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Austin, J. T., and Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal Constructs in Psychology: Structure, Process, and Content. Psychol. Bull. 120, 338–375. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.120.3.338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baylor, A. L., and Kim, Y. (2005). Simulating Instructional Roles through Pedagogical Agents. Int. J. Artif. Intell. Edu. 15, 95–115. doi:10.1007/s40593-015-0055-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickmore, T., and Gruber, A. (2010). Relational Agents in Clinical Psychiatry. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 18, 119–130. doi:10.3109/10673221003707538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickmore, T., Schulman, D., and Yin, L. (2010a). Maintaining Engagement in Long-Term Interventions with Relational Agents. Appl. Artif. Intell. 24, 648–666. doi:10.1080/08839514.2010.492259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickmore, T. W., Caruso, L., Clough-Gorr, K., and Heeren, T. (2005). ‘It's Just like You Talk to a Friend' Relational Agents for Older Adults. Interact. Comput. 17, 711–735. doi:10.1016/j.intcom.2005.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickmore, T. W., Pfeifer, L. M., Byron, D., Forsythe, S., Henault, L. E., Jack, B. W., et al. (2010b). Usability of Conversational Agents by Patients with Inadequate Health Literacy: Evidence from Two Clinical Trials. J. Health Commun. 15, 197–210. doi:10.1080/10810730.2010.499991

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickmore, T. W., Pfeifer, L. M., and Jack, B. W. (2009b). “Taking the Time to Care,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Boston, Massachusetts, April 4–9, 2009 (New York: ACM), 1265–1274. doi:10.1145/1518701.1518891

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickmore, T. W., Pfeifer, L. M., and Paasche-Orlow, M. K. (2009a). Using Computer Agents to Explain Medical Documents to Patients with Low Health Literacy. Patient Edu. Couns. 75, 315–320. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2009.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickmore, T. W., Puskar, K., Schlenk, E. A., Pfeifer, L. M., and Sereika, S. M. (2010c). Maintaining Reality: Relational Agents for Antipsychotic Medication Adherence. Interact. Comput. 22, 276–288. doi:10.1016/j.intcom.2010.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bravata, D. M., Smith-Spangler, C., Sundaram, V., Gienger, A. L., Lin, N., Lewis, R., et al. (2007). Using Pedometers to Increase Physical Activity and Improve Health. JAMA 298, 2296–2304. doi:10.1001/jama.298.19.2296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, M., O'Neill, N., van Woerden, H., Eslambolchilar, P., Jones, M., and John, A. (2016). Gamification and Adherence to Web-Based Mental Health Interventions: A Systematic Review. JMIR Ment. Health 3, e39. doi:10.2196/mental.5710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brownlow, S. (1992). Seeing Is Believing: Facial Appearance, Credibility, and Attitude Change. J. Nonverbal Behav. 16, 101–115. doi:10.1007/BF00990325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buller, D. B., and Street, R. L. (1992). “Physician–patient Relationships,” in Applications of Nonverbal Behavioral Theories and Research. Editor R. S. Feldman (New York: Psychology Press), 119–142.

Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., and Petty, R. E. (1984). The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion. Adv. Consumer Res. 11, 673–675.

Google Scholar

Carter, E. J., Mahler, M., and Hodgins, J. K. (2013). “Unpleasantness of Animated Characters Corresponds to Increased Viewer Attention to Faces,” in Proceedings of the ACM Symposium on Applied Perception, Dublin, Ireland: August 22–23, 2013, 35–40. doi:10.1145/2492494.2502059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaiken, S. (1987). “The Heuristic Model of Persuasion,” in Social Influence: The Ontario Symposium. Editors M. P. Zanna, J. M. Olson, and C. P. Herman (New York: Lawrence Erlbaum), 5, 3–39. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.66.3.460

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaiken, S. (1979). Communicator Physical Attractiveness and Persuasion. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 37, 1387–1397. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.37.8.1387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaiken, S., and Eagly, A. H. (1983). Communication Modality as a Determinant of Persuasion: The Role of Communicator Salience. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 45, 241–256. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.45.2.241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus Systematic Information Processing and the Use of Source versus Message Cues in Persuasion. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 39, 752–766. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.752

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaiken, S., and Maheswaran, D. (1994). Heuristic Processing Can Bias Systematic Processing: Effects of Source Credibility, Argument Ambiguity, and Task Importance on Attitude Judgment. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 66, 460–473. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.66.3.460

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charles, S. C., Wilbert, J. R., and Franke, K. J. (1985). Sued and Nonsued Physicians' Self-Reported Reactions to Malpractice Litigation. Am. J. Psychiatry 142, 437–440. doi:10.1176/ajp.142.4.437

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chattopadhyay, D., and MacDorman, K. F. (2016). Familiar Faces Rendered Strange: Why Inconsistent Realism Drives Characters into the Uncanny Valley. J. Vis. 16 (7), 7–25. doi:10.1167/16.11.7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, S., and Chaiken, S. (1999). “The Heuristic-Systematic Model in its Broader Context,” in Dual–process Theories in Social Psychology. Editors S. Chaiken, and Y. Trope (New York: The Guilford Press), 73–96.

Google Scholar

Chisholm-Burns, M. A., and Spivey, C. A. (2012). The ‘Cost' of Medication Nonadherence: Consequences We Cannot Afford to Accept. J. Am. Pharm. Assoc. 52, 823–826. doi:10.1331/JAPhA.2012.11088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chong, S., Ding, D., Byun, R., Comino, E., Bauman, A., and Jalaludin, B. (2017). Lifestyle Changes after a Diagnosis of Type 2 Diabetes. Diabetes Spectr. 30, 43–50. doi:10.2337/ds15-0044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1960). A Coefficient of Agreement for Nominal Scales. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 20, 37–46. doi:10.1177/001316446002000104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1973). Eta-squared and Partial Eta-Squared in Fixed Factor ANOVA Designs. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 33, 107–112. doi:10.1177/001316447303300111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conner, M., and Norman, P. (1996). The Role of Social Cognition in Health Behaviours. Maidenhead, England: Open University Press.

Cousin, G., Schmid Mast, M., and Jaunin-Stalder, N. (2013). Finding the Right Interactional Temperature: Do Colder Patients Need More Warmth in Physician Communication Style?. Soc. Sci. Med. 98, 18–23. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.08.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cutler, R. L., Fernandez-Llimos, F., Frommer, M., Benrimoj, C., and Garcia-Cardenas, V. (2018). Economic Impact of Medication Non-adherence by Disease Groups: A Systematic Review. BMJ Open 8, e016982. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2017-016982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dai, Z., and MacDorman, K. F. (2018). The Doctor's Digital Double: How Warmth, Competence, and Animation Promote Adherence Intention. PeerJ Comput. Sci. 4, e168. doi:10.7717/peerj-cs.168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring Individual Differences in Empathy: Evidence for a Multidimensional Approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeBono, K. G., and Harnish, R. J. (1988). Source Expertise, Source Attractiveness, and the Processing of Persuasive Information: A Functional Approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 55, 541–546. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.55.4.541

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeSmet, A., Van Ryckeghem, D., Compernolle, S., Baranowski, T., Thompson, D., Crombez, G., et al. (2014). A Meta-Analysis of Serious Digital Games for Healthy Lifestyle Promotion. Prev. Med. 69, 95–107. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.08.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diel, A., and MacDorman, K. F. (2021). Creepy Cats and Strange High Houses: Support for Configural Processing in Testing Predictions of Nine Uncanny Valley Theories. J. Vis. 21, 1–20. doi:10.1167/jov.21.4.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diel, A., Weigelt, S., and MacDorman, K. F. (2022). A Meta-Analysis of the Uncanny Valley's Independent and Dependent Variables. ACM Trans. Human Robot Interact. 11, 1–38. doi:10.1145/3470742

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dillard, J. P., and Shen, L. (2005). On the Nature of Reactance and its Role in Persuasive Health Communication. Commun. Monogr. 72, 144–168. doi:10.1080/03637750500111815

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Chaiken, S. (1993). The Psychology of Attitudes. Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Eriksson, K.-F., and Lindgärde, F. (1991). Prevention of Type 2 (Non-insulin-dependent) Diabetes Mellitus by Diet and Physical Exercise: The 6-year Malmo Feasibility Study. Diabetologia 34, 891–898. doi:10.1007/BF00400196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ettner, S. L. (1999). The Relationship Between Continuity of Care and the Health Behaviors of Patients. Med. Care 37, 547–555. doi:10.1097/00005650-199906000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, S., Wang, X., Wang, Q., Fang, J., Wu, Y., Yi, L., et al. (2018). The Uncanny Valley Effect in Typically Developing Children and its Absence in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. PLOS ONE 13, e0206343. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0206343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., and Glick, P. (2007). Universal Dimensions of Social Cognition: Warmth and Competence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 77–83. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., and Xu, J. (2002). A Model of (Often Mixed) Stereotype Content: Competence and Warmth Respectively Follow from Perceived Status and Competition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 82, 878–902. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, U. (2001). Mind Blindness and the Brain in Autism. Neuron 32, 969–979. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00552-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funke, F., and Reips, U.-D. (2012). Why Semantic Differentials in Web-Based Research Should Be Made from Visual Analogue Scales and Not from 5-point Scales. Field Methods 24, 310–327. doi:10.1177/1525822X12444061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gollwitzer, P. M., and Moskowitz, G. B. (1996). “Goal Effects on Action and Cognition,” in Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (New York: The Guilford Press), 361–399.

Google Scholar

Green, R. D., MacDorman, K. F., Ho, C.-C., and Vasudevan, S. (2008). Sensitivity to the Proportions of Faces that Vary in Human Likeness. Comput. Hum. Behav. 24, 2456–2474. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2008.02.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G. (1968). Cognitive Learning, Cognitive Response to Persuasion, and Attitude Change. New York: Academic Press.

Griskevicius, V., Shiota, M. N., and Neufeld, S. L. (2010). Influence of Different Positive Emotions on Persuasion Processing: A Functional Evolutionary Approach. Emotion 10, 190–206. doi:10.1037/a0018421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hale, J. L., Householder, B. J., and Greene, K. L. (2002). “The Theory of Reasoned Action,” in The Persuasion Handbook: Developments in Theory and Practice. Editors J. P. Dillard, and L. Shen (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publication), 14, 259–286.

Google Scholar

Hickson, G. B. (1994). Obstetricians' Prior Malpractice Experience and Patients' Satisfaction with Care. JAMA 272, 1583–1587. doi:10.1001/jama.1994.03520200039032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, C.-C., and MacDorman, K. F. (2017). Measuring the Uncanny Valley Effect. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 9, 129–139. doi:10.1007/s12369-016-0380-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, C.-C., MacDorman, K. F., and Pramono, Z. A. D. D. (2008). “Human Emotion and the Uncanny Valley,” in Proceedings of the 3rd ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human Robot Interaction, Amsterdam, March 12–15, 2008 (New York: ACM), 169–176. doi:10.1145/1349822.1349845

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, C.-C., and MacDorman, K. F. (2010). Revisiting the Uncanny Valley Theory: Developing and Validating an Alternative to the Godspeed Indices. Comput. Hum. Behav. 26, 1508–1518. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2010.05.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, P. M., Rumsfeld, J. S., Masoudi, F. A., McClure, D. L., Plomondon, M. E., Steiner, J. F., et al. (2006). Effect of Medication Nonadherence on Hospitalization and Mortality Among Patients with Diabetes Mellitus. Arch. Intern. Med. 166, 1836–1841. doi:10.1001/archinte.166.17.1836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horcajo, J., Petty, R. E., and Briñol, P. (2010). The Effects of Majority versus Minority Source Status on Persuasion: A Self-Validation Analysis. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 99, 498–512. doi:10.1037/a0018626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L. t., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria versus New Alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. A Multidis. J. 6, 1–55. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, D. L. (2003). Revisiting Sample Size and Number of Parameter Estimates: Some Support for the N:q Hypothesis. Struct. Equ. Model. A Multidis. J. 10, 128–141. doi:10.1207/S15328007SEM1001_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jelen, B., Richards, O. K., Whitman, S. A., Ongwere, T., Kresnye, K. C., and Siek, K. A. (2020). “Exploring the Use of Electronics to Customize Pervasive Health Technologies with Older Adult Crafters,” in Proceedings of the 14th EAI International Conference on Pervasive Computing Technologies for Healthcare, October 6–8, 2020 (New York: ACM), 166–178. doi:10.1145/3421937.3421976

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, W. L., Rickel, J. W., and Lester, J. C. (2000). Animated Pedagogical Agents: Face-To-Face Interaction in Interactive Learning Environments. Int. J. Artif. Intell. Edu. 11, 47–78.

Google Scholar

Kätsyri, J., Forger, K., Mäkäräinen, M., and Takala, T. (2015). A Review of Empirical Evidence on Different Uncanny Valley Hypotheses: Support for Perceptual Mismatch as One Road to the Valley of Eeriness. Front. Psychol. 6, 390. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelman, H. C., and Hovland, C. I. (1953). “Reinstatement” of the Communicator in Delayed Measurement of Opinion Change. J. Abnormal Soc. Psychol. 48, 327–335. doi:10.1037/h0061861

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, P., Parsons, T. D., Gratch, J., Leuski, A., and Rizzo, A. A. (2007). “Virtual Patients for Clinical Therapist Skills Training,” in International Workshop on Intelligent Virtual Agents (Berlin: Springer), 197–210. doi:10.1007/978-3-540-74997-4_19

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S. S., Kaplowitz, S., and Johnston, M. V. (2004). The Effects of Physician Empathy on Patient Satisfaction and Compliance. Eval. Health Prof. 27, 237–251. doi:10.1177/0163278704267037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirch, D. G., and Petelle, K. (2017). Addressing the Physician Shortage. JAMA 317, 1947–1948. doi:10.1001/jama.2017.2714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinsinger, F. (2018). The Unmet Challenge of Medication Nonadherence. Perm J. 22, 83–85. doi:10.7812/TPP/18-033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York: Guilford Press.

Koh, Y. J., and Sundar, S. S. (2010). Heuristic Versus Systematic Processing of Specialist Versus Generalist Sources in Online Media. Hum. Commun. Res. 36, 103–124. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2010.01370.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraft-Todd, G. T., Reinero, D. A., Kelley, J. M., Heberlein, A. S., Baer, L., and Riess, H. (2017). Empathic Nonverbal Behavior Increases Ratings of Both Warmth and Competence in a Medical Context. PLOS ONE 12, e0177758. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0177758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krakow, M. M., Yale, R. N., Jensen, J. D., Carcioppolo, N., and Ratcliff, C. L. (2018). Comparing Mediational Pathways for Narrative- and Argument-Based Messages: Believability, Counterarguing, and Emotional Reaction. Hum. Commun. Res. 44, 299–321. doi:10.1093/hcr/hqy002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landis, J. R., and Koch, G. G. (1977). The Measurement of Observer Agreement for Categorical Data. Biometrics 33, 159–174. doi:10.2307/2529310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and Representation: The Origins of "theory of mind.". Psychol. Rev. 94, 412–426. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.94.4.412

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levinson, W., Roter, D. L., Mullooly, J. P., Dull, V. T., and Frankel, R. M. (1997). Physician-patient Communication. The Relationship with Malpractice Claims Among Primary Care Physicians and Surgeons. JAMA 277, 553–559. doi:10.1001/jama.1997.0354031005103410.1001/jama.277.7.553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, M. A., and Rook, K. S. (1999). Social Control in Personal Relationships: Impact on Health Behaviors and Psychological Distress. Health Psychol. 18, 63–71. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.18.1.63

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, X., and Spence, P. R. (2018). Identity on Social Networks as a Cue: Identity, Retweets, and Credibility. Commun. Stud. 69, 461–482. doi:10.1080/10510974.2018.1489295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, X., Spence, P. R., and Lachlan, K. A. (2016). Social Media and Credibility Indicators: The Effect of Influence Cues. Comput. Hum. Behav. 63, 264–271. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Looije, R., Neerincx, M. A., and Cnossen, F. (2010). Persuasive Robotic Assistant for Health Self-Management of Older Adults: Design and Evaluation of Social Behaviors. Int. J. Human Comp. Stud. 68, 386–397. doi:10.1016/j.ijhcs.2009.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., and Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power Analysis and Determination of Sample Size for Covariance Structure Modeling. Psychol. Methods 1, 130–149. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.130

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDorman, K. F., and Chattopadhyay, D. (2016). Reducing Consistency in Human Realism Increases the Uncanny Valley Effect; Increasing Category Uncertainty Does Not. Cognition 146, 190–205. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDorman, K. F., Green, R. D., Ho, C.-C., and Koch, C. T. (2009). Too Real for comfort? Uncanny Responses to Computer Generated Faces. Comput. Hum. Behav. 25, 695–710. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDorman, K. F. (2019). In the Uncanny Valley, Transportation Predicts Narrative Enjoyment More Than Empathy, but Only for the Tragic Hero. Comput. Hum. Behav. 94, 140–153. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2019.01.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maddux, J. E. (1999). “Expectancies and the Social-Cognitive Perspective: Basic Principles, Processes, and Variables,” in How Expectancies Shape Experience. Editor I. Kirsch (Washington, DC: APA), 17–39. doi:10.1037/10332-001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marć, M., Bartosiewicz, A., Burzyńska, J., Chmiel, Z., and Januszewicz, P. (2019). A Nursing Shortage – A Prospect of Global and Local Policies. Int. Nurs. Rev. 66, 9–16. doi:10.1111/inr.12473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, L. R., Williams, S. L., Haskard, K. B., and DiMatteo, M. R. (2005). The Challenge of Patient Adherence. Ther. Clin. Risk Manag. 1, 189–199.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

McCroskey, J. C., and Teven, J. J. (1999). Goodwill: A Reexamination of the Construct and its Measurement. Commun. Monogr. 66, 90–103. doi:10.1080/03637759909376464

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonald, H. P., Garg, A. X., and Haynes, R. B. (2002). Interventions to Enhance Patient Adherence to Medication Prescriptions. JAMA 288, 2868–2879. doi:10.1001/jama.288.22.2868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonnell, R., Breidt, M., and Bülthoff, H. H. (2012). Render Me Real?. ACM Trans. Graph. 31, 1–11. doi:10.1145/2185520.2185587

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonnell, R., and Breidt, M. (2010). “Face Reality,” in ACM SIGGRAPH ASIA 2010 Sketches, Seoul, Korea, December 15–18, 2010 (New York: ACM), 1–2. doi:10.1145/1899950.1899991

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGuire, M., and Iuga, M. J. (2014). Adherence and Health Care Costs. Risk Manag. Healthc. Policy 7, 35–44. doi:10.2147/RMHP.S19801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGuire, W. J. (1978). “An Information-Processing Model of Advertising Effectiveness,” in Behavioral and Management Science in Marketing (New York: Ronald Press), 156–180.

Google Scholar

McKechnie, T. (2016). Call for Nurses: The Multi-Dimensional Manifestation of Nursing Shortage on Patient Care. QIHI J. Healthc. Improvement Patient Saf. 2 (2).

Google Scholar

Messner, M., Reinhard, M. A., and Sporer, S. L. (2008). Compliance through Direct Persuasive Appeals: The Moderating Role of Communicator's Attractiveness in Interpersonal Persuasion. Soc. Influ. 3, 67–83. doi:10.1080/15534510802045261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, N. H. (1997). Compliance with Treatment Regimens in Chronic Asymptomatic Diseases. Am. J. Med. 102, 43–49. doi:10.1016/S0002-9343(97)00467-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, N., Maruyama, G., Beaber, R. J., and Valone, K. (1976). Speed of Speech and Persuasion. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 34, 615–624. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.34.4.615

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montano, D. E., and Kasprzyk, D. (2015). “Theory of Reasoned Action, Theory of Planned Behavior, and the Integrated Behavioral Model,” in Health Behavior: Theory, Research and Practice (San Francisco: Jossey-Bass), 95–124.

Google Scholar

Morello, C. M., and Hirsch, J. D. (2017). Strategies for Addressing the Cost of Nonadherence in Diabetes. Am. J. Manag. Care 23, S247–S252.

Google Scholar

Mori, M., MacDorman, K. F., and Kageki, N. (2012). The Uncanny Valley [From the Field]. IEEE Robot. Automat. Mag. 19, 98–100. doi:10.1109/MRA.2012.2192811

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, K. M., Reiber, G., and Boyko, E. J. (2002). Diet and Exercise Among Adults with Type 2 Diabetes: Findings from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III). Diabetes Care 25, 1722–1728. doi:10.2337/diacare.25.10.1722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogawa, K., Bartneck, C., Sakamoto, D., Kanda, T., Ono, T., and Ishiguro, H. (2018). “Can an Android Persuade You?,” in Geminoid Studies: Science and Technologies for Humanlike Teleoperated Androids (Singapore: Springer), 235–247. doi:10.1007/978-981-10-8702-8_14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Hair, D. (1986). Patient Preferences for Physician Persuasion Strategies. Theor. Med. 7, 147–164.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Orji, R., Mandryk, R. L., and Vassileva, J. (2015). “Gender, Age, and Responsiveness to Cialdini's Persuasion Strategies,” in Persuasive Technology, Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Editors T. MacTavish, and S. Basapur (Cham, Switzerland: Springer), 147–159. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-20306-5_14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pallak, S. R., Murroni, E., and Koch, J. (1983). Communicator Attractiveness and Expertise, Emotional versus Rational Appeals, and Persuasion: A Heuristic versus Systematic Processing Interpretation. Soc. Cogn. 2, 122–141. doi:10.1521/soco.1983.2.2.122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, H., Bayliss, L. C., Ivory, J. D., Woodard, K., McCarthy, A., and MacDorman, K. F. (2014). Receptive to Bad Reception: Jerky Motion Can Make Persuasive Messages More Effective. Comput. Hum. Behav. 32, 32–39. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.11.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, H., and MacDorman, K. F. (2015). Sending an Avatar to Do a Human's Job: Compliance with Authority Persists Despite the Uncanny Valley. Presence. Teleop. Vir. Environ. 24, 1–23. doi:10.1162/PRES_a_00212

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, M. S., Asch, D. A., and Volpp, K. G. (2015). Wearable Devices as Facilitators, Not Drivers, of Health Behavior Change. JAMA 313, 459–460. doi:10.1001/jama.2014.14781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petterson, S. M., Liaw, W. R., Phillips, R. L., Rabin, D. L., Meyers, D. S., and Bazemore, A. W. (2012). Projecting US Primary Care Physician Workforce Needs: 2010-2025. Ann. Fam. Med. 10, 503–509. doi:10.1370/afm.1431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petty, R. E., and Wegener, D. T. (1998). Matching versus Mismatching Attitude Functions: Implications for Scrutiny of Persuasive Messages. Pers Soc. Psychol. Bull. 24, 227–240. doi:10.1177/0146167298243001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petty, R., Ostrom, T. M., and Brock, T. C. (2014). Cognitive Responses in Persuasion. New York: Psychology Press.

Pornpitakpan, C. (2004). The Persuasiveness of Source Credibility: A Critical Review of Five Decades' Evidence. J. Appl. Soc. Pyschol. 34, 243–281. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb02547.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prochaska, J. O., and DiClemente, C. C. (1983). Stages and Processes of Self-Change of Smoking: Toward an Integrative Model of Change. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 51, 390–395. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.51.3.390

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prochaska, J. O., and Marcus, B. H. (1994). “The Transtheoretical Model: Applications to Exercise,” in Advances in Exercise Adherence. Editor R. K. Dishman (Champaign, Illinois: Human Kinetics), 161–180.

Google Scholar

Prochaska, J. O., Redding, C. A., and Evers, K. E. (2015). “The Transtheoretical Model and Stages of Change,” in Health Behavior: Theory, Research, and Practice (San Francisco: Jossey-Bass), 125–148.

Google Scholar

Reips, U.-D., and Funke, F. (2008). Interval-level Measurement with Visual Analogue Scales in Internet-Based Research: VAS Generator. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 699–704. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rejeski, W. J., Ip, E. H., Bertoni, A. G., Bray, G. A., Evans, G., Gregg, E. W., et al. (2012). Lifestyle Change and Mobility in Obese Adults with Type 2 Diabetes. N. Engl. J. Med. 366, 1209–1217. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa1110294

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, D., and Caldwell, P. H. (2016). “Gamification to Improve Adherence to Clinical Treatment Advice: Improving Adherence to Clinical Treatment,” in Handbook of Research on Holistic Perspectives in Gamification for Clinical Practice (Hershey, PA: IGI Global), 47–77.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Rogers, T., Milkman, K. L., and Volpp, K. G. (2014). Commitment Devices. JAMA 311, 2065–2066. doi:10.1001/jama.2014.3485

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saeedi, P., Petersohn, I., Salpea, P., Malanda, B., Karuranga, S., Unwin, N., et al. (2019). Global and Regional Diabetes Prevalence Estimates for 2019 and Projections for 2030 and 2045: Results from the International Diabetes Federation Diabetes Atlas, 9th Edition. Diabetes Res. Clin. Pract. 157, 107843. doi:10.1016/j.diabres.2019.107843

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Safran, D. G., Taira, D. A., Rogers, W. H., Kosinski, M., Ware, J. E., and Tarlov, A. R. (1998). Linking Primary Care Performance to Outcomes of Care. J. Fam. Pract. 47, 213–220.

Google Scholar

Sami, W., Ansari, T., Butt, N. S., and Hamid, M. R. A. (2017). Effect of Diet on Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus: A Review. Int. J. Health Sci. (Qassim) 11, 65–71. doi:10.1002/dmrr.2515

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schein, C., and Gray, K. (2015). The Unifying Moral Dyad. Pers Soc. Psychol. Bull. 41, 1147–1163. doi:10.1177/0146167215591501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schindler, S., Zell, E., Botsch, M., and Kissler, J. (2017). Differential Effects of Face-Realism and Emotion on Event-Related Brain Potentials and Their Implications for the Uncanny Valley Theory. Sci. Rep. 7, 45003. doi:10.1038/srep45003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmid Mast, M., Hall, J. A., and Roter, D. L. (2007). Disentangling Physician Sex and Physician Communication Style: Their Effects on Patient Satisfaction in a Virtual Medical Visit. Patient Edu. Couns. 68, 16–22. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2007.03.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, J., Kaplan, S. H., Greenfield, S., Li, W., and Wilson, I. B. (2004). Better Physician-Patient Relationships are Associated with Higher Reported Adherence to Antiretroviral Therapy in Patients with HIV Infection. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 19, 1096–1103. doi:10.1111/j.1525-1497.2004.30418.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seyama, J., and Nagayama, R. S. (2007). The Uncanny Valley: Effect of Realism on the Impression of Artificial Human Faces. Presence Teleop. Vir. Environ. 16, 337–351. doi:10.1162/pres.16.4.337

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, R. S., Simpson, D. E., Lawrence, S. L., Talsky, A. M., Sobocinski, K. A., and Schiedermayer, D. L. (1989). A Survey of Sued and Nonsued Physicians and Suing Patients. Arch. Intern. Med. 149, 2190–2196. doi:10.1001/archinte.149.10.2190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sillice, M. A., Morokoff, P. J., Ferszt, G., Bickmore, T., Bock, B. C., Lantini, R., et al. (2018). Using Relational Agents to Promote Exercise and Sun Protection: Assessment of Participants' Experiences with Two Interventions. J. Med. Internet Res. 20, e48. doi:10.2196/jmir.7640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the Conditioned Reflex. New York: Pergamon.

Stavropoulou, C. (2011). Non-adherence to Medication and Doctor-Patient Relationship: Evidence from a European Survey. Patient Edu. Couns. 83, 7–13. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2010.04.039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, J.-P., and Ohler, P. (2018). Uncanny... but Convincing? Inconsistency Between a Virtual Agent's Facial Proportions and Vocal Realism Reduces its Credibility and Attractiveness, but Not its Persuasive Success. Interact Comput. 30, 480–491. doi:10.1093/iwc/iwy023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stonerock, G. L., and Blumenthal, J. A. (2017). Role of Counseling to Promote Adherence in Healthy Lifestyle Medicine: Strategies to Improve Exercise Adherence and Enhance Physical Activity. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 59, 455–462. doi:10.1016/j.pcad.2016.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Street, R. L. (1990). Communication in Medical Consultations: A Review Essay. Q. J. Speech 76, 315–332. doi:10.1080/00335639009383926

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamilmani, K., Rana, N. P., Wamba, S. F., and Dwivedi, R. (2021). The Extended Unified Theory of Acceptance and Use of Technology (UTAUT2): A Systematic Literature Review and Theory Evaluation. Int. J. Inf. Manage. 57, 102269. doi:10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2020.102269

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, A., Chaiken, S., and Henderson, M. D. (2002). “The Heuristic-Systematic Model of Social Information Processing,” in The Persuasion Handbook: Developments in Theory and Practice (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications), 195–211.

Google Scholar

Tuomilehto, J., Lindström, J., Eriksson, J. G., Valle, T. T., Hämäläinen, H., Ilanne-Parikka, P., et al. (2001). Prevention of Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus by Changes in Lifestyle Among Subjects with Impaired Glucose Tolerance. N. Engl. J. Med. 344, 1343–1350. doi:10.1056/NEJM200105033441801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Umberson, D. (1987). Family Status and Health Behaviors: Social Control as a Dimension of Social Integration. J. Health Soc. Behav. 28, 306–319. doi:10.2307/2136848

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waeber, B., Burnier, M., and Brunner, H. R. (2000). How to Improve Adherence with Prescribed Treatment in Hypertensive Patients?. J. Cardiovasc. Pharmacol. 35 (Suppl. 3), S23–S26. doi:10.1097/00005344-200035063-00006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, C. A., Cahir, C., Tecklenborg, S., Byrne, C., Culbertson, M. A., and Bennett, K. E. (2019). The Association between Medication Non‐adherence and Adverse Health Outcomes in Ageing Populations: A Systematic Review and Meta‐analysis. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 85, 2464–2478. doi:10.1111/bcp.14075

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Geigel, J., and Herbert, A. (2013). “Reading Personality: Avatar vs. Human Faces,” in Proceedings of the 2013 Humaine Association Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction, Geneva, September 2–5, 2013, 479–484. doi:10.1109/ACII.2013.85

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, T. L., and Sheeran, P. (2006). Does Changing Behavioral Intentions Engender Behavior Change? A Meta-Analysis of the Experimental Evidence. Psychol. Bull. 132, 249–268. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willson, P., and McNamara, J. R. (1982). How Perceptions of a Simulated Physician-Patient Interaction Influence Intended Satisfaction and Compliance. Soc. Sci. Med. 16, 1699–1704. doi:10.1016/0277-9536(82)90095-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wojciszke, B., Bazinska, R., and Jaworski, M. (1998). On the Dominance of Moral Categories in Impression Formation. Pers Soc. Psychol. Bull. 24, 1251–1263. doi:10.1177/01461672982412001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, W., and Kallgren, C. A. (1988). Communicator Attributes and Persuasion. Pers Soc. Psychol. Bull. 14, 172–182. doi:10.1177/0146167288141017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, L., Bickmore, T., and Cortés, D. E. (2010). “The Impact of Linguistic and Cultural Congruity on Persuasion by Conversational Agents,” in Intelligent Virtual Agents, Lecture Notes in Computer Science.. Editors J. Allbeck, N. Badler, T. Bickmore, C. Pelachaud, and A. Safonova (Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer), 343–349. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-15892-6_36

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zell, E., Aliaga, C., Jarabo, A., Zibrek, K., Gutierrez, D., McDonnell, R., et al. (2015). To Stylize or Not to Stylize?. ACM Trans. Graph. 34, 1–12. doi:10.1145/2816795.2818126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhuo, X., Zhang, P., and Hoerger, T. J. (2013). Lifetime Direct Medical Costs of Treating Type 2 Diabetes and Diabetic Complications. Am. J. Prev. Med. 45, 253–261. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2013.04.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zibrek, K., Kokkinara, E., and McDonnell, R. (2018). The Effect of Realistic Appearance of Virtual Characters in Immersive Environments - Does the Character's Personality Play a Role?. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 24, 1681–1690. doi:10.1109/TVCG.2018.2794638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziegele, M., and Jost, P. B. (2020). Not funny? The Effects of Factual versus Sarcastic Journalistic Responses to Uncivil User Comments. Commun. Res. 47, 891–920. doi:10.1177/0093650216671854

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: adherence, anthropomorphism, doctor–patient communication, heuristic–systematic model, interactive narratives, persuasive technologies, stereotype content model, uncanny valley effect

Citation: Dai Z and MacDorman KF (2021) Creepy, but Persuasive: In a Virtual Consultation, Physician Bedside Manner, Rather than the Uncanny Valley, Predicts Adherence. Front. Virtual Real. 2:739038. doi: 10.3389/frvir.2021.739038

Received: 09 July 2021; Accepted: 18 August 2021;
Published: 14 September 2021.

Edited by:

Funda Durupinar, University of Massachusetts Boston, United States

Reviewed by:

Matias Volonte, Northeastern University, United States
Richard Skarbez, La Trobe University, Australia

Copyright © 2021 Dai and MacDorman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Karl F. MacDorman, kmacdorm@indiana.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.