Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Vet. Sci. , 18 March 2025

Sec. Animal Behavior and Welfare

Volume 12 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2025.1549100

Analysis of sounds made by Bos taurus and Bubalus bubalis dams to their calves

dm Lenner
Ádám Lenner1*Zoltn Lajos PappZoltán Lajos Papp2Istvn KomlsiIstván Komlósi3
  • 1Doctoral School of Animal Science, University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
  • 2Faculty of Informatics, University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
  • 3Faculty of Agricultural and Food Sciences and Environmental Management, University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary

Audio and video recording techniques have advanced significantly in recent years, allowing newer opportunities for sound analysis. The grouping of cattle breeds or individuals based on the connections between their behavior and condition and their vocalizations is important from the point of view of animal welfare. Despite the numerous studies published about the acoustic characteristics of such sounds, there has not been an acoustic analysis regarding of cattle behavior and condition in isolation. The grey cattle and domestic buffalo cows, separated from their calves for a few minutes, are stressed and vocalize orally. In this study various methods were employed for the analysis of the sounds that water buffalo and grey cattle made after weaning. Differences have been found between the two species, but not between individuals. Their pitch varies over time for both species. The buffalo voice is three times more dynamic than that of the grey cattle on a logHz scale. Furthermore, a significant difference was found between relevant mel frequency cepstral coefficients adapted to animals. Our findings may be utilized in agriculture and bioacoustic procedures.

1 Introduction

The domestication of cattle and buffalo is different; while cattle were domesticated approximately 10,000 years ago (1), buffalo were domesticated 3,000–7,000 years ago (2). The water buffalo (Bubalus bubalis) is kept for both meat and dairy production (3), with a global population of 202 million individuals worldwide in 2020 (2), making its importance among farm animals indisputable, yet little analysis of its vocalization has been done. A Domesticated cattle and buffalo are highly gregarious and live in herds in nature and on farms (4). In their herds, there is a network between the individuals and they communicate with each other in various ways (5). They can recognize each other visually, by smell or by vocalization. Researched a correlation between the vocalization of cattle and their behavior. They distinguished six behavioral groups: ‘lying & ruminating’, ‘feeding related’, ‘social interaction’, ‘sexual behavior’, ‘stress-related behavior’ and ‘remaining behavior’ (6). Of the different behaviors, vocalization is present in 17.2% (7). The analysis of vocalization is a good opportunity to examine individuals of a herd (8). Cattle vocalization is affected by multiple factors, including farming method (9), age, dominance (10), weight and/or sex (11, 12), estrous climax (13), and castration, which is a highly stressful activity similar to selection (14). Some numerous studies have been published the intense variability between cows (1517).

Vocalization is triggered by a complex set of hormonal and nervous system reactions. The information on external events that modify emotions, or hormonal and homeostatic factors influencing mood, are perceived by limbic centers of the forebrain. Signals are then transferred via centers of the midbrain (periaqueductal grey) and the lower brainstem to effector muscles of the vocal system (8). Cattle make two main types of calls, forming them by adjusting their supra-laryngeal (above the larynx) vocal sound-producing organ. They use a nasalized low-frequency sound for close contact and an orally produced high-frequency sound for distant communication or for expressing an emotionally aroused state (1821). Vocalization is one of the most conspicuous behavioral changes in cattle, triggered by a feeling of discomfort (22, 23). As they are prey animals, vocalization within a livestock is minimal (8). When it can be heard, it has an importance from a biological point of view (22), for example in the case of weaning (5, 20, 24). Weaning is stressful for the cows. Oral vocalizations are a sign of stress (25, 26).

In the first hours after calving, vocalization is an important element in the development of the bond between the cow and her newborn calf (27, 28). F0 is the standard symbol for the fundamental frequency. Oral vocalization is triggered by isolation, pain or anxiety (10), while nasal sounds are usually produced in the first hours after calving [F0 = 81.17 ± 0.98 Hz; (12)], often used by dams towards their offspring in combination with licking (29). These nasal sounds also have a calming effect on the cows (30).

The parameters of sounds are affected by the anatomy of the larynx, the length, the thickness and the muscle tension of the vocal folds (31, 32). The filter selectively enhances or dampens specific frequency ranges of the source signal. As a result, a heterogeneous sound spectrum is created containing various formant frequency peaks (32, 33). The sound frequency of dams is higher during weaning too, like at the time of other dam-calf interactions (26, 34). Oral vocalization is generally connected with arousal (31). The frequency of the various sounds of vocalizing cattle is usually between 50 Hz and 1,250 Hz (4). The mean is between 120 and 180 Hz. It can be detected spectrographically up to 7 or 8 kHz; the peak call is in the 350 to 420 Hz range. Sometimes calls below 50 Hz, like the 31 Hz (10) newly weaned calves.

In terms of frequency, a cow’s hearing ranges from 23 Hz to 37,000 Hz (35). The cows have a sound threshold of 85 dB – 90 dB sound pressure level (36), and any range of environmental sounds exceeding 110 dB SPL may cause physical damage (37, 38).

Dams made high-frequency calls (HFC) during weaning, where visual contact was excluded (F0 = 152.8 ± 3.10 Hz) (12). Their normal range in the case of a 60 dB sound is between 23 Hz and 37 kHz. The highest sensitivity is at around 8 kHz (35). This important trait helps them notice predators in time.

The demand for innovative tools that collect and analyze information about the livestock and individuals is increasing. In precision livestock farming (PLF), is increasingly important nowadays, because farmer-cattle interaction is decreasing (39). Research is becoming rather interdisciplinary, aiming to detect connections between vocalization, the nervous system and the hormonal systems. From the point of view of animal welfare, vocalization can be very useful as exact feedback from the animal about its general state. The management of a farm or a plant is decisive for the success of animal welfare (40). The vocal individuality of high-frequency calls of cattle does not change in farming contexts of different emotional valence, including positive and negative emotional valence and situations (19). According to the literature, there is a significant difference between the vocalizations of lying individuals and the sounds recorded during other behaviors (83 ± 4.3 Hz versus 298 ± 8.0 Hz; p < 0.05). Mature dairy cattle have a significantly lower maximum frequency (Hz) than heifers (332.6 ± 0.2 Hz versus 218.5 Hz ± 0,3 Hz; p < 0.05) (6). The mean F0 high-frequency vocalization of beef cattle is 153 Hz, the formant frequencies are between 228 and 3,181 Hz, the average duration is 1.2 s and is made orally (12). In PLF the technology constantly measures the variables, such as movements, feed intake or oestrus (41), and thereby helps farmers control their livestock.

The mel frequency cepstral coefficient (MFCC) is regularly used for sound analysis and their categorization or grouping (42); therefore, we also employed it. Such coefficients have acoustic features that can be widely used in various applications later (4346). The mel scale-based cepstrum can be used well in the recognition of speech sounds. However, the mel scale is a distortion of the frequency scale that is adapted to human hearing; it is a scale with an even pitch sensation (47). The scale is based on people’s binaural hearing. This distortion depends on the shape of the human hearing organ. It is possible to start from the assumption that the hearing organ of these big animals differs only in size from that of humans. Mel frequency cepstrum analysis of the sounds indicated oestrus with 94% accuracy (44) other authors have achieved even better accuracy, 97% (48). Automated software is able to detect behavioral changes early, which in turn can lead to early responses, even to the treatment of a disease, thus reducing labor time and veterinary costs (49). Accurate knowledge of vocalization can be such a measure. Vocalization is an excellent tool for detecting animal welfare problems, in the field of agriculture; especially where individuals are not always visible, it can be used to assess animal welfare. The collection, observation and assessment of vocalization do not require animal interaction and are therefore stress-free. However, to do this, bovine vocalization software needs as accurate data as possible (50). Although analyzing vocalization is useful, limited research has been done on the analysis of the vocalization of cattle and its categorization. Further research is needed on vocalization analysis in order to reduce stress on animals and its temporal effects (10, 51). This paper analyses the sounds of grey cattle and water buffalo, among others, using the mel scale cepstrum. The cows that we studied vocalized in the weaning stress situation (52). We also aimed to detect differences in vocalization between the two species and between individuals (oral vocalization).

2 Materials and methods

2.1 The process of data collection

Our research site is a farm called Szamárhát, owned by Tiszatáj Közalapítvány, a public foundation. There, animal farming is extensive from spring to autumn and in winter, at the time of recording the sounds, the individuals are in barns. In 2019 and 2020 in the wintertime we recorded 14 calls of water buffalo cows and 13 calls of grey cattle cows. We applied multiple methods for the sound analysis of the 139 sound pieces. In MATLAB R20201a, we used the Polyfit function (sixth degree polynomial), in addition to spectrum analysis, mel-scale cepstrum and formant analysis. As prey animals, they vocalize only when justified (8), so cattle and buffalo cows had to be encouraged to vocalize. The situation was the same during each data collection. The audio recording took place between 8:00 and 11:00 am. Within 2–12 h after calving, the calves were taken for weight measurement and ear tagging. At that moment, a gate was placed between the dam and the offspring, as a physical barrier. The individuals heard and saw each other but were unable to physically contact each other. The dams made oral vocalization, a sign of stress (52). The vocalizing dams were standing. The eldest cow was 13 years old, the youngest one was 5. Each individual had already calved multiple times in the past. The sounds were recorded with a Sony IC Recorder AX412F whose sample rate is 32.000 Hz. Every time, we directed the recorder toward the cow from a distance of 2–3 meters. We aimed to avoid any interfering noise.

2.2 Statistical analysis

To determine the fundamental period or frequency of quasi-periodic signals, we utilized the Pitch function from the Matlab Audio Toolbox, which offers five methods. This function offers five methods. Among them, the Normalized Correlation Function (NCF) appeared to be the most appropriate (53), although it was often inaccurate (octave jump, see, Figure 1). We applied the Mann–Whitney U test to the samples. We examined the pitch using a two-sample t-test with the logarithm of the value.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The upper part of the figure shows the waveform of a water buffalo call. The lower part is its pitch function (solid line). Due to the usual octave jump, it is shifted up and down by one octave (dashed line). The function of the values we deemed correct was smoothed by fitting a suitable 6th degree polynomial.

3 Results and discussion

Cattle vocalizations provide essential information about the individuals. If we learn how to interpret this information correctly, it can be used to improve the management or welfare assessment. When under stress, cattle make sounds with a specific pattern (54). Despite the numerous studies published about the acoustic characteristics of such sounds (4, 10), there has not been an acoustic analysis regarding the behavior and the condition of cattle in isolation (55). Such an analysis could serve as a useful data source for surveillance systems (6). The bellow and the vocalization of grey cattle and water buffalo are similar to the vowels of human speech: they can be considered quasi-periodic signals, i.e., sound signals that cause a sense of pitch. While the vowels in speech are relatively short, the sounds of animals are long-lasting and quasi-stationary, i.e., their spectral properties change slowly and little. The typical number of shapes in a formant analysis of speech sounds is 6. Of these, only the first three characterize the speech sound, the others characterize the speaker. On this basis, five formants were considered sufficient for comparing the voices of the two species. We performed a mel scale-based cepstrum analysis of buffaloes and grey beef cows.

3.1 Intonation of vocalizations

Figure 1 shows the waveform of the vocalization of a water buffalo and the graph icon (solid line) of the pitch function. On the y-axis, the unit is the logarithm of the octave as a frequency ratio. In addition to the value provided by the pitch function, the figure contains the graph shifted up and down by one octave (dashed line). In our study, the fundamental frequency was partly automatically determined as a function of time, and then we fitted a sixth-degree polynomial to its logarithm (thick line). The result produced with NFC is the thin, continuous line the jumps one octave. This can be corrected with MATLAB.

The pitch of the vocalizations was examined separately for the two animal species. Some animal vocalizations do not have a constant pitch, they change slowly over time. The pitch values were determined the log2(Hz) scale. The mean pitches of each vocalization form the two samples for the two animal species. The Mann–Whitney U test, which we applied to the two samples, refuted (p-value = 7.52e−14) that the two samples come from the same distribution and that the two means are the same. Consequently, there is a significant difference between the pitches of the vocalizations of the two species. The two medians are 6.579 log2 (Hz) and 5.887 log2 (Hz) (i.e., 95.6 Hz and 59.18 Hz), their difference is 0.692 octaves, i.e., 4.15 whole intervals. The temporal means of the grey cattle’s vocalizations are on average much higher (Figure 2).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The mean pitch of the vocalizations was examined separately for the two animal species. The Mann–Whitney U test, which we applied to the two samples, refuted (p-value = 7.52e−14) that the two samples come from the same distribution and that the two means are the same. Figure shows that the pitch of grey cattle’s vocalizations is significantly higher than that of water buffalos.

The study also analyzed the pitch of the vocalizations and their range (Figure 3). The grey, circular points are the value pairs of a smaller population, they are meant to indicate the distribution of the value pairs in general. The different black marks also belong to one vocalization, and the same marks are different vocalizations of the same animal. The figures show that there is a large standard deviation for the individuals. Based on this methodology, we did not find differences between individuals of one species.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The relationship between the initial pitch and the sound range of the vocalizations. The grey circular points indicate the distribution of the value pairs (initial pitch, sound range) in general. The various black marks also belong to one vocalization, and the same marks are different vocalizations of the same animal. The figures show that there is a large standard deviation for the individuals.

We examined the average rate of pitch change. The pitch was again taken into account in log2(Hz). In the case of grey cattle and buffaloes, instead of the samples consisting of average rate values, the samples consisting of their logarithms can be considered as normally distributed. The applied Shapiro–Wilk normality test did not refute this in either case (p-value = 0.6494 or p-value = 0.8553). Applying the Welch Two Sample t-test to the two samples, we discarded the assumption that the medians of the two samples are the same (p-value = 2.36e-10). Figure 4 shows the parameters of the samples. The two medians are-1.349 log(log2(Hz)/s) and-0.199 log(log2(Hz)/s). Accordingly, the geometric means of the average rates of changes (we can now switch from log(log2(Hz)) to octaves) are 0.259 octaves/sec and 0.820 octaves/sec, which means that the average pitch change in 1 s is 1.6 whole intervals for grey cattle and 4.9 whole intervals for buffaloes. The tune of buffalo vocalizations is three times more dynamic than that of grey cattle.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The logarithm of the average rate of pitch change follows a normal distribution for both animal species. The Shapiro–Wilk normality test, which we applied to both samples, did not refute this in either case; therefore, we could apply the Welch Two Sample t-test and found that the dynamics of vocalizations are significantly different for the two animal species. The figure shows the result of this statistical examination. The tune of buffalo vocalizations is three times more dynamic than that of grey cattle.

The pitches are relatively low; consequently, it is particularly interesting when the energy is high at a few points in a period only. At such points, the sound resembles the rattle of a gun, although the lower limit of the hearing range is still ‘far away’ on the frequency scale. The extent to which the instantaneous energy is only high at a few points in one period of the waveform can be expressed by using power means. The property of power that we used is that the greater the parameter of the mean, the greater the weight with which greater values are included in the mean value. This property of power means is also used in the echo cancellation (Foundation, J.-M. V., www.speex.org) module of the speex codec program. The argument of the power mean will be the point-by-point square of one period of the quasi-periodic signal. We assumed that these values are all positive, so their power means can be formed. The degree of unevenness of the energy within the fundamental period was defined as the quotient of the 4-and 1-parameter power means. Figure 5 shows three periods of vocalization belonging to the smallest and the greatest value, H = energy unevenness.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The degree of unevenness of the energy within the fundamental period was defined as the quotient of the 4-and 1-parameter power means. It shows three periods of vocalization belonging to the smallest and the greatest value, H = energy unevenness.

We examined the relationship between these values for the two species. The energy unevenness measures of the vocalizations form the two patterns for the two animal species. The Mann–Whitney U test refuted (p-value4 = 0.001356) that the two samples come from the same distribution and that the two averages are the same. The averages are Hgrey cattle = 1.895, Hwater buffalo = 2.176.

The quality of animal vocalizations can be examined from the point of view of how much they move away from the continuous sound and become similar to a gun rattle, and how uneven the energy becomes within the fundamental period. To measure this, we introduced the quotient of two power means. The parameters of the power means are 4 and 1, and they refer to the energy values within the fundamental period. The figure shows three fundamental periods of four vocalization waveforms. The relevant energy unevenness quotient (H) is written above the graphs. As the figure shows, the more uneven the energy is, the higher is this value, for both grey cattle and water buffalo.

3.2 Spectral-based methods

Vowels are characterized by their formant structure. Only certain sounds in human speech are quasi-periodic. Vowels and voiced consonants are flexible. Sound recognition based on formant structure only works in the case of vowels. This is what we applied to animal vocalizations, to their quasi-periodic details. In the study, five formant frequencies were determined separately for grey cattle and water buffaloes. The formant structure is compressed by the spectrum into a number, into a series of 5–6 elements of formant frequency and width value pairs. Frequency is the more significant component of the pairs. As the function of frequency, the spectrum peaks at points that correspond to the formant frequencies. One of the characteristics of hearing is that the peaks overshadow their surroundings. This is the reason why we deal with the peaks only, concerning the sense. The width can determine the height of the peaks. In formant structures, the pairs are in an increasing order based on frequency. Figure 6 shows the parameters of the distributions (C1, B1), the pairs denote the median of the formant frequencies, separately for beef and buffalo. Only those columns can be compared that have the same index and do not differ significantly (Figure 6). Perhaps this method could be used to identify individuals, but there are not enough samples from each individual in this study.

Figure 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. The formant structures of vocalizations as quasi-periodic signals are standard characterizations of such signals. The figure shows that no significant difference between the two animal species can be detected using this method.

3.3 Mel scale cepstrum

Some studies have already incorporated human-derived algorithms to analyze and then recognize cattle vocalizations (44, 56). The MATLAB mfcc function returns the mel frequency cepstral coefficients (MFCCs) for the audio input (57).

The fundamental parameters of the function are the sound sample and the sampling frequency. Instead of the Fs actual sampling frequency, we chose Fs’ = 0.62*Fs so that the values of the members of the two populations are separated as much as possible. This value corresponds to what is known about the hearing range of animals, that the upper limit is around 30–40 kHz. The averages of the coefficients are 14–14 for cattle and buffalos, respectively. However, these averages are roughly the same for the two populations, except for the fourth and the eighth (Figure 7).

Figure 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. The averages of the mel frequency cepstral coefficients of the vocalizations show significant differences in the case of two coefficients, the 4th and 8th coefficients.

The difference can be verified statistically too. For both coefficients 4 (Figure 8) and 8 (Figure 9), the Mann–Whitney U test refuted (p-value4 = 2.2e-16, p-value8 = 5.9e-10) that the two samples come from the same distribution and that the two means are the same. The medians are M4grey cattle = 0.536, M4water buffalo = −0.910, M8grey cattle = 0.168, M8water buffalo = −0.389. We also demonstrate the distribution of coefficient pairs. (Figure 10).

Figure 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. The result of the statistical tests for the 4th mel frequency cepstral coefficient of vocalizations: in the case of grey cattle, this coefficient is significantly greater than that of water buffalo.

Figure 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. The result of the statistical tests for the 8th mel frequency cepstral coefficient of vocalizations: in the case of grey cattle, this coefficient is significantly greater than that of water buffalo.

Figure 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. The pairs consisting of the 4th and 8th coefficients of the mel frequency cepstral coefficients of the vocalizations. The empty circles represent the vocalizations of grey cattle and the filled circles are related to water buffalo.

4 Conclusion

The findings of bioacoustic studies of cattle have mainly been published concerning dairy cattle, focusing on the sounds that cattle make in response to weaning. To make use of such findings in advanced milk producing procedures in the future, it is necessary to better understand the sound kit of cattle and to interpret the information that the sounds convey. The specific acoustic parameters of the various cattle breeds need further research so that the species or breed effects can be excluded. In a large number of cattle farm animals neurobiological research is a difficult task that probably can be performed only with a small number of individuals in specialized laboratories. In this paper, we have analyzed the stress-induced oral vocalizations of water buffaloes and grey cattle using different methods. Concerning the acoustic features of vocalization, our data do not confirm the intense variability between cows, about which numerous studies have been published, but the difference between the two species was consistently detected. The pitch of individuals of the two species that we studied varies over time. The perception of pitch in terms of logHz value becomes a smooth variable, thus showing that the buffalo voice is three times more dynamic than that of the grey cattle. Our results can be utilized in the field of agriculture, for bioacoustic procedures.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics statement

The animals were treated in a manner according to the principles stated in Directive 2010/63/EU, regarding the protection of animals used for experimental and other scientific purposes, enforced in Hungary by Legislative Decrees Law No. XXVIII of 1998 on animal protection, therefore an ethics approval by an institutional review board is not necessary. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent was obtained from the owners for the participation of their animals in this study.

Author contributions

ÁL: Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft. ZP: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. IK: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. The publication is supported by the EFOP-3.6.3-VEKOP-16-2017-00008 project. The project is co-financed by the European Union and the European Social Fund.

Acknowledgments

The authors hereby thank the workers of Tiszatáj Foundation for permitting to make the observations on the Szamárhát Farm in Kesznyéten, Hungary.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Upadhyay, MR, Chen, W, Lenstra, JA, Goderie, CRJ, and MacHugh, DE. Genetic origin, admixture and population history of aurochs (Bos primigenius) and primitive European cattle. Heredity. (2017) 118:169–76. doi: 10.1038/hdy.2016.79

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Zhang, Y, Colli, L, and Barker, JSF. Asian water buffalo: domestication, history and genetics. Animal Genet. (2020) 51:177–91. doi: 10.1111/age.12911

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Bertoni, A, Álvarez-Macías, A, Mota-Rojas, D, Dávalos, JL, and Minervino, AHH. Dual-purpose water buffalo production systems in tropical latin america: bases for a sustainable model. Animals. (2021) 11:2910. doi: 10.3390/ani11102910

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Kiley, M. The vocalizations of ungulates, their causation and function. Z Tierpsychol. (1972) 31:171–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1972.tb01764.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Færevik, G, Jensen, MB, and Bøe, KE. Dairy calves social preferences and the significance of a companion animal during separation from the group. Animal Behav Sci. (2006) 99:205–21. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.10.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Meen, GH, Schellekens, MA, Slegers, MHM, Leenders, NLG, and van der Kooij, E. Sound analysis in dairy cattle vocalisation as a potential welfare monitor. Comput Electron Agric. (2015) 118:111–5. doi: 10.1016/j.compag.2015.08.028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Muñoz, JC, Huerta, M, Bueno, AL, and Rangel, R. Production and nutritional quality of forages in conditions humid tropics of Mexico. Rev Mex Ciencias Agrícolas. (2016) 16:3315–27. doi: 10.29312/remexca.v0i16.400

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Manteuffel, G, Puppe, B, and Schön, PC. Vocalization of farm animals as a measure of welfare. Animal Behav Sci. (2004) 88:163–82. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2004.02.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Schloeth, R. Das sozialleben des Camargue-rindes. Z Tierpsychol. (1961) 1961:574–627.

Google Scholar

10. Watts, JM, and Stookey, JM. Vocal behaviour in cattle: the animal's commentary on its biological processes and welfare. Animal Behav Sci. (2000) 67:15–33. doi: 10.1016/s0168-1591(99)00108-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Briefer, E, and McElligott, AG. Mutual mother-offspring vocal recognition in an ungulate hider species (Capra hircus). Animal Cogn. (2011) 14:585. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0396-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Padilla de la Torre, M, Brieferb, EF, Readera, T, and McElligottb, AG. Acoustic analysis of cattle (Bos taurus) mother–offspringcontact calls from a source–filter theory perspective. Animal Behav Sci. (2015) 163:58–68. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.11.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Schön, PC, Hamel, K, Puppe, B, Tuchscherer, A, and Kanitz, W. Altered vocalization rate during the estrous cycle in dairy cattle. Am Dairy Sci Assoc. (2007) 90:202–6. doi: 10.3168/jds.S0022-0302(07)72621-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Canozzi, MEA, Mederos, A, Zago, D, and Barcellos, J. A systematic review-meta-analysis of the effects of castration or dehorning on vocalization in beef cattle. Conference: IV Congresso Brasileiro de Bioética e Bem-estar Animal poster (2017). 2017 p.

Google Scholar

15. Barfield, CH, Tang-Martinez, Z, and Trainer, JM. Domestic Calvesbos taurus. Recognize their own mothers by auditory cues. Ethology. (1994) 97:257–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1994.tb01045.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Padilla De La Torre, M, Briefer, EF, Ochocki, BM, McElligott, AG, and Reader, T. Mother–offspring recognition via contact calls in cattle. Animal Behav. (2016) 114:147–54. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.02.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Yajuvendra, S, Lathwal, SS, Rajput, N, Raja, TV, Gupta, AK, Mohanty, TK, et al. Effective and accurate discrimination of individual dairy cattle through acoustic sensing. Animal Behav Sci. (2013) 146:11–8. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.03.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Ede, T, Lecorps, B, von Keyserlingk, MAG, and Weary, DM. Symposium review: scientific assessment of affective states in dairy cattle. J Dairy Sci. (2019) 102:10677–94. doi: 10.3168/jds.2019-16325

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Green, A, Clark, C, Favaro, L, Lomax, S, and Reby, D. Vocal individuality of Holstein - Friesian cattle is maintained across putatively positive and negative farming contexts. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:18468. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-54968-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Green, AC, Lidfors, LM, Lomax, S, Favaro, L, and Clark, CEF. Vocal production in postpartum dairy cows: temporal organization and association with maternal and stress behaviors. J Dairy Sci. (2021) 104:826–38. doi: 10.3168/jds.2020-18891

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Meagher, RK, Beaver, A, Weary, DM, and von Keyserlingk, MAG. A systematic review of the effects of prolonged cow–calf contact on behavior, welfare, and productivity. J Dairy Sci. (2019) 102:5765–83. doi: 10.3168/jds.2018-16021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Grandin, T. Cattle vocalizations are associated with handling and equipment problems at beef slaughter plants. Animal Behav Sci. (2001) 71:191–201. doi: 10.1016/s0168-1591(00)00179-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Watts, JM, and Stookey, JM. Effects of restraint and branding on rates and acoustic parameters of vocalization in beef cattle. Anim Behav Sci. (1999) 62:125–35. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(98)00222-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Mueller, R, and Schrader, L. Behavioural consistency during social separation and personality in dairy cows. Behaviour. (2005) 142:1289–306. doi: 10.1163/156853905774539346

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Flower, FC, and Weary, DM. Effects of early separation on the dairy cow and calf: 2. Separation at 1 day and 2 weeks after birth. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2001) 70:275–84. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(00)00164-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Lidfors, LM. Behavioural effects of separating the dairy calf immediately or4 days post-partum. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (1996) 49:269–83. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(96)01053-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Mota-Rojas, D, Bienboire-Frosini, C, Orihuela, A, Domínguez-Oliva, A, Villanueva García, D, Mora-Medina, P, et al. Mother-offspring bonding after calving in water Buffalo and other ruminants: sensory pathways and neuroendocrine aspects. Animals. (2024) 14:2696. doi: 10.3390/ani14182696

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Orihuela, A, Mota-Rojas, D, Strappini, A, Serrapica, F, Braghieri, A, Mora-Medina, P, et al. Neurophysiological mechanisms of cow–calf bonding in Buffalo and other farm animals. Animals. (2021) 11:1968. doi: 10.3390/ani11071968

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Hermann, E, and Stenum, N. Mother–calf behaviour during the first six hours after parturition In: JP Signoret, editor. Current topics in veterinary medicine and animal science. Welfare and husbandry of calves. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff Publishers (1982). 3–23.

Google Scholar

30. Lenner, A, Papp, ZL, Szabo, C, and Komlosi, I. Calming Hungarian Grey cattle in headlocks using processed nasal vocalization of a mother cow. Animals. (2024) 14:135. doi: 10.3390/ani14010135

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Briefer, E. Vocal expression of emotions in mammals: mechanisms of production and evidence. J Zool. (2012) 288:1–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2012.00920.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Taylor, AM, and Reby, D. The contribution of source-filter theory to mammal vocal communication research. J Zool. (2010) 280:221–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.2009.00661.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fant, G. Acoustic theory of speech production: With calculations based on x-ray studies of Russian articulations. The Hague: Mouton (1960).

Google Scholar

34. Hopster, H, O’Connell, JM, and Blokhuis, HJ. Acute effects of cow-calf separation on heart rate, plasma cortisol and behaviour in multiparous dairy cows. Animal Behav Sci. (1995) 44:1–8. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(95)00581-c

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Heffner, HE. Auditory awareness. Animal behaviour. Science. (1998) 57:259–68. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(98)00101-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Manci, KM, Gladwin, DN, Villella, R, and Cavendish, MG. Effects of aircraft noise and sonic booms on domestic animals and wildlife: a literature synthesis. Ft Collins: US Fish and Wildlife Service National Ecology Research Center (1988).

Google Scholar

37. Phillips, CJC. Housing, handling and the environment for cattle In: Principles of cattle production. E3rd ed: VetBooks, CABI Digital Library. (2018). 95–128.

Google Scholar

38. Pšenka, M, Šístková, M, Mihina, Š, and Gálik, R. Frequency analysis of noise exposure of dairy cows in the process of milking. Res Agric Eng. (2016) 62:185–9. doi: 10.17221/4/2015-RAE

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Henderson, J. (2011). Building US agricultural exports: one BRIC at a time.

Google Scholar

40. Grandin, T. The feasibility of using vocalization scoring as an indicator of poor welfare during cattle slaughter. Animal Behav Sci. (1998) 56:121–8. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(97)00102-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Röttgen, V, Becker, F, Tuchscherer, A, Wrenzycki, C, Düpjan, S, Schön, PC, et al. Vocalization as an indicator of estrus climax in Holstein heifers during natural estrus and superovulation. J Dairy Sci. (2018) 101:2383–94. doi: 10.3168/jds.2017-13412

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Sahidullah, M, and Saha, G. Design, analysis and experimental evaluation of block based transformation in MFCC computation for speaker recognition. Speech Commun. (2011) 54:543–65. doi: 10.1016/j.specom.2011.11.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Bishop, J.C., Falzon, G., Trotter, M., Kwan, P., and Meek, P.D. (2017). Sound analysis and detection, and the potential for precision livestock farming-a sheep vocalization case study. In Proceedings of the 1st Asian-Australasian conference on precision pastures and livestock farming, Hamilton, New Zealand 2017; pp. 1–7.

Google Scholar

44. Chung, Y, Lee, J, Oh, S, Park, D, and Chang, HH. Automatic detection of cow’s oestrus in audio surveillance system. Asian Austr J Anim Sci. (2013) 26:1030–7. doi: 10.5713/ajas.2012.12628201326,1030-1037

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Devi, I, Singh, P, Lathwal, SS, Dudi, K, and Singh, Y. Threshold values of acoustic features to assess estrous cycle phases in water buffaloes (Bubalus bubalis). Animal Behav Sci. (2019) 219:104838. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2019.104838

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Khunarsa, P, Mahawan, J, Nakjai, P, and Onkhum, N. Nondestructive determination of maturity of the monthong durian by mel-frequency cepstral coefficients (MFCCs) and neural network. Mech Mater. (2016) 2016:75–81. doi: 10.4028/www.scientific.net/AMM.855.75

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Rabiner, LR. Theory and applications of digital speech processing. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson (2010).

Google Scholar

48. Wang, J, Chen, H, Wang, J, Zhao, K, Li, X, Liu, B, et al. Identification of oestrus cows based on vocalisation characteristics and machine learning technique using a dual-channel-equipped acoustic tag. Animal. (2023) 17:100811. doi: 10.1016/j.animal.2023.100811

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Banhazi, T, and Black, J. Precision livestock farming: a suite of electronic systems to ensure the application of best practice management on livestock farms. Aust J Multi-discip Eng. (2009) 7:1–14. doi: 10.1080/14488388.2009.11464794

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Özmen, G, Özkan, IA, İnalc, S, Taşdemirb, S, and Çamc, M. Sound analysis to recognize cattle vocalization in a semi-open barn. J Eng Sci. (2021) 8:158–67. doi: 10.30855/gmbd.2022.01.15

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Haro, HA, Suarez-Usbeck, A, Burgos-Mayorga, A, Navarrete, J, and Ramonez, J. Vocalizacion como marcador de estres en ganado bovino. Vocal Excep. (2023) 13:126–31. doi: 10.54753/cedamaz.v13i1.1763

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Rushen, J, Wright, R, Johnsen, JF, Mejdell, CM, and de Passillé, AM. Reduced locomotor play behaviour of dairy calves following separation from the mother reflects their response to reduced energy intake. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2016) 177:6–11. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.01.023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Atal, BS. Automatic speaker recognition based on pitch contours. J Soc Am. (1972) 52:1687–97. doi: 10.1121/1.1913303

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Yeon, SC, Jeon, JH, Houpt, KA, Chang, HH, Lee, HC, and Lee, HJ. Acoustic features of vocalizations of Korean native cows (Bos taurus coreanea) in two different conditions. Animal Behav Sci. (2006) 101:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2006.01.013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Sauvé, CC, Beauplet, G, Hammill, MO, and Charrier, I. Mother–pup vocal recognition in harbour seals: influence of maternal behaviour, pup voice and habitat sound properties. Anim Behav. (2015) 2015:109–20. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.04.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Jahns, G. Call recognition to identify cow conditions - a call-recogniser translating calls to text. Comput Electron Agric. (2008) 62:54–8. doi: 10.1016/j.compag.2007.09.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Auditory Toolbox. Forrás. Available online at: https://engineering.purdue.edu/~malcolm/interval/1998-010/AuditoryToolboxTechReport.pdf (Accessed [October, 1998]).

Google Scholar

Keywords: vocalization, buffalo, beef, cepstrum, mel scale bio acoustic

Citation: Lenner Á, Papp ZL and Komlósi I (2025) Analysis of sounds made by Bos taurus and Bubalus bubalis dams to their calves. Front. Vet. Sci. 12:1549100. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2025.1549100

Received: 08 January 2025; Accepted: 24 February 2025;
Published: 18 March 2025.

Edited by:

Daniel Mota-Rojas, Metropolitan Autonomous University, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Marcelo Ghezzi, Universidad Nacional del Centro de Buenos Aires, Argentina
Antonio Verduzco-Mendoza, National Institute of Rehabilitation Luis Guillermo Ibarra Ibarra, Mexico

Copyright © 2025 Lenner, Papp and Komlósi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ádám Lenner, bGVubmVyNTAwdHRAZ21haWwuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset

95% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more