- 1Institute for Animal Welfare, Animal Behavior and Laboratory Animal Science, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- 2Institute for Veterinary Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- 3Department Sensors and Modelling, Leibniz Institute for Agricultural Engineering and Bioeconomy (ATB), Potsdam, Germany
Introduction: The novel object test is one of the three most common fear tests in veterinary science and employed in several different species. Although having been applied in several different studies in horses, it is surprising that there is no standardized test procedure available for these kinds of tests.
Methods: This study investigated the performance of the novel object test on 42 young Thoroughbred horses to determine the effect of sex (mare or stallion), test sites (round pen or paddock) and whether the horses had previously participated in an auction or not on the behavior during the novel object test.
Results: Differences in horses’ behavior during the novel object test were primarily attributed to the test sites. The animals showed significant (p < 0.05) intra-individual differences in the novel object test in the round pen and in the paddock. Sex did not affect the direct interaction with the novel object. The horses that had not participated in an auction seemed to actively perceive the novel object more quickly, so that the latency to first fixation on the object was significantly shorter.
Discussion: In order to obtain comparable results, it is recommended that novel object tests should be performed at the same location and under identical conditions. Furthermore, it is important to consider the individual behavior of each horse.
1 Introduction
The novel object test is a behavioral test designed to investigate exploratory and fear behavior of unknown objects when animals are confronted with an unfamiliar, usually stationary object in a defined enclosure. The novel object test is typically conducted on individual animals (1). Novel object tests are used in many studies with horses for various purposes. In some studies, the novel object test is used to assess the character or temperament of horses (2–4) or, for example, to select a suitable horse for a rider (5). Under the premise that the way an animal reacts to a novel object can indicate its levels of stress and anxiety but also exploratory motivation, curiosity and play behavior, novel object tests can be used for the assessment of animal welfare (6, 7) or are part of pharmacological studies (8). However, the literature lacks a standardized procedure for performing novel object tests in horses. Novel object tests are typically conducted in indoor arenas like round pens (8, 9) or a training hall (10–12) or paddocks (13, 14). In some studies, horses are exposed to unfamiliar objects in their stables to prevent external factors, such as handling or social separation, from affecting the results (7, 15). However, the specific method used is not consistent. Moreover, the age and previous experience of the animal may influence behavior during the novel object test. It is, however, important to note that it is difficult to separate age from the experiences a horse has had throughout its life (14). According to Bulens et al. (7), younger horses exhibited more object-related behavior during a novel object test which might indicate that younger horses are more curious than older horses. Graf et al. (16) found that older horses showed less fearful behavior when confronted with different unknown stimuli than younger horses and Visser et al. (17) showed that the physiological stress response to a novel object test decreased with increasing age as indicated by heart rate which might be attributed to a higher habituation to different situations in older horses. Age and previous experience should therefore be taken into account when assessing behavior during novel object tests. In other studies, the age and sex of the horses had only a minor influence on the results of the novel object test, much more consideration was given to the genetics of the animals (7, 18, 19). It is well-established that horses are capable of discriminating between different colors and shapes of objects. However, they do not exhibit generalization between objects that differ in both color and shape (20). The housing system also effects behavior during the novel object test (7). Research has shown that box housing is associated with increased activity levels during the novel object test (18). A significant difference when implementing the novel object test can be noted when humans are present or not. Research has shown that horses tend to touch the novel object more quickly when they are led by a handler on a rope (13). In general, as a prey species, being vulnerable to various stimuli is essential for horses and different test conditions may therefore influence their behavior and thus the results of behavioral tests.
The novel object test is recommended by various institutions. For instance, the Wageningen Welfare Assessment Protocol for horses suggests a novel object test with one person restraining the horse on a rope while another person presents a Rubik’s cube (6). The German Veterinary Association has included the novel object test in the protocol for the mandatory examination of Thoroughbred racehorses before their first race (9). The purpose of this behavioral test is to evaluate the stress level of racehorses in addition to their general behavioral assessment in the husbandry system and during handling. The test is intended to evaluate the horses’ mental and physical capacity to cope with the demands of a race and associated preparations. To achieve this, a modified version of the fear test described in the AWIN Welfare Assessment protocol for horses was selected. During the fear test, the horse is presented with an unfamiliar object in its stall, such as a bottle filled with stones, which is suspended from the ceiling without movement and then dropped in the subsequent step (21). Testing horses in their home stable has the advantage that novel object tests can be easily performed and repeated (22) and that results are not influenced by changing the environment, social isolation or handling (7, 15, 23). However, the test described in the protocol for racehorses is conducted in an arena instead of a box, allowing the horse more freedom of movement to exhibit flight reactions (9).
The German Veterinary Association provides precise recommendations for the performance of the novel object test. According to these recommendations, the test should be performed in a familiar, enclosed area that is preferably covered. To ensure accurate results, it is recommended to avoid any visual or acoustic distractions from food, conspecifics, or people during the test. Additionally, it is emphasized that the test should be conducted in the same location before the horse’s first training and first race to ensure comparability of results (9). The veterinarian conducting the test evaluates the outcome of the test, taking into account the entire examination as well as the husbandry environment and decides whether the horse is physically and mentally fit to cope with the demands of training and racing.
Valid interpretation of novel object tests requires comprehensive knowledge of environmental and individual factors that might influence the behavior during a novel object test. To the best of the authors´ knowledge, previous studies have paid little attention to the influence of different environments, sex and the horses’ previous life experiences in terms of their behavior during the novel object test. The aim of this study therefore was to determine if the horse’s behavior during the novel object test and consequently the test results are impacted by the environment and whether there are sex-specific differences. Given that a horse’s previous experiences can impact its behavior, it is critical to consider this potential influencing factor as well.
2 Materials and methods
This study and its procedure were approved by the State Office for Consumer Protection of Saarland (Landesamt für Verbraucherschutz Saarland). It is not considered to be an animal experiment requiring authorization.
2.1 Animals
Forty-two Thoroughbred horses (19 stallions and 23 mares) were selected from a stable in Germany specialized in the preparation of young racehorses. The horses were housed in individual stalls, but were allowed to graze in small groups on pastures for at least 2 h on 5 days a week. They were fed on hay and stable’s own mixture of high-energy, racing-horse feed. The horses were between 23 and 27 months old and had experience in handling. The general handling of the horses included leading them by halter and rope or a bridle, as well as daily contact with the stable staff when feeding and cleaning the stalls. Additionally, the horses experienced regular veterinary and farrier care. The study was conducted in February. At the time of the study, 36 of the selected horses (18 mares and 18 stallions) had been in pre-training for approximately 4–5 months. The other 6 horses (1 stallion and 5 mares) had experience in handling, but had not yet been under saddle. Of the 36 horses that had already been under saddle, 24 had previously been at auction and 12 had not been at auction. Of the 6 horses that had not yet been ridden, 3 had been to an auction and 3 had not been to an auction. If horses are sold at auction as yearlings, they are usually accustomed to being bridled, transported and being housed in an unfamiliar environment. It is reasonable to assume that a horse that has been to an auction has gained significantly more experience, which may affect its behavior in a novel object test. On the other hand, it is also possible that the abrupt social separation and potentially unfamiliar handling practices during an auction lead to negative associations with novel environments which could potentially cause higher stress responses during a novel object test. Whether the horses have participated in an auction or not was therefore also taken into consideration. None of the horses had any experience in behavioral testing.
2.2 Implementation of novel object test
Novel object tests were carried out in February 2023 at two different test sites. The first test site was a standard round pen (Figure 1) surrounded by a horse walker commonly found on horse farms. At a height of two meters, the round pen was surrounded by opaque wooden walls. The round pen had a diameter of 14 meters and was filled with a mixture of sand. The entire facility was covered with the roof in the center of the enclosure made of plexiglass, allowing for daylight to enter. The second test site for this study was a paddock (Figure 2). The sand paddock, with a side length of 18 by 18 meters, was surrounded by a wooden fence, including an electric wire, and a metal gate. The horses were familiar with the round pen and associated horse walker due to regular use during training. However, since both the stallions and mares were allowed to go out into large pastures in small groups, the horses were not fully accustomed to being kept alone in the round pen or on a single paddock.
Figure 1. The novel object used during the test in the round pen; the pool raft shaped like a mermaid’s tail placed in the center of the testing site.
Figure 2. The novel object used during the test in a paddock; the pool raft ‘clown fish’ was placed in the center of the testing site.
All horses were tested once in the round pen. In addition, 12 horses (6 mares and 6 stallions) completed a second novel object test in the paddock 3 days later. Due to the limited amount of time that was available to conduct this study and to be able to test all horses on the same day in order to reduce the impact of different whether conditions on the results, only 12 horses were subjected to a second test in the paddock. In preparation for the novel object test, an inflatable pool raft was placed in the center of the test site. In order to rule out the possibility of familiarization with the object, two different pool rafts were used in the two different test locations. For the round pen, the pool raft in shape of a “mermaid’s tail” (approx. 170 × 85 × 100 cm) served as the unknown object, whereas the object “clown fish” (approx. 155 × 86.5 × 39 cm) was used in the paddock. The horses were led to the selected test site by a known person, turned with their heads toward the entrance, and then released from the rope and halter. The person left the test site immediately and a timer was set for 10 min once the entrance door was closed. The persons conducting the test moved out of sight so that the horses could not see or hear them to minimize their influence on the horses’ behavior during the novel object test. The horses were always in sight to enable the researchers to intervene quickly in case of an emergency. Two GoPro hero 11 black action cameras (GoPro, Inc., San Mateo, CA, USA) facing the novel object were installed on opposite sides, allowing video recordings of the horses’ behavior during the entire test situation. The horses were habituated to the cameras through previous video recordings that were made during the horse’s daily routine in their stables and on pasture. The novel object test was completed precisely after 10 min, irrespective of the horses’ preceding behavioral responses. After each test, the test area was cleared of feces and uneven ground was leveled. If the test object was moved by the horse during the test, it was returned to its original position in the center of the test area. To minimize the influence of female pheromones on the behavior of the stallions, the stallions were tested before the mares on each of the test days.
2.3 Assessment of behavior during novel object test
Evaluation was performed by an independent observer who was experienced in equine behavior studies. The videos were randomized and blinded for the observation. The software “The Observer XT 16” (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, Netherlands, version 16.0.1203) was used to code the type, number and duration of certain behaviors according to a previously developed ethogram (Table 1). The ethogram was developed based on a previously defined standard ethogram but was specifically adapted for this study in order to enable a comprehensive and consistent evaluation of the horses’ behavior during the novel object test. All behaviors exhibited by the horses were included and each behavior was defined with precision. The time required for the horse to initially fixate, approach and touch the unknown object was observed, as well as the horse’s manner of interaction with the novel object. In addition to the video-based data collection, the horses were fitted with activity sensors (GT9XLink, ActiGraph, Pensacola, FL, USA). These sensors were attached to the left side of the horse’s neck using a collar and recorded relevant activities such as standing, walking, trotting, cantering and rolling (24). All horses were familiarized with wearing the sensor collars for several hours before the novel object tests. None of the horses showed any aversive behavior that was related to the attachment of the sensor collars.
2.4 Statistical analysis
Due to technical issues, two video recordings and three sensor recordings from the novel object tests in the round pen could not be analyzed. As a result, data analysis included a total of 12 novel object tests in the paddock and 40 (video) / 39 (sensor) tests in the round pen. All statistical analyses were performed using R (25). The effect of test site (round pen or paddock), sex (mare or stallion) and auction (whether the horses participated in an auction or not) on the behavior during the novel object test was evaluated using linear or generalized linear mixed effects models with test site, sex and auction as well as the interaction between sex and test site and sex and auction as fixed effects and horse as random effect. The interaction between test site and auction was not included since the horses that had been tested twice in different test sites, had all been previously to an auction. By including horse as random effect, it was accounted for within-subject correlation in horses that were tested twice. Depending on the dependent variable being continuous (durations) or discrete (count data), linear or negative binomial generalized linear mixed effects models were fitted using the ‘lmer’ or ‘glmer.nb’ function in R. The significance of the fixed effects on the behavior during the novel object test was evaluated by comparing full models to models without the fixed effects by means of likelihood ratio tests using the ‘anova’ function in R. Normality of the error distributions of the linear regression models was evaluated by means of normal probability plots of the residuals. The results are presented as box plots and estimated marginal means. Results that are presented in Table 2 are provided as estimated marginal means ± standard error. In order to describe the behavior of the horses during the novel object tests as comprehensively as possible, a relatively large number of different behavioral parameters was recorded during this study. To reduce the dimensionality of the data and to find associations between different behavioral parameters, principal component analysis was conducted using the ‘prcomp’ function in R. Subsequently, a kmeans cluster analysis of the coordinates of the different variables on the principal components (retaining only those components with an eigenvalue >1) was carried out. The resulting clusters were visualized in the loadings plot of the first two principal components.
Table 2. Statistical analysis of the behavior during the novel object test (results are presented as estimated marginal means ± standard error).
3 Results
The most significant differences in behavior during the novel object test were related to the impact of the test site. When comparing the behavior of the 12 horses that completed the novel object test on both test sites, the following observations can be made: 11 out of 12 horses had touched the novel object in the paddock. However, 4 of these 12 horses did not touch the object in the round pen. Comparison of the effect of test sites showed multiple significant differences: The latency time for the first fixation on the object (Figure 3A) and the first approach (Figure 3C) was higher in the round pen. However, the effects of test site, sex and whether the horses had attended an auction on the latency time to touch the object for the first time were not statistically significant. When the novel object test was performed in the round pen, the horses fixated the novel object longer (Figure 4A). The number of fixations on the object (Figure 4B), approaches to the object (Figure 4C) and touches of the object with the muzzle (Figure 4D) were higher in the round pen. With regard to those behaviors that are not directly related to the novel object, blowing (Figure 5A) and defecating (Figure 5C) were more frequent in the round pen than in the paddock. On the other hand, the behaviors chewing and licking, squeaking, standing at the exit (Figure 5B), flinching and shaking (Figure 5D) were more frequent in the paddock than in the round pen. Depending on the test site, activity sensor data indicated that the horses spent more time walking (Figure 6A) and less time galloping (Figure 6C) in the round pen than in the paddock. Moreover, the horses spent more time standing alert (Figure 6D) in the round pen than in the paddock.
Figure 3. Latency time until the first fixation of the object (Latency first fix obj) compared between test sites (A; paddock: n = 12, round pen: n = 40) and between horses that participated in an auction (n = 39) or not (n = 13) (B) as well as latency time until the first approach toward the object (Latency first approach obj) compared between test sites (C) and sexes (D). The box plots display the distribution of data by indicating the median and the interquartile range (IQR). Individual data points from mares (n = 27) are shown either as red circles (horses that participated in an auction, n = 21) or red squares (horses that did not participate in an auction, n = 6). Individual data points from stallions (n = 25) are shown in blue circles (n = 18) or squares (n = 7) accordingly. The marginal means estimated by the mixed effects models are shown as blue asterisks. p-values result from negative-binomial regression models.
Figure 4. Duration of fixation of the object (Duration fix obj) (A), number of fixations of the object (Nr fix obj) (B), number of approaches toward the object (Nr approach obj) (C) and number of touches of the object with the muzzle (Nr touch obj muzzle) (D) compared between test sites (paddock: n = 12, round pen: n = 40). The box plots display the distribution of data by indicating the median and the interquartile range (IQR). Individual data points from mares (n = 27) are shown either as red circles (horses that participated in an auction, n = 21) or red squares (horses that did not participate in an auction, n = 6). Individual data points from stallions (n = 25) are shown in blue circles (n = 18) or squares (n = 7) accordingly. The marginal means estimated by the mixed effects models are shown as blue asterisks. p-values result from linear regression models for durations and negative-binomial regression models for count data.
Figure 5. Number of blowing (Nr blowing) (A), number of standing at the exit (Nr standing exit) (B) number of defecations (Nr defecating) (C) and number of shaking (Nr shaking) (D) compared between test sites (paddock: n = 12, round pen: n = 40). The box plots display the distribution of data by indicating the median and the interquartile range (IQR). Individual data points from mares are shown either as red circles (horses that participated in an auction, n = 21) or red squares (horses that did not participate in an auction, n = 6). Individual data points from stallions are shown in blue circles (n = 18) or squares (n = 7) accordingly. The marginal means estimated by the mixed effects models are shown as blue asterisks. p-values result from negative-binomial regression models.
Figure 6. Duration walking (Duration WAL) during the novel object test compared between test sites (paddock: n = 12, round pen: n = 39) (A), duration trotting (Duration TRO) compared between sexes (B) as well as duration galloping (Duration GAL) (C) and standing alert (Duration SAL) (D) compared between test sites. The box plots display the distribution of data by indicating the median and the interquartile range (IQR). Individual data points from mares (n = 27) are shown either as red circles (horses that participated in an auction, n = 19) or red squares (horses that did not participate in an auction, n = 8). Individual data points from stallions (n = 24) are shown in blue circles (n = 18) or squares (n = 6) accordingly. The marginal means estimated by the mixed effects models are shown as blue asterisks. p-values result from linear regression models for durations and negative-binomial regression models for count data.
Some behavioral parameters also appeared to be significantly associated with sex. Mares approached the novel object for the first time (Figure 3D) earlier than stallions. Stallions sniffed the ground (Figure 7A) longer and pawed (Figure 7C) more frequently than mares. Vocalization (Figure 7B) and standing at the exit (Figure 7D) were more striking in mares. Mares also appeared to be more active, with longer durations trotting (Figure 6B) than stallions.
Figure 7. Duration of sniffing the ground (Duration sniff ground) (A), Number of vocalizations (Nr neighing) (B), number of pawing (Nr pawing) (C) and number of standing at the exit (Nr standing exit) (D) compared between sexes (stallions: n = 25, mares: n = 27). The box plots display the distribution of data by indicating the median and the interquartile range (IQR). Individual data points from mares are shown either as red circles (horses that participated in an auction, n = 21) or red squares (horses that did not participate in an auction, n = 6). Individual data points from stallions are shown in blue circles (n = 18) or squares (n = 7) accordingly. The marginal means estimated by the mixed effects models are shown as blue asterisks. p-values result from linear regression models for durations and negative-binomial regression models for count data.
When comparing whether or not the horses had participated in an auction, the following significant results could be demonstrated: The latency time until the first fixation of the object (Figure 3B) as well as the duration of sniffing the ground (Figure 8A) was longer for the horses that attended an auction. Furthermore, the horses that did not participate in an auction were standing at the exit (Figure 8B) more often.
Figure 8. Duration of sniffing the ground (Duration sniff ground) (A) and number of standing at the exit (Nr standing exit) (B) compared between horses that participated in an auction (n = 39) or not (n = 13). The box plots display the distribution of data by indicating the median and the interquartile range (IQR). Individual data points from mares are shown either as red circles (horses that participated in an auction, n = 21) or red squares (horses that did not participate in an auction, n = 6). Individual data points from stallions are shown in blue circles (n = 18) or squares (n = 7) accordingly. The marginal means estimated by the mixed effects models are shown as blue asterisks. p-values result from linear regression models for durations and negative-binomial regression models for count data.
Cluster analysis was performed using the first 9 principal components accounting for 77% of the variation in the data. The resulting clusters are visualized in the loadings plot of the first two principal components (Figure 9A). The principal component and k means cluster analysis show that certain behavioral parameters that have been observed during the novel object test were more associated with each other than others and formed clusters of parameters that jointly contribute to the principal components. Six main clusters of highly associated behaviors could be identified and are visualized in Figure 9A. When comparing the individual data points between test sites (Figure 9B) and sexes (Figure 9C), it becomes apparent that the point cloud for the tests conducted on the paddock is slightly shifted toward the upper left quadrant of the plot, indicating that the horses exhibited less engagement with the novel object. The point cloud for the mares is slightly shifted toward active and anxious behavior, whereas the point cloud for stallions is slightly shifted toward exploratory and comfort behavior. However, both point clouds overlap in the region of behaviors related to the novel object.
Figure 9. Correlation (loadings) between the different behavioral variables and the first (PC 1) and second principal component (PC 2) (A). The variation explained by the principal components is given in % of total variability in the axe’s labels. The quality of representation of the variables by the principal components (cos2) is indicated by the color of the arrows and labels. The further away a variable is from the origin of the plot, the more important it is to interpret these components. The closer to the center of the plot a variable is, the less important it is for the first two components. Variables whose arrows point in the same direction are positively correlated and variables whose arrows point in opposite directions are negatively correlated to each other. The clusters identified by means of kmeans clustering of the coordinates of the variables on the first 9 principal components (eigenvalue >1) are indicated by different coloring of the data points associated with the variables. Individual data points for PC 1 and PC 2 compared between novel object tests conducted in the round pen (green) or paddock (dark blue) (B) and compared between mares (red) and stallions (light blue) (C). Individual data points form horses that participated in an auction are shown as circles, horses that did not participate in an auction are shown as squares (B,C).
4 Discussion
The results of this study show considerable inter-individual differences in the behavior of horses during the novel object test. Moreover, the behavior of the horses was significantly influenced by the external circumstances of the test situation since it could be shown that the test site had a significant effect on the behavior of the horses. The horses fixated the novel object faster and made the first approach toward the object earlier in the paddock than in the round pen. On the other hand, the total duration for which the horses fixated the object was shorter and the number of fixations as well as the number of approaches toward the object and touches of the object with the muzzle was lower in the paddock. Thus, the horses engaged with the object earlier but for a shorter time when the test took place in the paddock. Little interaction with the novel object may indicate fear or stress (7). On the other hand, exploration of the novel object, such as sniffing or nibbling, is evaluated as absence of anxiety (26). The difference in these behaviors between test sites may indicate that in the environment that is more open, the horses are less afraid to approach the novel object but are too distracted by the surroundings to explore the object more closely. However, the faster fixation and approach toward the novel object and the lower extent of explorative behavior toward the object may also be attributed to a habituation effect from the first test although the object and location was different. In view of the influence of the testing environment of the behavior during the novel object test, the question arises to what extent the influence of the test conditions was currently taken into account in the interpretation of the results of novel object tests. The German Veterinary Society recommends including a novel object test in the ‘Examination protocol for racehorses before the start of training and first start of life’. Although it is recommended that the test should be performed in a familiar, enclosed area that is preferably covered, test environments will vary depending on the local conditions of the stable. The test environments evaluated in this study were chosen to reflect different conditions that can be typically found in stables accommodating racehorses. It is also recommended that the tests prior to the start of training and the horse’s first race should be performed on the same test site on both occasions to ensure comparability. The results of our study support this recommendation as they demonstrated that the same horses exhibited significantly different behavior when tested in different environments. Moreover, the protocol recommends a maximum test duration of 5 min. The latency for the horse to olfactory explore the novel object for the first time, approach it and touch it is measured but no further interpretation of the behavioral test results is provided here (9).
The recommendation to perform the novel object test at the same location both times is supported, but the question of feasibility arises. Many horses are no longer housed in the same barn before the first training session and later before the first race, making it difficult to compare data. Given the findings of this study, it may be worthwhile to consider whether the recommended 5-min test time is sufficient, as the average latency time until the horses touched the object for the first time was higher than 5 min.
To ensure a consistent interpretation of the results, it is crucial to consider the test location. Comparing results obtained in a paddock with those from a round pen is not appropriate. As horses are prey species and highly sensitive to their environment, any external stimulus can affect the results of the novel object test. Therefore, it is recommended to compare the results of novel object tests only within individuals under the same conditions. The presence of humans also influences the horse’s behavior, which is why indirect behavioral observation using cameras should be chosen.
The study did not sufficiently investigate the extent to which familiarity with the test site affects the results. If feasible, it is advisable to acclimatize the test location to relate the animals’ reaction to the novel object rather than the unfamiliar environment (19). The horses in the study were familiar with the test sites but they were not familiar with being kept alone in the round pen or paddock. Horses are obligate social creatures, and social separation can be a significant stressor that affects their behavior (27). Additionally, habituation to the test site can be considered as training for social separation (13). On the other hand, previous experiences with the test environments should also be considered. Horses that experienced stressful situations in the same or similar environment, for example during training, may also exhibit higher stress levels during the novel object test. In summary, the location of the test significantly influences the results of the novel object test. It is unclear whether these differences in results are due to environmental factors such as increased visual, olfactory, and acoustic stimuli or exposure to weather conditions on the paddock, or if the differences are due to habituation to the test areas. As this study clearly indicates that different test environments lead to different results of novel object tests, one of the key issues when planning experiments involving behavioral tests is an appropriate choice of the test environment.
The novel object test is a behavioral assessment that measures escape and exploratory behavior which are not primarily sex-specific (2). The studies by Wolff et al. (19) and Lesimple et al. (18) were unable to assign any further significance to age and sex in the novel object test. When considering the sex of the horses, it is also important to take into account their age and sexual maturity. The horses in this study were between 23 and 27 months old, which suggests that they have reached sexual maturity, as it typically occurs at an age of 12–20 months (28). Our study found no sex-specific differences in the direct interaction of the horses with the novel object with the exception that mares tended to approach the novel object significantly earlier than stallions. However, since we used a very comprehensive ethogram, we also observed many individual behaviors that were not directly related to the novel object. Some of these behaviors appear to be influenced by sex. It could be shown that regardless of the test site, stallions sniff the ground longer than mares. Sniffing the ground can be classified as a form of exploratory behavior, which allows horses to gain new experiences and learn independently (28). Moreover, sniffing is a behavior that is especially used by stallions to gain information about other horses. Pawing was also more frequently observed in stallions than in mares, which also seems to be related to a more pronounced exploratory behavior in stallions compared to mares. Moreover, it is evident that the mares were more active during the novel object test, exhibiting a markedly higher duration of trotting than the stallions. Visser et al. (29) demonstrated that horses kept in pairs trotted and galloped more during a novel object test than horses kept individually. Increased activity can also be observed as a reaction of the horses to social isolation (30). Compared to stallions, the behavior of mares seems to be more strongly characterized by the effect of social isolation which is also indicated by the higher number of vocalizations and higher occurrence of standing at the exit. Increased vocalization has been associated with anxious behavior and may be an indicator of stress (7). The sex-specificity of behaviors associated with exploration of the environment and social isolation must be interpreted in relation to the natural behavior of horses. Stallions usually leave their natal herd at the age of approximately 2 years and may therefore be less dependent on their strong social bonds (19).
The findings of this study suggest that participation in an auction may have some influence, albeit limited. It was observed that horses that had participated in an auction exhibited a longer latency period until the first fixation of the object and spent more time sniffing the ground. However, further research is necessary to clarify the significance of these results. It appears that the horses that had participated in the auction may have been more accustomed to different visual stimuli. Nevertheless, there were no significant differences in terms of latency to first approach and touch the object, and no differences in direct interactions with the novel object between the groups. However, horses that did not have participated in an auction tended to stand longer close to the exit, which may be a reaction to social isolation. The horses investigated in this study were all about the same age but had different previous experiences probably resulting in different degrees of habituation to unknown situations. According to Visser et al. (17), it was observed that horses tend to exhibit less reactivity confronting unfamiliar objects as they age. The study also found that older horses approach the object more quickly, spent less time galloping and trotting, and more time exploring their surroundings. Further investigations could explore whether these results are solely attributed to the age of the animals or if previous experiences, such as participation in an auction or exposure to different housing or training conditions, may have an influence. A 2018 study examined the rearing conditions of foals, with a particular focus on the weaning process from the mare, and found that these conditions may affect the behavioral development of the horses, which in turn may indirectly affect the outcomes of a novel object test (31).
In view of the complexity of the horse’s behavior in response to unfamiliar situations, the question arises whether the interpretation of novel object tests on the basis of single isolated parameters is adequate. Thus, it would be desirable to reduce the number of parameters to be evaluated without losing information. One way to tackle this problem could be to systematically aggregate several small-scale behavioral parameters into fewer functional groups based on their correlation and joint contribution in explaining the observed variability by means of principal component analysis and clustering. Principal component analysis provides a low dimensional representation of the high dimensional data set of behavioral parameters collected within this study and was primarily intended to give a visual approximation of the systematic information contained in the multivariate data. It has to be noted, however, that this visual approximation can only be partial as scatter plots take only two dimensions into account, and hence only explain a part of the variability of the data. Unless the information in the data is truly contained in two or three dimensions, most visualizations will only give a limited view of the multivariate phenomenon. This study identified six main clusters of highly associated behaviors that are also functionally related. The cluster, including behaviors like increased activity (trotting and galloping), vocalization, defecating, tailswishing, standing at the exit etc., which can most likely be associated with fear and stress (30, 32) is very well separated from the clusters including those behaviors that are related to the exploration of the novel object and the direct test environment. Thus, horses exhibiting signs of high levels of excitement tend to pay less attention to the novel object or the direct testing environment. This supports the assumption that low engagement with the novel object indicates fear or stress and high engagement with the object indicates the absence of fear (26). It could also be demonstrated a clear separation between the duration and number of times the horses were sniffing at or touching the novel object with the muzzle or forelimb and the number and duration of fixations. This observation emphasizes the importance of distinguishing between the different levels of engagement with the novel object. Horses that spend considerable time looking at the novel object and maybe even approach it by taking some steps toward it do not necessarily explore the novel object by sniffing and touching it with the muzzle or forelimb. Moreover, kicking toward the novel object was associated with active behavior, especially bucking, but clearly separated from the behaviors describing a curious exploration of the object. Thus, this kind of behavior may be interpreted as defensive behavior when confronting the object and seems more associated with anxious behavior. Although this observation cannot be substantiated by literature that shows a clear association between kicking behavior and anxious or defensive behavior. On the other hand, behaviors that are related to the exploration of the environment (e.g., sniffing the ground and pawing) and comfort behavior like rolling are positively correlated with the exploration of the novel object. Thus, this further supports the assumption that horses exhibiting those behaviors seem to be less anxious.
In order to transfer the data from this study to the entire population of thoroughbred horses, it would be beneficial to increase the number of animals involved. Furthermore, the sample of horses utilized in this study encompasses a range of variability within the horse population. This includes horses of diverse pedigrees, early and late maturing horses, and horses of different sexes. One limitation of the study is that only one age group of horses was examined. It is also noteworthy that only 12 horses were tested in both the round pen and the paddock. The study design was deliberately chosen to determine which differences in behavior were due to the test location and not to inter-individual differences between the horses. The order in which the novel object test was performed should also be considered. For practical reasons, the 12 horses were all tested in the round pen first, and the second novel object test was conducted in the paddock. Because the individual novel object tests differed in the objects used, it is not possible to conclusively determine how the subjective perception of the objects influenced the horses’ behavior during the test. It would have been beneficial to test half of the horses with one object and the other half with the other object in order to avoid the effect different objects might have on the behavior of the horses during the test. Another limitation of this study is that only one independent observer evaluated the behavior during the novel object tests. Interrater reliability could therefore not be obtained.
5 Conclusion
In conclusion, it may be advantageous to establish a standardized method for executing and interpreting novel object tests with horses. The test should be performed in a calm environment to minimize external influences. It is recommended to use a round pen with visual barriers, as horses are highly sensitive prey animals and may be easily distracted by external factors. The timing of the test should also be taken into consideration. To promote accurate results, it is suggested to choose a time frame with expected minimal interference with other horses, personnel, or agricultural machinery and a duration extending 5 min. For consistency, it is crucial to perform the novel object test at the same location especially if several tests are compared to each other. Moreover, sex and previous experience of the horses should be considered as influencing factors when assessing behavior during novel object tests.
Data availability statemement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The animal studies were approved by the State Office for Consumer Protection of Saarland (Landesamt für Verbraucherschutz Saarland). It is not considered to be an animal experiment requiring authorization. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent was not obtained from the owners for the participation of their animals in this study because the animal keeper (owner of the horse farm and trainer of the horses) approved the investigations with the owners’ consent.
Author contributions
LK: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. KK: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MS: Formal analysis, Software, Writing – original draft. RM: Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. GH: Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. CT-R: Conceptualization, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. MW: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This publication is part of the HorseWatch project, which is financially supported by the German Federal Ministry of Food and Agriculture (BMEL) based on a decision of the Parliament of the Federal Republic of Germany, granted by the Federal Office for Agriculture and Food (BLE; grant number 2820HS017).
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to express their gratitude to Monika Venner, Nastasja Volz-Degel, and their entire team for their invaluable support and assistance in the practical implementation of the study.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Forkman, B, Boissy, A, Meunier-Salaün, M-C, Canali, E, and Jones, RB. A critical review of fear tests used on cattle, pigs, sheep, poultry and horses. Physiol Behav. (2007) 92:340–74. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.03.016
2. Goslar, Kristina (2011): Temperament and character assessment of riding horses (temperaments-und Charakterbeurteilung bei Reitpferden). Available at:https://elib.tiho-hannover.de/dissertations/goslark_ws11 (Accessed on 29 July 2024).
3. Lansade, L, Bouissou, M-F, and Erhard, HW. Fearfulness in horses: a temperament trait stable across time and situations. Animal Behav Sci. (2008) 115:182–200. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2008.06.011
4. Le Scolan, N, Hausberger, M, and Wolff, A. Stability over situations in temperamental traits of horses as revealed by experimental and scoring approaches. Behav Process. (1997) 41:257–66. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(97)00052-1
5. Visser, EK, van Reenen, CG, van der Werf, JTN, Schilder, MBH, Knaap, JH, Barneveld, A, et al. Heart rate and heart rate variability during a novel object test and a handling test in young horses. Physiol Behav. (2002) 76:289–96. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00698-4
6. Wageningen UR Livestock Research (2011): Welfare monitoring system: assessment protocol for horses. Version 2.0. Available at:https://edepot.wur.nl/238619 (Accessed July 29, 2024).
7. Bulens, A, Sterken, H, van Beirendonck, S, van Thielen, J, and Driessen, B. The use of different objects during a novel object test in stabled horses. J Vet Behav. (2015) 10:54–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.09.002
8. Eichler, F, Ehrle, A, Machnik, M, Jensen, KC, Wagner, S, Baudisch, N, et al. Behavioral observations, heart rate and cortisol monitoring in horses following multiple oral administrations of a cannabidiol containing paste (part 2/2). Front Vet Sci. (2023) 10:1–13. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2023.1305873
9. Becker, Christina, and Köhler, Michael (2022): Examination protocol for racehorses before the first start of life (Protokoll zur Untersuchung eines Rennpferdes vor dem ersten start). Available at:https://bundestieraerztekammer.de/tieraerzte/leitlinien/downloads/Protokoll_zur_Untersuchung_eines_Rennpferdes_vor_dem_Trainingsbeginn.pdf (Accessed on 29 July 2024).
10. Christensen, JW, Zharkikh, T, and Ladewig, J. Do horses generalise between objects during habituation? Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2008) 114:509–20. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2008.03.007
11. Lee, KE, Kim, JG, Lee, H, and Kim, BS. Behavioral and cardiac responses in mature horses exposed to a novel object. J Animal Sci Technol. (2021) 63:651–61. doi: 10.5187/jast.2021.e51
12. Powell, BB, Horvath, KC, Gilliam, TL, Sibille, KT, Keil, A, Miller-Cushon, EK, et al. Behavioral and physiological reactions to a sudden novel object in the weanling horse: quantitative phenotypes for future GWAS. Genes. (2023) 14:P. 593. doi: 10.3390/genes14030593
13. Gorecka, A, Bakuniak, M, Chruszczewski, MH, and Jezierski, TA. A note on the habituation to novelty in horses: handler effect. Animal Sci Papers Rep. (2007) 25:143–52.
14. Christensen, JW, Beblein, C, and Malmkvist, J. Development and consistency of fearfulness in horses from foal to adult. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2020) 232:105106. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2020.105106
15. Popescu, Silvana, Diugan, Eva, Oros, Daniela, and Stepan, Caius A.; Danci, Liana C.; Borda, Cristin (2016): Behavioural responses in stallions to a novel object. In: Bull UASVM Vet Med. 73:443–444. doi: 10.15835/buasvmcn-vm
16. Graf, P, von Borstel Uta, K, and Gauly, M. Practical considerations regarding the implementation of a temperament test into horse performance tests: results of a large-scale test run. J Vet Behav. (2014) 9:329–40. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.08.004
17. Visser, EK, Van Reenen, CG, Hopster, H, Schilder, MBH, Knaap, JH, Barneveld, A, et al. Quantifying aspects of young horses’ temperament: consistency of behavioural variables. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2001) 74:241–58. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(01)00177-0
18. Lesimple, C, Fureix, C, LeScolan, N, Richard-Yris, M-A, and Hausberger, M. Housing conditions and breed are associated with emotionality and cognitive abilities in riding school horses. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2011) 129:92–9. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2010.11.005
19. Wolff, A, Hausberger, M, and Le Scolan, N. Experimental tests to assess emotionality in horses. Behav Process. (1997) 40:209–21. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(97)00784-5
20. Christensen, JW, Zharkikh, T, and Chovaux, E. Object recognition and generalisation during habituation in horses. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2011) 129:83–91. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2010.11.016
21. Dalla Costa, E., Dai, F., Lebelt, D., Scholz, P., Barbieri, S., Canali, E., et al. (2015): AWIN 2015. AWIN Welfare assessment protocol for horses. Available at:https://air.unimi.it/retrieve/dfa8b992-42a0-748b-e053-3a05fe0a3a96/AWINProtocolHorses.pdf (accessed on 29 July 2024).
22. Gorecka, A, Jastrzebska, E, Sosnowska, Z, Jaworski, Z, Jezierski, TA, and Chruszczewski, MH. Reactivity to humans and fearfulness tests: field validation in polish cold blood horses. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2011) 133:207–15. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.05.011
23. Malmkvist, J, Poulsen, JM, Luthersson, N, Palme, R, Christensen, JW, and Søndergaard, E. Behaviour and stress responses in horses with gastric ulceration. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2012) 142:160–7. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.10.002
24. Kirsch, Katharina, Klitzing, Lara, Wiegard, Mechtild, Thöne-Reineke, Christa, and Hoffmann, Gundula (2023): Analysis of behaviour during a novel object test in horses using collar-worn inertial sensors and artifical intelligence. In: Kuratorium für Technik und Bauwesen in der Landwirtschaft e.V. (KTBL) (Hg.): Aktuelle Arbeiten zur artgemäßen Tierhaltung. 55. Internationale Arbeitstagung "Angewandte Ethologie bei Nutztieren". Freiburg, 115–123.
25. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2017).
26. Valenchon, M, Lévy, F, Fortin, M, Leterrier, C, and Lansade, L. Stress and temperament affect working memory performance for disappearing food in horses, Equus caballus. Animal Behav. (2013) 86:1233–40. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.09.026
27. Kiley-Worthington, M. The behavior of horses in relation to management and training — towards ethologically sound environments. J Equine Vet. (1990) 10:62–75. doi: 10.1016/S0737-0806(06)80088-2
28. Zeitler-Feicht, MH. Handbuch Pferdeverhalten. Ursache, Therapie und Prophylaxe von Problemverhalten. 3., völlig neu bearbeitete, erweiterte und gestaltete Auflage. Stuttgart: Ulmer (Reiterbibliothek) (2015).
29. Visser, EK, Ellis, AD, and van Reenen, CG. The effect of two different housing conditions on the welfare of young horses stabled for the first time. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2008) 114:521–33. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2008.03.003
30. Bagshaw, CS, Ralston, SL, and Fisher, H. Behavioral and physiological effect of orally administered tryptophan on horses subjected to acute isolation stress. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (1994) 40:1–12. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(94)90083-3
31. Lansade, L, Foury, A, Reigner, F, Vidament, M, Guettier, E, Bouvet, G, et al. Progressive habituation to separation alleviates the negative effects of weaning in the mother and foal. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2018) 97:59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.07.005
Keywords: animal welfare, behavioral test, equine behavior, horse, novel object test, thoroughbred racehorses
Citation: Klitzing L, Kirsch K, Schindler M, Merle R, Hoffmann G, Thöne-Reineke C and Wiegard M (2025) Exploring factors that influence the behavior response to novel object tests in young thoroughbred horses: investigating sex, test site and auction history. Front. Vet. Sci. 11:1478350. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2024.1478350
Edited by:
T. Bas Rodenburg, Utrecht University, NetherlandsReviewed by:
Denise Pereira Leme, Santa Catarina State University, BrazilEmily Kieson, Equine International Corp., United States
Wiktoria Janicka, University of Life Sciences of Lublin, Poland
Copyright © 2025 Klitzing, Kirsch, Schindler, Merle, Hoffmann, Thöne-Reineke and Wiegard. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Lara Klitzing, bGFyYS5rbGl0emluZzJAZnUtYmVybGluLmRl