Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Vet. Sci., 30 July 2024
Sec. Veterinary Epidemiology and Economics
This article is part of the Research Topic Insights in Veterinary Epidemiology and Economics: 2023 View all 5 articles

Exposure variables in veterinary epidemiology: are they telling us what we think they are?

  • 1Department of Population Health Sciences, VA-MD College of Veterinary Medicine, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, VA, United States
  • 2Department of Population Medicine, Ontario Veterinary College, University of Guelph, Guelph, ON, Canada
  • 3Department of Large Animal Clinical Sciences, College of Veterinary Medicine, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, United States
  • 4Center for Outcomes Research and Epidemiology, College of Veterinary Medicine, Kansas State University, Manhattan, KS, United States

This manuscript summarizes a presentation delivered by the first author at the 2024 symposium for the Calvin Schwabe Award for Lifetime Achievement in Veterinary Epidemiology and Preventive Medicine, which was awarded to Dr. Jan Sargeant. Epidemiologic research plays a crucial role in understanding the complex relationships between exposures and health outcomes. However, the accuracy of the conclusions drawn from these investigations relies upon the meticulous selection and measurement of exposure variables. Appropriate exposure variable selection is crucial for understanding disease etiologies, but it is often the case that we are not able to directly measure the exposure variable of interest and use proxy measures to assess exposures instead. Inappropriate use of proxy measures can lead to erroneous conclusions being made about the true exposure of interest. These errors may lead to biased estimates of associations between exposures and outcomes. The consequences of such biases extend beyond research concerns as health decisions can be made based on flawed evidence. Recognizing and mitigating these biases are essential for producing reliable evidence that informs health policies and interventions, ultimately contributing to improved population health outcomes. To address these challenges, researchers must adopt rigorous methodologies for exposure variable selection and validation studies to minimize measurement errors.

1 Introduction

John Snow, considered the father of modern epidemiology, published his conclusions regarding the Broad Street pump being the source of the Cholera epidemic in the Soho district of London in 1855. In terms of scientific advances this is still a relatively modern development and epidemiology is thus a relatively young science. To put this in perspective, we are equidistant from John Snow’s publication “On the mode of communication of cholera” now as he was from Sir Isaac Newton’s publication about the laws of motion (1687) at the time he presented that publication.

Given the foundations of this branch of science and the most pressing health-related issues facing human populations at the time, it is no surprise that the early developments in the field of epidemiology were rooted in determining the cause(s) of infectious diseases. In this model, causal factors are those that are responsible for health impacts or modifications of health and each factor that contributes to disease occurrence is considered a component cause of disease. Any combination of factors that produce disease are considered a sufficient cause of disease, and causal factors that are required for the disease to develop are termed necessary causes. However, as we have moved from studying infectious causes of disease to non-infectious disease outcomes, such as cancer and aging in both humans and other animal species, we have increased the complexity of exposure measurement within the field. This is because with non-infectious outcomes there may be no necessary cause for a particular health outcome. In fact, any single component cause may only make a small contribution to the disease etiology. This perspective aims to elucidate the importance of appropriate selection of exposure variables within the field of veterinary epidemiology, though many of the concepts apply to human populations as well.

2 Challenges with exposure variables

Rothman and Greenland (1) described the concept of causation due to multiple component causes as being an incomplete causal mechanism unless or until all of the component conditions or events that are necessary for the outcome to occur have reached a set of minimal conditions or thresholds. Thus, each of those components must be accurately measured to determine causality. An additional complexity is that most diseases can be caused by more than one causal mechanism, a concept called multicausality, and each of these mechanisms involves the collective action of a multitude of component causes (1). Knowledge of which components are part of the multiple component causes and how they should be measured is necessary prior to occurrence of the outcome of interest in order to determine causality.

When measuring exposures, it is also important to consider the timing of the exposure on the individual or population in terms of when the exposure occurs in relation to the individual’s development or life stage. This is important because the timing of the exposure can cause tremendous variability in the outcomes that may occur. An excellent example is the exposure to the steroidal alkaloid, cyclopamine, in sheep during pregnancy. Ewes can become exposed to this potent teratogen through ingestion of the plant Veratrum californicum resulting in synophthalmia (cyclopia) formation in the embryonic lamb. However, cyclopamine is rapidly eliminated from the ewe and ingestion of the plant only on gestational days 13 or 14 results in craniofacial malformations being exhibited (2).

In addition to the timing of exposure in relation to the individual’s development, duration of exposure may also be associated with outcomes. In a prospective human birth cohort study conducted in Cincinnati, Ohio, early life exposure to traffic-related air pollution was associated with wheezing regardless of the age at which exposure occurs (3). However, increased risk for asthma was only identified in children exposed to traffic-related air pollution from birth to the age of seven (3). This illustrates that, even within the same population cohort, the duration of time exposed to the same exposure risk did influence disease occurrence.

Another complication is that many of the observational studies used in veterinary epidemiology are retrospective. However, it is not always possible to measure exposure variables retrospectively, as it is often the case that there are no measurable indicators of past exposures. For instance, dietary intake during childhood has been shown to affect adult risk of breast cancer in human females (4), but there are few adult individuals who have detailed descriptions of the types and amounts of foods they consumed as toddlers.

The total number of exposures of interest have increased considerably, too. In a recently published manuscript by Sargeant et al. (5), the authors evaluated 200 observational studies published in the veterinary literature between 2020 and 2022. The number of variables assessed during the screening step in these studies averaged over 20, with a maximum of more than 175. The average number of independent variables evaluated in the final models used in the studies was approximately 14.

The exposure variables being examined themselves have also become much more complex. For instance, food selections for companion animals have become more diverse (6), and different diet types have been associated with different health outcomes (7, 8). Environmental risk factors being examined in relation to health outcomes in animals include those related to the natural environment (9), built environment (10), and the chemical environment (11). Researchers are examining the role that psychosocial (12) and cognitive states (13) play in health outcomes in animals as well. Of course, we also are learning more about the role that genetic predispositions play in the outcome of disease, especially cancers (14), in animal species.

This increasing complexity and numeracy of exposures of interest has likely contributed to an increase in errors related to measurement of exposures (15, 16). It is thought that inaccurate exposure measurements are one of the main sources of bias in epidemiologic research. The magnitude of this bias is likely underappreciated (16). For instance, if we have a well measured variable that correlates with the true exposure of interest with a correlation coefficient of 0.7, we might consider that to be an acceptably strong relationship between the two variables. However, in this instance if we observe a risk ratio of 1.7 in our exposure variable with a correlation coefficient of 0.7, it would indicate that the true risk ratio associated with the exposure of interest is 3.0, nearly two-fold higher than what was measured. Of course, exposure estimates can be either under- or overestimated when measurement errors occur (17).

With the era of veterinary medical “Big Data” having begun (18), one might assume that measurement errors can be overcome by the use of enormous datasets with large numbers of observations. This assumption likely originates from the probability theory known as the law of large numbers wherein by taking the average of an increasing number of random observations sampled from a population it allows for convergence on the true value of the mean. However, measurement errors impact epidemiologic data analyses in several ways, including creating bias in, and affecting the precision of, the exposure effect estimate (17). Thus, a larger sample size will not necessarily move exposure effect estimates closer to their real values and may affect the precision of the estimate, but not the bias resulting in a very precise, but biased estimate. So a larger sample size might be able to compensate for the loss in precision that is caused by measurement error, but the bias created when the reliability of the measurement is low may need a 50-fold or more increase in sample size in order to compensate for the error (19, 20).

It is not uncommon for veterinary researchers to use proxy variables in lieu of directly measuring the true variable of interest. One type of proxy measure that is used with some frequency in epidemiologic research is distance. That is to say that we use the distance from an exposure of interest as a proxy measure for the amount of exposure. In many cases, investigators are able to measure distance from the exposure with a high degree of accuracy, but the true amount of exposure may not always be equal at equal distances from the source of exposure. For instance, a virus or fine particulate matter that is dispersed through the air and travels from a source of exposure like a silver mine (21) or a poultry house (22) does not travel uniformly in all directions away from the source of exposure. Factors such as wind direction and speed, the deposition process, and pathogen decay rate must be considered in order for true exposure to be estimated. Similarly, all animals in a closed barn may not receive the same exposure from an airborne pathogen due to differences in air flow within the building based on location of fans and doors and variables such as temperature and humidity. However, distance is regularly used as a proxy measure for exposure without accounting for variables that might differentially impact the way in which distance from a source of exposure should be interpreted in both human (23) and animal (24) health research.

It is also not uncommon for veterinary researchers to create variables to define exposures of interest. For example, there have been several studies that have examined the effect of brachycephaly, or a shortened skull shape, on health outcomes in dogs (2527). However, there is not a standardized definition of the term brachycephaly being used across these studies. One study (25) used morphometric measurements to define dogs as brachycephalic, another (26) used a list of 13 dog breeds to define their brachycephalic cohort, and a third (27) used a list of more than 30 dog breeds to define their brachycephalic cohort, and that list did not incorporate all of the 13 breeds included in the previous study. Thus, the same exposure variable was ostensibly being examined, but on close inspection it becomes apparent that though the same label is being affixed, the term does not mean the same thing in each of these instances. This means that at least some of the animals or even entire breeds being studied must be misclassified when we compare results across studies.

3 Proposed solutions

Given that inaccurate exposure measurements are one of the main sources of bias in epidemiologic research, it seems prudent that we, as a discipline, make every effort to reduce the impact on our understanding of health. One of the most straightforward ways we can do this is by directly measuring exposure variables of interest. Foregoing the use of proxy measurements whenever feasible and realistic to do so will decrease bias and increase the accuracy of our exposure measurements. This will in turn allow us to observe risk ratios that are closer to the true effect and will enhance our understanding of disease etiologies.

When it is not possible to directly measure the exposure variable of interest, it is imperative that rational proxy measurements are used. Thoughtfully considering how the proxy measure may vary from the true exposure variable and taking those variables into account is crucial. Furthermore, it is imperative that the process through which the proxy variable was decided upon by the investigators be described in the methods section of the report associated with the work. Transparency around the decision-making process is critical so that readers can evaluate and determine how close a proxy measurement is to the true variable of interest.

Directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) or causal diagrams can also be used for selecting appropriate exposure variables as they provide a clear representation of the assumed causal relationships between variables. By mapping out these relationships, DAGs help to identify and distinguish between confounders, mediators, and colliders, thus preventing biased estimates of the exposure-outcome association (28). When used to guide the selection of variables to control for, they help to ensure that the chosen variables isolate the causal effect of the exposure on the outcome, rather than introducing bias or masking the true relationship.

Further, we must be consistent in our use of defined exposures. Using similar terminology with different inclusion criteria across studies makes research replication difficult, if not impossible. Our profession has a strong history of successfully using consensus statements to provide our community with information about topics as varied as the diagnosis and treatment of diseases to reporting guidelines for use when conducting research (2934). Consensus statements also can be used to define exposure variables that can be uniformly applied across research endeavors.

Lastly, failure to recognize the impact of poorly measured exposure variables should not be tolerated. They should, in fact, be considered a serious flaw in research proposals and manuscripts submitted for publication. Erroneous measurements can lead to biased results that may not be sufficiently understood, even when they are recognized by the researchers. Several methods of quantitative bias analysis and “good practices” for their application have been developed (35). Acknowledging the presence of errors in the measurement of exposure variables in the discussion section of a manuscript should not be considered an adequate or acceptable practice.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

AR: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JS: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. AO'C: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. DR: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the Association for Veterinary Epidemiology and Preventive Medicine (AVEPM) and the Conference of Research Workers in Animal Diseases (CRWAD) for support of the Schwabe Symposium and the 2024 Calvin W. Schwabe Award winner, Dr. Jan Sargeant. The Calvin W. Schwabe Award is presented annually by the AVEPM to honor lifetime achievement in veterinary epidemiology and preventive medicine.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Rothman, KJ, and Greenland, S. Causation and causal inference in epidemiology. Am J Public Health. (2005) 95:S144–50. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.059204

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Welch, KD, Panter, KE, Lee, ST, Gardner, DR, Stegelmeier, BL, and Cook, D. Cyclopamine-induced synophthalmia in sheep: defining a critical window and toxicokinetic evaluation. J Appl Toxicol. (2009) 29:414–21. doi: 10.1002/jat.1427

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Brunst, KJ, Ryan, PH, Brokamp, C, Bernstein, D, Reponen, T, Lockey, J, et al. Timing and duration of traffic-related air pollution exposure and the risk for childhood wheeze and asthma. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. (2015) 192:421–7. doi: 10.1164/rccm.201407-1314OC

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Michels, KB, Rosner, BA, Chumlea, WC, Colditz, GA, and Willett, WC. Preschool diet and adult risk of breast cancer. Int J Cancer. (2006) 118:749–54. doi: 10.1002/ijc.21407

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Sargeant, JM, O’Connor, AM, Renter, DG, and Ruple, A. What question are we trying to answer? Embracing causal inference. Front Vet Sci. (2024) 11:1402981. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2024.1402981

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Morgan, G, Williams, N, Schmidt, V, Cookson, D, Symington, C, and Pinchbeck, G. A Dog’s dinner: factors affecting food choice and feeding practices for UK dog owners feeding raw meat-based or conventional cooked diets. Prev Vet Med. (2022) 208:105741. doi: 10.1016/j.prevetmed.2022.105741

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Groat, EF, Williams, NJ, Pinchbeck, G, Warner, B, Simpson, A, and Schmidt, VM. UK dogs eating raw meat diets have higher risk of Salmonella and antimicrobial-resistant Escherichia coli faecal carriage. J Small Anim Pract. (2022) 63:435–41. doi: 10.1111/jsap.13488

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Giacometti, F, Magarotto, J, Serraino, A, and Piva, S. Highly suspected cases of salmonellosis in two cats fed with a commercial raw meat-based diet: health risks to animals and zoonotic implications. BMC Vet Res. (2017) 13:224. doi: 10.1186/s12917-017-1143-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Tangtrongsup, S, Scorza, AV, Reif, JS, Ballweber, LR, Lappin, MR, and Salman, MD. Seasonal distributions and other risk factors for Giardia duodenalis and Cryptosporidium spp. infections in dogs and cats in Chiang Mai, Thailand. Prev Vet Med. (2020) 174:104820. doi: 10.1016/j.prevetmed.2019.104820

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Rowe, E, Browne, W, Casey, R, Gruffydd-Jones, T, and Murray, J. Risk factors identified for owner-reported feline obesity at around one year of age: dry diet and indoor lifestyle. Prev Vet Med. (2015) 121:273–81. doi: 10.1016/j.prevetmed.2015.07.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Takashima-Uebelhoer, BB, Barber, LG, Zagarins, SE, Procter-Gray, E, Gollenberg, AL, Moore, AS, et al. Household chemical exposures and the risk of canine malignant lymphoma, a model for human non-Hodgkin’s lymphoma. Environ Res. (2012) 112:171–6. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2011.12.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Cannas, S, Berteselli, GV, Piotti, P, Talamonti, Z, Scaglia, E, Stefanello, D, et al. Stress and Cancer in dogs: comparison between a population of dogs diagnosed with Cancer and a control population - a pilot study. Maced Vet Rev. (2016) 39:201–8. doi: 10.1515/macvetrev-2016-0088

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Thomovsky, SA, and Ogata, N. A Canine’s behavior and cognitive state as it relates to immobility and the success of physical rehabilitation in the non-ambulatory spinal cord patient. Front Vet Sci. (2021) 8:599320. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2021.599320

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Leeb, T, Bannasch, D, and Schoenebeck, JJ. Identification of genetic risk factors for monogenic and complex canine diseases. Annu Rev Anim Biosci. (2023) 11:183–205. doi: 10.1146/annurev-animal-050622-055534

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Jurek, AM, Maldonado, G, Greenland, S, and Church, TR. Exposure-measurement error is frequently ignored when interpreting epidemiologic study results. Eur J Epidemiol. (2006) 21:871–6. doi: 10.1007/s10654-006-9083-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Brakenhoff, TB, Mitroiu, M, Keogh, RH, Moons, KGM, Groenwold, RHH, and van Smeden, M. Measurement error is often neglected in medical literature: a systematic review. J Clin Epidemiol. (2018) 98:89–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2018.02.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. van Smeden, M, Lash, TL, and Groenwold, RHH. Reflection on modern methods: five myths about measurement error in epidemiological research. Int J Epidemiol. (2020) 49:338–47. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyz251

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Paynter, AN, Dunbar, MD, Creevy, KE, and Ruple, A. Veterinary big data: when data goes to the dogs. Animals. (2021) 11:1872. doi: 10.3390/ani11071872

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Devine, OJ, and Smith, JM. Estimating sample size for epidemiologic studies: the impact of ignoring exposure measurement uncertainty. Stat Med. (1998) 17:1375–89. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0258(19980630)17:12<1375::AID-SIM857>3.0.CO;2-D

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. McKeown-Eyssen, GE, and Tibshirani, R. Implications of measurement error in exposure for the sample sizes of case-control studies. Am J Epidemiol. (1994) 139:415–21. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a117014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Kim, CS, Anthony, TL, Goldstein, D, and Rytuba, JJ. Windborne transport and surface enrichment of arsenic in semi-arid mining regions: examples from the Mojave Desert, California. Aeolian Res. (2014) 14:85–96. doi: 10.1016/j.aeolia.2014.02.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Ssematimba, A, Hagenaars, TJ, and de Jong, MCM. Modelling the wind-borne spread of highly pathogenic avian influenza virus between farms. PLoS One. (2012) 7:e31114. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031114

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Schultz, AA, Peppard, P, Gangnon, RE, and Malecki, KMC. Residential proximity to concentrated animal feeding operations and allergic and respiratory disease. Environ Int. (2019) 130:104911. doi: 10.1016/j.envint.2019.104911

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Slizovskiy, IB, Conti, LA, Trufan, SJ, Reif, JS, Lamers, VT, Stowe, MH, et al. Reported health conditions in animals residing near natural gas wells in southwestern Pennsylvania. J Environ Sci Health A Tox Hazard Subst Environ Eng. (2015) 50:473–81. doi: 10.1080/10934529.2015.992666

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Packer, RMA, Hendricks, A, Tivers, MS, and Burn, CC. Impact of facial conformation on canine health: brachycephalic obstructive airway syndrome. PLoS One. (2015) 10:e0137496. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137496

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Gruenheid, M, Aarnes, TK, McLoughlin, MA, Simpson, EM, Mathys, DA, Mollenkopf, DF, et al. Risk of anesthesia-related complications in brachycephalic dogs. J Am Vet Med Assoc. (2018) 253:301–6. doi: 10.2460/javma.253.3.301

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. O’Neill, DG, Pegram, C, Crocker, P, Brodbelt, DC, Church, DB, and Packer, RMA. Unravelling the health status of brachycephalic dogs in the UK using multivariable analysis. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:17251. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-73088-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Pearce, N, and Lawlor, D. Causal inference—so much more than statistics. Int J Epidemiol. (2016) 45:1895–903. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyw328

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Ruple, A, Sargeant, JM, Selmic, LE, and O’Connor, AM. The standards of reporting randomized trials in pets (PetSORT): methods and development processes. Front Vet Sci. (2023) 10:1137774. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2023.1137774

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Sargeant, JM, Ruple, A, Selmic, LE, and O’Connor, AM. The standards of reporting trials in pets (PetSORT): explanation and elaboration. Front Vet Sci. (2023) 10:1137781. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2023.1137781

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Sargeant, JM, O’Connor, AM, Gardner, IA, Dickson, JS, and Torrence, MEconsensus meeting participants, et al. The REFLECT statement: reporting guidelines for randomized controlled trials in livestock and food safety: explanation and elaboration. Zoonoses Public Health. (2010) 57:105–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1863-2378.2009.01312.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. O’Connor, AM, Sargeant, JM, Gardner, IA, Dickson, JS, Torrence, ME, Dewey, CE, et al. The REFLECT statement: methods and processes of creating reporting guidelines for randomized controlled trials for livestock and food safety. J Vet Intern Med. (2010) 24:57–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-1676.2009.0441.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Sargeant, JM, O’Connor, AM, Dohoo, IR, Erb, HN, Cevallos, M, Egger, M, et al. Methods and processes of developing the strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology - veterinary (STROBE-vet) statement. Prev Vet Med. (2016) 134:188–96. doi: 10.1016/j.prevetmed.2016.09.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. ACVIM Consensus Statement TaskforceHinchcliff, KW, Morley, PS, DiBartola, SP, Taylor, SD, and Harrell, KA. ACVIM-endorsed statements: consensus statements, evidence-based practice guidelines and systematic reviews. J Vet Intern Med. (2023) 37:1957–65. doi: 10.1111/jvim.16869

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Lash, TL, Fox, MP, MacLehose, RF, Maldonado, G, McCandless, LC, and Greenland, S. Good practices for quantitative bias analysis. Int J Epidemiol. (2014) 43:1969–85. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu149

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: exposure variables, variable selection, observational studies, veterinary epidemiology, causation

Citation: Ruple A, Sargeant JM, O’Connor AM and Renter DG (2024) Exposure variables in veterinary epidemiology: are they telling us what we think they are? Front. Vet. Sci. 11:1442308. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2024.1442308

Received: 01 June 2024; Accepted: 22 July 2024;
Published: 30 July 2024.

Edited by:

Ioannis Magouras, University of Bern, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Ane Nødtvedt, Norwegian University of Life Sciences, Norway

Copyright © 2024 Ruple, Sargeant, O’Connor and Renter. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Audrey Ruple, aruple@vt.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.