Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Vet. Sci., 07 February 2024
Sec. Veterinary Humanities and Social Sciences
This article is part of the Research Topic Animals and Social Interaction View all 3 articles

Owner expectations and surprises of dog ownership experiences in the United Kingdom

  • Dogs Trust, London, United Kingdom

Introduction: Although many owners are satisfied by dog ownership, large numbers of dogs are relinquished annually, with an estimated 130,000 dogs cared for each year by rescue organisations in the UK. Unrealistic ownership expectations are a potential factor in the decision to relinquish and therefore understanding what surprises owners about the realities of ownership and how this meets their expectations is vital.

Methods: Using a retrospective cross-sectional cohort study design, as part of Dogs Trust’s National Dog Survey 2021, owners were asked ‘what has surprised you most about owning a dog?’ and to classify how their experiences had compared with their expectations on a list of aspects of ownership as either more than, less than or as expected. Free text responses (n= 2,000) were analysed using reflexive thematic analysis in NVivo Pro (v.12 QSR) and a quantitative summary of classified expectations (n=354,224) was conducted in R.

Results: Many aspects of ownership were reported to be as expected, however a discrepancy between expectation and reality regarding some aspects was revealed. The cost of vet visits was greater than expected for the majority of respondents (52%), whilst other factors that often exceeded expectations included buying/rehoming cost (33%) and amount of patience needed (25%). Damage to furniture was less than expected for many (50%) as was damage to garden (33%). From the thematic analysis, four themes were generated that reflected what surprised owners most about ownership: emotional connectedness of human–dog relationships; dog’s impact on human health/wellbeing; understanding what dogs are like; and meeting the demands of ownership.

Conclusion: Overall these results aid our understanding of dog-human interactions, highlighting the complexity of the dog-owner relationship which may come with unanticipated costs. Whilst this study’s results are reassuring given many aspects of ownership were as expected, and surprises were often positive, some areas had greater impacts than expected, raising opportunities for intervention, resources or support. The aim would be to manage owners’ expectations prior to acquisition or ensure these are more realistically met, reducing the likelihood of negative welfare implications for both dog and owner.

1 Introduction

Dogs are one of the most popular companion animal species across the world, including in the United Kingdom where dogs are owned by an estimated one-quarter of adults (1). Although many owners report satisfaction with their dogs and their relationships with them (2), significant numbers of dogs face negative welfare situations, such as relinquishment or euthanasia. Each year, in the UK, an estimated 130,000 dogs are cared for by rescue organisations (3). Factors associated with dog relinquishment are varied, including dog behaviour, owner ill-health, relocation or housing issues, lack of time, and financial costs (4, 5). Another factor associated with some cases of dog relinquishment is unrealistic or unmet owner expectations (4, 6). Unrealistic expectations for ownership were cited as a main reason for adopters returning dogs to shelters in 7 to 13% of cases (7, 8). Current evidence indicates a variety of dog- or ownership-related aspects associated with mismatched expectations. For instance, people adopting dogs from a UK charity who found the work and effort in looking after their dog to be more than they had expected had 9.9 times higher odds of giving their dog back to the shelter than people who found the effort required to be less than expected (9). Similarly, adopters returning their dog to the shelter within the first three months of adoption had significantly higher expectations for dog health and desirable behaviour, as well as the development of the human–dog bond, compared with non-returning owners (10). Excessive financial costs associated with dog ownership is another reported reason for relinquishment that suggests a discrepancy between expectations and reality (11).

Previous research has explored owner’s pre-acquisition expectations. In a recent survey of UK puppy purchasers, the misconception that some “designer crossbreeds” (e.g., Cocker Spaniel X Poodle, the “Cockapoo”) are “hypoallergenic” and thus pose a reduced risk to owner’s allergies was found to motivate owner demand for the purchase of such “designer crossbreeds” (12). This finding indicates potential risks to both dog and human welfare if owner expectations regarding a dog’s hypoallergenicity are not met. Other research suggests that owners of doodles (i.e., poodle hybrids, such as the “Cockapoo”) underestimated the maintenance and grooming needs of doodle dogs (13). Inadequate grooming can lead to potentially serious dog welfare consequences (14). In another survey, conducted in Australia, many prospective dog adopters anticipated health benefits of dog ownership to include increased walking (89%) and physical fitness (52%) (15). Respondents also expected improvements to mental health, through greater happiness (89%), and decreased stress (74%), loneliness (61%) and depression (57%). In the same study, dog training and dog behavioural issues were common expected challenges of dog ownership, anticipated by 62 and 50% of respondents, respectively. Expectations of dog ownership are shaped by experience and knowledge about dog behaviours and ownership requirements (16). Furthermore, evidence indicates that people with greater knowledge about animal care, health, behaviour, training and costs have more realistic expectations about dog ownership (e.g., the effort required) than people with less knowledge (17).

The relationship between the current perceptions of aspects of dog ownership experience and owner’s prior expectations has not yet been fully explored in a sample of current owners. Whilst there is an existing body of research about owners’ expectations surrounding dog ownership, there is a data gap regarding whether such expectations are perceived to be subsequently met. Furthermore, recent acquisitions during/since the COVID-19 pandemic has seen a potential increase to the pet dog population, with negative fallout possible particularly if dogs were acquired impulsively and/or with unrealistic expectations of ownership (18). Understanding which aspects of dog ownership surprise owners, and in what ways, is therefore a critical step in addressing dog relinquishment and optimising welfare for both humans and dogs. Using a sample of current UK dog owners, the objective of this study was therefore to retrospectively explore dog owners’ current perceptions of their expectations of dog ownership, reflecting on whether certain aspects of ownership were more, less or equal to what they expected. This study also investigated demographic/owner factor variables for their association with aspects that surprised respondents. Furthermore, the qualitative analysis aimed to broaden the scope of understanding of this topic, by identifying further aspects of dog ownership that surprised owners and produce richer insights into owners’ experiences.

2 Methods

Ethical approval for this study was granted by Dogs Trust Ethical Review Board (reference ERB050).

2.1 Data collection

This study used a retrospective cross-sectional cohort design, with an online survey used as the data collection tool. The “National Dog Survey 2021”, developed by Dogs Trust, collected responses from dog owners in the UK between 10th September to 25th October 2021. Full survey methodology including tool development, study participants and data collection has been previously described (19).

2.2 Data analysis

To explore dog owners’ reflections on their expectations of dog ownership a convergent mixed-methods study design inspired the data analysis (20). Quantitative and qualitative data were collected in parallel, analysed independently and then interpreted together in a comparative and contrasting way. Data included within the qualitative analysis were responses to the free-text survey question “What has surprised you most about owning a dog?”, whilst the quantitative analysis summarises findings from the question “After owning a dog, please tell us which of the following are less, more or as you expected?” where respondents were asked to classify 13 functional areas of dog ownership (Figure 1) based on their experiences, as being “less than expected”, “as expected” or “more than expected”.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Percentage of respondents (n = 354,046) stating that their expectations of dog ownership (based on a series of 13 statements) were either less than, more than, or as expected.

2.2.1 Quantitative

Following the data cleaning methodology as described in Anderson et al. (19), data were imported into R (version 4.1.3) (21) and the distribution of the data checked. Descriptive statistics were then collated based on responses to the question “After owning a dog, please tell us which of the following are less, more or as you expected?”, and variables of interest were compared using the mean number of surprises (combining both more than and less than expected responses) cross tabulated to identify differences between groups. Variables of interest included age of the dog, due to the potential differences in experiences related to the current life stage of their dog, as well as number of dogs in the household as presence of multiple dogs may impact the comparison of expectations versus experience. Due to the previous literature highlighting that younger owners may be more likely to experience unrealistic expectations of dog ownership, owner age category was included within our analysis. Finally, acquisition factors such as source of acquisition and price paid for dog were included due to potential differences in owners’ expectations when acquiring their dog through different sources. Statistical comparisons were considered inappropriate due to the large sample size, increasing the likelihood of introduction of false positive results.

2.2.2 Qualitative

From the full dataset where answers were provided to the question “What has surprised you most about owning a dog?” (n = 273,899), a random subsample of 2,000 free-text responses was generated in R using slice_sample() from all comments that were not blank and contained at least 3 characters. These responses were imported into NVivo Pro (v.12 QSR) and analysed following a reflexive thematic analysis approach (22, 23). The research question that guided the qualitative analysis was “What aspects of dogs or ownership surprised owners since their dog’s acquisition?” This closely resembled the free-text question from which the data were obtained. An experiential orientation to data interpretation was adopted as we aimed to prioritise owners’ own accounts of their experiences and perspectives. One author (KH) familiarised herself with the data by reading all 2,000 free-text responses, generating initial codes from the data and then organising them into meaningful groups from which themes were constructed. Data were inductively coded, in that coding was driven by the content of the responses, rather than pre-determined codes. As coding progressed, some codes were modified. From the initial coding, categories were identified which were then collated into themes. The themes linked ideas and concepts within grouped codes that represented overall patterns of meaning that the researcher interpreted from the data. Following the same process, another author (KA) independently coded 1,000 responses from the subsample. This was done not with the goal of achieving greater reliability or accuracy between the coders, but rather to deepen our reflexive engagement with the data, for instance by identifying any overlooked areas in our respective analyses. Three authors (KH, KA, and RMC) met to review KH and KA’s construction of themes and relevant data references, and collaboratively established the final themes.

Whilst an inductive approach was employed for coding and theme development, we recognize that the qualitative researcher always, unavoidably, brings their pre-existing knowledge to the analysis. As researchers in the field of dog welfare research, both coders (and the wider research team) were familiar with prior research on the topic being explored. Most authors also had dog ownership experience. We acknowledge that the team’s pre-existing knowledge and experience may have informed our understanding of respondents’ experiences and the aspects they found surprising.

Including the full dataset within the qualitative analysis was neither feasible, for manual coding, nor necessary, to meet the goal of this study’s qualitative aspect. In line with research conducted within a qualitative paradigm, the aim of this study’s qualitative analysis was to explore some of the range and diversity of experience amongst dog owners, rather than present a quantified representation of the data. Through engagement with the subsample outlined above, including appraisal of the breadth of the study’s aim and the richness of the individual data items, the researchers determined that this subsample size had adequate “information power” (24) to meet this study’s aim.

3 Results

A total of 354,046 respondents owning dogs in the UK completed the survey. Respondents were asked to complete one survey per household and for their most recently acquired dog. Full demographic summaries of respondents are available in a previous publication (19).

3.1 Quantitative analysis

Respondents were asked to classify a series of statements based on their current perception of ownership as either as “expected”, “less than expected” or “more than expected”, with the overall results of this study revealing insights in areas of ownership, indicating several aspects of ownership in which owners reported that their expectations were commonly under- or over-met (Figure 1). The greatest discrepancies where expectations were exceeded were the cost of vet visits (52% of respondents), cost of buying/rehoming a dog (32%) and patience needed to deal with behaviour (25%). Areas that were often less than what was expected included damage to furniture and other items (50%) and damage to garden (33%).

When considering the number of surprises experienced by respondents, both “more than expected” and “less than expected”, the mean number of surprises reported decreased steadily with an increase in owner age category. Respondents in younger age categories experienced a greater number of surprises, while older owners more frequently reported ownership aspects were as expected (Table 1). Younger respondents more frequently reported surprises around: amount of noise and activity, cost of feeding, and toys/beds, damage to furniture, social life impacts, patience needed to deal with behaviour, amount of mess, and time needed for exercise and training. One exception was the cost of vet visits, which was reported to be “more than expected” across all age groups; furthermore, the “more than expected” surprise at cost of vet visits also was more frequently reported as dog age increased.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The mean number of surprises overall, and less than or more than, when classifying 13 statements about aspects of dog ownership as more than, less than or as expected.

A higher number of mean surprises was recorded in those who owned fewer and younger dogs (Table 1). Those with fewer dogs also more frequently reported that damage to garden and furniture was “less than expected”. Owners of younger (particularly 0–2 years) dogs more frequently reported that damage to garden, amount of mess, time needed for training and patience needed were “more than expected” compared to owners with older dogs. The mean number of surprises was also lower for owners who acquired their dogs from rehoming centres, and higher in those getting dogs from general selling or pet selling websites. Those who acquired their dogs from rehoming centres also reported higher frequency of “less than expected” than other sources.

Finally, those who paid more for their dogs had a higher mean number of surprises and reported higher numbers of “more than expected” within their responses (Table 1). The amount of patience needed to deal with their dog’s behaviour was reported as “more than expected” more frequently by owners that had acquired their dog from foreign rehoming websites or pet selling websites. Where owners had acquired their dogs from websites, both pet and general sites, higher frequencies of “more than expected” were reported by owners for: more noise and activity, damage to furniture and garden, amount of mess and mud in the house, and time needed for training compared to other sources. Finally, owners sourcing their dogs from Kennel Club websites reported number of vet visits to be “more than expected” more frequently than owners who acquired their dogs from any other source.

3.2 Qualitative analysis

Of the codes generated from the responses, four distinct but interlinking themes were constructed: Emotional Connectedness of Human–Dog Relationships; Dog’s Impact on Human Health or Wellbeing; Meeting the Demands of Dog-Ownership, and; Understanding What Dogs are Like. Together, these themes reflect both emotional and practical dimensions of dog ownership and, overall, illustrate that dogs occupied a more prominent place in their owners’ lives than they had anticipated. The themes are outlined in Table 2. Data excerpts contained in Table 2 are referenced in the written account below, to illustrate the themes. In addition to these themes, some people commented that nothing had surprised them. These respondents often linked their lack of surprise to their previous ownership experience (e.g., “I have always had dogs so no surprises really.”), suggesting that owners’ expectations are shaped, in part, by their dog ownership history.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Key themes from the thematic analysis relating to surprises associated with dog ownership when asked “What has surprised you most about owning a dog?”

3.2.1 Emotional connectedness of human-dog relationships

This theme encapsulates respondents’ surprise regarding the intimacy of relationships established between themselves and their dogs. One valued aspect of human-dog relationships, that surprised some owners, was the company dogs provide (1a). Beyond occupying a purely functional role in owners’ lives, however, many respondents alluded to deeper human–dog relationships, commenting that they enjoyed friendship with their dog (1b), or considered their dog to be deeply embedded in the family unit (1c). Emphasising the emotional bond they share with their dogs, some respondents described the relationship with their dog as though the dog was akin to a child (1d; 1e). Many owners expressed surprise regarding the amount or depth of love and/or affection that they felt towards their dog and/or that they received from their dog (1f; 1 g). The love that dogs give their owner was often valued for its “unconditional” quality (1 h). Owners emphasised a few types of close relationships with their dogs (e.g., friendship or family) that were often described as developing quickly (1i). Many owners described feeling more attached to their dog than expected (1j), with some commenting that they miss them when they are not together or that they cannot now imagine life without their dog (or a dog) (1 k). Whilst some respondents focused on the attachment they felt to their dog, others noted that this feeling was reciprocal as they described a strong mutual bond between themselves and their dog (1 l).

3.2.2 Dogs’ impact on human health or wellbeing

This theme encapsulates participants’ surprise regarding the impact that their dog – or aspects of dog ownership – was perceived to have had on the health or wellbeing of themselves or other people, most typically a family or household member. Most references to health or wellbeing highlighted positive perceived changes to psychosocial aspects of health (i.e., mental, emotional, and social). Many respondents referred to improvements, since dog acquisition, in areas of mental health, including intrinsic positive feelings of happiness or enjoyment (2a; 2b). Sometimes these outcomes were associated with the time owners spent with their dog, including through specific activities, such as walking (2b), or as a result of the affection and love dogs give their owner (2c). Here, this theme connects to the theme Emotional Connectedness of Human-Dog Relationships. For some owners, dogs were felt to increase feelings of ease (2c), mitigate loneliness (2d), and provide purpose or motivation to get up or go outside (2e). Some respondents noted specific mental health conditions that they felt their dog had helped to alleviate (2c; 2f). Responses suggested that, in some cases, the perceived improvements to mental health may have been mediated (at least partially) by the emotional support or comfort that dogs were widely reported to provide, particularly during difficult periods in a person’s life (2 g; 2 h). Respondents were also surprised by the increase in social interactions they had experienced as a dog owner, sometimes forming social connections with other people through walking their dog (2i). Dogs were described as acting as a catalyst for conversation with strangers who they would not otherwise interact with (2j). As well as interactions and connections with strangers, some participants commented that their family dynamic had improved since acquiring their dog, with the dog perceived to have brought family members together (2 k). In addition to the emphasis that many respondents placed on impacts on psychosocial health, some respondents also highlighted benefits to physical health, achieved through increased exercise via walking (2 l).

However, some respondents expressed surprise at how they perceived dog ownership to have compromised their wellbeing. Worries about meeting their dog’s needs through optimising their health and happiness were reported (2 m), with some suggesting that their concerns were associated with their close relationship with, or attachment to, their dog (2n). Respondents also related their attachment to their dog with the emotional distress owners experience as dogs age and die (2o). This aspect of this theme should thus be interpreted in conjunction with the theme Emotional Connectedness of Human–Dog Relationships. A further threat to owner’s wellbeing was the emotional strain associated with the often-reported “hard work” involved in raising a puppy or owning a dog with challenging behaviour (2p), which was an aspect of the theme Meeting the Demands of Dog Ownership.

3.2.3 Meeting the demands of dog ownership

This theme is characterised by respondents’ surprise regarding the extent of dogs’ needs and how owners meet these. Meeting the demands of ownership was described by respondents in two primary ways: (1) through the provision of largely tangible things, for instance time (i.e., time spent training or exercising) or money, and; (2) how fulfilling their dog’s needs was associated with an all-encompassing caregiving role performed by the owner. Together, these aspects reflect the practical and affective dimensions of meeting the demands of dog ownership.

The first aspect of this theme concerned owners’ surprise regarding the extent of owner involvement or the amount of resources required to fulfil a dog’s needs. Some respondents emphasised their surprise regarding the amount of time and attention dog ownership involved (3a; 3b). For instance, owners noted that the amount of time required to meet their dog’s training needs was greater than expected, with some suggesting that they have found training to be an ongoing process, rather than an activity that can be fully completed (3c). For some respondents, dog ownership had been greater or harder work than anticipated, often due to the amount of training or care required (3d). An emphasis on more hard work was particularly expressed by owners of puppies (3d) or rescue dogs (3e), with these owners associating the hard work with inherent challenges they perceive to accompany these types of dogs. However, several respondents expressed that the hard work was worth it, given how rewarding they found dog ownership to be (3f). As well as the demand on owner’s time and effort, some respondents had not expected the financial costs associated with dog ownership to be so high. Veterinary costs were a commonly reported surprising expense. Some owners perceived veterinarians to be greedy (3 g), for instance by “upselling” procedures (3 h) or charging more than owners anticipated for issues they considered to be routine or minor (3i). The cost of insurance was also reported as surprising by a minority of owners, with some placing emphasis on the rising cost of insurance as dogs age (3j).

In attempting to fulfil their dog’s needs, some participants emphasised the level of commitment (3 k) or responsibility that they found to be required of dog owners, or that they felt towards their dog. A few respondents explicitly associated their sense of responsibility with their desire to optimise their dog’s happiness (3 l) or meet their training needs (3 m). Owners’ commitment to their dog’s wellbeing led them to perceive their lives as being organised around their dog’s needs. One consequence of this was a sense of restriction around owners’ spontaneity, with some viewing their dog as a tie – for instance, limiting their ability to travel (3n). A minority of respondents also noted their surprise that society – particularly hospitality businesses – is not always welcoming to dogs, adding to the additional forward planning required when wanting to go out and about with their dogs (3o).

3.2.4 Understanding what dogs are like

This theme encapsulates elements that comprise how dogs act, or who they are perceived to be as individuals, primarily in terms of their temperament, abilities and behaviour. One aspect that commonly surprised respondents was the amount of personality or character their dog has (4a). Specific personality traits noted were predominantly positive, including loyalty (4b), lovingness (4b), sociability or friendliness (4c), and fun (4d). Some respondents were surprised by how well-behaved their dog is (4e). There was an emphasis on dogs’ individuality as an aspect that surprised people, with some commenting on differences between different dogs’ temperaments (4c; 4e; 4f). As well as the individuality of dogs, some respondents reported their surprise regarding perceived breed-based differences in temperament (4f). A further aspect that surprised some owners was their dog’s activity or energy levels. A few people with previous ownership experience commented on the difference between energy levels in their current and formerly owned dogs (4 g). A small minority of respondents were surprised by the amount of time their dog spends sleeping (4 h).

Some respondents were surprised by how intelligent they perceived their dog to be, sometimes inferred from their dog’s ease of learning (4i). As well as general intelligence, dogs’ communicative abilities with people were also noted, with some owners commenting on their dog’s remarkable ability to understand and respond to human emotions and moods (4j).

A dog’s ability to adapt to their new surroundings surprised some respondents, particularly those who had adopted their dog (e.g., acquired from a rehoming organization). Some were surprised by how well or quickly their dog had settled in (4 k).

However, some aspects of dog temperament or behaviour were less positively regarded, as some respondents reported being surprised by elements of their dog’s behaviour they considered problematic. A range of specific issues were noted, including barking, which a minority of participants reported was a problem either for themselves or others (4 l). Separation-related behaviours (e.g., crying when left home alone) surprised a few others (4 m). A minority of respondents commented on behavioural issues associated with puppies, including puppy teething or biting (4n) and behavioural regression during adolescence (4o). Dog’s individuality was highlighted in some owners’ accounts of behavioural issues, whereby individual dogs had been more or less challenging than other dogs owned by the respondent (4 m).

A minority of participants were surprised by the messy and dirty aspects of their dog or dog ownership. The amount of hair that dogs shed was a commonly reported issue, with some respondents describing dog hair as ubiquitous in the home (4p). Some respondents reported aversion around their dog’s bodily functions, for instance referring to the amount of poo the dog produces (4q).

4 Discussion

The aim of this study was to explore how the experience of dog ownership compared with owner expectations in a sample of UK dog owners. Overall, the results of this study provide initial insights into certain areas of expectations surrounding dog ownership where owners may experience discrepancies and surprises related to dog ownership. The findings highlight several aspects of dog ownership that surprised owners in both positive and negative ways, which can be utilised to guide future research in this area as well as develop interventions aimed at supporting dog owners to reduce the likelihood of negative fallout, such as relinquishments, due to these discrepancies.

One key area of surprise for owners within this study was around meeting the demands of dog ownership, which encompassed both the extent of dogs’ needs and the capability of owners to meet these. This was identified through both the qualitative thematic analysis and the quantitative analysis of graded statements. Within this theme, it was common for respondents to be surprised by factors related to costs associated with dog ownership. Common findings within this study were unexpected costs relating to the care of the dog, such as feeding them, providing necessities such as beds and toys, and keeping them healthy. Previous research has also demonstrated that unexpected or excessive financial costs are perceived as a challenge in dog ownership (15). Within the quantitative data, the greatest surprise reported by owners was the costs related to veterinary care, with owners reporting this more frequently as the age of the dog increased, suggesting unmet expectations can occur some distance in time from acquisition. This increase in surprise of vet costs in owners of older dogs may either reflect a perceived increase in the costs associated with health conditions particularly prevalent in older dogs, or a wider perceived increase in service costs over time. This was also a key sub-theme within the qualitative analysis where many were surprised at the cost of care, particularly things they themselves considered minor or routine. This suggests that some owners may perceive veterinary surgeries to be overcharging or profiteering, when in reality many veterinary procedures, including routine ones, are cost intensive. With the UK’s funded public health system, owners may be unlikely to compare veterinary healthcare costs to that of a human healthcare setting, raising the question as to how owners benchmark veterinary costs. Research from the United States suggests that pet owners and veterinarians may differ in how they relate to veterinary costs (25). When discussing the costs of veterinary care in focus groups, pet owners suggested that animal care should come before profit, while veterinarians focused on the tangible aspects of their services (e.g., time) and felt that their work is undervalued. While some existing research in other countries has explored public perceptions of the veterinary profession (26), there is a lack of evidence pertaining to UK pet owners’ perceptions of the veterinary profession. Further research into public perception of the veterinary profession and the veterinarian-client relationship in the UK would be beneficial to understand this further, as well as to better understand cost-related barriers to obtaining veterinary care and ways to mitigate these. Breed can also impact on expectations of costs, with owners of brachycephalic breeds more prone to underestimate the veterinary cost of their dog due to the high disease burden seen in these breeds (27). Financial burden may occur where expectations of costs are exceeded. With financial reasons often reported as factors associated with the relinquishment of pets (2830) and in particular cats and dogs, it is important that owners are aware of and understand the potential lifetime financial costs, particularly routine costs, involved in dog ownership prior to acquisition.

Successful human-dog relationships often rely on met expectations, with ownership offering a symbiotic relationship for both dogs and their owners. As part of this relationship, it is imperative that owners invest the necessary time to provide their dogs with what they need, for both their dog’s welfare and their own, with research highlighting owners may experience negative wellbeing due to feeling they had not met their dog’s needs or expectations (31). Within this study, another area of surprise was the amount of time and effort needed to provide the necessary care for a dog. Often owners felt like their lives were organised around their dogs to fulfil their dog’s needs, and the level of commitment and responsibility was often noted as a surprise. Misconceptions of the amount of time needed for elements such as exercise and training may exist, particularly to those who may not conduct sufficient research before acquiring their dog or acquire a dog ill-suited to their lifestyle. A common owner-related reason for relinquishment of dogs is due to lack of time to spend with the dog, not being able to provide the time they need (15, 3234), whilst studies also highlight the increase in responsibility as a challenge of dog ownership (15, 27, 33). Scarlett et al. (34) found this was most common for owners of younger (less than 2 years) dogs, with 70% of owners having owned their dog less than a year, suggesting that in many cases this may be due to unrealistic expectations of time requirements, rather than changes of circumstances. Misconceptions of what specific breeds need has also been highlighted, with a study highlighting that owners of brachycephalic breeds were commonly surprised at the level of exercise and maintenance their dogs required (27).

Training is an important element of dog ownership and, within this study, the amount dogs needed often exceeded their owners’ expectations and there was surprise that this was a continual ongoing process and need throughout the dog’s entire life. Problematic behaviours are common reasons for relinquishment, posing significant concern and challenges to dog owners (15, 17, 31, 33, 34) and without adequate training and appropriate behaviour modification, subsequent undesirable behaviours may develop. Some aspects of dog behaviour were reported negatively by respondents with many owners being surprised by their dog’s problematic behaviour. This included issues such as separation-related behaviour and barking. Previous research has highlighted that dogs that behaved in unexpected ways resulted in reduced emotional closeness and attachment to dog – highlighting potential damage to the human-animal bond if expectations are not met (27). Furthermore, many owners were surprised by the amount of attention their dog needed, as well as the amount of patience; this was particularly true for those owning puppies and rescue dogs. While this unmet expectation has the potential to jeopardise the human-animal bond, for many the hard work was however worth it due to the rewards of ownership.

Another key finding of this study was the surprise that dog owners had around understanding what their dogs are like. When asked to construct their “ideal” dog, in a study by King et al. (35), owners reported a number of physical and behavioural traits that were important to them. This included traits such as friendliness and obedience, with women preferring traits such as calm and sociable and men selecting traits such as energetic and faithful. Evidence further suggests owners acquire dogs to provide a source of companionship, and therefore likely select a breed reported to offer a suitable personality and temperament to match their expectations (15, 36, 37). Despite this, within our research many respondents were surprised by elements of the way their dogs were with regards to their individual personalities and temperament, how much character they had, as well as their levels of activity and energy. It was also common for owners to report surprises when comparing their current dog to previously owned dogs, such as differences in temperament and energy levels. Research has shown owners may be more likely to return/relinquish a dog when comparing them to a previously owned dog’s needs and traits, due to being less tolerant of what they may consider misbehaviour (15). Dog needs and behaviour can both vary greatly between and within breeds (38, 39), and therefore expecting dogs to behave similarly to previously owned dogs, particularly if the same breed, may result in unrealistic expectations and ultimately negative outcomes.

The qualitative analysis provided deeper insight into factors not captured by the quantitative data in this study, such as the emotional connectedness of the dog-owner relationship, which was often reported as a surprise by owners. Existing literature suggests that companionship is a commonly expected benefit of dog ownership (15), with numerous studies reporting that companionship for the owner is a primary motivation for dog acquisition (37, 4042). Companionship for others in the household (including children, adults or dogs) is another common reason for getting a dog (37). Therefore, this study’s qualitative findings indicate that the perceived amount and depth of companionship provided by dogs may be even greater than expected. Similarly, whilst previous research has found that dogs are often understood as family members (36, 4349) and that, in Britain, pets have been considered as friends since the end of the 17th century (50), the closeness of owner-dog relationships nevertheless surprised many of our respondents.

Consistent with previous literature (47, 51) respondents associated strong human-dog attachments with the emotional support or comfort they perceived their dogs as providing them with. Whilst evidence indicates that prospective owners often anticipate mental and physical benefits (15, 36), many respondents in this study reported these as taking them by surprise, suggesting that this is another aspect of ownership that may be difficult to fully comprehend prior to acquisition. Despite owners’ surprise regarding the emotional benefits of dogs or dog ownership, this study’s qualitative findings also highlighted some respondents’ surprise around the more unpleasant emotions associated with dog ownership. Feelings of worry and guilt about whether they were sufficiently meeting their dog’s needs took some owners by surprise. This perceived negative impact of ownership on owner wellbeing has been similarly identified in previous research (51). Perceptions of pets as a source of worry were highlighted in studies of pet ownership experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic, with owner’s worries often linked to restricted access to veterinary care, food supply chain issues, or exercise restrictions during this time (5254). Beyond everyday worries, owners in this study also reflected on their surprise regarding the emotional distress felt when dogs die. Some respondents’ comments suggest that greater attachment with a dog may be positively associated with the severity of grief experienced or anticipated when dogs die, which echoes findings from previous research (51, 5558). Consistent with the contradictory findings in existing literature (51) our research suggests both positive and negative perceived impacts of dog ownership on owner’s wellbeing exist. Overall impacts of dog ownership (whether positive and/or negative) on owner wellbeing is likely dependent on the individual dog, owner and their unique relationship (51).

This study’s findings showed that many aspects of dog ownership were reported as surprising. Evidence suggests that around half of dog owners carry out pre-acquisition research ahead of acquiring their dog (59). It is therefore perhaps unsurprising that many owners report elements of ownership as not what they expected. In those that do conduct pre-acquisition research (and from appropriate sources), this disparity in their expectations may be due to prospective owners having difficulty in fully comprehending the lived reality prior to acquisition. In order to address this, interventions could include physical preparations such as spending time caring for dogs, e.g., co-care of a friend’s dog/fostering, on top of the desk-based research owners are encouraged to conduct before acquiring their dogs could, in order to prepare owners for the realities of dog ownership. Further research into expectations around dog ownership is warranted, as well as assessing interventions aimed at encouraging pre-acquisition research. Preparing owners ahead of acquisition of a dog may well aid understanding around the process of acquiring a dog, and reduce the likelihood of negative fallout such as decreased welfare, damage to the human-animal bond and relinquishment or euthanasia.

5 Limitations

This study indicates a number of areas that owners may experience discrepancies in expectations versus experience regarding dog ownership, however this study’s findings are subject to several limitations. Firstly, a lack of information regarding respondents’ expectations before acquisition precludes us from making direct comparisons between pre- and post-acquisition expectations and experiences. For example, we do not know how cautious or optimistic they were. Our data also did not capture respondents’ previous dog ownership experience, with previous dog experience shown to be an influential factor on owner expectations (16); this limits the conclusions drawn from the data somewhat. Secondly, our data are cross-sectional, and we expect that respondents would have owned their dogs for varying lengths of time. The length of time the dog had been owned could have affected respondents’ perceptions and may also introduce recall bias relying on respondents to recall information from a long time ago. For instance, previous research has found that dog adopters’ expectations for ownership relative to their experience changes over time: owning a dog was considered easier than they had expected by an increasing proportion of adopters over time (10). Finally, the population of dogs within this study could be considered “successful” relationships, given they are still living with their owner, suggesting that many owners whose experience violates their expectations may simply manage this. However due to limited questions in our survey regarding welfare, we are unable to comment on the impacts of mismanaged expectations on the welfare or both owner and/or dogs, nor can we predict subsequent outcomes from this study, such as relinquishment or euthanasia. Further research in this area would benefit from longitudinal study design exploring owners expectations both pre- and post-acquisition, collecting additional information such as that listed above within the limitations of this research to allow for a more thorough understanding of the discrepancy between expectations and actual lived reality. Furthermore, understanding how expectation discrepancies and surprises of ownership have impacted dog owners and outcomes such as relinquishment is warranted to provide deeper insight in this area.

6 Conclusion

Dog ownership can be very valuable to humans; therefore, it is unsurprising that they are most commonly owned companion animal in many countries, including the UK. Often attention is drawn towards the perks of ownership, and the realities and consequences therefore may not be widely considered by potential owners. Overall, this study indicates that owners’ reflections on dog ownership are complex and that this type of human-animal relationship involves forming close relationships often at a greater cost (e.g., financial and time) than anticipated. This study highlights that particular aspects of dog ownership, such as the strength of bonds and the extent of the benefits and challenges, may also be difficult to fully comprehend prior to ownership. Successful relationships, where owners are satisfied with their dogs, may rely on preconceived expectations being met and therefore our findings suggest a need to instill realistic expectations of dogs, and dog ownership, in aspirant owners to optimise dog and human health and welfare.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because the data are not publicly available due to consent not collected from participants to share the raw data. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to research@dogstrust.org.uk.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Dogs Trust Ethical Review Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

KA: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. KH: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. RAC: Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. BC: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. RMC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by Dogs Trust and the authors are salaried employees of Dogs Trust.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the Dogs Trust Marketing, Communications and Digital teams for supporting the development of the research and all dog owners who responded to the survey in this study. We are also grateful to Dr. Melissa Upjohn, Dr. Lauren Samet and Dr. Sara Owczarczak-Garstecka for comments on an earlier draft of this paper.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. PDSA . PDSA Animal Wellbeing (PAW) Report [Internet]. (2021) (Accessed October 25, 2021). Available at: https://www.pdsa.org.uk/what-we-do/pdsa-animal-wellbeing-report/paw-report-2021

Google Scholar

2. Neidhart, L , and Boyd, R . Companion animal adoption study. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (2002) 5:175–92. doi: 10.1207/S15327604JAWS0503_02

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Clark, CC , Gruffydd-Jones, T , and Murray, JK . Number of cats and dogs in UK welfare organisations. Vet Rec. (2012) 170:493–3. doi: 10.1136/vr.100524

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Lambert, K , Coe, J , Niel, L , Dewey, C , and Sargeant, JM . A systematic review and meta-analysis of the proportion of dogs surrendered for dog-related and owner-related reasons. Prev Vet Med. (2015) 118:148–60. doi: 10.1016/j.prevetmed.2014.11.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Coe, JB , Young, I , Lambert, K , Dysart, L , Nogueira Borden, L , and Rajić, A . A scoping review of published research on the relinquishment of companion animals. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (2014) 17:253–73. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2014.899910

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. New, JC , Salman, MD , King, M , Scarlett, JM , Kass, PH , and Hutchison, JM . Characteristics of shelter-relinquished animals and their owners compared with animals and their owners in U.S. pet-owning households. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (2000) 3:179–201. doi: 10.1207/S15327604JAWS0303_1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Powell, L , Reinhard, C , Satriale, D , Morris, M , Serpell, J , and Watson, B . Characterizing unsuccessful animal adoptions: age and breed predict the likelihood of return, reasons for return and post-return outcomes. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:8018. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-87649-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Hawes, SM , Kerrigan, JM , Hupe, T , and Morris, KN . Factors informing the return of adopted dogs and cats to an animal shelter. Animals. (2020) 10:1573. doi: 10.3390/ani10091573

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Diesel, G , Pfeiffer, DU , and Brodbelt, D . Factors affecting the success of rehoming dogs in the UK during 2005. Prev Vet Med. (2008) 84:228–41. doi: 10.1016/j.prevetmed.2007.12.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Powell, L , Lee, B , Reinhard, CL , Morris, M , Satriale, D , Serpell, J, et al. Returning a shelter dog: the role of owner expectations and dog behavior. Animals. (2022) 12:1053. doi: 10.3390/ani12091053

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Miller, DD , Staats, SR , Partlo, C , and Rada, K . Factors associated with the decision to surrender a pet to an animal shelter. J Am Vet Ass. (1996) 209:738–42. doi: 10.2460/javma.1996.209.04.738

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Burnett, E , Brand, CL , O’Neill, DG , Pegram, CL , Belshaw, Z , Stevens, KB, et al. How much is that doodle in the window? Exploring motivations and behaviours of UK owners acquiring designer crossbreed dogs (2019-2020). Canine Med Genet. (2022) 9:8. doi: 10.1186/s40575-022-00120-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Hladky-Krage, B , and Hoffman, CL . Expectations versus reality of designer dog ownership in the United States. Animals. (2022) 12:3247. doi: 10.3390/ani12233247

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Watson, E , and Niestat, L . Osseous lesions in the distal extremities of dogs with strangulating hair mats. Vet Radiol Ultrasound. (2021) 62:37–43. doi: 10.1111/vru.12924

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Powell, L , Chia, D , McGreevy, P , Podberscek, AL , Edwards, KM , Neilly, B, et al. Expectations for dog ownership: perceived physical, mental and psychosocial health consequences among prospective adopters. PLoS One. (2018) 13:e0200276. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0200276

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Bouma, EMC , Vink, LM , and Dijkstra, A . Expectations versus reality: long-term research on the dog–owner relationship. Animals. (2020) 10:772. doi: 10.3390/ani10050772

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. O’Connor, R , Coe, JB , Niel, L , and Jones-Bitton, A . Effect of adopters’ lifestyles and animal-care knowledge on their expectations prior to companion-animal guardianship. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (2016) 19:157–70. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2015.1125295

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Carroll, GA , Torjussen, A , and Reeve, C . Companion animal adoption and relinquishment during the COVID-19 pandemic: Peri-pandemic pets at greatest risk of relinquishment. Front Vet Sci. (2022) 9:9. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2022.1017954

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Anderson, KL , Casey, RA , Cooper, B , Upjohn, MM , and Christley, RM . National dog survey: describing UK dog and ownership demographics. Animals. (2023) 13:1072. doi: 10.3390/ani13061072

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Creswell, J , and Plano-Clark, V . Designing and conducting mixed methods research. Third ed. Los Angeles: SAGE Publications Inc (2017).

Google Scholar

21. R Core Team R: A language and environment for statistical computing [internet]. (Accessed September 12, 2022). Available at: https://www.r-project.org

Google Scholar

22. Braun, V , and Clarke, V . Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual Res Psychol. (2006) 3:77–101. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Braun, V , and Clarke, V . Reflecting on reflexive thematic analysis. Qual Res Sport Exerc Health. (2019) 11:589–97. doi: 10.1080/2159676X.2019.1628806

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Malterud, K , Siersma, VD , and Guassora, AD . Sample size in qualitative interview studies. Qual Health Res. (2016) 26:1753–60. doi: 10.1177/1049732315617444

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Coe, JB , Adams, CL , and Bonnett, BN . A focus group study of veterinarians’ and pet owners’ perceptions of the monetary aspects of veterinary care. J Am Vet Med Ass. (2007) 231:1510–8. doi: 10.2460/javma.231.10.1510

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Kedrowicz, AA , and Royal, KD . A comparison of public perceptions of physicians and veterinarians in the United States. Vet Sci. (2020) 7:50. doi: 10.3390/vetsci7020050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Packer, RMA , O’Neill, DG , Fletcher, F , and Farnworth, MJ . Great expectations, inconvenient truths, and the paradoxes of the dog-owner relationship for owners of brachycephalic dogs. PLoS One. (2019) 14:e0219918. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0219918

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Dolan, E , Scotto, J , Slater, M , and Weiss, E . Risk factors for dog relinquishment to a Los Angeles municipal animal shelter. Animals. (2015) 5:1311–28. doi: 10.3390/ani5040413

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Eagan, BH , Gordon, E , and Protopopova, A . Reasons for guardian-relinquishment of dogs to shelters: animal and regional predictors in British Columbia, Canada. Front Vet Sci. (2022) 9:9. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2022.857634

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Salman, MD , New, JG Jr, Scarlett, JM , Kass, PH , Ruch-Gallie, R , and Hetts, S . Human and animal factors related to relinquishment of dogs and cats in 12 selected animal shelters in the United States. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (1998) 1:207–26. doi: 10.1207/s15327604jaws0103_2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Barcelos, AM , Kargas, N , Maltby, J , Hall, S , and Mills, DS . A framework for understanding how activities associated with dog ownership relate to human well-being. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:11363. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-68446-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Diesel, G , Smith, H , and Pfeiffer, DU . Factors affecting time to adoption of dogs re-homed by a charity in the UK. Anim Welf. (2007) 16:353–60. doi: 10.1017/S0962728600027160

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Jensen, JBH , Sandøe, P , and Nielsen, SS . Owner-related reasons matter more than behavioural problems—a study of why owners relinquished dogs and cats to a Danish animal shelter from 1996 to 2017. Animals. (2020) 10:1064. doi: 10.3390/ani10061064

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Scarlett, JM , Salman, MD , New, JG Jr, and Kass, PH . Reasons for relinquishment of companion animals in U.S. animal shelters: selected health and personal issues. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (1999) 2:41–57. doi: 10.1207/s15327604jaws0201_4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. King, T , Marston, LC , and Bennett, PC . Describing the ideal Australian companion dog. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2009) 120:84. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.04.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Holland, KE , Mead, R , Casey, RA , Upjohn, MM , and Christley, RM . Why do people want dogs? A mixed-methods study of motivations for dog acquisition in the United Kingdom. Front Vet Sci. (2022) 9:9. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2022.877950

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Packer, RMA , Brand, CL , Belshaw, Z , Pegram, CL , Stevens, KB , and O’Neill, DG . Pandemic puppies: characterising motivations and behaviours of UK owners who purchased puppies during the 2020 COVID-19 pandemic. Animals. (2021) 11:2500. doi: 10.3390/ani11092500

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Morrill, K , Hekman, J , Li, X , McClure, J , Logan, B , Goodman, L, et al. Ancestry-inclusive dog genomics challenges popular breed stereotypes. Science. (2022) 376:eabk0639. doi: 10.1126/science.abk0639

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Hammond, A , Rowland, T , Mills, DS , and Pilot, M . Comparison of behavioural tendencies between “dangerous dogs” and other domestic dog breeds – evolutionary context and practical implications. Evol Appl. (2022) 15:1806–19. doi: 10.1111/eva.13479

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Jagoe, A , and Serpell, J . Owner characteristics and interactions and the prevalence of canine behaviour problems. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (1996) 47:31–42. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(95)01008-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Kobelt, AJ , Hemsworth, PH , Barnett, JL , and Coleman, GJ . A survey of dog ownership in suburban Australia—conditions and behaviour problems. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2003) 82:137–48. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(03)00062-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Endenburg, N , and Bouw, J . Motives for acquiring companion animals. J Econ Psychol. (1994) 15:191–206. doi: 10.1016/0167-4870(94)90037-X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Franklin, A . Human-nonhuman animal relationships in Australia: an overview of results from the first national survey and follow-up case studies 2000-2004. Soc Anim. (2007) 15:7–27. doi: 10.1163/156853007X169315

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Franklin, A . ‘Be[a]ware of the dog’: a post-humanist approach to housing. Hous Theory Soc. (2006) 23:137–56. doi: 10.1080/14036090600813760

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Power, E . Furry families: making a human-dog family through home. Soc Cult Geogr. (2008) 9:535–55. doi: 10.1080/14649360802217790

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Fox, R . Animal behaviours, post-human lives: everyday negotiations of the animal-human divide in pet-keeping. Soc Cult Geogr. (2006) 7:525–37. doi: 10.1080/14649360600825679

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Charles, N . ‘Animals just love you as you are’: experiencing kinship across the species barrier. Sociology. (2014) 48:715–30. doi: 10.1177/0038038513515353

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Charles, N , and Davies, CA . My family and other animals: pets as kin. Sociol Res Online. (2008) 13:13–26. doi: 10.5153/sro.1798

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Laurent-Simpson, A . Just like family: how companion animals joined the household. New York: NYU press (2021).

Google Scholar

50. Thomas, K . Man and the natural world. London: Penguin (1984).

Google Scholar

51. Merkouri, A , Graham, TM , O’Haire, ME , Purewal, R , and Westgarth, C . Dogs and the good life: a cross-sectional study of the association between the dog–owner relationship and owner mental wellbeing. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.903647

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Ratschen, E , Shoesmith, E , Shahab, L , Silva, K , Kale, D , Toner, P, et al. Human-animal relationships and interactions during the Covid-19 lockdown phase in the UK: investigating links with mental health and loneliness. Triberti S, editor. PLoS one. (2020) 15:e0239397. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0239397

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Applebaum, JW , Tomlinson, CA , Matijczak, A , McDonald, SE , and Zsembik, BA . The concerns, difficulties, and stressors of caring for pets during COVID-19: results from a large survey of U.S. pet owners. Animals. (2020) 10:1882. doi: 10.3390/ani10101882

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Holland, KE , Owczarczak-Garstecka, SC , Anderson, KL , Casey, RA , Christley, RM , Harris, L, et al. “More attention than usual”: a thematic analysis of dog ownership experiences in the UK during the first COVID-19 lockdown. Animals. (2021) 11:240. doi: 10.3390/ani11010240

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Archer, J , and Winchester, G . Bereavement following death of a pet. Br J Psychol. (1994) 85:259–71. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02522.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Field, N , Orsini, L , Gavish, R , and Packman, W . Role of attachment in response to pet loss. Death Stud. (2009) 33:334–55. doi: 10.1080/07481180802705783

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Wrobel, TA , and Dye, AL . Grieving pet death: normative, gender, and attachment issues. OMEGA J Death Dying. (2003) 47:385–93. doi: 10.2190/QYV5-LLJ1-T043-U0F9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Eckerd, LM , Barnett, JE , and Jett-Dias, L . Grief following pet and human loss: closeness is key. Death Stud. (2016) 40:275–82. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2016.1139014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Mead, R , Holland, KE , Casey, RA , Upjohn, MM , and Christley, RM . “Do your homework as your heart takes over when you go looking”: factors associated with pre-acquisition information-seeking among prospective UK dog owners. Animals. (2023) 13:1015. doi: 10.3390/ani13061015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dog, expectations, human-animal bond, pet ownership, dog acquisition

Citation: Anderson KL, Holland KE, Casey RA, Cooper B and Christley RM (2024) Owner expectations and surprises of dog ownership experiences in the United Kingdom. Front. Vet. Sci. 11:1331793. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793

Received: 01 November 2023; Accepted: 25 January 2024;
Published: 07 February 2024.

Edited by:

Mika Simonen, University of Helsinki, Finland

Reviewed by:

Jo Hockenhull, University of Bristol, United Kingdom
Otto Segersven, University of Helsinki, Finland

Copyright © 2024 Anderson, Holland, Casey, Cooper and Christley. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Katharine L. Anderson, Katharine.Anderson@dogstrust.org.uk

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.