Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Vet. Sci., 30 January 2020
Sec. Animal Behavior and Welfare

Effects of Dark Brooder Rearing and Age on Hypothalamic Vasotocin and Feather Corticosterone Levels in Laying Hens

  • 1Behaviour and Welfare Research Group, Department of Farm Animal Health, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands
  • 2UMC Utrecht Brain Center, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, Netherlands
  • 3Department of Animal Science, Aarhus University, Tjele, Denmark

Chickens cannot independently thermoregulate at hatch and lack opportunity to behaviorally thermoregulate with a hen in the egg layer industry, thus barns are heated to thermoneutral temperatures. Dark brooders are low-energy-consuming hot plates, which may be environmentally advantageous while providing welfare-enhancing aspects of maternal care (i.e., shelter and separation of active and inactive individuals). Dark brooder use has been demonstrated to decrease injurious pecking and mortality well into the production period of layers. To further understand hen development around lay onset and effects of dark brooders on the brain and HPA-axis, we examined effects of rearing with dark brooders on expression of vasotocin (AVT) in the hypothalamus and corticosterone (CORT) in the feathers of in total 48 layer Isa Warren hens at 16 w and 28 w of age (n = 12 per age and treatment). An age-dependent decreased number of AVT-positive neurons was seen in the medial preoptic area, medial preoptic nucleus, paraventricular nucleus, rostral part (prepeduncular hypothalamus), and lateral preoptic area. Trends to effects of brooder rearing were found in both anteromedial preoptic nucleus and supraoptic nucleus, with dark brooder reared animals showing higher mean counts of AVT-positive neurons in both areas. No interactions between brooder raising and age were observed in AVT-positive neuron count. CORT levels were higher in primary wing feathers from 28 week old hens than in those from 16 week hens. No main effects of rearing with dark brooders or interactions between age and treatment were found on CORT levels. The age-dependent effects seen in the hypothalamus and CORT aids in further understanding of the development of chickens around puberty. The use of brooders tended to increase AVT expression in the anteromedial preoptic nucleus and supraoptic nucleus, an indication that dark brooder rearing may affect physiological responses regulated by these areas. The lack of effect of dark brooders on CORT in feathers is at the least an indication that the use of dark brooders is not stressful; in combination with the benefits of dark brooders on injurious pecking, fearfulness and early mortality, this pleads for the use of dark brooders in on-farm situations.

Introduction

Chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus) hatch with limited ability to produce warmth by thermogenesis (1); upon emergence of thermoregulation at 2 weeks of age, chicks are still unable to maintain their normal body temperature at 21°C or lower ambient temperature (2). Mature thermoregulation is not present until ~4 weeks of age, at least in part due to replacement of downs by feathers. Therefore, chicks' survival in the first weeks depends on their own ability to thermoregulate behaviorally, unless aided artificially.

Under natural conditions, chicks thermoregulate by regularly seeking warmth under a broody hen. In the egg layer industry, adding broody mother hens to flocks of typically several thousand birds in homogenous age groups is not feasible for practical, disease prevention, and economic reasons. Thus, breeding barns are heated to temperatures that allow the chicks to maintain a healthy body temperature, usually starting at 34°C and gradually decreasing to 20–22°C over a period of 4 weeks (3). Barn heating has several drawbacks. First, heating a large space costly in terms of economics and environmental impact. Second, in conventionally heated barns, chicks are not provided with shelter, dark areas, or resting spaces, aspects normally provided by a broody hen which may be important to their development (47).

A possible alternative to barn heating is the use of dark brooders: low-energy-consuming hot plates surrounded by horizontal flaps, creating a dark and warm space. Chicks creep under the brooders when they need extra heat, mimicking the use of a broody hen to thermoregulate behaviorally. This allows barn temperature to be reduced, leading to a considerable saving in energy consumption (Unpublished data). Brooders may also have positive effects on welfare problems in the egg layer industry. Chicks bred with broody hens are less fearful than chicks bred without mother hens (8). Likewise, brooders have been shown to reduce the level of fear in chicks (7). Fear is a strong stressor that inhibits the immune system, growth, feed exploitation, and egg production (9), and may also induce or exacerbate feather pecking in laying hens (10). There is strong evidence that the use of dark brooders reduces feather pecking not only during rearing, but as a long-lasting effect into the laying period (6, 7, 1113).

Dark brooders provide aspects of maternal care that may help chicks in situations where a broody hen is not present (i.e., on farm). Maternal deprivation has been reported to affect the neuro-endocrine response to stress in mammals (1416). Moreover, an increase in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis response correlates with maternally deprived animals' fearful reactions to novel situations (17). In avian species, quality of maternal care has been related to corticosterone (CORT) levels [Aphelocoma coerulescens; (18)]. In young chicks of domestic fowl, auditory and olfactory cues from a mother hen have positive effects on learning and memory (19), and stress levels (20). Maternal care in chicks also promotes exploratory behavior (21). Both fearfulness (22) and knowledge about potential threats is communicated from (surrogate) mother hens to chicks (23, 24), suggesting that (aspects of) maternal care may have long-term effects on (among other things) the HPA-axis, including CORT release.

Maternal care, or lack thereof, can also lead to long-term changes in neuroanatomy visible even in adulthood in humans and rodents (25). Previous studies from our group have demonstrated long-lasting effects to maternal care on chicken brain; maternal care to layer chicks in early life caused increased expression of arginine vasotocin (AVT; a vasopressin ortholog found in birds, reptiles and fish) in the medial preoptic area of the hypothalamus (26) and alterations in differences of cell size between the two hemispheres in the hippocampus (27) when brains of adult animals were examined. AVT receptor expression is also modulated by CORT in avians (28), indicating a link between the two systems.

Given the impracticality of housing broody hens with chicks in industrial situations on the one hand, and the desirability of introducing aspects of maternal care to improve welfare and reduce energy use on the other hand, investigation of the effects of the use of dark brooders during rearing on physiological parameters related to stress and welfare is of strong interest. Previously published results from the present group of animals demonstrated strong positive effects of dark brooders on plumage condition, wound reduction and reduction in mortality rates (13). To further examine mechanisms behind these changes, we therefore investigated effects of rearing hens with a dark brooder from hatch through 6 weeks of age on CORT concentrations in feathers and on AVT expression in the hypothalamus at 16 and at 28 weeks of age.

Materials and Methods

All procedures involving animals were in accordance with the Danish Ministry of Justice Law No. 382 (10 June 1987) and Acts 333 (19 May 1990), 726 (9 September 1993) and 1016 (12 December 2001). This study is part of a larger project that also evaluated the effects of brooders on welfare, including behavior, fear, injurious pecking damage, mortality and production parameters (7, 13).

Animals

The chickens used in this study were non-beak trimmed Isa Warren layers, obtained when 1 day old from TopÆg Aps, Viborg, Denmark.

Housing

Animals were housed at the animal housing facilities at Aarhus University, Foulum, in Denmark. Upon arrival groups of 100–103 individuals were placed in 22 pens, each measuring 4 × 2 × 2 m (Length × Width × Height). When 16 weeks old the group sizes were reduced to 50 birds/group. There were four different brooder treatments (each consisting of four replicate groups) differing in size of the available area under the brooder per chick (54 or 72 cm2) and in whether the brooder was raised at regular intervals [for more details see (13)]. Due to the time consuming and costly nature of the procedures applied in the present study, only one of the brooder treatment, i.e., four treatment groups, and only five of the six control groups were included.

In the dark brooder treatment selected, one water-heated dark brooder (Jyden Bur A/S) of 120 × 60 cm was present in each of the pens. The dark brooders were 15 cm from the ground in the “lowered” position such that the chicks could access a warm and dark area under the brooders; the chicks could all fit under the brooder and were regularly observed to do so. Every 4 h on days 0–4, the dark brooders were lifted 140 cm from the ground for 10 min to mimic the behavior of a mother hen when initiating an active period. After day 4 of age, the brooders remained in the lowered position. The brooders were then permanently raised to 200 cm above the ground when the chickens reached 41 days of age. Throughout the time that the dark brooders were present in the pens, the dark brooders were used as a primary heat source, with the temperature under the brooder being 34°C for the first 3 days, then lowered a half-degree each day until 20°C was reached on day 30. The room temperature in the dark brooder group was kept at 24°C on day 0 and 1, 22°C on day 3 and 4 and then kept constant at 20°C. The control groups were housed in an adjacent identical room where the room temperature was kept at the same temperature schedule as used for the temperature under the brooders. See Table 1 for a schematic representation of temperatures for both groups. In order to minimize pen-effect, all groups were reallocated over the 22 pens at day 44 of age.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Schematic overview of the room temperatures used for control and dark brooder groups and the temperature under the dark brooders.

Experimental Animals

For the histological and feather CORT investigations 2–3 animals from each of the control groups and three animals from each of the brooder groups were selected. This was done both at 16 w of age (113–114 d of age, just before group size reduction) and again at 28 w (198–199 d) of age, giving a total of 48 animals for the present experiment (N = 12 per treatment per age). Animals were selected randomly but were required to fall within the 25th and 75th percentile with regards to body weight, as determined by weighing another group of animals from the same strain, age and supplier housed in the same facilities. None of the pullets had come into lay at 16 weeks of age. In addition to being within the 25th and 75th percentile with regards to body weight, the hens selected at 28 weeks of age also had a comb of a normal size and color, indicating that they were in lay.

Feather Collection and Brain Dissection

The selected animals were sacrificed by cervical dislocation. Primary wing feathers 2 and 8 were plucked from both wings from each chicken, protected from light with aluminum foil and stored at room temperature until processing. Brains were dissected then immersion fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde. After 24 h, brains were transferred to 70% ethanol and stored at 4°C until sectioning.

Brain Sectioning and Immunohistochemistry

Before sectioning, the chicken brains were embedded in gelatin and stored in a 30% sucrose solution at 4°C. The gelatin embedded brains were sliced according to the chicken atlas (29), with sections collected from rostral to plate 13 until caudal to plate 15 using a vibratoom (Leica VT1200S) set at 40 μm. Sections were collected in four series, thus with 160 μm between each section and stored in tubes of 0.12 M phosphate buffer saline (PBS) and 0.1% sodium azide at 4°C.

Single-Label Immunohistochemistry

Procedures for AVT immunohistochemistry (IHC) were as in Hewlett at al. (26). Briefly, IHC was done using a Vector labs Elite ABC kit (Brunschwig Chemie, Amsterdam) on free-floating sections. Unless otherwise stated, all steps were done at room temperature and by placing the sections on a low speed shaker to help evenly expose the sections to the solutions. Sections were placed into 0.12 M PBS for 10 min. Subsequent washing steps were in 0.05 M tris-buffered saline (TBS) for 5 min and repeated three times. Sections were then washed before incubation in 0.3% H2O2 in methanol for 10 min to remove endogenous peroxidase. After another washing step, the sections were incubated for 1 h in 5% normal goat serum (NGS) blocking buffer in 0.03% triton-X100 in TBS (TBST). Excess buffer was removed and the slides incubated in primary antiserum for AVT (rabbit anti-AVT 1:2,500, kind gift from Prof. S. Blähser) in 1% NGS-TBST blocking buffer for 1 h at room temperature and then at 4°C for 20 h. The following day, sections were washed three times in TBST before incubation for 1 h in secondary antiserum (goat anti-rabbit IgG 1:200, DAKO, Denmark) in 1% NGS-TBST blocking buffer. After washing, sections were incubated in 1% avidin/biotin solution (Vectastain ABC-Elite kit) in TBS for 45 min. Sections were then washed before being stained with 3,3'-Diaminobenzidine (DAB, Sigma) with 0.02% H2O2 peroxidase in TBS for 120 s, then washed again. Finally, sections were mounted onto SuperFrostplus® slides, allowed to dry overnight, processed through a clearing and dehydration series of ethanol and xylene to remove lipids and water, and cover-slipped using DePeX (Serva Electrophoresis, Heidelberg).

Antiserum Specificity

To prevent cross-reaction to the related peptide mesotocin, the AVT specific rabbit serum was absorbed to oxytocin (the mammalian equivalent of mesotocin) coupled to CNBR-activated Sepahrose4B according to the manufacturer's protocol (GE Healthcare, the Netherlands) and the non-binding fraction of the antiserum was used. In addition, the specificity of the antiserum specific for AVT in chicken tissue was demonstrated by replacing the primary antibody with normal rabbit IgG as a control for non-specific interaction. This IHC study was possible through generous contribution of Prof. S. Blähser of pure Rabbit anti-AVT serum.

AVT-Positive Neuron Counting

All suitable sections obtained from the staining procedure were subsequently counted according to the atlas of the chicken brain by Puelles et al. (29). All of the slides which were undamaged and in the area of interest were counted, which equaled 3–9 slides per animal. AVT-positive neurons were counted in the following hypothalamic areas: medial preoptic area (MPA); medial preoptic nucleus (MPO); paraventricular nucleus, rostral part (prepeduncular hypothalamus) (PaRO); lateral preoptic area (LPO); anteromedial preoptic nucleus (AMPO); supraoptic nucleus (SO). All slides were counted manually and then averaged among the total slides used per chicken. If a region was not present in all the slides, then that region was averaged over the slides in which that area was present. All counting was done using a Jenaval microscope under the magnification (100×) and (200×). The final statistics were conducted on observations from a single observer, but at the beginning and the end of measuring, counting was conducted by two observers to “benchmark” and ensure that the counting had not changed during the course of analysis. Photographs of the slides for reporting were taken using an Olympus BX40 at (40×).

CORT Extraction

CORT Extraction from feathers was performed using a modified version of the protocol described in Bortolotti et al. (30), as previously described in von Eugen et al. (31) and Zeinstra et al. (32). Extraction began by cleaning the feathers using aerosol solution of methanol. The calamus and rachis were removed and only the vane region was used for analysis. The feathers were cut in slices of roughly 5 mm2 flakes. The feathers were measured in length before and after cutting and then weighed. The extraction itself consisted of sonicating the feathers in a water bath for 30 min and subsequently incubating the flakes in an 80% methanol solution, 5 mL with an additional 1 mL/0.03 g of feather, overnight. The samples were centrifuged the following day at 2,200 g for 10 min at room temperature (20°C). The supernatant was then divided into 4 Eppendorf tubes per feather sample and dried using a vacuum pump (Speedvac, Savant AES 1000-240) at a medium dry rate for 3 h. Dried residue was later reconstituted using the respective buffers from the kit used for analysis.

CORT Enzyme Immunoassay (EIA)

CORT EIA was performed using the CORT EIA Kit (Cayman Chemical Company). After buffer, sample and reagent preparation, standards were made according to protocol ranging from 5,000 to 8.2 pg/mL. All reagents, standards, samples and buffers were added to the wells according to a scheme and allowed to incubate for 2 h on a shaker. The plate was then washed 5 times in wash buffer. Right before use, Ellmans reagent was reconstituted and added to the plate and allowed to incubate for 1 h at room temperature in total darkness. The plate was then subsequently read at 405 nm. All values are expressed as pg CORT per mg of feather vane.

The results from the CORT EIA obtained from the Microplate reader (DTX 880, Beckman Coulter) were analyzed using an excel file provided from the company that produced the kit (Cayman Chemical Company). The excel results were then statistically analyzed using SAS.

Statistical Analysis

The assumption of normality was assessed using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test (PROC UNIVARIATE, SAS Inst. Inc., Cary, NC). For the IHC results, it was not possible to achieve a normal distribution to both conditions using transformations, thus a Friedman's two-way non-parametric analysis was performed on the ranked values with age and treatment (brooder vs. control) as main effects, and age × treatment interaction.

Visual inspection of the data indicated a possible outlier in the corticosterone data from the control group; this was statistically confirmed with a Grubbs' test, also called the ESD method (extreme studentized deviate), thus data from one control animal were removed from this analysis. The CORT levels expressed in pg/mg feather were normally distributed following exclusion of this outlier, thus ANOVA analysis was performed. A repeated measures ANOVA was conducted with feather (2 and 8) as repeated measure, and with the main effects age (16 weeks vs. 28 weeks) and treatment (brooder vs. control). The experimental unit was individual animal.

Results

Hypothalamic AVT Levels

The number of AVT-positive neurons in the MPA, MPO, PaRO and LPO was strongly influenced by age, with higher numbers of AVT-positive neurons in all four areas in the 16 w old animals compared to the 28 w old animals (see Figure 1, Table 2). Trends to effects of brooder raising were found in both AMPO and SO, with dark brooder reared animals showing higher mean counts of AVT-positive neurons in both areas. No interactions between brooder raising and age were observed.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Number of AVT-positive neurons counted per region of interest (mean ± SEM). (A) MPA, medial preoptic area; (B) MPO, medial preoptic nucleus; (C) PaRO, paraventricular nucleus, rostral part (prepeduncular hypothalamus); (D) AMPO, anteromedial preoptic nucleus; (E) SO, supraoptic nucleus; (F) LPO, lateral preoptic area. 16w, 16 weeks of age; 28w, 28 weeks of age.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Statistical results of AVT-positive neuron count comparisons (Friedman's Two-Way Nonparametric ANOVA).

Feather CORT

CORT levels were higher in primary wing feathers from 28 week old hens than in those from 16 week hens (Figure 2). This effect was seen in both feathers 2 and feather 8, and a main effect of age was confirmed statistically (see Table 3). No main effects of rearing with a brooder or interactions between effects were found on CORT levels in the feathers sampled.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Bars indicate concentrations of CORT in pg per mg of feather vane measured in primary wing feather 2 (A) and primary wing feather 8 (B) from hens at 16 weeks (16 w) and 28 weeks (28 w) of age. Bars indicate average, error bars indicate SEM.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Statistical results of CORT in primary wing feathers (pg CORT/mg feather, repeated measures ANOVA).

Discussion

Age had an effect on both AVT expression in the hypothalamic areas MPA, MPO, PaRO and LPO, and CORT concentrations in feathers, with CORT levels higher in older animals and the number of AVT-positive neurons higher in younger animals. Rearing with brooders did not significantly alter CORT or AVT levels, though brooder rearing did produce a trend to increased AVT in AMPO and SO.

AVT Expression in Relation to Dark Brooders and Age

The AVT-positive immunoreactive neuron distribution observed in the present study is comparable to that previously reported in chicken, i.e., from our lab by Hewlett et al. (26), and previously by Viglietti-Panzica (33) and Tennyson et al. (34). Broadly similar distributions have been reported in other avian species, such as starling, duck and Japanese quail by Bons (35), Panzica et al. (36) and Goossens et al. (37), and in the dark-eyed junco by Panzica et al. [Junco hyemalis, (38)]. Although brooder rearing did not significantly alter the number of AVT-positive neurons, trends to increased numbers of AVT-immunopositive neurons were seen in the SO and the AMPO. This alteration in AVT cannot directly be linked to positive or negative welfare states; given that both excitatory and inhibitory transmitters are localized in the hypothalamus, sometimes even colocalizing with neuropeptides within a single neuron (39). However, the trend to effect indicates a potential link- be it positive or negative- with the physiological functions in which the SO and AMPO play a role.

There is a wide body of evidence linking the SO to physiological functions in avians, specifically to osmotic and hemodynamic regulation. Osmotic stress and hypotensic stress increase AVT expression in the chicken SO (4042). Osmotic stress also increases c-fos expression in AVT-positive neurons in the SO in chicken, zebrafinch, ring dove, and Japanese quail (43), and increases neuron size in the SO in quail (44, 45). It is interesting in this context that animals reared with brooders spent less time drinking during the first 4 days of life, probably due to a more efficient thermoregulation resulting in less respiratory evaporation (13).

The specific role of the AMPO has been less well-described. Lesions in the AMPO in rats have been linked to disruptions of the pre-ovulatory LH surge and reduction in prolactin surge (46, 47). The AMPO also undergoes changes in rat in relation to parity (48). The mammalian equivalent to AVT, vasopressin, has been shown to stimulate an LH surge when injected directly into the preoptic area (including the AMPO) (49). In all, this indicates a potential link between increased AVT in the AMPO and ovulation, which could mean that dark brooders may be beneficial to ovulation and thus potentially to egg laying.

Age-dependent decreases in AVT were observed in the MPA, MPO, PaRO, and LPO areas of the hypothalamus. The age-related expression of AVT may be related to sexual development and lay onset in the chickens in the present study. The MPO has been linked to nest-exploration behavior in starlings, with higher estradiol, higher preproenkephalin (PENK) mRNA and lower levels of D1 and D2 dopamine receptor mRNA in the MPO in female starlings which had explored a nest than those which had not (50). Onset of laying in Isa Warren hens is generally around 19 weeks of age, with peak production at 26 weeks of age. The 16-week hens in the current study would have been just prior to the start of laying, while the 28 week hens are at peak production levels. AVT expression in ovaries has been strongly linked to oviposition in avian species (5153). Previous studies in quail examining age-dependency of AVT expression in ovaries found no evidence of AVT expression in ovaries at 4 weeks of age (just prior to lay onset), high expression at 12 weeks of age (at peak egg production), and lowered expression at 78 weeks (at low egg production levels) (54), underlining the role of AVT in sexual maturity in avian species.

Interestingly, all of the areas examined showed either significant effects of age or a trend to an effect of brooder rearing, but not both. No interactions were seen between the two main effects. This indicates a quite strict topographical separation of effects in the areas examined.

CORT Levels in Feathers Increase From 16 to 28 Weeks of Age

In the current study, higher levels of CORT were found in feathers taken from 28 week old hens compared to 16 week old pullets. A major event that takes place between these two ages is onset of laying, i.e., the transition from pullet to a sexually mature hen. It is not known exactly how long CORT is stored in feathers, or how long it takes for circulating CORT to accumulate in feathers, but it is a reasonable assumption that an increase observed in feathers at a later age is a reflection of higher circulating CORT levels at later ages. No effect of brooder rearing was seen on CORT levels in feathers. This may indicate that either effects of brooders are too subtle to be measured in the long-term accumulation of CORT in feathers, or, alternatively, that the use of brooders does not directly affect the HPA-axis. Further research in this area is warranted.

The age-dependent increase in feather CORT observed may be related to onset of laying in the hens in the current study. In avian species, stress is strongly linked to delay in lay onset [reviewed in (55)]. Direct manipulation of the HPA-axis also impacts lay onset: oral administration of CORT for three seven-day periods in drinking water before onset of laying, delays lay onset in layer hens (56). The HPA-axis, and specifically cortisol, has also been linked to the onset of sexual maturity in other species. In humans, an increase in urinary cortisol is seen in perimenarche that is correlated to both gynecological and chorological age (57). Increased salivary cortisol response to the Leiden Public Speaking Task as a stressor correlated with self-reported pubertal development in humans (58). In rats, an increased cortisol response to an acute stressor is seen in a series of studies examining pre-pubertal rats compared to adult rats [reviewed in (59)]. In all, an increase in cortisol or CORT around the timepoint of sexual maturity is frequently seen in various species, and may explain the age-dependent feather corticosterone increase in the present study.

Conclusions

Age was found to affect the expression of AVT in the hypothalamus of chickens, and an age-dependent increase in CORT was seen in feathers of the hens. This information aids in further understanding of the development of chickens around puberty. The use of brooders tended to affect AVT expression in some areas of the hypothalamus. The lack of effect of dark brooders on CORT in feathers is at the least an indication that the use of dark brooders is not stressful; given previously shown positive benefits of dark brooders, e.g., prevention of injurious pecking, reduced fearfulness and lower mortality, this pleads for the use of dark brooders in on-farm situations.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

All procedures involving animals were in accordance with the Danish Ministry of Justice Law No. 382 (10 June 1987) and Acts 333 (19 May 1990), 726 (9 September 1993) and 1016 (12 December 2001). This study is part of a larger project that also evaluated the effects of brooders on welfare, including behavior, fear, injurious pecking damage, mortality and production parameters (7, 13).

Author Contributions

AR conceived and designed the study with RN. RN and AR drafted the manuscript with help from EZ. EZ and AD performed laboratory work, analysis of stained slides, and statistical analysis.

Funding

This work was financed by the Green Development and Demonstration Programme (GUDP), established under the Ministry of Food, Agriculture, and Fisheries, Denmark and by Utrecht University, the Netherlands.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Prof. Dr. vét. Sabine Blähser for providing the AVT-specific antiserum and to J.G. Lozeman-van't Klooster for biotechnical assistance. We would also like to thank MHJ Agroteknik A/S for the development of the water-heated brooders and the technical support during the installation.

References

1. Myhre K. Behavioral temperature regulation in neonate chick of bantam hen (Gallus-domesticus). Poult Sci. (1978) 57:1369–75. doi: 10.3382/ps.0571369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Wekstein DR, Zolman JF. Ontogeny of heat production in chicks. Fed. proc. (1969) 28:1023–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

3. Thiele H-H. Management recommendations for rearing pullets for alternative housing systems. Lohmann Inf. (2007) 42:14–24.

Google Scholar

4. Riber AB, Nielsen BL, Ritz C, Forkman B. Diurnal activity cycles and synchrony in layer hen chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2007) 108:276–87. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2007.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Riber AB, Wichman A, Braastad BO, Forkman B. Effects of broody hens on perch use, ground pecking, feather pecking and cannibalism in domestic fowl (Gallus gallus domesticus). Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2007) 106:39–51. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2006.07.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Gilani A-M, Knowles TG, Nicol CJ. The effect of dark brooders on feather pecking on commercial farms. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2012) 142:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Riber AB, Guzman DA. Effects of dark brooders on behavior and fearfulness in layers. Animals. (2016) 6:3. doi: 10.3390/ani6010003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Roden C, Wechsler B. A comparison of the behaviour of domestic chicks reared with or without a hen in enriched pens. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (1998) 55:317–26. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(97)00073-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Barnett JL, Hemsworth PH, Cronin GM, Newman EA, McCallum TH, Chilton D. Effects of pen size, partial stalls and method of feeding on welfare-related behavioral and physiological-responses of group-housed pigs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (1992) 34:207–20. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(05)80116-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Rodenburg TB, van Krimpen MM, de Jong IC, de Haas EN, Kops MS, Riedstra BJ, et al. The prevention and control of feather pecking in laying hens: identifying the underlying principles. World's Poult Sci J. (2013) 69:361–74. doi: 10.1017/S0043933913000354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Johnsen PF, Kristensen HH. Effect of brooder quality on the early development of feather pecking behaviour in domestic chicks. In: Oester H, Wyss C, editors. Proceedings of the 6th European Symposium on Poultry Welfare. Zollikofen (2001). p. 209–12.

Google Scholar

12. Jensen AB, Palme R, Forkman B. Effect of brooders on feather pecking and cannibalism in domestic fowl (Gallus gallus domesticus). Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2006) 99:287–300. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.10.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Riber AB, Guzman DA Effects of different types of dark brooders on injurious pecking damage and production-related traits at rear and lay in layers. Poult Sci. (2017). 96:3529–38. doi: 10.3382/ps/pex177

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Menard JL, Champagne DL, Meaney MJP. Variations of maternal care differentially influence 'fear' reactivity and regional patterns of cFos immunoreactivity in response to the shock-probe burying test. Neuroscience. (2004) 129:297–308. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Barha CK, Pawluski JL, Galea LAM. Maternal care affects male and female offspring working memory and stress reactivity. Physiol Behav. (2007) 92:939–50. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Nishi M, Horii-Hayashi N, Sasagawa T, Matsunaga W. Effects of early life stress on brain activity: Implications from maternal separation model in rodents. Gen Comp Endocrinol. (2013) 181:306–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2012.09.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Francis DD, Meaney MJ. Maternal care and the development of stress responses. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (1999) 9:128–34. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)80016-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Schoech SJ, Rensel MA, Wilcoxen TE. Here today, not gone tomorrow: long-term effects of corticosterone. J Ornithol. (2012) 153:217–26. doi: 10.1007/s10336-012-0820-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Field SE, Rickard NS, Toukhsati SR, Gibbs ME. Maternal hen calls modulate memory formation in the day-old chick: the role of noradretialine. Neurobiol Learn Mem. (2007) 88:321–30. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Madec I, Gabarrou J-F, Pageat P. Influence of a maternal odorant on copying strategies in chicks facing isolation and novelty during a standardized test. Neuroendocrinol Lett. (2008) 29:507–11.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

21. de Margerie E, Peris A, Pittet F, Houdelier C, Lumineau S, Richard-Yris MA. Effect of mothering on the spatial exploratory behavior of quail chicks. Dev Psychobiol. (2013) 55:256–64. doi: 10.1002/dev.21019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Houdelier C, Lumineau S, Bertin A, Guibert F, De Margerie E, Augery M, et al. Development of fearfulness in birds: genetic factors modulate non-genetic maternal influences. PLoS ONE. (2011) 6:e14604. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Edgar JL, Paul ES, Nicol CJ. Protective mother hens: cognitive influences on the avian maternal response. Anim Behav. (2013) 86:223–9. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Edgar JL, Held S, Paul ES, Pettersson I, I'Anson Price R, Nicol CJ. Social buffering in a bird. Anim Behav. (2015) 105:11–19. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Knop J, Joels M, van der Veen R. The added value of rodent models in studying parental influence on offspring development: opportunities, limitations and future perspectives. Curr Opin Psychol. (2017) 15:174–81. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Hewlett SE, Zeinstra EC, van Eerdenburg FJCM, Rodenburg TB, van Kooten PJS, van der Staay FJ, et al. Hypothalamic vasotocin and tyrosine hydroxylase levels following maternal care and selection for low mortality in laying hens. BMC Vet Res. (2014) 10:167. doi: 10.1186/1746-6148-10-167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Nordquist RE, Zeinstra EC, Rodenburg TB, van der Staay FJ. Effects of maternal care and selection for low mortality on tyrosine hydroxylase concentrations and cell soma size in hippocampus and nidopallium caudolaterale in adult laying hen. J Anim Sci. (2013) 91:137–46. doi: 10.2527/jas.2012-5227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kang SW, Kuenzel WJ. Regulation of gene expression of vasotocin and corticotropin-releasing hormone receptors in the avian anterior pituitary by corticosterone. Gen Comp Endocrinol. (2014) 204:25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2014.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Puelles L, Martinez-de-la-Torre M, Paxinos G, Watson C, Martínez S. The Chicken Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. An Atlas Featuring Neuromeric Subdivisions and Mammalian Homologies. Elsevier Inc. (2007).

Google Scholar

30. Bortolotti GR, Marchant T, Blas J, Cabezas S. Tracking stress: localisation, deposition and stability of corticosterone in feathers. J Exp Biol. (2009) 212:1477–82. doi: 10.1242/jeb.022152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. von Eugen K, Nordquist RE, Zeinstra E, van der Staay FJ. Stocking density affects stress and anxious behavior in the laying hen chick during rearing. Animals. (2019) 9:53. doi: 10.3390/ani9020053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Zeinstra EC, Oeben L, van der Staay FJ, Nordquist RE. Methoden voor de bepaling van corticosteron in veren en cortisol in haren als mogelijke lange-termijn indicator voor dierenwelzijn. Biotechniek. (2015) 54:17–22.

Google Scholar

33. Viglietti-Panzica C. Immunohistochemical study of the distribution of vasotocin reacting neurons in avian diencephalon. J Hirnforsch. (1986) 27:559–66.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

34. Tennyson VM, Hou-Yu A, Nilaver G, Zimmerman EA. Immunocytochemical studies of vasotocin and mesotocin in the hypothalamo-hypophysial system of the chicken. Cell Tissue Res. (1985) 239:279–91. doi: 10.1007/BF00218005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Bons N. The topography of mesotocin and vasotocin systems in the brain of the domestic mallard and Japanese quail: immunocytochemical identification. Cell Tissue Res. (1980) 213:37–51. doi: 10.1007/BF00236919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Panzica GC, Fiori MG, Viglietti-Panzica C. Vasotocin fibers in the mesencephalon and pons of the domestic fowl. An immunohistochemical study. Neurosci Lett. (1986) 68:155–9. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(86)90133-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Goossens N, Blahser S, Oksche A, Vandesande F, Dierickx K. Immunocytochemical investigation of the hypothalamo-neurohypophysial system in birds. Cell Tissue Res. (1977) 184:1–13. doi: 10.1007/BF00220523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Panzica GC, Plumari L, Garcia-Ojeda E, Deviche P. Central vasotocin-immunoreactive system in a male passerine bird (Junco hyemalis). J Comp Neurol. (1999) 409:105–17. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19990621)409:1<105::AID-CNE8>3.0.CO;2-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Schöne C, Burdakov D. Glutamate and GABA as rapid effectors of hypothalamic “peptidergic” neurons. (2012) 6:81. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Chaturvedi CM, Newton BW, Cornett LE, Koike TI. An in situ hybridization and immunohistochemical study of vasotocin neurons in the hypothalamus of water-deprived chickens. Peptides. (1994) 15:1179–87. doi: 10.1016/0196-9781(94)90140-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Chaturvedi CM, Cornett LE, Koike TI. Arginine vasotocin gene expression in hypothalamic neurons is up-regulated in chickens drinking hypertonic saline: an in situ hybridization study. Peptides. (1997) 18:1383–8. doi: 10.1016/S0196-9781(97)00190-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Jaccoby S, Singh AB, Cornett LE, Koike TI. Arginine vasotocin gene expression and secretion during osmotic stimulation and hemorrhagic hypotension in hens. Gen Comp Endocrinol. (1997) 106:327–37. doi: 10.1006/gcen.1997.6872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Sharp PJ, Li Q, Talbot RT, Barker P, Huskisson N, Lea RW. Identification of hypothalamic nuclei involved in osmoregulation using fos immunocytochemistry in the domestic hen (Gallus domesticus), Ring dove (Streptopelia risoria), Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) and Zebra finch (Taenopygia guttata). Cell Tissue Res. (1995) 282:351–61. doi: 10.1007/BF00319125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Oksche A, Wilson WO, Farner DS. The hypothalamic neurosecretory system of Coturnix coturnix japonica. Z Zellforch Microsk Anat Histochem. (1963) 61:688–709. doi: 10.1007/BF00342619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Konishi T, Kato M. Light-induced rhythmic changes in the hypothalamic neurosecretory activity in Japanese quail, Coturnix coturnix japonica. Endocrinol Jpn. (1967) 14:239–45. doi: 10.1507/endocrj1954.14.239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Ronnekleiv OK, Kelly MJ. Plasma prolactin and luteinizing hormone profiles during the estrous cycle of the female rat: effects of surgically induced persistent estrus. Neuroendocrinology. (1988) 47:133–41. doi: 10.1159/000124903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Ma YJ, Kelly MJ, Ronnekleiv OK. Pro-gonadotropin-releasing hormone (ProGnRH) and GnRH content in the preoptic area and the basal hypothalamus of anterior medial preoptic nucleus/suprachiasmatic nucleus-lesioned persistent estrous rats. Endocrinology. (1990) 127:2654–64. doi: 10.1210/endo-127-6-2654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Featherstone RE, Fleming AS, Ivy GO. Plasticity in the maternal circuit: effects of experience and partum condition on brain astroctye number in female rats. Behav Neurosci. (2000) 114:158–72. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.114.1.158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Palm IF, van der Beek EM, Wiegant VM, Buijs RM, Kalsbeek A. The stimulatory effect of vasopressin on the luteinizing hormone surge in ovariectomized, estradiol-treated rats is time-dependent. Brain Res. (2001) 901:109–16. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02309-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Spool JA, Jay MD, Riters LV. Nest box exploration may stimulate breeding physiology and alter mRNA expression in the medial preoptic area of female European starlings. J Exp Biol. (2018) 221: jeb174441. doi: 10.1242/jeb.174441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Saito N, Kinzler S, Koike TI. Arginine vasotocin and mesotocin levels in theca and granulosa layers of the ovary during the oviposition cycle in hens (Gallus domesticus). Gen Comp Endocrinol. (1990) 79:54–63. doi: 10.1016/0016-6480(90)90087-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Sasaki T, Shimada K, Saito N. Changes of AVT levels in plasma, neurohypophysis and hypothalamus in relation to oviposition in the laying hen. Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol. (1998) 121:149–53. doi: 10.1016/S1095-6433(98)10116-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Seth R, Xu YX, Grossmann R, Chaturvedi CM. Changes in expression of AVT and AVT receptor (VT1) gene in hypothalamus and shell gland in relation to egg laying in white leghorn hen. Gen Comp Endocrinol. (2004) 137:177–86. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2004.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Srivastava R, Cornett LE, Chaturvedi CM. Age-dependent expression of AVT and its oxytocic-like receptor VT3 in the shell gland of Japanese quail, Coturnix coturnix japonica. Gen Comp Endocrinol. (2010) 165:47–52. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2009.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Wang XJ, Liu L, Zhao JP, Jiao HC, Lin H. Stress impairs the reproduction of laying hens: an involvement of energy. World's Poult Sci J. (2017) 73:845–56. doi: 10.1017/S0043933917000794

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Shini S, Shini A, Huff GR. Effects of chronic and repeated corticosterone administration in rearing chickens on physiology, the onset of lay and egg production of hens. Physiol Behav. (2009) 98:73–7. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Legro RS, Lin HM, Lloyd T, Demers LM. Urinary free cortisol increases in adolescent caucasian females during perimenarche. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. (2003) 88:215–9. doi: 10.1210/jc.2002-020256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Van den Bos E, de Rooij M, Miers AC, Bokhorst CL, Westenberg PM. Adolescents' increasing stress response to social evaluation: pubertal effects on cortisol and alpha-amylase during public speaking. Child Dev. (2014) 85:220–236. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Klein ZA, Romeo RD. Changes in hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal stress responsiveness before and after puberty in rats. Horm Behav. (2013) 64:357–63. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dark brooder, feather corticosterone, hypothalamic vasotocin, avian, vasopressin, cortisol

Citation: Nordquist RE, Zeinstra EC, Dougherty A and Riber AB (2020) Effects of Dark Brooder Rearing and Age on Hypothalamic Vasotocin and Feather Corticosterone Levels in Laying Hens. Front. Vet. Sci. 7:19. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2020.00019

Received: 02 May 2019; Accepted: 10 January 2020;
Published: 30 January 2020.

Edited by:

Sabine G. Gebhardt-Henrich, University of Bern, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Alexandra Harlander, University of Guelph, Canada
Jean-Loup Rault, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Austria

Copyright © 2020 Nordquist, Zeinstra, Dougherty and Riber. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rebecca E. Nordquist, r.e.nordquist1@uu.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.