- Adaptive Rationality Group, Centre of Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, Penryn, United Kingdom
Emergent evidence of aspects of sociality, such as social structure and social learning, across many vertebrate taxa, warrant more detailed consideration of their influence on welfare outcomes for wildlife. Sociality can be dynamic across organismal development, it can: provide protection through safety in numbers; may influence breeding outcomes via mate choice and alloparental care; can influence foraging success through transmission of social information and co-operation; and it can provide opportunities for the spread of novel behavior. Social learning itself provides an important mechanism for resilience in changing environments, but also has the potential to increase vulnerability or facilitate the spread of maladaptive behaviors. The welfare consequences of vertebrates living in social groups are explored using Wilson's 10 qualities of sociality as a framework, and the implications of human activities are discussed. Focus to date has been on the importance of social networks for the welfare of farmed or captive animals. Here I consider the importance of social networks and sociality more generally for the welfare of wildlife and explore Mellor's five domain model for animal welfare within the context of wildlife sociality.
Introduction
Sociality is a measure of the degree to which animals interact or form long-term or transient associations and is prevalent across a broad range of vertebrate and invertebrate taxa. Animal welfare, on the other hand, is a multidimensional field, measured through a range of criteria from health and comfort, to the ability to express natural behaviors (1). While some natural behaviors may be considered, many aspects of sociality are often overlooked in animal welfare assessment and this is particularly the case for wildlife, where social settings may be complex.
Wild animal conservation and individual and group welfare are deeply intertwined (2). While the social environment is undoubtedly of significance to both the conservation and welfare of many species, quantifying the welfare implications of aspects of sociality can be challenging in the wild. The social environment has been described as being comprised of non-random and heterogeneous social interactions (3, 4). The social environment of vertebrate taxa is highly diverse, ranging from: the complex third order alliances of male bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.) (5); to matriarchal African elephant societies (Loxodonta sp.) (6); to the hierarchical group dynamics of flocking birds (7). For long-lived, wide ranging species, such as blue whales (Balaenoptera musculus), there are also important spatio-temporal considerations for interpretation of behavior and associations (8).
Aspects of sociality, such as social structure and social transmission of information, across many vertebrate taxa, warrant a more detailed exploration of their influence on individual and group welfare outcomes for wildlife. Quantifying the welfare of wild animals presents a number of unique challenges. Wildlife welfare is often framed in terms of physiology or individual behavior. Nevertheless, better understanding of the processes of social behavior may provide important insights for the welfare of wildlife. While the focus of this review is vertebrates, there are some notable invertebrate taxa, such as cephalopods (9), which might also benefit from similar consideration.
Wilson's 10 qualities of sociality (10) have previously been suggested as a framework for evaluating the importance of sociality for individual and group welfare for cetaceans (whales and dolphins) (11). Here I attempt to expand these ideas to examine how sociality may be important for the welfare of a range of vertebrate taxa: exploring the risks and benefits of sociality to animal welfare; investigating the relationship between sociality and the impacts of human-induced rapid environmental change (HIREC) (12) on sociality and animal welfare. I then examine this new perspective within the context of the five domains model (13) of animal welfare.
Sociality and Living in Groups
There are a number of potential ecological and animal welfare benefits from living in groups which include: predator defense, cooperative foraging, mating opportunities, and reduced vulnerability to infanticide (14). However, there are also risks and costs associated with group living, such as: increased risk of spread of disease, or increased conspicuousness to predators and competition for resources (3). Living in groups can also influence individual reproductive fitness, gene flow and spatial distribution (10, 15) and it has been argued that the buffering effects of social support may be relevant to farm animal welfare (16). There are likely many facets of sociality which have implications for both individual and group welfare in wildlife.
E.O Wilson listed 10 “qualities” of sociality (10) which are used to understand and classify conspecific groups. These characteristics of animal societies have also been used to provide a framework for evaluating the importance of sociality for individual and group welfare in cetaceans (whales and dolphins) (11). Here this framework is used to examine Wilson's 10 qualities of sociality within the context of wildlife welfare, more generally (Table 1).
Table 1. Wilson's 10 “qualities” of sociality (10) and welfare considerations for wild animals [adapted from Brakes (11)].
Risks and Benefits of Sociality
Within the vertebrate taxa there are a variety of social structures, types of association between individuals, and transmission pathways for social information. As a result the relationship between sociality and welfare across vertebrate taxa is multifaceted (see Table 2). Undoubtedly, sociality provides benefit to individual welfare, for example in predator defense, alloparental care, or by increasing foraging success through the transmission of information about food sources or other types of public information, or through co-operative foraging. But sociality also has associated risks, such as the increased probability of the spread of disease, or conspicuousness to predators. These risks and benefits can change according to the environment and may also be dynamic across the developmental stages of an organism.
Table 2. Summary of the importance of social living for wild animal welfare [after Brakes (11)].
While the risk of spread of disease and parasitic burdens associated with living in social groups has been extensively studied, sociality has, nevertheless, evolved independently in many taxa, demonstrating that these risks are overall offset by the benefits of sociality.
Social Support Hypotheses
The potential benefits of group living are varied (Table 1), but social support may specifically provide beneficial effects to the recipient, irrespective of whether or not the recipient is being challenged; or social partners may modulate or downregulate the impact of stressors on the recipient's homeostasis [a process known as stress buffering (30)]. Stress buffering has been investigated for farm animal welfare and it has been suggested that farmers could exploit animals' natural ability to benefit from their social group to obtain better welfare outcomes (16). Arguably, for some highly social species of wildlife it may also be the case that better management can be achieved through strategic use of elements of animal sociality to enhance welfare outcomes.
Social Learning, Animal Culture, and Welfare
Social learning—“learning that is influenced by observation of, or interactions with, another animal (typically a conspecific) or its products” (31)—has been observed across a broad range of vertebrate taxa (32) and can be important for conservation efforts (21, 33, 34). Social learning can act more rapidly than the intergenerational process of Darwinian selection (21) and some facets of sociality may also hold important insights for wild animal welfare. For example, social learning can provide mechanisms for resilience to ecological or anthropogenic stressors, such as reduced prey abundance [for example through diversification of foraging strategies (22, 35, 36)]. In contrast, it can also result in the transmission of socially learnt conservative foraging specializations, such as those found in killer whales (Orcinus orca), resulting in the species being less, rather than more, resilient to fluctuations in prey abundance (21).
Social learning also has the potential to facilitate the spread of maladaptive behaviors, which can evolve much more rapidly than genetic selection can counter them (21, 37). This is particularly relevant to animal welfare in human-wildlife conflict situations, both in relation to the spread of behaviors that lead to human-wildlife conflict (38) and in relation to facilitating resolution of these conflicts (39, 40). In all cases, a better understanding of the processes of social learning may help mitigation efforts and improve welfare outcomes for wildlife.
Social learning can also result in more persistent behavioral traits developing into “animal cultures.” Whitehead and Rendell (41) define culture as: “information or behaviors—shared within a community—which is acquired from conspecifics through some form of social learning.” But the interplay between animal culture and individual or group welfare within wild vertebrates may be complex. Cultural behavior may shape both social relationships and social structure (42), or even act as a marker of group identity (41, 43). Although social network analyses have been used to examine the welfare implications of disrupting these social systems (44) (see section Tools for Assessing Sociality and Wildlife Welfare), most studies have focused on farm or captive animals and the implications of these aspects of sociality for wild animal welfare warrant further exploration.
Human Impacts on Sociality and Wildlife Welfare
The impact of human activities on wildlife and habitat is ubiquitous across virtually all ecosystems and HIREC is now a widely acknowledged phenomenon (12). From deforestation, to climate change and ocean acidification, wildlife around the globe is challenged by rapid environmental change, but aspects of sociality may provide opportunities for resilience to this rapid change, or increase vulnerabilities (45). It is timely to examine how sociality may influence wild animal welfare and responses to HIREC.
HIREC has the potential to influence all behavioral domains associated with social behavior, from breeding to communication, foraging and migration. A method to classify this vast array of threats from an individual welfare perspective is to consider acute and chronic implications for sociality and welfare (Table 3).
Cognition, Sociality, and Wildlife Welfare
Understanding and predicting how individuals behave in response to HIREC may be important for evaluating aspects of sociality relevant to wildlife welfare. For example, understanding how individual innovations arise may be relevant for predicting—and setting up optimal conditions for—the spread of novel behavior through social learning. Wildlife behavioral responses are governed by cognitive processes ranging from perceptual processes to learned behavior. Understanding these cognitive processes and the effect of HIREC associated stress, may also assist in reducing human impacts on wildlife (46). It has been argued, using the same rationale, that if cognition underlies a behavior that is relevant to welfare, then understanding these cognitive processes may help in achieving better welfare outcomes for species such as cetaceans (11) and this is arguably also the case for other wildlife.
Greggor et al. (46) argue that “cognitive theory can thus help predict how best to manipulate and exploit attentional biases, innate responses, and learning tendencies to enhance conservation efforts.” For example, they argue that mitigation methods will only be effective if they are reliably perceived by the target species (e.g., preventing birds colliding with human-made structures) and that this perception is rapidly learned. The same argument can been made for a range of wildlife mitigation activities, including: acoustic “pingers” on fishing nets designed to prevent dolphins from becoming entangled and boundaries designed to prevent crop raiding. The success of some mitigation methods may also be contingent on the social transmission of information, which in turn is contingent on other aspects of sociality such as social structure.
Tools for Assessing Sociality and Wildlife Welfare
Asher et al. (47) argued for expanding the scope and development of new quantitative methods for the analysis of various aspects of behavioral organization as indicators for animal welfare, including: fractal analysis, temporal methods, social network analysis (SNA), and agent-based modeling and simulation. Social network analysis has now begun to emerge as a tool for assessing animal welfare. Kleinhappel et al. (44) argue that SNA is as yet, underused in the field of animal welfare and suggest a number of opportunities for using these methods, particularly in relation to the welfare of captive animals. However, SNA also has important application for assessing the welfare of wild populations.
SNA can be used to detect and describe the patterns and quality of interactions among individuals, with implications for physical health (e.g., disease transmission), psychological health (e.g., stress and social buffering), and social well-being (e.g., group stability) (48). These types of analyses have been used to investigate animal welfare in captive settings [e.g., for captive elephants (49) and primates (47)] and also have value in wild settings. For example, Godfrey et al. (50), demonstrated the link between parasite infection patterns and the connectivity of individual lizards within a population. It has further been postulated that SNA could be used to track behavioral changes and predict and prevent disease outbreak within groups (44).
Extending the Five Domains Model to Incorporate Sociality in Wildlife
There are several models that might be applicable for expanding our consideration of wild animal welfare in relation to sociality. Hinde's framework for classifying aspects of sociality (51) might for example provide a basis for attempting to maintain optimal conditions of sociality from a welfare perspective within wild populations. However, trying to ensure optimality in the wild is both challenging to define and difficult in practice.
A more realistic approach may be to use the five domains model of animal welfare (13). While generally applied to domestic and captive animals, where some degree of compromise in welfare circumstances is implicit, the five domains model may be a more practical framework for managing welfare issues arising from human impacts on wild animal sociality. It is suggested here that the five domains model may be useful to avoid directly aversive situations resulting from poor management of the sociality of wildlife, for example through the removal of repositories of social knowledge (25). Instead, it may be better to try to manage wild environments so that socially complex systems can thrive and avoid situations that will hinder their development. The five domains model itself resulted from recognition of the need to provide positive experiences for animals (beyond the five freedoms framework) (52) and this approach may be useful for exploring how we can monitor and potentially assist social systems to flourish in the wild.
The objective of the five domains model is to “draw attention to areas that are relevant to both animal welfare assessment and management” and focus on the “presence or absence of various internal physical/functional states and external circumstances that give rise to welfare-relevant negative and/or positive mental experiences, i.e., affects” (13). In formulating the five domains model, Mellor “carefully and cautiously” considered the affects of the first four domains on nutrition, environment, health and behavior, on the fifth domain of mental state.
Considering some of the issues raised in this review, here I have attempted to explore some of the possible implications of sociality for wildlife welfare within the context of the five domains model (Table 4). This list is not intended to be exhaustive, but instead is a first exploration.
Table 4. Potential interface between sociality and wild animal welfare as it relates to the five domains model (13) of animal welfare.
Play and the Five Domains Model
The stated objective of the five domains model is to provide opportunities for positive welfare (13) and it follows that one such opportunity might be granted through play in wildlife. Play is variable across species, has a variety of definitions (54, 55) and its proximate mechanisms and ultimate functions are not well understood. Play can indicate good welfare conditions, but can also be an indication of stressful situations, for example resulting in reduced parental care (56). Play has a variety of roles and it has been argued that play is usually the first behavior to disappear when welfare is compromised: when animals are stressed, anxious, hungry or unwell (57, 58). It has been suggested that play may be a reliable indicator of psychological and physical well-being (56). However, using play as a definitive indicator for optimal or good welfare can be confounded by several factors including: the heterogeneity of play; the fact that play can be a coping strategy; factors with no obvious relation to animal welfare can influence intrinsic playfulness within individuals; and the fact there are circumstances in which negative welfare can actually result in more play. Thus, play cannot definitely be considered an indicator of optimal or good welfare (59). But play can also be dynamic across life stages and arguably may be important for positive mental state in some species (see Table 4). For example, it has been argued that play may be socially contagious and therefore capable of spreading good welfare in groups (56).
Play behavior may also be of value in relation to innovation. In addition to having a potentially positive influence on mental states, there is potentially reciprocity between the social aspects of play, innovation and how innovation may then spread in wild populations. Bateson (57) argues that numerous functions for play have been proposed but they are not mutually exclusive and there are indications that those individuals who play most are most likely to survive and reproduce.
Potentially, the reciprocity between play and vital rates may manifest through the social transmission of innovative behaviors. Although fitness outcomes are not always synonymous with welfare outcomes, associations and bonds forged through play may influence the spread of information across a network and the trial and error aspects of play may help individuals accumulate latent information, which may facilitate innovation as their environment changes. Thus, it can be argued that play may be an important part of the behavioral repertoire of some wildlife for maintaining resilience.
Conclusion
The interplay between sociality and the welfare of wildlife is multifaceted. The widespread evolution of sociality, social learning, and in some instances even animal culture, among vertebrate taxa is evidence that overall the benefits of social living out-weight the costs. Nevertheless, the individual and group welfare issues associated with the processes of social living are undoubtedly important for the consideration of wild animal welfare.
Previous explorations of the interface between sociality and animal welfare focus predominantly on domestic or captive animals: an entirely different paradigm from evaluating the implications of sociality on free ranging vertebrate organisms. There are many avenues for further research in this field (16), including for example, capitalizing on the mechanisms of social learning for the restoration and rehabilitation of wildlife populations (53, 60). But perhaps the most pressing are instances where human activities significantly disrupt social systems and can influence individual and group welfare (24, 25).
The principle objective of the five domains model is to move beyond an animal welfare focus of survival, toward individuals thriving. In order to meet the objective of thriving wild vertebrate populations, it is essential to incorporate aspects of sociality into their welfare assessments.
Author Contributions
The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.
Funding
Funding for open access of this article was kindly supplied by the Institutional APC Fund, University of Exeter.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
I would like to thank Sasha Dall for commenting on an earlier version of this text and Andy Butterworth, Mark Simmonds, and Clive Phillips for valuable conversations which have helped shape some of the thoughts presented here.
References
1. Botreau R, Veissier I, Butterworth A, Bracke MBM, Keeling LJ. Definition of criteria for overall assessment of animal welfare. Anim Welf. (2007) 16:225–8. doi: 10.1016/S0008-6215(00)00097-5
2. Paquet PCD, Darimont CT. Wildlife conservation and animal welfare: two sides of the same coin? Anim Welf. (2010) 19:177–90.
4. Croft DP, James R, Krause DJ. Exploring Animal Social Networks. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press (2008).
5. Connor RC, Krützen M. Male dolphin alliances in Shark Bay : changing perspectives in a 30-year study. Anim Behav. (2015) 2015:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.02.019
6. McComb K, Shannon G, Durant SM, Sayialel K, Slotow R, Poole J, et al. Leadership in elephants: the adaptive value of age. Proc Biol Sci. (2011) 278:3270–6. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0168
7. Nagy M, Ákos Z, Biro D, Vicsek T. Hierarchical group dynamics in pigeon flocks. Nature. (2010) 464:890–3. doi: 10.1038/nature08891
8. Lomac-Macnair K, Smultea MA. Blue Whale (Balaenoptera musculus) Behavior and Group Dynamics as Observed from an Aircraft off Southern California. Anim Behav Cogn. (2016) 3:1–21. doi: 10.12966/abc.02.01.2016
9. Fiorito G, Scotto P. Observational learning in Octopus vulgaris. Science. (1992) 1992:545. doi: 10.1126/science.256.5056.545
10. Wilson EO. Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Belknap Press (1975). Available online at: https://books.google.com/books?id=v7lV9tz8fXAC&pgis=1
11. Brakes P. Social change in cetacean populations resulting from human influences. In: Butterworth A, editor. Marine Mammal Welfare: Human Induced Change in the Marine Environment and its Impacts on Marine Mammal Welfare (Cham: Springer Nature) (2017). p. 147–60.
12. Sih A, Ferrari MCO, Harris DJ. Evolution and behavioural responses to human-induced rapid environmental change. Evol Appl. (2011) 4:367–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1752-4571.2010.00166.x
13. Mellor DJ. Operational details of the five domains model and its key applications to the assessment and management of animal welfare. Animals. (2017) 7:60. doi: 10.3390/ani7080060
14. Silk JB. The adaptive value of sociality in mammalian groups. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (2007) 362:539–59. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1994
15. Dall SRX, Giraldeau LA, Olsson O, McNamara JM, Stephens DW. Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends Ecol Evol. (2005) 20:187–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.010
16. Rault JL. Friends with benefits: social support and its relevance for farm animal welfare. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2012) 136:1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.10.002
17. Johnstone R, Cant M. The evolution of menopause in cetaceans and humans: the role of demography. Proc Biol Sci. (2010) 277:3765–71. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0988
18. Whitehead H. Analyzing Animal Societies: Quantitative Methods for Vertebrate Social Analysis. University of Chicago Press (2008). Available online at: https://books.google.com/books?id=zD1gwUbW57gC&pgis=1 (Accessed September 21, 2015)
19. Farine DR, Aplin LM, Sheldon BC, Hoppitt W. Interspecific social networks promote information transmission in wild songbirds. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. (2015) 282:2804. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.2804
20. Gregg JD, Project DC, Dudzinski KM, Smith H V. Do dolphins eavesdrop on the echolocation signals of conspecifics? Int J Comp Psychol. (2007) 20:65–88.
21. Whitehead H. Conserving and managing animals that learn socially and share cultures. Learn Behav. (2010) 38:329–36. doi: 10.3758/LB.38.3.329
22. Ansmann IC, Parra GJ, Chilvers BL, Lanyon JM. Dolphins restructure social system after reduction of commercial fisheries. Anim Behav. (2012) 84:575–81. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.06.009
23. Brent LJN, Franks DW, Cant MA, Croft DP, Brent LJN, Franks DW, et al. Ecological knowledge, leadership, and the evolution of menopause in killer whales. Curr Biol. (2015) 25:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.037
24. Williams R, Lusseau D. A killer whale social network is vulnerable to targeted removals. Biol Lett. (2006) 2:497–500. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2006.0510
25. McComb K, Moss C, Durant SM, Baker L, Sayialel S. Matriarchs as repositories of social knowledge in African elephants. Science. (2001) 292:491–4. doi: 10.1126/science.1057895
26. Tomkins JL, Andrews S. Fluctuating asymmetry. Encycl Life Sci. (2001) 2001:1–5. doi: 10.1055/s-0031-1275301
27. Swaddle JP. Fluctuating asymmetry, animal behavior, and evolution. Adv Study Behav. (2003) 32:169–205. doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(03)01004-0
28. CMS. Report of the CMS Scientific Council Workshop on the Conservation Implications of Cetacean Culture. (2014). Available online at: http://www.cms.int/sites/default/files/document/Inf_10_14_ScC_WG_Rpt_on_Cetacean_Culture_Eonly.pdf
29. McNamara JM, Dall SRX. Information is a fitness enhancing resource. Oikos. (2010) 119:231–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.17509.x
30. Cohen S, Wills TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol Bull. (1985) 98:310–57. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310
31. Heyes CM. Social learning in animals: categories and mechanisms. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. (1994) 69:207–31. Available online at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8054445 (Accessed September 21, 2015)
32. Whiten A. A second inheritance system: the extension of biology through culture. Interface Focus. (2017) 7:20160142. doi: 10.1098/rsfs.2016.0142
33. Laiolo P, Jovani R. The emergence of animal culture conservation. Trends Ecol Evol. (2007) 22:5. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2006.10.007
34. Snijders L, Blumstein DT, Stanley CR, Franks DW. Animal social network theory can help wildlife conservation. Trends Ecol Evol. (2017) 32:567–77. doi: 10.1016/J.TREE.2017.05.005
35. Krützen M, Mann J, Heithaus MR, Connor RC, Bejder L, Sherwin WB. Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2005) 102:8939–43. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500232102
36. Tinker MT, Bentall G, Estes JA. Food limitation leads to behavioral diversification and dietary specialization in sea otters. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2008) 105:560–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0709263105
37. Richerson PJ, Boyd R. Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press (2005). doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9744.2009.01005.x
38. Thode AM, Wild L, Mathias D, Straley J, Lunsford C. A comparison of acoustic and visual metrics of sperm whale longline depredation. J Acoust Soc Am. (2014) 135:3086–100. doi: 10.1121/1.4869853
39. Griffin AS. Social learning about predators: a review and prospectus. Anim Learn Behav. (2004) 32:131–40. doi: 10.3758/BF03196014
40. Morehouse AT, Graves TA, Mikle N, Boyce MS. Nature vs. nurture: evidence for social learning of conflict behaviour in grizzly bears. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0165425. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165425
41. Whitehead H, Rendell L. The Cultural Lives of Whales and Dolphins. University of Chicago Press (2015). Available online at: https://books.google.com/books?id=GeBXBQAAQBAJ&pgis=1 (Accessed September 21, 2015)
42. Cantor M, Shoemaker LG, Cabral RB, Flores CO, Varga M, Whitehead H. Multilevel animal societies can emerge from cultural transmission. Nat Commun. (2015) 6:8091. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9091
43. van Leeuwen EJC, Cronin KA, Haun DBM. A group-specific arbitrary tradition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn. (2014)1421–5. doi: 10.1007/s10071-014-0766-8
44. Kleinhappel TK, John EA, Pike TW, Wilkinson A, Burman OHP. Animal welfare: a social networks perspective. Sci Prog. (2016) 99:68–82. doi: 10.3184/003685016X14495640902331
45. Convention on the Conservation of Migratory Species of Wild Animals 3 rd Meeting of the Sessional Committee of the CMS Scientific Council (ScC-SC3) Report of the CMS Workshop On Conservation Implications Of Animal Culture Social Complexity. (2018). Available online at: https://www.cms.int/cami/sites/default/files/document/cms_scc-sc3_inf.8_animal-culture-workshop-2018-report_e.pdf (Accessed August 22, 2018)
46. Greggor AL, Clayton NS, Phalan B, Thornton A. Comparative cognition for conservationists. Trends Ecol Evol. (2014) 29:489–95. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2014.06.004
47. Asher L, Collins LM, Ortiz-pelaez A, Drewe JA, Nicol CJ, Pfeiffer DU. Recent advances in the analysis of behavioural organization and interpretation as indicators of animal welfare. J R Soc Interface. (2009) 6:1103–19. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2009.0221
48. Beisner BA, McCowan B. Social networks and animal welfare. In: Krause J, James R, Franks D, and Croft D, editors. Animal Social Networks. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2015). p. 111–22. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199679041.003.0011
49. Coleing A. The application of social network theory to animal behaviour. Biosci Horizons. (2009) 2:32–43. doi: 10.1093/biohorizons/hzp008
50. Godfrey SS, Bull CM, James R, Murray K. Network structure and parasite transmission in a group living lizard, the gidgee skink, Egernia stokesii. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. (2009) 63:1045–56. doi: 10.2307/40295394
52. Mellor DJ, Beausoleil NJ. Extending the “Five Domains” model for animal welfare assessment to incorporate positive welfare states. Anim Welf. (2015) 24:241–53. doi: 10.7120/09627286.24.3.241
53. Mueller T, O'Hara RB, Converse SJ, Urbanek RP, Fagan WF. Social learning of migratory performance. Science. (2013) 341:999–1002. doi: 10.1126/science.1237139
54. Barreto R, Flora A, Oliveira S, André AE, Rossi O, Luana AE, et al. Play behaviour in nonhuman animals and the animal welfare issue. Artic J Ethol. (2010) 28:1–5. doi: 10.1007/s10164-009-0167-7
56. Held SDE, Špinka M. Animal play and animal welfare. Anim Behav. (2011) 81:891–9. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.007
57. Bateson P. Play, playfulness, creativity and innovation. Anim Behav Cogn. (2014) 2:99. doi: 10.12966/abc.05.02.2014
58. Sharpe LL, Clutton-Brock TH, Brotherton PNM, Cameron EZ, Cherry MI. Experimental provisioning increases play in free-ranging meerkats. Anim Behav. (2002) 64:113–21. doi: 10.1006/ANBE.2002.3031
59. Ahloy-Dallaire J, Espinosa J, Mason G. Play and optimal welfare: does play indicate the presence of positive affective states? Behav Process. (2017) 156:3–15. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.11.011
Keywords: sociality, animal welfare, social learning, animal culture, social structure, vertebrates, five domains model, wildlife
Citation: Brakes P (2019) Sociality and Wild Animal Welfare: Future Directions. Front. Vet. Sci. 6:62. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2019.00062
Received: 27 September 2018; Accepted: 12 February 2019;
Published: 19 March 2019.
Edited by:
Chris Walzer, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, AustriaReviewed by:
Michael Toscano, University of Bern, SwitzerlandJudith L. Stella, Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service (USDA), United States
Copyright © 2019 Brakes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Philippa Brakes, p.brakes@exeter.ac.uk