Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 15 August 2024
This article is part of the Research Topic Asymmetry in the Central Nervous System: Functional Implications - Volume II View all 5 articles

Occlusal effects on text reading: an eye-tracker study

  • 1Department of Translational Research and of New Surgical and Medical Technologies, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
  • 2Institute of Management, Scuola Superiore Sant'Anna, Pisa, Italy
  • 3Department of Physics, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
  • 4Department of Surgical, Medical and Molecular Pathology and Critical Care Medicine, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
  • 5Maxillofacial Surgery Unit, Italian Stomatology Institute, Milan, Italy
  • 6Department of Developmental Neuroscience, IRCCS Fondazione Stella Maris, Pisa, Italy

Introduction: Asymmetric electromyographic (EMG) activity during teeth clenching has been linked to cognitive impairment, as evaluated by the Spinnler-Tognoni matrices test, and to asymmetric pupil size (anisocoria). Anisocoria indicates an asymmetric Locus Coeruleus activity, leading to an asymmetric hemispheric excitability worsening cognitive performance. Bite splint wearing corrects EMG asymmetry, reduces anisocoria and improves cognitive performance. This study explores the possible effect of EMG asymmetry on oculomotor behavior during text reading.

Methods: In subjects showing different degrees of EMG asymmetry during clenching, the number and duration of fixation periods during a reading task, performed under two different occlusal conditions were analyzed. The first lecture was executed with a dental impression (imprint) interposed between the dental arches (corrected condition) and the second one with the arches in direct contact (habitual condition), without clenching effort. The imprint reduced the EMG asymmetries during clenching.

Results: In both occlusal conditions, total reading time correlated with duration of fixations, but not with their number. An inverse relation was observed between the number of fixations and their duration across individuals. Fixation frequency and duration were positively and negatively correlated with the amplitude of EMG asymmetry, respectively. Differently, total reading time was not related to the EMG asymmetry. When switching from the corrected to the habitual condition, an increase in the number of fixations and a reduction in their duration was observed, while total reading time could be either increased or decreased. An increased fixation frequency was observed in most of the subjects, while a reduced duration only among individuals with shorter reading times in habitual condition.

Discussion: In the habitual condition, EMG asymmetry influences reading patterns (more saccades/shorter fixations, less saccades/longer fixations) in our sample. The changes in text reading behavior elicited by occlusal correction can be explained by assuming that occlusal disharmony negatively interferes with the reading task by increasing the number of saccades necessary for text scanning. This finding may also indicate an increased difficulty in processing of visual information. The potential involvement of trigeminal pathways in the relation between occlusal factors and oculomotor control is discussed.

1 Introduction

The asymmetry of hemispheric activity may induce a cognitive impairment. Research on brain injuries shows that a unilateral parietal lesion leads to cognitive visuo-spatial impairment (unilateral neglect), which is not observed when both sides of the brain are symmetrically injured (Lomber and Payne, 1996), suggesting that the disability arises by the inter-hemispheric imbalance, rather than the size of the damaged area. In humans, unilateral neglect may be greatly reduced by (1) depressing the activity of the undamaged hemisphere (Koch et al., 2012; Andres et al., 2020) or (2) stimulating the vestibular and neck proprioceptive afferents to the damaged hemisphere (Kerkhoff, 2001). These findings suggest that brain’s capability to represent space depends on maintaining a proper balance of activity between specific brain regions and peripheral afferents. According to De Cicco et al. (2017) trigeminal sensorimotor asymmetries can also impair cognitive functions in subjects with otherwise intact brains. Specifically, an asymmetry in the masseter electromyographic (EMG) activity during clenching was found to negatively affect cognitive visuo-spatial performance, as measured using the Spinnler-Tognoni matrices test (Spinnler and Tognoni, 1987), performed without occlusal effort. Moreover, this sensorimotor trigeminal unbalance diminished the positive effect of chewing on cognitive visuo-spatial performance (Tramonti Fantozzi et al., 2021b). The use of occlusal correction, either through an orthotic device or an implant prosthesis, reduced the trigeminal asymmetry and improved visuo-spatial performance (De Cicco et al., 2014, 2016; Tramonti Fantozzi et al., 2021a,c,d), suggesting a causal relationship between trigeminal asymmetries and cognitive impairment. It is possible that the proprioceptive trigeminal asymmetry introduces an asymmetry within the spatial representation of one’s body. Such an unbalanced representation could disturb cognitive processes. Interestingly, in these studies, the EMG asymmetry observed during clenching was strongly and positively correlated with an asymmetry in pupil size (anisocoria) evaluated without any occlusal effort (De Cicco et al., 2016; Tramonti Fantozzi et al., 2021a,c, 2023). Since pupil size is considered a proxy of the activity of noradrenergic Locus Coeruleus (LC) neurons (Silvetti et al., 2013; Murphy et al., 2014; Hoffing and Seitz, 2015; Kihara et al., 2015; Joshi et al., 2016; Reimer et al., 2016; Einhäuser, 2017; Mathôt, 2018; Joshi and Gold, 2020), the anisocoria could arise from an asymmetric LC discharge. This asymmetry may lead to an asymmetric body representation or else to an imbalance in the hemispheric activity. In both instances, a detrimental effects on cognitive functions would occurr, mirroring the phenomenon described by Lomber and Payne (1996). So, whatever the mechanism could be (altered body representation, hemispheric unbalance), the LC asymmetry may be the link between the trigeminal asymmetry and to the cognitive deficits. Notably, correcting occlusal issues not only reduced the trigeminal asymmetry but also the associated anisocoria (De Cicco et al., 2016; Tramonti Fantozzi et al., 2021a,c, 2023).

The main goal of the present investigation was to verify whether, in a population of adult subjects showing an asymmetric masseter EMG activity during clenching, the presence of trigeminal asymmetry elicits specific changes in the oculomotor performance evaluated during a reading. The number of saccades and the duration of fixations were evaluated using an eye-tracker. Each subject performed the reading task under two different occlusal conditions: with the dental arches in direct contact (habitual condition) and with a dental impression (imprint) interposed between them (corrected condition). The imprint used to correct the occlusion was designed to eliminate or minimize the EMG asymmetry during clenching. According to our hypothesis, we expected that oculomotor impairments would be present when the reading task was performed without imprint.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Subjects

Experiments were carried out in 40 subjects (age: 38.38 ± 10.05 years; females (n = 17): 36.00 ± 9.82 years; males (n = 23): 40.13 ± 10.06 years), who signed a written informed consent. Considering the side with higher EMG activity as the hypertonic side and the side with lower EGM activity as the hypotonic side, subjects showed a baseline EMG asymmetry greater than 20%. The asymmetry was evaluated as follow:

200EMGhypertonicsideEMGhypotonicsideEMGhypertonicside+EMGhypotonicside

All subjects were native Italian speakers and unaffected by neurological or psychiatric disorders. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the experimental protocol was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Pisa University (endorsement 39/2022).

2.2 Masseter electromyographic activity evaluation

During clenching, right and left masseter EMG activities were recorded by a K6-I MyoTronics system (sample rate: 720 Hz, cut-off frequency: 15 Hz, notch filter: 50 Hz) using Duotrode Ag/AgCl electrodes (inter-electrode distance: 19.5 mm, MyoTronics, Seattle, WA, USA), placed as described by De Cicco et al. (2014). Automatically filtered EMG traces were fully rectified, and the values of burst duration and average EMG activity were computed. The clenching effort (2–4 s) began and ended upon experimenter’s request.

2.3 Occlusal correction: dental impression manufacturing

Subjects received 15 min of transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) of trigeminal motor branches (Nnoaham and Kumbang, 2008), using four couples of electrodes, positioned on both sides at the level of the incisura sigmoidea and the submental region. Biphasic (cathodal/anodal) current pulses (0.54 ms duration, 21–25 mA intensity), delivered by two I.A.C.E.R. stimulators (Martellago, Venice, Italy), induced repeated contractions of the masseters and mandible depressor muscles. The stimuli for the left and the right sides were adjusted to achieve symmetric muscle activation, with the frequency set at 40and 0.618 Hz for mandible depressor and elevator muscles, respectively. These patterns led to an alternating contraction of the masseters and a tonic contraction of depressor muscles, resulting in small amplitude mandibular movements (1 mm). After TENS, the mandibular resting posture was lower. A dental impression was then obtained by placing a self-hardening material between the dental arches. When the subject wore this dental impression during a clenching effort, the EMG asymmetry was greatly reduced or abolished.

2.4 Oculomotor assessment

After creating the dental impressions (imprint), all subjects participated in two consecutive reading sessions to assess their oculomotor performance. In the first session, the subject wore the dental impression (corrected condition), while in the second session, their dental arches were in direct contact (habitual condition). It has been observed that simply repeating the reading task enhance reading ease, leading to shorter total reading time and reductions in both the frequency and the duration of fixation periods (Hyönä and Niemi, 1990; Raney and Rayner, 1995; Raney et al., 2000). Therefore, we chose to begin the experiment under the occlusal condition that, based on our hypothesis, was supposed to result in the best reading performance. In this way, the lack of improvement or a worsening of the reading performance in the second test could have been attributed to the effects of dental impression removal.

2.4.1 Apparatus

Gaze data were collected at 50 Hz using a 1,750 Tobii eye-tracker system (Tobii Technologies, Stockholm, Sweden). The instrument detects gaze position through a corneal reflection technique and is endowed with a 17” TFT monitor (1.280 × 1,024 resolution). The subject is illuminated by five near-infrared diodes placed above (three) and below (two) the screen. An image of the subject is obtained by an infrared light sensitive camera placed below the screen, between the bottom diodes. The subjects seated in comfortable chair at 60 cm from the screen, that was placed on a desk. The test was performed in an experimental room, under artificial lightening, with closed shutters. During the entire duration of the experiment, the context was never modified, and screen’s luminance was maintained constant. At the start of the reading session, a 16 points calibration of the eye-tracker was done for each subject.

2.4.2 Stimuli

During the reading session, subjects were instructed to silently read a specific text displayed on the monitor. The text was in Italian language and consisted of 29 lines, each with an average of 8 words per line. Font size, font type and text content were kept consistent across conditions for all the subjects. Since no time constraints were imposed, participants were free to modulate their reading speed.

2.4.3 Oculomotor behavior during the reading task

Fixations and saccades were directly identified by the eye-tracker. For each subject, the total number of fixations, the frequency of fixations per unit time (s), and their average duration of fixations (fixation duration) were computed. The number of fixations corresponds to that of saccadic movements performed by the subjects to scan the text. Moreover, the total reading time was recorded.

2.5 Statistical analysis

No outlier rejection was applied to the population analyzed. Oculomotor data were preliminary tested for distribution normality by the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test and by the Shapiro–Wilk test. The results of this screening have been shown in Table 1. Since the more demanding test (Shapiro–Wilk) indicated a significant deviation from normality (Table 1A), a Log10 transform was applied to the data. The transformation gave rise to a normal distribution for all the oculomotor parameters analyzed (Table 1B). Data were represented as mean ± standard deviation (SD). Between condition comparisons were performed by paired t-test. Correlations between variables were investigated by linear regression analysis. In addition to these classic parametric statistics, more robust tests less biased by data distribution were performed for both regression analysis (rank Spearman, Gamma and Kendall Tau correlation) and between-condition comparisons (Wilcoxon test). Most of statistical analyses were performed with Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS, version 20). For non-parametric correlations the software STATISTICA (version 6.0) was utilized. The level of statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Results of the normality test performed (Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Shapiro–Wilk) for the different ocular metrics recorded in the two occlusal conditions.

3 Results

3.1 Effects of occlusal correction on EMG activity

In the habitual condition, the EMG activity recorded on the hypotonic and hypertonic side corresponded to 61.9 ± 40.0 μV and to 126.0 ± 43.0 μV, respectively (t-test: p < 0.0005). Following occlusal correction, these EMG values were rebalanced (hypotonic: 143.0 ± 55.4 μV, hypertonic: 147.2 ± 51.0 μV, t-test: p = 0.090). The increase in EMG activity was significant for both sides (t-test: p < 0.0005). These modifications led to a significant reduction of the absolute value of EMG asymmetry in the corrected condition (from 76.8 ± 42.4% to 12.0 ± 9.8%, t-test: p < 0.0005). All these findings were confirmed by the Wilcoxon test. Basal values of EMG asymmetry and their reduction after occlusal correction were not correlated with subjects’ age. Linear regression analysis indicated that the (habitual - corrected) changes in EMG asymmetry were significantly correlated with those observed in the EMG activity recorded on the hypotonic side (Figure 1A). This finding was confirmed by non-parametric models of correlation (see Methods). Differently, no correlation was observed with the EMG changes recorded on the hypertonic side (Figure 1B).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The differences in the absolute value of EMG asymmetry between by the habitual and the corrected conditions are plotted as a function of the EMG activity changes observed on the hypotonic (A) and hypertonic (B) side. A: The continuous line corresponds to the regression line for all the plotted points, the relative equation is displayed in the corresponding panel.

3.2 Effects of changing occlusal condition on oculomotor measures and total reading time

A summary of the different oculomotor metrics recorded under corrected and habitual condition is displayed in Table 2. The corrected condition was associated with a lower fixation number compared to the habitual one. The same held true also for the number of fixations occurring per time unit. On the contrary, fixation duration was higher in corrected than in the habitual condition. Changing from the corrected to the habitual condition, no significant difference was observed in the total reading time. However, as shown in Figure 2, individual subjects could either show increases (circles) or decreases (dots) in this parameter. We will refer to these groups of subjects as Habitual Slower (HS) and Habitual Faster (HF), respectively. It is of interest that the increase in number of fixations characterized both HF (corrected: 2.02 ± 0.16 Log10(Nos.), habitual: 2.11 ± 0.16 Log10(Nos.), t-test: p = 0.019, Figure 3A) and HS (corrected: 2.02 ± 0.17 Log10(Nos.), habitual: 2.11 ± 0.13 Log10(Nos.), t-test: p = 0.001, Figure 3B) subjects. At variance, the decrease in fixation duration when passing from corrected to habitual condition was found among HF (corrected: 2.59 ± 0.20 Log10(ms); habitual: 2.43 ± 0.19 Log10(ms), t-test: p < 0.0005, Figure 3C) but not among HS subjects [corrected: 2.53 ± 0.24 Log10(ms); habitual: 2.50 ± 0.21 Log10(ms), t-test: p = 0.310, Figure 3D]. Nearly identical results were observed when data were compared by the Wilcoxon test. No differences were found between HF and HS subjects in terms of age, EMG asymmetry, oculomotor parameters and reading times evaluated in both corrected and habitual conditions.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean ± SD values of oculomotor parameters evaluated in the corrected and habitual condition, compared by paired t-test and Wilcoxon test.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Relation between the total reading times observed in habitual and in corrected conditions. The dashed line represents equal values for the two conditions. Dots and circles represent subjects showing a decrease and an increase of their reading time, respectively, when changing from the corrected to the habitual condition.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Comparison of fixation number (A,B) and duration values (C,D) obtained in habitual (ordinates) and corrected condition (abscissas) in subjects showing a decrease (A,C) or an increase (B,D) in their total reading time when switching from the corrected to the habitual condition. The dashed lines represent equal values for the two conditions.

3.3 Correlations between oculomotor measures and EMG asymmetry

When subjects were tested in the corrected condition, linear regression analysis did not show any significant correlation between total reading time/oculomotor measurements and the corresponding level of EMG asymmetry. Differently, in the habitual condition, the number of fixations (Figure 4A) and their duration (Figure 4B) were positively and negatively correlated with the EMG asymmetry, respectively. A positive correlation with EMG asymmetry was also found when the number of fixations was divided by the total reading time (number of fixations per second, Figure 4C). Differently, no significant correlation was observed between the total reading time and the EMG asymmetry (R = 0.212, p = 0.189). All these correlations were confirmed by applying non-parametric models. As shown in Figure 5, in both the corrected (Figure 5A) and the habitual (Figure 5B) conditions, fixation number and fixation duration were strongly (and negatively) correlated. While the total reading time was independent upon the fixation frequency, it was positively correlated with the fixation duration in both the corrected (Figure 5C) and the habitual (Figure 5D) conditions. These finding were confirmed by non-parametric regression models.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Habitual condition: correlations between the absolute value of EMG asymmetry and the fixation numbers (A), the fixation duration (B) and the fixation number per time unit (C). Continuous lines correspond to the regression lines for all the plotted points, the equations of which are displayed in the corresponding panel.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Correlations between oculomotor performance parameters and total reading time. The upper row represents the relation between number and duration of fixations in corrected (A) and habitual (B) conditions. The lower row displays the relation between total reading time and fixation duration in corrected (C) and habitual (D) conditions. Continuous lines correspond to the regression lines for all the plotted points, whose equations are displayed in the corresponding panel.

3.4 Changes in oculomotor measures and EMG activity/asymmetry elicited by occlusal correction

The differences in the number of fixations (Figure 6A), in fixation duration (Figure 6B) and in the frequency of fixations per second (Figure 6C) observed by switching from the corrected to the habitual condition (Log habitual - Log corrected) were influenced by the relative changes in (habitual-corrected) EMG asymmetry, as indicated by linear regression analysis. This observation was confirmed by non-parametric correlations.

Figure 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. The changes in fixation number (A), in fixation duration (B) and in fixation number per sec (C) elicited by switching from the corrected to the habitual condition are plotted as a function of the relative change in EMG asymmetry. Continuous lines correspond to the regression lines for all the plotted points, the equations of which are displayed in the corresponding panel.

4 Discussion

In this study, we investigated oculomotor behaviour during a reading task in subjects under two different occlusal conditions: corrected and habitual. For all participants, the habitual condition was associated to an asymmetrical development of masseter EMG activity during clenching. Previous findings showed that, in this condition, cognitive visuo-spatial performance and task-induced mydriasis are significantly reduced (De Cicco et al., 2014, 2016; Tramonti Fantozzi et al., 2021a,c,d).

Reading fluently requires coordinated perceptual, cognitive, and motor skills (Reichle and Reingold, 2013). The phenomenon of fixation, closely related to visual functions, is a brief period (Rayner, 1998) during which the reader extracts and processes information associated with the word currently fixed (Liversedge and Findlay, 2000; Degno and Liversedge, 2020). The duration and/or location of fixations can be influenced by several factors, including words frequency and predictability (Rayner and Duffy, 1986; Rayner and Well, 1996; Rayner et al., 2006; Schotter et al., 2012). Brain regions actively involved in modulating fixations are the reticular formation (RF), the cerebellum, the superior colliculus, the parietal and the frontal cortex (Krauzlis et al., 2017). The neuronal mechanism responsible for regulating ocular fixation during reading involves several phases: (1) the transmission of the signal from the retina to the visual cortex, (2) the visual representation of the fixed word, (3) lexical processing of the word, and (4) the programming of saccadic movements (Reichle and Reingold, 2013).

4.1 Limitations of the study

The main limitation of the present study is the lack of a reversal in the order of the two occlusal conditions (corrected, habitual) under which the reading task was performed. The lack of reversal poses the problem of how to reasonably dissect the effects of the condition from those of repeating twice the same test (the corrected condition was tested before the habitual condition). In this respect, the data present in the literature allow to draw some conclusion despite the methodological flaw. Indeed, there is a large body of evidence that the second reading of a text results in a total reading time reduction and in a decrease in both the frequency and the duration of fixation periods (Hyönä and Niemi, 1990; Raney and Rayner, 1995; Raney et al., 2000). Reading difficulties, such as dyslexia, are typically characterized by the opposite pattern, i.e., an increase in both the number and the duration of fixation and an extension of the total reading time (Hutzler and Wimmer, 2004). This pattern has been observed also in patients with macular degeneration (Ktistakis et al., 2023), Alzheimer’s disease (Lueck et al., 2000; Fernández et al., 2013, 2015) and aphasia-based alexia (Hux et al., 2024). So, in the present study, the lack of an improvement or a worsening of oculomotor parameters during the second session suggests a negative effect of the habitual condition on text reading. Indeed, according to our hypothesis, the balanced (corrected) occlusal condition tested first should have been that associated to a better oculomotor control. Another point that allows to distinguish the effect of occlusal condition from those of task repetition is the fact that, in the former case, the changes observed in oculomotor behavior should be correlated with the associated changes in EMG asymmetry. This was indeed observed in the present study, as shown in Figure 6.

Finally, another limitation of the study was the lack of measuring the saccade amplitudes, whose acquisition would have allowed a better understanding of the changes in oculomotor behavior elicited by changing the occlusal condition. It has to be pointed out, however, that given the fixed length of the read text and the relative small number of regressions performed during reading (Rayner, 2009; 10–15% of the time), the changes in saccade amplitude can be expected to be strongly (and negatively) correlated to those in saccade (fixation) frequency.

4.2 EMG asymmetry and oculomotor behaviour during a Reading task

Findings from this study suggest that oculomotor behaviour is modified by the occlusal condition.

In our study, during the second reading session, we did not observe an average decrease in the total reading time. Conversely, we noticed an increase of the frequency of fixations and a decrease in the average duration of fixations. This suggests that removing the imprint for the second reading session might have partially negated the typically positive impact that a second reading has on oculomotor metrics, consisting in a decrease in fixation frequency and duration. This implies that the influence of occlusal condition on reading performance may be significant enough to override the decrease in fixation frequency expected from the familiarity and repetition of the reading task. This hypothesis is supported by the finding that the increase in fixation frequency, induced by switching from corrected to the habitual condition, was positively correlated with the corresponding increase in EMG asymmetry during clenching. Since the duration of fixations was on the average reduced, as would have been expected during a second repetition of the reading task, it could be proposed that occlusal disharmony is hampering the sequence of eye movements more than the processing of visual information, which determines the duration of the fixation period. In some subjects, fixation duration showed little changes or a frank increase switching from corrected to habitual condition: consequently, they were reading faster with the imprint interposed between the arches. In this restricted population, the occlusal disharmony has not only increased the number of fixations but has also counterbalanced or overwhelmed the decrease in fixation duration expected by task repetition.

Beyond the changes in oculomotor metrics elicited by changing the occlusal condition, it has to be stressed that within our population oculomotor metrics in habitual occlusion are related to the trigeminal asymmetry: subjects with larger asymmetries show in fact higher number of fixations of shorter duration with respect to less asymmetric subjects. In other words, trigeminal asymmetry seems to determine the reading pattern of the individual subjects. This gives rise, within the population, to a trade-off between fixation duration and frequency, with an inverse relationship between the two oculomotor metrics. Higher number of fixations is indicative of a reduced saccadic amplitude and could be attributable, in principle, to a reduction in the span of text that can be processed by the reader, leading to a shift in reading dynamics/patterns characterized by an increased number of fixations (i.e., a reduced saccade amplitude) with shorter durations. Higher numbers of fixations and reduction of their duration with respect to normal controls has been also observed in offspring of late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (O-LOAD), as reported by Fernández et al. (2022). The authors concluded that “This eye movement pattern could be considered an early marker of oculomotor impairment.” They claimed that the control group processed and integrated the full meaning of a sentence through larger saccades and spending more time on the fixated words, whereas the O-LOAD group had difficulties in extracting information from the text, being constrained to a reading pattern characterized by shorter saccades and less time on the fixated word. In other words, the O-LOAD subjects could not analyse the same span of words as the control group.

The reading difficulties elicited by occlusal disharmony could be linked to a diminished capacity in processing visual input coming from the parafoveal region, which is crucial for efficient reading (Schotter et al., 2012): this would explain the higher fixation number and reduced fixation duration observed in our study. A parafoveal vision decline was observed in older readers (Payne and Federmeier, 2017). Indeed, these subjects showed a reduced ability to quickly combine the meanings of words seen in their peripheral and foveal vision (Payne and Federmeier, 2017). This could be related to neuroanatomical and functional changes induced by aging, particularly in the frontal cortex (Fabiani, 2012). Interestingly, in the presence of a trigeminal sensory-motor imbalance, a reduced performance was observed in the Spinnler-Tognoni test, consisting in the retrieval of target numbers within numerical matrices that must be scanned line by line (Spinnler and Tognoni, 1987). When the asymmetry was corrected by bite/oral prosthesis placement, it was observed an improvement in scanning velocity of the matrix, as well as in the speed of target retrieval, without any increase in error rate. These results contradicted the well-known trade-off between speed and accuracy (Fitts, 1954) and could be well explained by an increase in the subject’s ability to extract parafoveal information. This results in a higher number of items that can be processed simultaneously, allowing a faster execution of matrices scanning and targets retrieval. Based on these findings, the decrease in fixation duration following imprint removal could be regarded somehow differently than an improvement, in reading performance due to the repetition of the task. It could represent a change in reading pattern imposed by a reduced capability of processing visual information following the insurgence of a condition of occlusal disharmony. Further investigation is needed to clarify this issue.

4.3 Trigeminal and oculomotor pathways

Some of the central connections of trigeminal system may potentially account for an occlusal interference with oculomotor behaviour (Zhang et al., 2011; Liang et al., 2017). Indeed, trigeminal input reaches RF (Langer and Kaneko, 1990) and vestibular nuclear (Buisseret-Delmas et al., 1999) regions, as well as the nucleus prepositus hypoglossi (McCrea and Baker, 1985; Buisseret-Delmas et al., 1999) and the superior colliculus (Ndiaye et al., 2000), which have part in oculomotor control. These anatomo-functional relationships could account for pathological interference of mandibular movements with eye movements (Pratt et al., 1984; Zhang et al., 2011), as well as for the apparent interference existing between occlusal condition and ocular vergence (Marchili et al., 2016; Caruso et al., 2018). In principle, an asymmetry in the trigeminal input may complicate oculomotor control by affecting the activity of the structures mentioned above, resulting in the need of a larger number of saccades to read the same amount of text. Although we cannot completely reject this hypothesis, it must be pointed out that the observed changes in oculomotor behavior resembles to an adjustment in reading dynamics rather than to an oculomotor impairment.

Subjects with asymmetric sensorimotor trigeminal activity during clenching were characterized by anisocoria. Giving the established relationship between pupil size and LC activity, this finding suggests that in our sample the trigeminal asymmetry may lead to an asymmetry in the basal LC activity. The pathways linking proprioceptive trigeminal activity to LC activity have been recently reviewed (De Cicco et al., 2017). Fibres from the mesencephalic trigeminal nucleus reach the LC directly or through the reticular formation. In turn noradrenergic fibres from the LC contact mesencephalic trigeminal ganglion cells (Takahashi et al., 2010). Few LC axons enter the trigeminal motor nucleus where they enhance motoneuronal excitability (Samuels and Szabadi, 2008). These feedback projections may potentially enhance proprioceptive trigeminal and LC activity during chewing, giving rise to a progressive enhancement in cognitive performance (Tramonti Fantozzi et al., 2023). A predominant trigeminal sensorimotor activity on one side may therefore lead to an asymmetry in LC discharge, which can be further enhanced by the reciprocal LC connection between the two sides (Jones and Yang, 1985), which are inhibitory in nature (Cedarbaum and Aghajanian, 1977).

The condition could be detrimental for higher level sensory-motor and cognitive processes (De Cicco et al., 2014, 2016; Tramonti Fantozzi et al., 2019, 2021a,c), either by inducing an asymmetric body representation, or else an asymmetric hemispheric excitability, akin to the phenomenon described by Lomber and Payne (1996). The scanning dynamic of the text can be affected by such an asymmetry, contributing to the increased number of fixations (saccades) observed in the habitual occlusion condition compared to the occlusion corrected condition.

In conclusion, the influence of occlusion condition on oculomotor behaviour can be attributed to both the asymmetric trigeminal input to oculomotor structures and the effects of trigeminal asymmetries on higher visuo-spatial functions.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study is freely available in an online repository at: https://osf.io/7xe5f/.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Bioethics Commettee of the Pisa University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MPTF: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Visualization. VDC: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Validation, Writing – review & editing. AB: Writing – review & editing. EC: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. LB: Funding acquisition, Resources, Writing – review & editing. DDC: Writing – review & editing. Pd'A: Writing – review & editing. UF: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. DM: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the University of Pisa (Ateneo 2022-2023 to LB) and the Italian Space Agency (Pe-15 to DM).

Acknowledgments

We would like to acknowledge Cristina Pucci, Paolo Orsini, and Francesco Montanari for their excellent technical contribution and support. We also thank Dr. Žan Zelič for helpful discussion.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Andres, M., Masson, N., Larigaldie, N., Bonato, M., Vandermeeren, Y., and Dormal, V. (2020). Transcranial electric stimulation optimizes the balance of visual attention across space. Clin. Neurophysiol. 131, 912–920. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.12.415

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buisseret-Delmas, C., Compoint, C., Delfini, C., and Buisseret, P. (1999). Organisation of reciprocal connections between trigeminal and vestibular nuclei in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 409, 153–168. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19990621)409:1<153::AID-CNE11>3.0.CO;2-#

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caruso, S., Gatto, R., Capogreco, M., and Nota, A. (2018). Association of Visual Defects and Occlusal Molar Class in children. Biomed. Res. Int. 2018:7296289. doi: 10.1155/2018/7296289

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cedarbaum, J. M., and Aghajanian, G. K. (1977). Catecholamine receptors on locus coeruleus neurons: pharmacological characterization. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 44, 375–385. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(77)90312-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Cicco, V., Barresi, M., Tramonti Fantozzi, M. P., Cataldo, E., Parisi, V., and Manzoni, D. (2016). Oral implant-prostheses: new teeth for a brighter brain. PLoS One 11:e0148715. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0148715

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Cicco, V., Cataldo, E., Barresi, M., Parisi, V., and Manzoni, D. (2014). Sensorimotor trigeminal unbalance modulates pupil size. Arch. Ital. Biol. 152, 1–12

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

De Cicco, V., Tramonti Fantozzi, M. P., Cataldo, E., Barresi, M., Bruschini, L., Faraguna, U., et al. (2017). Trigeminal, visceral and vestibular inputs may improve cognitive functions by acting through the locus Coeruleus and the ascending reticular activating system: a new hypothesis. Front. Neuroanat. 11:130. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2017.00130

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Degno, F., and Liversedge, S. P. (2020). Eye movements and fixation-related potentials in Reading: a review. Vision (Basel) 4:11. doi: 10.3390/vision4010011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Einhäuser, W. (2017). “The pupil as marker of cognitive processes” in Computational and cognitive neuroscience of vision. ed. Q. Zhao (Singapore: Springer Singapore), 141–169.

Google Scholar

Fabiani, M. (2012). It was the best of times, it was the worst of times: a psychophysiologist’s view of cognitive aging. Psychophysiology 49, 283–304. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01331.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, G., Castro, L. R., Schumacher, M., and Agamennoni, O. E. (2015). Diagnosis of mild Alzheimer disease through the analysis of eye movements during reading. J. Integr. Neurosci. 14, 121–133. doi: 10.1142/S0219635215500090

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, G., González, A. P., Abulafia, C., Fiorentini, L., Agamennoni, O., and Guinjoan, S. M. (2022). Oculomotor abnormalities during Reading in the offspring of late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 19, 212–222. doi: 10.2174/1567205019666220413075840

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, G., Mandolesi, P., Rotstein, N. P., Colombo, O., Agamennoni, O., and Politi, L. E. (2013). Eye movement alterations during reading in patients with early Alzheimer disease. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 54, 8345–8352. doi: 10.1167/iovs.13-12877

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J. Exp. Psychol. 47, 381–391. doi: 10.1037/h0055392

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffing, R. C., and Seitz, A. R. (2015). Pupillometry as a glimpse into the neurochemical basis of human memory encoding. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 765–774. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00749

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutzler, F., and Wimmer, H. (2004). Eye movements of dyslexic children when reading in a regular orthography. Brain Lang. 89, 235–242. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00401-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hux, K., Knollman-Porter, K., Bevelhimer, A., and Wallace, S. E. (2024). Variations in eye-gaze behaviors evident when five adults with aphasia-based Alexia read multisentence passages. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 33, 173–188. doi: 10.1044/2023_AJSLP-23-00160

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyönä, J., and Niemi, P. (1990). Eye movements during repeated reading of a text. Acta Psychol. 73, 259–280. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(90)90026-C

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, B. E., and Yang, T. Z. (1985). The efferent projections from the reticular formation and the locus coeruleus studied by anterograde and retrograde axonal transport in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 242, 56–92. doi: 10.1002/cne.902420105

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Joshi, S., and Gold, J. I. (2020). Pupil size as a window on neural substrates of cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 466–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.03.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Joshi, S., Li, Y., Kalwani, R. M., and Gold, J. I. (2016). Relationships between pupil diameter and neuronal activity in the locus coeruleus, colliculi, and cingulate cortex. Neuron 89, 221–234. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerkhoff, G. (2001). Spatial hemineglect in humans. Prog. Neurobiol. 63, 1–27. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(00)00028-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kihara, K., Takeuchi, T., Yoshimoto, S., Kondo, H. M., and Kawahara, J. I. (2015). Pupillometric evidence for the locus coeruleus-noradrenaline system facilitating attentional processing of action-triggered visual stimuli. Front. Psychol. 6:827. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00827

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koch, G., Bonnì, S., Giacobbe, V., Bucchi, G., Basile, B., Lupo, F., et al. (2012). θ-Burst stimulation of the left hemisphere accelerates recovery of hemispatial neglect. Neurology 78, 24–30. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31823ed08f

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krauzlis, R. J., Goffart, L., and Hafed, Z. M. (2017). Neuronal control of fixation and fixational eye movements. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 372:20160205. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0205

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ktistakis, E., Simos, P., Tsilimbaris, M. K., and Plainis, S. (2023). Efficacy οf wet age-related macular degeneration treatment οn Reading: a pilot study using eye-movement analysis. Optom. Vis. Sci. 100, 670–678. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0000000000002064

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Langer, T. P., and Kaneko, C. R. (1990). Brainstem afferents to the oculomotor omnipause neurons in monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 295, 413–427. doi: 10.1002/cne.902950306

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, H., Zhang, J., Luo, P., Zhu, H., Qiao, Y., Su, A., et al. (2017). Electromyography and Fos immunostaining study establish a possible functional link between trigeminal proprioception and the oculomotor system in rats. J. Biomed. Res. 31, 256–263. doi: 10.7555/JBR.31.20160127

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liversedge, S. P., and Findlay, J. M. (2000). Saccadic eye movements and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 6–14. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01418-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lomber, S. G., and Payne, B. R. (1996). Removal of two halves restores the whole: reversal of visual hemineglect during bilateral cortical or collicular inactivation in the cat. Vis. Neurosci. 13, 1143–1156. doi: 10.1017/S0952523800007781

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lueck, K. L., Mendez, M. F., and Perryman, K. M. (2000). Eye movement abnormalities during reading in patients with Alzheimer disease. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. 13, 77–82

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Marchili, N., Ortu, E., Pietropaoli, D., Cattaneo, R., and Monaco, A. (2016). Dental occlusion and ophthalmology: a literature review. Open Dent. J. 10, 460–468. doi: 10.2174/1874210601610010460

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathôt, S. (2018). Pupillometry: psychology, physiology, and function. J. Cogn. 1:16. doi: 10.5334/joc.18

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrea, R. A., and Baker, R. (1985). Anatomical connections of the nucleus prepositus of the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 237, 377–407. doi: 10.1002/cne.902370308

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, P. R., O’Connell, R. G., O’Sullivan, M., Robertson, I. H., and Balsters, J. H. (2014). Pupil diameter covaries with BOLD activity in human locus coeruleus. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 4140–4154. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22466

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ndiaye, A., Pinganaud, G., Vander Werf, F., Buisseret-Delmas, C., and Buisseret, P. (2000). Connections between the trigeminal mesencephalic nucleus and the superior colliculus in the rat. Neurosci. Lett. 294, 17–20. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)01519-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nnoaham, K. E., and Kumbang, J. (2008). Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) for chronic pain. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 4:CD003222. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003222.pub2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Payne, B. R., and Federmeier, K. D. (2017). Event-related brain potentials reveal age-related changes in parafoveal-foveal integration during sentence processing. Neuropsychologia 106, 358–370. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratt, S. G., Beyer, C. K., and Johnson, C. C. (1984). The Marcus Gunn phenomenon. A review of 71 cases. Ophthalmology 91, 27–30. doi: 10.1016/S0161-6420(84)34331-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raney, G. E., and Rayner, K. (1995). Word frequency effects and eye movements during two readings of a text. Canad. J. Exp. Psychol. 49, 151–173. doi: 10.1037/1196-1961.49.2.151

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raney, G. E., Therriault, D. J., and Minkoff, S. R. B. (2000). Repetition effects from paraphrased text: evidence for an integrated representation model of text representation. Discourse Process. 29, 61–81. doi: 10.1207/S15326950dp2901_4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychol. Bull. 124, 372–422. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayner, K. (2009). The 35th sir Frederick Bartlett lecture: eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 62, 1457–1506. doi: 10.1080/17470210902816461

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayner, K., and Duffy, S. A. (1986). Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Mem. Cogn. 14, 191–201. doi: 10.3758/BF03197692

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayner, K., Reichle, E. D., Stroud, M. J., Williams, C. C., and Pollatsek, A. (2006). The effect of word frequency, word predictability, and font difficulty on the eye movements of young and older readers. Psychol. Aging 21, 448–465. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.448

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayner, K., and Well, A. D. (1996). Effects of contextual constraint on eye movements in reading: a further examination. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 3, 504–509. doi: 10.3758/BF03214555

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reichle, E. D., and Reingold, E. M. (2013). Neurophysiological constraints on the eye-mind link. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:361. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00361

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimer, J., McGinley, M. J., Liu, Y., Rodenkirch, C., Wang, Q., McCormick, D. A., et al. (2016). Pupil fluctuations track rapid changes in adrenergic and cholinergic activity in cortex. Nat. Commun. 7:13289. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13289

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Samuels, E. R., and Szabadi, E. (2008). Functional neuroanatomy of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus: its roles in the regulation of arousal and autonomic function part I: principles of functional organisation. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 6, 235–253. doi: 10.2174/157015908785777229

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schotter, E. R., Angele, B., and Rayner, K. (2012). Parafoveal processing in reading. Atten. Percept. Psychophysiol. 74, 5–35. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0219-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvetti, M., Seurinck, R., van Bochove, M. E., and Verguts, T. (2013). The influence of the noradrenergic system on optimal control of neural plasticity. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:160. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00160

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spinnler, H., and Tognoni, G. (1987). The Italian group on the neuropsychological study of aging. Italian standardization and classification of neuropsychological tests. Ital. J. Neurol. Sci. 6, 1–120.

Google Scholar

Takahashi, T., Shirasu, M., Shirasu, M., Kubo, K., Onozuka, M., Sato, S., et al. (2010). The locus coeruleus projects to the mesencephalic trigeminal nucleus in rats. Neurosci. Res. 68, 103–106. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2010.06.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tramonti Fantozzi, M. P., De Cicco, V., Argento, S., De Cicco, D., Barresi, M., Cataldo, E., et al. (2021a). Trigeminal input, pupil size and cognitive performance: from oral to brain matter. Brain Res. 1751:147194. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2020.147194

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tramonti Fantozzi, M. P., De Cicco, V., d’Ascanio, P., Cataldo, E., De Cicco, D., Bruschini, L., et al. (2023). Trigeminal stimulation and visuospatial performance: the struggle between chewing and trigeminal asymmetries. Biomedicine 11:2307. doi: 10.3390/biomedicines11082307

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tramonti Fantozzi, M. P., De Cicco, V., De Cicco, D., d’Ascanio, P., Cataldo, E., Bruschini, L., et al. (2021b). Chewing and cognitive improvement: the side matters. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 15:749444. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2021.749444

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tramonti Fantozzi, M. P., Diciotti, S., Tessa, C., Castagna, B., Chiesa, D., Barresi, M., et al. (2019). Unbalanced occlusion modifies the pattern of brain activity during execution of a finger to thumb motor task. Front. Neurosci. 13:499. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00499

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tramonti Fantozzi, M. P., Lazzarini, G., De Cicco, V., Briganti, A., Argento, S., De Cicco, D., et al. (2021c). The path from trigeminal asymmetry to cognitive impairment: a behavioral and molecular study. Sci. Rep. 11:4744. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-82265-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tramonti Fantozzi, M. P., Marconi, O., Simoni, F., De Cicco, V., De Cicco, D., Cataldo, E., et al. (2021d). Coupling between trigeminal-induced asymmetries in locus Coeruleus activity and cognitive performance. Symmetry 13:1676. doi: 10.3390/sym13091676

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Liang, H., Luo, P., and Xiong, H. (2011). Unraveling a masticatory - oculomotor neural pathway in rat: implications for a pathophysiological neural circuit in human? Int. J. Physiol. Pathophysiol. Pharmacol. 3, 280–287

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Keywords: occlusal correction, EMG asymmetry, Locus Coeruleus, text reading, eye movements

Citation: Tramonti Fantozzi MP, De Cicco V, Bazzani A, Cataldo E, Bruschini L, De Cicco D, d’Ascanio P, Faraguna U and Manzoni D (2024) Occlusal effects on text reading: an eye-tracker study. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 18:1409251. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2024.1409251

Received: 29 March 2024; Accepted: 02 August 2024;
Published: 15 August 2024.

Edited by:

Adonis Moschovakis, University of Crete, Greece

Reviewed by:

Vito Enrico Pettorossi, University of Perugia, Italy
Marco Barone, LUM University "Giuseppe Degennaro", Italy

Copyright © 2024 Tramonti Fantozzi, De Cicco, Bazzani, Cataldo, Bruschini, De Cicco, d’Ascanio, Faraguna and Manzoni. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Diego Manzoni, diego.manzoni@unipi.it

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.