Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 13 January 2022
This article is part of the Research Topic The Cognitive Neuroscience of Visual Working Memory, Volume II View all 5 articles

Dependence of Working Memory on Coordinated Activity Across Brain Areas

  • 1School of Cognitive Sciences, Institute for Research in Fundamental Sciences (IPM), Tehran, Iran
  • 2Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
  • 3Cognitive Systems Laboratory, Control and Intelligent Processing Center of Excellence (CIPCE), School of Electrical and Computer Engineering, College of Engineering, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran

Neural signatures of working memory (WM) have been reported in numerous brain areas, suggesting a distributed neural substrate for memory maintenance. In the current manuscript we provide an updated review of the literature focusing on intracranial neurophysiological recordings during WM in primates. Such signatures of WM include changes in firing rate or local oscillatory power within an area, along with measures of coordinated activity between areas based on synchronization between oscillations. In comparing the ability of various neural signatures in any brain area to predict behavioral performance, we observe that synchrony between areas is more frequently and robustly correlated with WM performance than any of the within-area neural signatures. We further review the evidence for alteration of inter-areal synchrony in brain disorders, consistent with an important role for such synchrony during behavior. Additionally, results of causal studies indicate that manipulating synchrony across areas is especially effective at influencing WM task performance. Each of these lines of research supports the critical role of inter-areal synchrony in WM. Finally, we propose a framework for interactions between prefrontal and sensory areas during WM, incorporating a range of experimental findings and offering an explanation for the observed link between intra-areal measures and WM performance.

Introduction

Working memory (WM), as a basic cognitive function, contributes to our goal-oriented behaviors such as decision-making, problem-solving, language comprehension, and learning (Gazzaniga and Ivry, 2013). Persistent activity has been the traditional signature for implicating an area in WM (Kojima and Goldman-Rakic, 1982; Funahashi et al., 1989; Miller et al., 1996); however, persistent activity is rarely a strong predictor of memory performance (in terms of percent correct, accuracy, or faster reaction times; Table 1), raising questions about whether it is the best indicator of an area’s contribution to memory maintenance. Moreover, many areas show such persistent spiking activity during the delay period of a WM task (i.e., delay activity), suggesting that memory maintenance may depend on distributed activity across multiple brain areas (Christophel et al., 2017). This hypothesis leads to the question of how these active areas interact during the task.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Neural signatures of working memory (WM) within areas and their relationship to behavior.

Synchronized activity between brain areas provides a potential means to modulate communication during WM and other tasks (Varela et al., 2001; Fries, 2005, 2015; Sakurai and Takahashi, 2008; Canolty et al., 2010; Fell and Axmacher, 2011; Luczak et al., 2013; Canavier, 2015; Yuste, 2015; Avena-Koenigsberger et al., 2018; Singer, 2018; Hahn et al., 2019).

In this review, we summarize findings on changes in oscillatory and synchronized activity within and between brain areas during WM, including correlations with behavioral performance, impairments associated with mental disorders, and causal manipulations. Finally, we suggest a framework for interactions between prefrontal and visual areas which offers an explanation for why success in WM tasks relies on inter-areal synchrony.

Population-Level Signatures Predict the Behavioral Performance on Working Memory Tasks

The activity of individual neurons often fails to predict WM performance. A summary of studies which reported the correlation (or lack thereof) between delay-period spiking or oscillatory activity within a single brain area and behavior is shown in Table 1; it includes both single-neuron firing rate studies, and population-level measures based on local field potentials (LFPs) in non-human primates or intracranial recordings in humans (ECoG). LFP activity, which reflects a combination of local activity and sub-threshold network input (Buzsáki et al., 2012; Friston et al., 2015), provides a window onto local oscillatory activity and synchronization between areas (for a review of EEG findings, see Fell and Axmacher, 2011). Proportionally, very few studies measuring persistent activity in single cells report a correlation with behavioral performance, and several fail to find such a performance correlation (Nakamura and Kubota, 1995; Siegel et al., 2009; Liebe et al., 2012; Rigotti et al., 2013; Sarma et al., 2015; Antzoulatos and Miller, 2016; Parthasarathy et al., 2017; Bahmani et al., 2018; Lundqvist et al., 2018; Rezayat et al., 2021). However, we should note that many studies measuring persistent spiking activity in non-human primates are not optimized for finding such behavioral correlations, since animals are extensively trained, often leaving few error trials for analysis (Pessoa et al., 2002). Meanwhile the publication bias against reporting negative results (i.e., a lack of behavioral correlation) will introduce bias in the opposite direction (Leavitt et al., 2017). The role of persistent spiking activity in the maintenance of WM is an active subject of debate in the field (Dedoncker et al., 2016; Christophel et al., 2017; Leavitt et al., 2017; Constantinidis et al., 2018; Miller et al., 2018); some activity-silent theories of WM depend on changes in synaptic weights rather than ongoing spiking activity (Mongillo et al., 2008; Stokes, 2015), which would certainly explain the lack of a strong behavioral correlation for delay period spiking activity. Table 1 summarizes cases where behavioral correlations of spiking or LFP activity are reported.

Population-level signatures (such as LFP) are more likely than single neuron activity to predict WM performance (see Table 1). The LFP power spectrum provides a representation of oscillatory activity in different frequency bands, which may also relate to the relative timing of activity within an area or to fluctuations in synaptic input (Buzsáki et al., 2012). WM modulation of LFP power has been reported in the prefrontal (Lundqvist et al., 2016, 2018), parietal (Pesaran et al., 2008), and sensory areas (Siegel et al., 2009; Barr et al., 2010; Helfrich and Knight, 2016; Lundqvist et al., 2016, 2018) of monkeys and in the prefrontal cortex (Howard et al., 2003; Noy et al., 2015; Johnson et al., 2018a), hippocampus (van Vugt et al., 2010; Ni et al., 2017), medial temporal lobe (Howard et al., 2003; Courtney, 2008; Ni et al., 2017; Johnson et al., 2018a), sensory areas (Noy et al., 2015), and parietal cortex (Noy et al., 2015) of humans. Within PFC, γ band activity increased during encoding and retrieval of information but decreased during the delay period, while β band power showed the opposite pattern (Lundqvist et al., 2016, 2018); deviation from this pattern of activity predicted errors (Lundqvist et al., 2018). Within parietal cortices, both spiking activity and the γ band LFP power during the delay predicted the animal’s choice (Pesaran et al., 2002). In sensory areas which lack persistent spiking activity, αβ band LFP power increased during the delay period of a WM task (Mendoza-Halliday et al., 2014; Bahmani et al., 2018). This change in power was correlated with performance (Bahmani et al., 2018) and reflected the content of WM (Mendoza-Halliday et al., 2014).

In addition to modulating power in different LFP frequency bands, WM also alters oscillatory synchronization, and spike timing relative to these oscillations within an area [often measured via Phase-Phase Locking (PPL) or Spike Phase Locking (SPL), respectively]. Oscillatory synchrony within sensory areas was measured by phase locking between different sites within MT (Tallon-Baudry et al., 2001; Bahmani et al., 2018), V4 (Tallon-Baudry et al., 2004), or IT (Tallon-Baudry et al., 2001, 2004), all of which showed increases in αβ band phase locking during WM. Phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) measures interactions between different frequency bands (more specifically, the phase of one frequency and the amplitude of another, typically higher frequency); WM induces changes in PAC in the hippocampus (Axmacher et al., 2010), and across different layers of PFC (Bastos et al., 2018) and MT (Maris et al., 2011). In PFC, the αβ phase in the deep layers modulated γ band activity in the superficial layers (Bastos et al., 2018). Within PFC and sensory areas, the maintenance of WM is accompanied by modulation of SPL in the β and θ bands (Siegel et al., 2009). Interestingly, the specific phase values at which spikes were locked predicted the content of WM and behavior (Siegel et al., 2009). Similarly, in MT, average spiking activity didn’t reflect the content of WM, but the SPL in the αβ band did (Bahmani et al., 2018). Moreover, greater SPL during WM corresponded with enhanced processing of sensory input (Bahmani et al., 2018). Similarly, WM information in spiking activity varied with the θ cycle in area V4 (Lee et al., 2005). As described, SPL analysis provides a widow onto the temporal coding of spiking activity, which sometimes reveals information not detectable in rate coding over longer time windows. These phase locking measures within an area are also more likely than the firing rate of individual neurons to correlate with performance.

Synchronization Between Areas During Working Memory Predicts Performance

Many areas have delay activity during WM (Christophel et al., 2017; Leavitt et al., 2017; Sreenivasan and D’Esposito, 2019), raising the question of whether interactions between these areas contribute to memory maintenance. Here we review evidence that changes in synchrony between areas occur during WM: there is evidence for PFC interacting with sensory areas, the parietal cortex, and the hippocampus during WM (summarized in Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Summary of studies examining synchrony between areas during working memory (WM). Brain schematics of the monkey (left) and human brain (right), and areas recorded (green) in studies reporting measurements of synchrony between areas. Gray arrows indicate areas recorded in the same study, labeled with the frequency band in which WM-induced changes in synchrony between the areas were reported.

Let’s begin by briefly outlining three conceptual models which describe the role of PFC’s interactions with sensory cortex, parietal cortex, and hippocampal areas during WM: sensory recruitment, distributed network, and activation of long-term memory models. In all of these models the PFC is believed to be crucial, based on extensive literature on its role in executive function (Jacobsen and Nissen, 1937; Piaget, 1964; Niki, 1974; Kojima and Goldman-Rakic, 1982; Funahashi et al., 1989; Miller et al., 1996). The sensory recruitment model seeks to describe the interaction between PFC and sensory areas during WM (Pasternak and Greenlee, 2005), suggesting that sensory areas maintain detailed sensory memories under control of the PFC. Distributed network models of WM posit that interactions between association areas (such as parietal and prefrontal cortex) are necessary for memory maintenance (Leavitt et al., 2017). In the activation of long-term memory theory, PFC-hippocampal interactions maintain WM via activation of long-term memory representations (Eriksson et al., 2015; Loaiza and Halse, 2019; Rhodes and Cowan, 2019); it has also been suggested that information must pass through WM before entering long-term memory (Eriksson et al., 2015; Loaiza and Halse, 2019; Rhodes and Cowan, 2019). Synchrony between the PFC and areas associated with long-term memory (such as the hippocampus) could reflect either of these processes. In all of these scenarios (sensory recruitment, distributed network, or activation of long-term memory representations), synchronized activity across areas plays a key role in WM tasks (Leszczyński et al., 2015). In the following paragraphs we discuss some evidence for each of these interactions.

First, several studies report synchrony between PFC and sensory areas during WM. Phase synchrony between PFC and temporal cortex increased during memory maintenance (Liebe et al., 2012; Rezayat et al., 2021). Figure 2 shows that synchrony between brain areas reflects the content of WM. Phase locking between PFC and IT, specifically in the β band, reflected both the identity and the location of a remembered object (Figure 2A). These inter-areal synchrony measures were also related to WM performance (Table 2). Synchrony between PFC and V4, specifically in the θ band, predicted memory performance (Liebe et al., 2012), as did β band synchrony between PFC and IT cortex (Rezayat et al., 2021). This inter-areal phase locking and spike-phase synchrony was correlated with performance even when within-area signatures showed little or no relationship to memory performance (Rezayat et al., 2021).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Synchronized activity between brain areas reflected the content of WM. (A) Phase-phase locking (PPL) between PFC (FEF) and temporal cortex (IT) encoded the identity (top) and location (bottom) of the sample object during a delayed-match-to-sample task [adapted from Rezayat et al. (2021)]. Heatmap shows the difference in PPL between conditions (different object identities or locations) across time and frequency. (B) LFP-LFP coherence between PFC and parietal cortex encoded the identity (top) and location (bottom) of the sample object during the delayed-match-to-sample task [adapted from Salazar et al. (2012)]. Heatmap shows the difference in coherence between conditions (different object identities or locations) across time and frequency.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Neural signatures of WM between areas and their relationship to behavior.

There are many studies suggesting that the content of WM is maintained via the interaction of association areas across a distributed network (Christophel et al., 2017; Leavitt et al., 2017); indeed, the frontoparietal network is thought to play a key role in a variety of cognitive functions (Sarnthein et al., 1998; Zaksas et al., 2001; Babiloni et al., 2004; Olesen et al., 2004; Freedman and Assad, 2006; Hamidi et al., 2008; Salazar et al., 2012; Zhou et al., 2012; Dotson et al., 2014, 2018; Antzoulatos and Miller, 2016; Rose et al., 2016; Mackey and Curtis, 2017; Violante et al., 2017; Jacob et al., 2018; Wang S. et al., 2019), including WM. Phase synchrony between prefrontal and parietal cortex, specifically in the β band, reflected the content of WM (Salazar et al., 2012; Dotson et al., 2014; Antzoulatos and Miller, 2016; Figure 2B). Frontoparietal β band synchrony also predicted WM performance (Antzoulatos and Miller, 2016). In human intracranial recordings, frontoparietal delta and θ band oscillations modulated WM representations (Johnson et al., 2018b). Much of the evidence for the involvement of frontoparietal synchrony in WM comes from human EEG and MEG research, including evidence of phase synchronization (Babiloni et al., 2004; Deiber et al., 2007; Rutman et al., 2010; Zanto et al., 2011) and phase-amplitude coupling (Reinhart and Nguyen, 2019). Cross frequency coupling measured by PAC in human ECoG is observed between many brain areas (Bruns and Eckhorn, 2004; Maris et al., 2011) and these interactions are related to frontoparietal connectivity (Figueroa-Vargas et al., 2020). All of this evidence points toward a role for frontoparietal synchrony during WM.

The role of long-term memory in WM (Jensen and Lisman, 2005) remains controversial. The observation of prefrontal-hippocampal synchrony during WM maintenance (Brincat and Miller, 2015) seems consistent with the suggestion that the hippocampus supports WM by activating long-term memory representations (Eriksson et al., 2015; Loaiza and Halse, 2019; Rhodes and Cowan, 2019). In support of recruitment of the hippocampus during WM, there is evidence for an increase in the γ band power within PFC and the hippocampus during WM (Brzezicka et al., 2019), and imaging studies have shown an interaction between PFC and the hippocampus during WM tasks (Gluth et al., 2015; Calabro et al., 2020). Interestingly, similar interactions between PFC and the hippocampus are observed during the transformation of information from short-term memory to long-term memory during sleep (Helfrich et al., 2019) [Another version of the long term memory activation hypothesis suggests that prefrontal-parietal synchrony reflects an attentional pointer to information stored in long term memory (Ruchkin et al., 2003)]. Studies on the effect of hippocampal lesions on WM performance somewhat complicate the picture. Both human clinical studies (Spiers et al., 2001) and induced hippocampal lesions in monkeys (Zola et al., 2000) indicate that the hippocampus is not necessary for simple WM performance with short delays; however, there is evidence that the hippocampus contributes to short-term spatial memory (Jarrard, 1993; Kessels et al., 2001), and to more complex WM tasks requiring higher-order binding or associations (Yonelinas, 2013). This suggests that the prefrontal-hippocampal interactions observed during WM serve a purpose other than simple WM maintenance, perhaps contributing to maintaining more complex associations or bindings within WM, in addition to potentially reflecting the transfer of information to long term memory.

Dysfunctions in Oscillations and Synchrony During Working Memory Occur in Brain Disorders

If oscillations and synchrony are important for normal brain function, one might expect them to be disrupted in various mental disorders, and this is indeed the case (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2006). These changes in synchrony are important not only for understanding the mechanism of the underlying pathology, but also as a potential non-invasive biological diagnostic (Gandal et al., 2012), which may be detectable early in the disease process, and for developing treatments (Strüber and Herrmann, 2020). As a core cognitive function, the impairment of WM appears in many different disorders including schizophrenia (Park and Holzman, 1992; Goldman-Rakic, 1994; Roitman et al., 2000; Peled et al., 2001; Kim et al., 2003; Micheloyannis et al., 2006; Basar-Eroglu et al., 2007; Haenschel et al., 2007, 2009; Badcock et al., 2008; Pachou et al., 2008; Barr et al., 2010, 2017; Griesmayr et al., 2014; Lett et al., 2014; Wu et al., 2014; Pinal et al., 2015; Senkowski and Gallinat, 2015; Van Snellenberg et al., 2016; Kang et al., 2018; Ma et al., 2018; Appaji et al., 2020; Murphy N. et al., 2020), bipolar disorder (Wu et al., 2014; Appaji et al., 2020), autism spectrum disorder (Bangel et al., 2014; Rabiee et al., 2018), Parkinson’s disease (Siegert et al., 2008; Cools and D’Esposito, 2011; Zhao et al., 2018; Harrington et al., 2020), psychosis (Gold et al., 2019), Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Martinussen et al., 2005; Wolf et al., 2009; Matt Alderson et al., 2013; Bédard et al., 2014; Wu et al., 2017; Zammit et al., 2018; Jang et al., 2020), and depression (Shan et al., 2018). The role of synchronized oscillations in different brain disorders has been thoroughly examined elsewhere (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2006), and there is much recent interest in identifying non-invasive and quantitative signatures for different disorders (Başar, 2013; Nimmrich et al., 2015; Asllani et al., 2018). Here we provide a brief overview of some key lines of research related to changes of inter-areal synchrony in disorders affecting WM. Desynchronization across brain areas has been reported for schizophrenia (Peled et al., 2001; Kim et al., 2003; Micheloyannis et al., 2006; Haenschel et al., 2007; Pachou et al., 2008; Griesmayr et al., 2014; Lett et al., 2014), autism spectrum disorder (Bangel et al., 2014), Parkinson’s disease (Siegert et al., 2008), ADHD (Zammit et al., 2018; Jang et al., 2020) and psychosis (Hudgens-Haney et al., 2017). Abnormal cortical synchrony in PPL (measured with EEG) has been reported in schizophrenia during WM within dorsolateral prefrontal (Kang et al., 2018), posterior parietal (Kang et al., 2018), and visual cortices (Kang et al., 2018; for review see Brennan et al., 2013). Imaging-based connectivity measures showed lower connectivity between cortical areas in schizophrenia (Kim et al., 2003). Autism groups have less β band synchronization across multiple brain areas, as measured by MEG (Bangel et al., 2014). In the autism spectrum disorders and Williams syndrome there is reduced β band coherence and stronger γ band oscillations during perceptual tasks (Castelhano et al., 2015). ADHD is associated with a decrease in functional connectivity across prefrontal and parietal cortex (Wolf et al., 2009; Bédard et al., 2014). There is a significant different in θ band phase synchrony across the frontoparietal network in the ADHD group, measured by EEG (Jang et al., 2020). In an animal model of schizophrenia, globally administering an N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist, WM was impaired; this WM disruption was accompanied by enhanced α and low-γ band power, and dampening of the β band oscillations in the lPFC, both during the delay period and between trials (Ma et al., 2018). Additionally, WM deficits are accompanied by poor interregional synchrony in rodent models of schizophrenia (Sigurdsson et al., 2010). However, optogenetically inducing delta oscillations in the thalamic projection to the hippocampus impairs WM performance in rodents- so not all oscillatory manipulations are beneficial (Duan et al., 2015; Rahman et al., 2021). In summary, a variety of brain disorders characterized by WM impairments also show evidence of changes in synchronization between brain areas, supporting the hypothesis that such synchronization is important for WM performance.

Manipulation of Inter-Areal Synchrony Alters Working Memory Performance

Whether synchrony and oscillations have a role in information processing in the brain, or are primarily epiphenomenal, has long been a subject of debate. The best way to test the functional role of synchrony is through causal experiments that selectively alter synchronous activity across brain areas. This is most directly accomplished by simultaneously manipulating activity across multiple areas (although manipulations of one area sometimes have indirect effects on synchrony). In comparing the frequency of changes in WM performance for studies manipulating activity in just one vs. multiple areas (Table 3), we observe that those manipulating multiple areas more reliably impacted WM performance, consistent with an important functional role for inter-areal synchrony in WM performance. Effects of the manipulations on performance are summarized in Table 3, and effects on brain activity, oscillations, or synchrony in Table 4.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Causal manipulations of oscillations or synchrony and their effect on WM.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Causal manipulations of oscillations or synchrony and effect on neural measurements.

Manipulations which can alter oscillatory activity in a single area are sometimes sufficient to impact WM performance (Table 3), as well as modulating activity elsewhere in the brain (Table 4). Manipulations are carried out using a variety of techniques, as indicated in the third column of each table. Electrical stimulation with subdural electrodes over the superior frontal gyrus, in the same frequency range as endogenous activity during WM (θ -α), reduced subjects’ reaction times (Alagapan et al., 2019a). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) applies a low DC current to pairs of electrodes placed on the head. There are mixed results for the effect of tDCS over prefrontal cortex. Some studies reported that tDCS boosted WM performance when applied over the PFC (Fregni et al., 2005; Jones et al., 2015; Bourbon-Teles and Soto, 2019; Cespon et al., 2019; Wang S. et al., 2019; Boudewyn et al., 2020), while others showed no effect or reduced performance (Hoy et al., 2015; Hill et al., 2017; Ikeda et al., 2019; Luque-Casado et al., 2019; Wang S. et al., 2019; Murphy O. W. et al., 2020). Within sensory areas, tDCS of right fusiform regions involved in face representation and memory specifically enhanced WM performance for faces but not scenes (Brunyé et al., 2017). Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), which applies a high intensity magnetic pulse on the area under a coil, also has mixed impacts on WM performance. TMS over PFC can improve WM performance (Hamidi et al., 2008; Beynel et al., 2019; Guo et al., 2019; Barbosa et al., 2020), but not always (Zanto et al., 2011; Lee and D’Esposito, 2012; Lorenc et al., 2015). Another technique, transcranial alternative current stimulation (tACS), uses an AC current (at some specific frequency) rather than a DC current, allowing intentional targeting of specific oscillatory frequencies. With this method the frequency of stimulation can be chosen to match the frequency of intrinsic oscillations recorded at the site, which can increase the effect of the manipulation on performance (Hoy et al., 2015; Alekseichuk et al., 2016; Murphy O. W. et al., 2020).

In spite of the popularity of tDCS, tACS, and TMS, the exact effect of these types of stimulation on brain activity remains a topic of investigation (Liu et al., 2018). tACS can modulate oscillatory activity (Liu et al., 2018). tDCS increased neural excitability and spontaneous activity in the area under the anodal electrode (Rahman et al., 2013; Krause et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2018; Kunori and Takashima, 2019). TMS increased spiking activity and selectivity in the affected region (Pasley et al., 2009; Mueller et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2015; Romero et al., 2019). Note that although tDCS and TMS do not directly induce a particular oscillatory frequency based on the stimulation parameters, they can nevertheless sometimes produce frequency-specific changes in power within the stimulated area (Boudewyn et al., 2020) or elsewhere in the brain (Ni et al., 2016; Hill et al., 2017), and alter oscillatory synchrony between areas (Zanto et al., 2011; Jones et al., 2020). TMS of one area can also modulate processing elsewhere in the brain. TMS of PFC modulated the fidelity of information in visual cortex (as measured by patterns of fMRI activity) during a WM task (Lorenc et al., 2015). Disrupting PFC with TMS during memory encoding diminished top-down modulation of activity in posterior cortex during encoding, which predicted the subsequent decrement in WM accuracy (Zanto et al., 2011; Lee and D’Esposito, 2012). TMS over parietal cortex can reactivate the latent content of WM, as measured by performance and patterns of fMRI activity (Rose et al., 2016). These studies show that manipulation in one part of the network can be sufficient modulate neural activity and impact task performance.

Manipulation of neural activity in multiple areas has also been used to more directly control the synchrony between areas, with more reliable effects on behavioral performance. Synchronized θ band tACS of frontoparietal networks enhanced performance on a demanding verbal WM task (Violante et al., 2017). This stimulation increased parietal activity (measured via fMRI), which correlated with behavioral performance. Synchronous stimulation of both sites was critical for this effect; stimulation of prefrontal cortex alone did not improve performance in the same study (Violante et al., 2017), nor was performance improved by prefrontal or parietal θ or γ band stimulation in another study evaluating visual WM (Pahor and Jaušovec, 2018). Intracranial stimulation of two areas in the frontoparietal network showed a similar effect: in-phase stimulation decreased phase lag between areas and enhanced WM performance, but antiphase stimulation increased the phase lag between areas and had no effect on performance (Alagapan et al., 2019b). tDCS of the frontoparietal network enhanced WM performance and increased coupling between the θ band phase in PFC and the γ band amplitude in parietal cortex (Jones et al., 2020). Synchronized θ band stimulation of frontal and temporal areas increased subsequent synchrony between areas (measured with EEG), and enhanced WM performance in the elderly (Reinhart and Nguyen, 2019). Neither prefrontal nor temporal stimulation alone, nor asynchronous stimulation of both areas, improved performance in the same study. In another recent study, the time of stimulation was controlled relative to the phase of endogenous activity in another area (Berger et al., 2019). The timing of TMS stimulation of parietal cortex (relative to the phase of the θ oscillation in frontal cortex) determined whether it enhanced or suppressed parietal γ activity. Stimulation at the trough of the frontal θ oscillation enhanced the γ band activity and performance; stimulation at the θ peak had the opposite effect (Berger et al., 2019). With appropriate tACS intensity and frequency, synchronous activity across areas can be manipulated (Rahman et al., 2013; Krause et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2018). Lower frequency stimulation with higher field intensity can impose synchrony in the network (Liu et al., 2018), and some tACS has been shown to modulate the timing of spiking activity rather than firing rate in primates (Krause et al., 2019). As one example of the power of this approach, such modulation of synchrony can control epileptic activity (Bikson et al., 2001; Sunderam et al., 2006; Berényi et al., 2012; Desai et al., 2016; Asllani et al., 2018).

Although neurophysiological findings have recently shown modulations of β band synchrony during WM (Siegel et al., 2009; Salazar et al., 2012; Dotson et al., 2014; Lundqvist et al., 2016; Bastos et al., 2018; Zanos et al., 2018), and modulations in this band are often predictive of WM performance (Tallon-Baudry et al., 2004; Siegel et al., 2009; Mendoza-Halliday et al., 2014; Brincat and Miller, 2015; Antzoulatos and Miller, 2016; Bahmani et al., 2018; Lundqvist et al., 2018; Rezayat et al., 2021), this frequency range has yet to be used in stimulation studies examining WM. However, note that β band stimulation is common during motor tasks, reviewed in Wischnewski et al. (2019). Microstimulation or optogenetic manipulations in animals hold the potential to modulate synchrony in a more spatially precise manner, testing the role of synchrony in maintaining specific representations. Indeed, rodent studies have reported selective modulation of synchrony by optogenetic methods (Kidder et al., 2021; Quirk et al., 2021). Once we better understand the role of synchrony, the ability to selectively manipulate it could provide treatments for neural disorders (Sreeraj et al., 2019).

Role of Synchrony in Working Memory

A variety of functions have been proposed for inter-areal synchrony, which we briefly survey here before introducing our own interpretation specific to its role in WM. Synchrony between areas is widely hypothesized to alter the efficacy of communication or information transfer between them (Fries, 2015); this has been most extensively studied for the γ band (for review see Hahn et al., 2019). However, many neural signatures of WM involve coupling in the β or θ band (detailed above), and the role of these frequencies in modulating the efficacy of communication remains comparatively less explored. One possibility is that lower frequency synchronization can provide a temporal framework for higher frequency synchrony (Sauseng et al., 2009; Siebenhühner et al., 2016); PAC measures may reflect the nesting of γ band oscillations within lower frequency oscillations, as observed in WM studies (Maris et al., 2011; van der Meij et al., 2012; Jiang et al., 2015). For β band synchrony two main roles have been suggested (Miller et al., 2018). One is that β band synchrony links the phase of deep layers of PFC to the γ band oscillation in the superficial layers (Bastos et al., 2018). In this scenario the β band is a source of internal inhibitory control, and the power in the β and γ bands are anticorrelated (Lundqvist et al., 2016, 2018; Bastos et al., 2018). Another potential role of β oscillations, proposed based on modeling results, is that they underlie the formation of dispersed neuronal ensembles (Kopell et al., 2011), and ensemble activity could represent the content of WM. A third possible role of β oscillations is to help drive changes in synaptic weights: the timing of neural activity relative to β oscillations has been shown to affect synaptic plasticity (Zanos et al., 2018), and plasticity is the basis of activity-silent WM models (Mongillo et al., 2008; Stokes, 2015).

Here we introduce an overall framework for the interactions between PFC and visual cortex during WM (Figure 3), which explains a constellation of experimental results, including the close tie between signatures of inter-areal coordination and WM performance. A full description of the theoretical framework and all the relevant literature will require an entire separate review article; here we provide an outline of the key concepts and most relevant literature.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. A framework describing prefrontal-visual interactions and the functional role of coordinated oscillations during WM. Description moves counter-clockwise from upper left. WM-dependent spiking activity: spiking activity within PFC (red) represents the content of WM. This activity is sent from PFC to visual areas via direct projections (red projection), recruiting sensory areas. WM-induced distant oscillation: within visual areas (blue), the top-down WM input drives an αβ-frequency oscillation. Oscillation-dependent spiking activity: the combination of this αβ oscillation and a neuron’s sensitivity to sensory input will determine its spike timing relative to the local αβ oscillation. Spikes are sent from visual areas to PFC (blue projection). WM-induced local oscillation: WM activity within PFC also drives an αβ oscillation within PFC, which will be phase-locked with that in visual areas. Oscillation gates input efficacy: the phase of the αβ oscillation within PFC will gate the efficacy of visual input, providing a mechanism to preserve the information contained in spike timing relative to the oscillation. Visual inputs to PFC target visuomotor neurons, and evoked activity in PFC will in turn guide behaviors (for example, eye movements), with the net result that incoming stimuli matching the content of WM are more likely to influence behavior. This model is based on results that have been reported in one or more visual areas including V4, MT, and IT, for spatial or object WM (see text for references).

Overall, the emerging picture is that PFC spiking activity representing the content of WM is sent to visual areas during WM maintenance (Merrikhi et al., 2017). This PFC “persistent activity” sent to visual areas recruits them by increasing the power of αβ oscillations within those areas. The PFC-induced oscillation in visual areas modulates the likelihood and timing of action potentials based on the sensitivity of the neuron. This allows visual neurons to reflect their sensitivity to visual stimuli in the timing of their spikes relative to the local oscillation: more sensitive neurons generate spikes earlier than less sensitive neurons in response to the WM-induced oscillation. This sensory information in the timing of spikes can be read out via a phase code when considering the timing of spikes relative to the phase of αβ oscillations (Bahmani et al., 2018). This information, encoded in the phase of spikes relative to the WM-induced αβ oscillation, can be decoded by electrophysiologists recording the spiking and LFP activity, but how can it be conveyed to downstream areas which receive only spiking activity? In order to access the information encoded in spike timing downstream areas must also have an oscillatory reference. Multiple groups have reported an increase in PPL between oscillations in prefrontal and visual areas during WM, as reviewed above (e.g., Liebe et al., 2012; Mendoza-Halliday et al., 2014; Daume et al., 2017; Rezayat et al., 2021). It is likely that the same source (i.e., persistent PFC activity) that drives the αβ oscillation in visual areas also generates a similar, coherent oscillation in areas receiving input from those visual areas. Thus, in this perspective, the inter-areal PPL observed during WM is a signature of sharing a similar oscillatory frame of reference. Another study from our group, tracing the fate of visual input to the FEF during WM, provides a clue to a potential mechanism for reading out the information contained in the timing of incoming spikes: the efficacy of visual inputs to PFC neurons increases at the location held in WM (Noudoost et al., 2021). Building on this observation, we propose that the coherent oscillation in the receiving area allows dynamic gating of arriving spikes, such that spikes arriving at a certain phase will more effectively drive the post-synaptic neurons; indeed, such changes in the sensitivity of an area to input based on the phase of local oscillations have previously been reported, albeit in the gamma band (Cardin et al., 2009; Knoblich et al., 2010; Yonelinas, 2013; Ni et al., 2016), and are consistent with the phase coding observed within PFC during WM (Siegel et al., 2009). Thus, the phase locking between visual and prefrontal areas during WM (e.g., Rezayat et al., 2021) is a signature of a shared oscillatory frame of reference, which both controls the relative timing of spike generation in visual areas and dynamically gates visual input efficacy in prefrontal areas—encoding and decoding information, respectively, in spike timing relative to the oscillation. These modulations of spike timing within visual areas, in combination with a coherent oscillation in prefrontal areas, mean that incoming sensory stimuli matching the content of WM will be more likely to drive prefrontal activity, and thus to guide behavior. WM relies on this recruitment of sensory areas by prefrontal areas, and thus, having a shared oscillatory frame of reference between areas is critical for WM performance, as reviewed in this manuscript.

A schematic illustrating the key components of the proposed framework for the prefrontal recruitment of sensory areas during WM is shown in Figure 3. It should be noted that thinking within the suggested framework will refine our definitions of some concepts:

Representation is the sensitivity of a neuron to current input, including subthreshold modulations not visible in extracellular recordings (e.g., spiking activity). The WM-induced oscillation can facilitate the expression of that representation in the form of spiking activity for the purpose of inter-areal communication.

Sensory recruitment is the process of facilitating sensory areas to express their representation in the form of spiking activity, which enables WM to take advantage of these areas’ greater visual selectivity. In order to recruit extrastriate visual areas, the FEF part of prefrontal cortex directly sends these areas persistent WM-related activity, which drives an αβ oscillation within them.

Feedback: Although the projections from PFC to visual areas directly convey only spiking activity, notably including the content of WM, the purpose of this feedback is not merely to replicate that information in visual areas, but rather to drive a coherent oscillation between the two areas. This feedback-induced shared oscillatory frame of reference enables both phase-dependent encoding of visual information in visual areas, and decoding of that information using oscillation-dependent input efficacy in PFC.

As noted, we have limited ourselves to an overview of the framework and the associated interpretation of phase locking between areas; a full and detailed survey of the evidence for each component of the proposed model is beyond the scope of the current review. This framework provides an answer to several puzzles in the literature of sensory recruitment by WM (and more broadly the top-down control of sensory signals). Why would FEF, which does not have strong feature selectivity, show persistent activity during various forms of WM (Clark et al., 2012)? We propose that this FEF activity serves to drive a common oscillatory frame of reference in both V4 and FEF. Why do sensory areas with sufficient feature selectivity to satisfy WM requirements only show very weak modulations in their firing rate during memory maintenance? In this framework, the representational enhancement in these areas can only be traced in relation to WM-dependent oscillations. How would aligning spikes to a certain oscillation in visual areas benefit sensory processing, if that phase information is not sent along with spikes to the next area? We suggest that the area receiving visual input also has a copy of the phase reference, as evidenced by both the coherence of the oscillations, and the timing of visual spikes relative to the phase of PFC oscillations. Finally, of course, the framework offers an explanation for the main focus of this review- the close link between inter-areal coherence and WM performance.

Concluding Remarks

We have reviewed evidence that oscillatory coupling between areas is crucial for WM. A mechanistic framework for understanding the necessity of such coupling is briefly described (Figure 3). Certain aspects of the proposed framework have yet to be directly tested. For example, what is the circuit mechanism driving coherent oscillations in visual and prefrontal areas? Does the role of oscillations in controlling spike timing in visual cortex rely on the same cellular mechanism that gates the efficacy of inputs in prefrontal cortex? Do aspects of the proposed framework apply to prefrontal-visual interactions outside of WM? The answers to these questions hold the potential to transform our understanding of prefrontal control, sensory representation, and the role of inter-areal communication in cognitive tasks.

Author Contributions

ER, KC, and BN wrote the manuscript. M-RD and BN contributed to the development of ideas and discussion.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health grants R01EY02694, R01NS113073, and R01EY031477 to BN and an Unrestricted Grant from Research to Prevent Blindness, Inc., to Moran Eye Center, University of Utah.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Alagapan, S., Riddle, J., Huang, W. A., Hadar, E., Shin, H. W., and Fröhlich, F. (2019b). Network-targeted, multi-site direct cortical stimulation enhances working memory by modulating phase lag of low-frequency oscillations. Cell Rep. 29, 2590–2598.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.10.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alagapan, S., Lustenberger, C., Hadar, E., Shin, H. W., and Fröhlich, F. (2019a). Low-frequency direct cortical stimulation of left superior frontal gyrus enhances working memory performance. Neuroimage 184, 697–706. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alekseichuk, I., Turi, Z., de Lara, G. A., Antal, A., and Paulus, W. (2016). Spatial working memory in humans depends on theta and high gamma synchronization in the prefrontal cortex. Curr. Biol. 26, 1513–1521. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antzoulatos, E. G., and Miller, E. K. (2016). Synchronous beta rhythms of frontoparietal networks support only behaviorally relevant representations. eLife 5:e17822. doi: 10.7554/eLife.17822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appaji, A., Nagendra, B., Chako, D. M., Padmanabha, A., Jacob, A., Hiremath, C. V., et al. (2020). Relation between retinal vascular abnormalities and working memory impairment in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Asian J. Psychiatr. 49:101942. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.101942

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arif, Y., Spooner, R. K., Wiesman, A. I., Proskovec, A. L., Rezich, M. T., Heinrichs-Graham, E., et al. (2020). Prefrontal multielectrode transcranial direct current stimulation modulates performance and neural activity serving visuospatial processing. Cereb. Cortex 30, 4847–4857. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armstrong, K. M., Chang, M. H., and Moore, T. (2009). Selection and maintenance of spatial information by frontal eye field neurons. J. Neurosci. 29, 15621–15629. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4465-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asllani, M., Expert, P., and Carletti, T. (2018). A minimally invasive neurostimulation method for controlling abnormal synchronisation in the neuronal activity. PLoS Comput. Biol. 14:e1006296. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avena-Koenigsberger, A., Misic, B., and Sporns, O. (2018). Communication dynamics in complex brain networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 17–33. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axmacher, N., Henseler, M. M., Jensen, O., Weinreich, I., Elger, C. E., and Fell, J. (2010). Cross-frequency coupling supports multi-item working memory in the human hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 3228–3233. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911531107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axmacher, N., Schmitz, D. P., Wagner, T., Elger, C. E., and Fell, J. (2008). Interactions between medial temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, and inferior temporal regions during visual working memory: a combined intracranial EEG and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 28, 7304–7312. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1778-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Carducci, F., Vecchio, F., Rossi, S., Babiloni, F., Cincotti, F., et al. (2004). Functional frontoparietal connectivity during short-term memory as revealed by high-resolution EEG coherence analysis. Behav. Neurosci. 118, 687–697. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.4.687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badcock, J. C., Badcock, D. R., Read, C., and Jablensky, A. (2008). Examining encoding imprecision in spatial working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 100, 144–152. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bahmani, Z., Daliri, M. R., Merrikhi, Y., Clark, K., and Noudoost, B. (2018). Working memory enhances cortical representations via spatially specific coordination of spike times. Neuron 97, 967–979.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bangel, K. A., Batty, M., Ye, A. X., Meaux, E., Taylor, M. J., and Doesburg, S. M. (2014). Reduced beta band connectivity during number estimation in autism. Neuroimage Clin. 6, 202–213. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbosa, J., Stein, H., Martinez, R. L., Galan-Gadea, A., Li, S., Dalmau, J., et al. (2020). Interplay between persistent activity and activity-silent dynamics in the prefrontal cortex underlies serial biases in working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 1016–1024. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-0644-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barr, M. S., Farzan, F., Tran, L. C., Chen, R., Fitzgerald, P. B., and Daskalakis, Z. J. (2010). Evidence for excessive frontal evoked gamma oscillatory activity in schizophrenia during working memory. Schizophr. Res. 121, 146–152. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.05.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barr, M. S., Rajji, T. K., Zomorrodi, R., Radhu, N., George, T. P., Blumberger, D. M., et al. (2017). Impaired theta-gamma coupling during working memory performance in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 189, 104–110. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.01.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Başar, E. (2013). Brain oscillations in neuropsychiatric disease. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 15, 291–300. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2013.15.3/ebasar

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basar-Eroglu, C., Brand, A., Hildebrandt, H., Kedzior, K. K., Mathes, B., and Schmiedt, C. (2007). Working memory related gamma oscillations in schizophrenia patients. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 64, 39–45. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastos, A. M., Loonis, R., Kornblith, S., Lundqvist, M., and Miller, E. K. (2018). Laminar recordings in frontal cortex suggest distinct layers for maintenance and control of working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, 1117–1122. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1710323115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bédard, A.-C. V., Newcorn, J. H., Clerkin, S. M., Krone, B., Fan, J., Halperin, J. M., et al. (2014). Reduced prefrontal efficiency for visuospatial working memory in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 53, 1020–1030.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berényi, A., Belluscio, M., Mao, D., and Buzsáki, G. (2012). Closed-loop control of epilepsy by transcranial electrical stimulation. Science 337, 735–737. doi: 10.1126/science.1223154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, B., Griesmayr, B., Minarik, T., Biel, A. L., Pinal, D., Sterr, A., et al. (2019). Dynamic regulation of interregional cortical communication by slow brain oscillations during working memory. Nat. Commun. 10:4242. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-12057-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beynel, L., Davis, S. W., Crowell, C. A., Hilbig, S. A., Lim, W., Nguyen, D., et al. (2019). Online repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation during working memory in younger and older adults: a randomized within-subject comparison. PLoS One 14:e0213707. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bikson, M., Lian, J., Hahn, P. J., Stacey, W. C., Sciortino, C., and Durand, D. M. (2001). Suppression of epileptiform activity by high frequency sinusoidal fields in rat hippocampal slices. J. Physiol. 531, 181–191. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.0181j.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boran, E., Fedele, T., Steiner, A., Hilfiker, P., Stieglitz, L., Grunwald, T., et al. (2020). Dataset of human medial temporal lobe neurons, scalp and intracranial EEG during a verbal working memory task. Sci. Data 7:30. doi: 10.1038/s41597-020-0364-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boudewyn, M. A., Scangos, K., Ranganath, C., and Carter, C. S. (2020). Using prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to enhance proactive cognitive control in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 45, 1877–1883. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-0750-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourbon-Teles, J., and Soto, D. (2019). Assessing the role of the left dorsal frontal cortex in working memory guidance: Attentional or Mnemonic? A neurostimulation study. Neuroscience 411, 140–149.

Google Scholar

Brennan, A. M., Harris, A. W. F., and Williams, L. M. (2013). Functional dysconnectivity in schizophrenia and its relationship to neural synchrony. Expert Rev. Neurother. 13, 755–765. doi: 10.1586/14737175.2013.811899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brincat, S. L., and Miller, E. K. (2015). Frequency-specific hippocampal-prefrontal interactions during associative learning. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 576–581. doi: 10.1038/nn.3954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brody, C. D., Hernández, A., Zainos, A., and Romo, R. (2003). Timing and neural encoding of somatosensory parametric working memory in macaque prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1196–1207. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruns, A., and Eckhorn, R. (2004). Task-related coupling from high- to low-frequency signals among visual cortical areas in human subdural recordings. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 51, 97–116. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunyé, T. T., Moran, J. M., Holmes, A., Mahoney, C. R., and Taylor, H. A. (2017). Non-invasive brain stimulation targeting the right fusiform gyrus selectively increases working memory for faces. Brain Cogn. 113, 32–39. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brzezicka, A., Kamiński, J., Reed, C. M., Chung, J. M., Mamelak, A. N., and Rutishauser, U. (2019). Working memory load-related theta power decreases in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex predict individual differences in performance. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 31, 1290–1307. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01417

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buschman, T. J., Siegel, M., Roy, J. E., and Miller, E. K. (2011). Neural substrates of cognitive capacity limitations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 11252–11255. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1104666108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G., Anastassiou, C. A., and Koch, C. (2012). The origin of extracellular fields and currents–EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 407–420. doi: 10.1038/nrn3241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calabro, F. J., Murty, V. P., Jalbrzikowski, M., Tervo-Clemmens, B., and Luna, B. (2020). Development of hippocampal-prefrontal cortex interactions through adolescence. Cereb. Cortex 30, 1548–1558. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canavier, C. C. (2015). Phase-resetting as a tool of information transmission. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 31, 206–213. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canolty, R. T., Ganguly, K., Kennerley, S. W., Cadieu, C. F., Koepsell, K., Wallis, J. D., et al. (2010). Oscillatory phase coupling coordinates anatomically dispersed functional cell assemblies. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 17356–17361. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008306107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardin, J. A., Carlén, M., Meletis, K., Knoblich, U., Zhang, F., Deisseroth, K., et al. (2009). Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature 459, 663–667. doi: 10.1038/nature08002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castelhano, J., Bernardino, I., Rebola, J., Rodriguez, E., and Castelo-Branco, M. (2015). Oscillations or Synchrony? Disruption of neural synchrony despite enhanced gamma oscillations in a model of disrupted perceptual coherence. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 2416–2426. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00863

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, S. E., Towers, J. P., Wallis, J. D., Hunt, L. T., and Kennerley, S. W. (2018). Reconciling persistent and dynamic hypotheses of working memory coding in prefrontal cortex. Nat. Commun. 9:3498. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05873-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cespon, J., Rodella, C., Miniussi, C., and Pellicciari, M. C. (2019). Behavioural and electrophysiological modulations induced by transcranial direct current stimulation in healthy elderly and Alzheimer’s disease patients: a pilot study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 130, 2038–2052. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christophel, T. B., Klink, P. C., Spitzer, B., Roelfsema, P. R., and Haynes, J. D. (2017). The distributed nature of working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 111–124. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, K. L., Noudoost, B., and Moore, T. (2012). Persistent spatial information in the frontal eye field during object-based short-term memory. J. Neurosci. 32, 10907–10914. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1450-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Constantinidis, C., Funahashi, S., Lee, D., Murray, J. D., Qi, X.-L., Wang, M., et al. (2018). Persistent spiking activity underlies working memory. J. Neurosci. 38, 7020–7028. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2486-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cools, R., and D’Esposito, M. (2011). Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol. Psychiatry 69, e113–e125. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Courtney, S. M. (2008). An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex. Science 279, 1347–1351. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5355.1347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daume, J., Gruber, T., Engel, A. K., and Friese, U. (2017). Phase-amplitude coupling and long-range phase synchronization reveal frontotemporal interactions during visual working memory. J. Neurosci. 37, 313–322. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2130-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dedoncker, J., Brunoni, A. R., Baeken, C., and Vanderhasselt, M.-A. (2016). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in healthy and neuropsychiatric samples: influence of stimulation parameters. Brain Stimul. 9, 501–517. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deiber, M. P., Missonnier, P., Bertrand, O., Gold, G., Fazio-Costa, L., Ibañez, V., et al. (2007). Distinction between perceptual and attentional processing in working memory tasks: a study of phase-locked and induced oscillatory brain dynamics. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 158–172. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desai, S. A., Rolston, J. D., McCracken, C. E., Potter, S. M., and Gross, R. E. (2016). Asynchronous distributed multielectrode microstimulation reduces seizures in the dorsal tetanus toxin model of temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Stimul. 9, 86–100. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dotson, N. M., Hoffman, S. J., Goodell, B., and Gray, C. M. (2018). Feature-based visual short-term memory is widely distributed and hierarchically organized. Neuron 99, 215–226. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.05.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dotson, N. M., Salazar, R. F., and Gray, C. M. (2014). Frontoparietal correlation dynamics reveal interplay between integration and segregation during visual working memory. J. Neurosci. 34, 13600–13613. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1961-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duan, A. R., Varela, C., Zhang, Y., Shen, Y., Xiong, L., Wilson, M. A., et al. (2015). Delta frequency optogenetic stimulation of the thalamic nucleus reuniens is sufficient to produce working memory deficits: relevance to schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 1098–1107. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksson, J., Vogel, E. K., Lansner, A., Bergström, F., and Nyberg, L. (2015). Neurocognitive architecture of working memory. Neuron 88, 33–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fell, J., and Axmacher, N. (2011). The role of phase synchronization in memory processes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 105–118. doi: 10.1038/nrn2979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Figueroa-Vargas, A., Cárcamo, C., Henríquez-Ch, R., Zamorano, F., Ciampi, E., Uribe-San-Martin, R., et al. (2020). Frontoparietal connectivity correlates with working memory performance in multiple sclerosis. Sci. Rep. 10:9310. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-66279-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freedman, D. J., and Assad, J. A. (2006). Experience-dependent representation of visual categories in parietal cortex. Nature 443, 85–88. doi: 10.1038/nature05078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fregni, F., Boggio, P. S., Nitsche, M., Bermpohl, F., Antal, A., Feredoes, E., et al. (2005). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enhances working memory. Exp. Brain Res. 166, 23–30. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2334-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fries, P. (2005). A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fries, P. (2015). Rhythms for cognition: communication through coherence. Neuron 88, 220–235. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Bastos, A. M., Pinotsis, D., and Litvak, V. (2015). LFP and oscillations-what do they tell us? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 31, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. J., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989). Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey’s dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 61, 331–349. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.61.2.331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandal, M. J., Edgar, J. C., Klook, K., and Siegel, S. J. (2012). Gamma synchrony: towards a translational biomarker for the treatment-resistant symptoms of schizophrenia. Neuropharmacology 62, 1504–1518. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gazzaniga, M., and Ivry, R. B. (2013). Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind: Fourth International Student Edition. New York, NY: WW Norton.

Google Scholar

Gehrig, J., Michalareas, G., Forster, M.-T., Lei, J., Hok, P., Laufs, H., et al. (2019). Low-frequency oscillations code speech during verbal working memory. J. Neurosci. 39, 6498–6512. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0018-19.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gluth, S., Sommer, T., Rieskamp, J., and Büchel, C. (2015). Effective connectivity between hippocampus and ventromedial prefrontal cortex controls preferential choices from memory. Neuron 86, 1078–1090. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gold, J. M., Barch, D. M., Feuerstahler, L. M., Carter, C. S., MacDonald, A. W., Ragland, J. D., et al. (2019). Working memory impairment across psychotic disorders. Schizophr. Bull. 45, 804–812. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sby134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1994). Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 6, 348–357.

Google Scholar

Griesmayr, B., Berger, B., Stelzig-Schoeler, R., Aichhorn, W., Bergmann, J., and Sauseng, P. (2014). EEG theta phase coupling during executive control of visual working memory investigated in individuals with schizophrenia and in healthy controls. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 14, 1340–1355. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0272-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, Z., Jiang, Z., Jiang, B., McClure, M. A., and Mu, Q. (2019). High-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation could improve impaired working memory induced by sleep deprivation. Neural Plast. 2019:7030286. doi: 10.1155/2019/7030286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haenschel, C., Bittner, R. A., Waltz, J., Haertling, F., Wibral, M., Singer, W., et al. (2009). Cortical oscillatory activity is critical for working memory as revealed by deficits in early-onset schizophrenia. J. Neurosci. 29, 9481–9489. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1428-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haenschel, C., Uhlhaas, P. J., and Singer, W. (2007). Synchronous oscillatory activity and working memory in schizophrenia. Pharmacopsychiatry 40, S54—-S61.

Google Scholar

Hahn, G., Ponce-Alvarez, A., Deco, G., Aertsen, A., and Kumar, A. (2019). Portraits of communication in neuronal networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 117–127. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0094-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haller, M., Case, J., Crone, N. E., Chang, E. F., King-Stephens, D., Laxer, K. D., et al. (2018). Persistent neuronal activity in human prefrontal cortex links perception and action. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 80–91. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0267-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamidi, M., Tononi, G., and Postle, B. R. (2008). Evaluating frontal and parietal contributions to spatial working memory with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Res. 1230, 202–210. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrington, D. L., Shen, Q., Vincent Filoteo, J., Litvan, I., Huang, M., Castillo, G. N., et al. (2020). Abnormal distraction and load-specific connectivity during working memory in cognitively normal Parkinson’s disease. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 1195–1211. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helfrich, R. F., and Knight, R. T. (2016). Oscillatory dynamics of prefrontal cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 916–930. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helfrich, R. F., Lendner, J. D., Mander, B. A., Guillen, H., Paff, M., Mnatsakanyan, L., et al. (2019). Bidirectional prefrontal-hippocampal dynamics organize information transfer during sleep in humans. Nat. Commun. 10:3572. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-11444-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helfrich, R. F., Schneider, T. R., Rach, S., Trautmann-Lengsfeld, S. A., Engel, A. K., and Herrmann, C. S. (2014). Entrainment of brain oscillations by transcranial alternating current stimulation. Curr. Biol. 24, 333–339. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, A. T., Rogasch, N. C., Fitzgerald, P. B., and Hoy, K. E. (2017). Effects of prefrontal bipolar and high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation on cortical reactivity and working memory in healthy adults. Neuroimage 152, 142–157. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, M. W., Rizzuto, D. S., Caplan, J. B., Madsen, J. R., Lisman, J., Aschenbrenner-Scheibe, R., et al. (2003). Gamma oscillations correlate with working memory load in humans. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1369–1374. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoy, K. E., Bailey, N., Arnold, S., Windsor, K., John, J., Daskalakis, Z. J., et al. (2015). The effect of γ-tACS on working memory performance in healthy controls. Brain Cogn. 101, 51–56. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y., Matysiak, A., Heil, P., König, R., and Brosch, M. (2016). Persistent neural activity in auditory cortex is related to auditory working memory in humans and nonhuman primates. eLife 5:e15441. doi: 10.7554/eLife.15441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hudgens-Haney, M. E., Ethridge, L. E., Knight, J. B., McDowell, J. E., Keedy, S. K., Pearlson, G. D., et al. (2017). Intrinsic neural activity differences among psychotic illnesses. Psychophysiology 54, 1223–1238. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hussar, C., and Pasternak, T. (2010). Trial-to-trial variability of the prefrontal neurons reveals the nature of their engagement in a motion discrimination task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 21842–21847. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009956107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ikeda, T., Takahashi, T., Hiraishi, H., Saito, D. N., and Kikuchi, M. (2019). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation induces high gamma-band activity in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during a working memory task: a double-blind, randomized, crossover study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:136. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacob, S. N., Hähnke, D., and Nieder, A. (2018). Structuring of abstract working memory content by fronto-parietal synchrony in primate cortex. Neuron 99, 588–597.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacob, S. N., and Nieder, A. (2014). Complementary roles for primate frontal and parietal cortex in guarding working memory from distractor stimuli. Neuron 83, 226–237. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobsen, C. F., and Nissen, H. W. (1937). Studies of cerebral function in primates. IV. The effects of frontal lobe lesions on the delayed alternation habit in monkeys. J. Comp. Psychol. 23, 101-112.

Google Scholar

Jang, K.-M., Kim, M.-S., and Kim, D.-W. (2020). The dynamic properties of a brain network during spatial working memory tasks in college students with ADHD Traits. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:580813. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.580813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarrard, L. E. (1993). On the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat. Behav. Neural Biol. 60, 9–26. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(93)90664-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Lisman, J. E. (2005). Hippocampal sequence-encoding driven by a cortical multi-item working memory buffer. Trends Neurosci. 28, 67–72. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, H., Bahramisharif, A., van Gerven, M. A. J., and Jensen, O. (2015). Measuring directionality between neuronal oscillations of different frequencies. Neuroimage 118, 359–367. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, E. L., Adams, J. N., Solbakk, A.-K., Endestad, T., Larsson, P. G., Ivanovic, J., et al. (2018a). Dynamic frontotemporal systems process space and time in working memory. PLoS Biol. 16:e2004274. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2004274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, E. L., King-Stephens, D., Weber, P. B., Laxer, K. D., Lin, J. J., and Knight, R. T. (2018b). Spectral imprints of working memory for everyday associations in the frontoparietal network. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 12:65. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2018.00065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, K. T., Johnson, E. L., and Berryhill, M. E. (2020). Frontoparietal theta-gamma interactions track working memory enhancement with training and tDCS. Neuroimage 211:116615. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, K. T., Stephens, J. A., Alam, M., Bikson, M., and Berryhill, M. E. (2015). Longitudinal neurostimulation in older adults improves working memory. PLoS One 10:e0121904. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kambara, T., Brown, E. C., Jeong, J.-W., Ofen, N., Nakai, Y., and Asano, E. (2017). Spatio-temporal dynamics of working memory maintenance and scanning of verbal information. Clin. Neurophysiol. 128, 882–891. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kambara, T., Brown, E. C., Silverstein, B. H., Nakai, Y., and Asano, E. (2018). Neural dynamics of verbal working memory in auditory description naming. Sci. Rep. 8:15868. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-33776-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamiński, J., Sullivan, S., Chung, J. M., Ross, I. B., Mamelak, A. N., and Rutishauser, U. (2017). Persistently active neurons in human medial frontal and medial temporal lobe support working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 590–601. doi: 10.1038/nn.4509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, S. S., MacDonald, A. W. III, Chafee, M. V., Im, C.-H., Bernat, E. M., Davenport, N. D., et al. (2018). Abnormal cortical neural synchrony during working memory in schizophrenia. Clin. Neurophysiol. 129, 210–221. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.10.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessels, R. P., de Haan, E. H., Kappelle, L. J., and Postma, A. (2001). Varieties of human spatial memory: a meta-analysis on the effects of hippocampal lesions. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 35, 295–303. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(01)00058-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khursheed, F., Tandon, N., Tertel, K., Pieters, T. A., Disano, M. A., and Ellmore, T. M. (2011). Frequency-specific electrocorticographic correlates of working memory delay period fMRI activity. Neuroimage 56, 1773–1782. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kidder, K. S., Miles, J. T., Baker, P. M., Hones, V. I., Gire, D. H., and Mizumori, S. J. Y. (2021). A selective role for the mPFC during choice and deliberation, but not spatial memory retention over short delays. Hippocampus 31, 690–700. doi: 10.1002/hipo.23306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J.-J., Kwon, J. S., Park, H. J., Youn, T., Kang, D. H., Kim, M. S., et al. (2003). Functional disconnection between the prefrontal and parietal cortices during working memory processing in schizophrenia: a[15(O)]H2O PET study. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 919–923. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.5.919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, T., Allen, E. A., Pasley, B. N., and Freeman, R. D. (2015). Transcranial magnetic stimulation changes response selectivity of neurons in the visual cortex. Brain Stimul. 8, 613–623. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.01.407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knoblich, U., Siegle, J. H., Pritchett, D. L., and Moore, C. I. (2010). What do we gain from gamma? Local dynamic gain modulation drives enhanced efficacy and efficiency of signal transmission. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:185. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kojima, S., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1982). Delay-related activity of prefrontal neurons in rhesus monkeys performing delayed response. Brain Res. 248, 43–50. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)91145-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopell, N., Whittington, M. A., and Kramer, M. A. (2011). Neuronal assembly dynamics in the beta1 frequency range permits short-term memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 3779–3784. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1019676108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kornblith, S., Buschman, T. J., and Miller, E. K. (2016). Stimulus load and oscillatory activity in higher cortex. Cereb. Cortex 26, 3772–3784. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kornblith, S., Quian Quiroga, R., Koch, C., Fried, I., and Mormann, F. (2017). Persistent single-neuron activity during working memory in the human medial temporal lobe. Curr. Biol. 27, 1026–1032. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause, M. R., Vieira, P. G., Csorba, B. A., Pilly, P. K., and Pack, C. C. (2019). Transcranial alternating current stimulation entrains single-neuron activity in the primate brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 5747–5755. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1815958116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause, M. R., Zanos, T. P., Csorba, B. A., Pilly, P. K., Choe, J., Phillips, M. E., et al. (2017). Transcranial direct current stimulation facilitates associative learning and alters functional connectivity in the primate brain. Curr. Biol. 27, 3086–3096. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunori, N., and Takashima, I. (2019). Evaluation of acute anodal direct current stimulation-induced effects on somatosensory-evoked responses in the rat. Brain Res. 1720, 146318. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2019.146318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leavitt, M. L., Mendoza-Halliday, D., and Martinez-Trujillo, J. C. (2017). Sustained activity encoding working memories: not fully distributed. Trends Neurosci. 40, 328–346. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H., Simpson, G. V., Logothetis, N. K., and Rainer, G. (2005). Phase locking of single neuron activity to theta oscillations during working memory in monkey extrastriate visual cortex. Neuron 45, 147–156. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, T. G., and D’Esposito, M. (2012). The dynamic nature of top-down signals originating from prefrontal cortex: a combined fMRI-TMS study. J. Neurosci. 32, 15458–15466. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0627-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leszczyński, M., Fell, J., and Axmacher, N. (2015). Rhythmic working memory activation in the human hippocampus. Cell Rep. 13, 1272–1282. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.09.081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lett, T. A., Voineskos, A. N., Kennedy, J. L., Levine, B., and Daskalakis, Z. J. (2014). Treating working memory deficits in schizophrenia: a review of the neurobiology. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 361–370. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.07.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liebe, S., Hoerzer, G. M., Logothetis, N. K., and Rainer, G. (2012). Theta coupling between V4 and prefrontal cortex predicts visual short-term memory performance. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 456–462. doi: 10.1038/nn.3038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, A., Vöröslakos, M., Kronberg, G., Henin, S., Krause, M. R., Huang, Y., et al. (2018). Immediate neurophysiological effects of transcranial electrical stimulation. Nat. Commun. 9:5092. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-07233-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loaiza, V. M., and Halse, S. C. (2019). Where working memory meets long-term memory: the interplay of list length and distractors on memory performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 45, 1455–1472. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenc, E. S., Lee, T. G., Chen, A. J.-W., and D’Esposito, M. (2015). The effect of disruption of prefrontal cortical function with transcranial magnetic stimulation on visual working memory. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:169. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luczak, A., Bartho, P., and Harris, K. D. (2013). Gating of sensory input by spontaneous cortical activity. J. Neurosci. 33, 1684–1695. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2928-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundqvist, M., Herman, P., Warden, M. R., Brincat, S. L., and Miller, E. K. (2018). Gamma and beta bursts during working memory readout suggest roles in its volitional control. Nat. Commun. 9:394. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02791-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundqvist, M., Rose, J., Herman, P., Brincat, S. L. L., Buschman, T. J. J., and Miller, E. K. K. (2016). Gamma and beta bursts underlie working memory. Neuron 90, 152–164. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luque-Casado, A., Fogelson, N., Iglesias-Soler, E., and Fernandez-del-Olmo, M. (2019). Exploring the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation over the prefrontal cortex on working memory: a cluster analysis approach. Behav. Brain Res. 375, 112144. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, L., Skoblenick, K., Johnston, K., and Everling, S. (2018). Ketamine alters lateral prefrontal oscillations in a rule-based working memory task. J. Neurosci. 38, 2482–2494. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2659-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackey, W. E., and Curtis, C. E. (2017). Distinct contributions by frontal and parietal cortices support working memory. Sci. Rep. 7:6188. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-06293-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maris, E., van Vugt, M., and Kahana, M. (2011). Spatially distributed patterns of oscillatory coupling between high-frequency amplitudes and low-frequency phases in human iEEG. Neuroimage 54, 836–850. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinussen, R., Hayden, J., Hogg-Johnson, S., and Tannock, R. (2005). A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 44, 377–384. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000153228.72591.73

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matt Alderson, R., Kasper, L. J., Hudec, K. L., and Patros, C. H. G. (2013). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and working memory in adults: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 27, 287–302. doi: 10.1037/a0032371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendoza-Halliday, D., Torres, S., and Martinez-Trujillo, J. C. (2014). Sharp emergence of feature-selective sustained activity along the dorsal visual pathway. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1255–1262. doi: 10.1038/nn.3785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merrikhi, Y., Clark, K., Albarran, E., Parsa, M., Zirnsak, M., Moore, T., et al. (2017). Spatial working memory alters the efficacy of input to visual cortex. Nat. Commun. 8:15041. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyers, E. M., Qi, X. L., and Constantinidis, C. (2012). Incorporation of new information into prefrontal cortical activity after learning working memory tasks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 4651–4656. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1201022109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Micheloyannis, S., Pachou, E., Stam, C. J., Breakspear, M., Bitsios, P., Vourkas, M., et al. (2006). Small-world networks and disturbed functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 87, 60–66. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.06.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., Erickson, C. A., and Desimone, R. (1996). Neural mechanisms of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex of the macaque. J. Neurosci. 16, 5154–5167. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-16-05154.1996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., Lundqvist, M., and Bastos, A. M. (2018). Working Memory 2.0. Neuron 100, 463–475. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mongillo, G., Barak, O., and Tsodyks, M. (2008). Synaptic theory of working memory. Science 319, 1543–1546. doi: 10.1126/science.1150769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mueller, J. K., Grigsby, E. M., Prevosto, V., Petraglia, F. W., Rao, H., Deng, Z.-D., et al. (2014). Simultaneous transcranial magnetic stimulation and single-neuron recording in alert non-human primates. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1130–1136. doi: 10.1038/nn.3751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, N., Ramakrishnan, N., Walker, C. P., Polizzotto, N. R., and Cho, R. Y. (2020). Intact auditory cortical cross-frequency coupling in early and chronic schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry 11:507. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, O. W., Hoy, K. E., Wong, D., Bailey, N. W., Fitzgerald, P. B., and Segrave, R. A. (2020). Transcranial random noise stimulation is more effective than transcranial direct current stimulation for enhancing working memory in healthy individuals: behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. Brain Stimul. 13, 1370–1380. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2020.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myroshnychenko, M., Seamans, J. K., Phillips, A. G., and Lapish, C. C. (2017). Temporal dynamics of hippocampal and medial prefrontal cortex interactions during the delay period of a working memory-guided foraging task. Cereb. Cortex 27, 5331–5342. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura, K., and Kubota, K. (1995). Mnemonic firing of neurons in the monkey temporal pole during a visual recognition memory task. J. Neurophysiol. 74, 162–178. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.74.1.162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ni, B., Wu, R., Yu, T., Zhu, H., Li, Y., and Liu, Z. (2017). Role of the hippocampus in distinct memory traces: timing of match and mismatch enhancement revealed by intracranial recording. Neurosci. Bull. 33, 664–674. doi: 10.1007/s12264-017-0172-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ni, J., Wunderle, T., Lewis, C. M., Desimone, R., Diester, I., and Fries, P. (2016). Gamma-rhythmic gain modulation. Neuron 92, 240–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niki, H. (1974). Prefrontal unit activity during delayed alternation in the monkey. I. Relation to direction of response. Brain Res. 68, 185–196. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(74)90388-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nimmrich, V., Draguhn, A., and Axmacher, N. (2015). Neuronal network oscillations in neurodegenerative diseases. Neuromol. Med. 17, 270–284. doi: 10.1007/s12017-015-8355-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noudoost, B., Clark, K. L., and Moore, T. (2021). Working memory gates visual input to primate prefrontal neurons. eLife 10:e64814. doi: 10.7554/eLife.64814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noy, N., Bickel, S., Zion-Golumbic, E., Harel, M., Golan, T., Davidesco, I., et al. (2015). Intracranial recordings reveal transient response dynamics during information maintenance in human cerebral cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 3988–4003. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olesen, P. J., Westerberg, H., and Klingberg, T. (2004). Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 75–79. doi: 10.1038/nn1165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pachou, E., Vourkas, M., Simos, P., Smit, D., Stam, C. J., Tsirka, V., et al. (2008). Working memory in schizophrenia: an EEG study using power spectrum and coherence analysis to estimate cortical activation and network behavior. Brain Topogr. 21, 128–137. doi: 10.1007/s10548-008-0062-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pahor, A., and Jaušovec, N. (2018). The effects of theta and gamma tacs on working memory and electrophysiology. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:651. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S., and Holzman, P. S. (1992). Schizophrenics show spatial working memory deficits. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 49, 975–982. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820120063009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parthasarathy, A., Herikstad, R., Bong, J. H., Medina, F. S., Libedinsky, C., and Yen, S.-C. (2017). Mixed selectivity morphs population codes in prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1770–1779. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0003-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pasley, B. N., Allen, E. A., and Freeman, R. D. (2009). State-dependent variability of neuronal responses to transcranial magnetic stimulation of the visual cortex. Neuron 62, 291–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pasternak, T., and Greenlee, M. W. (2005). Working memory in primate sensory systems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 97–107. doi: 10.1038/nrn1603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peled, A., Geva, A. B., Kremen, W. S., Blankfeld, H. M., Esfandiarfard, R., and Nordahl, T. E. (2001). Functional connectivity and working memory in schizophrenia: an EEG study. Int. J. Neurosci. 106, 47–61. doi: 10.3109/00207450109149737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pesaran, B., Nelson, M. J., and Andersen, R. A. (2008). Free choice activates a decision circuit between frontal and parietal cortex. Nature 453, 406–409. doi: 10.1038/nature06849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pesaran, B., Pezaris, J. S., Sahani, M., Mitra, P. P., and Andersen, R. A. (2002). Temporal structure in neuronal activity during working memory in macaque parietal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 805–811. doi: 10.1038/nn890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pessoa, L., Gutierrez, E., Bandettini, P., and Ungerleider, L. (2002). Neural correlates of visual working memory: fMRI amplitude predicts task performance. Neuron 35, 975–987. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00817-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piaget, J. (1964). Part I: Cognitive development in children: Piaget development and learning. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2, 176–186. doi: 10.1002/tea.3660020306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinal, D., Zurrón, M., Díaz, F., and Sauseng, P. (2015). Stuck in default mode: inefficient cross-frequency synchronization may lead to age-related short-term memory decline. Neurobiol. Aging 36, 1611–1618. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pipa, G., Städtler, E. S., Rodriguez, E. F., Waltz, J. A., Muckli, L. F., Singer, W., et al. (2009). Performance- and stimulus-dependent oscillations in monkey prefrontal cortex during short-term memory. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 3:25. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.025.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polanía, R., Nitsche, M. A., Korman, C., Batsikadze, G., and Paulus, W. (2012). The importance of timing in segregated theta phase-coupling for cognitive performance. Curr. Biol. 22, 1314–1318. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qi, X.-L., Meyer, T., Stanford, T. R., and Constantinidis, C. (2011). Changes in prefrontal neuronal activity after learning to perform a spatial working memory task. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2722–2732. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quirk, C. R., Zutshi, I., Srikanth, S., Fu, M. L., Devico Marciano, N., Wright, M. K., et al. (2021). Precisely timed theta oscillations are selectively required during the encoding phase of memory. Nat. Neurosci. 24, 1614–1627. doi: 10.1038/s41593-021-00919-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabiee, A., Vasaghi-Gharamaleki, B., Samadi, S. A., Amiri-Shavaki, Y., Alaghband-Rad, J., Seyedin, S., et al. (2018). Impaired nonverbal working memory in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Med. J. Islam. Repub. Iran 32:107. doi: 10.14196/mjiri.32.107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahman, A., Reato, D., Arlotti, M., Gasca, F., Datta, A., Parra, L. C., et al. (2013). Cellular effects of acute direct current stimulation: somatic and synaptic terminal effects. J. Physiol. 591, 2563–2578. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.247171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahman, F., Nanu, R., Schneider, N. A., Katz, D., Lisman, J., and Pi, H.-J. (2021). Optogenetic perturbation of projections from thalamic nucleus reuniens to hippocampus disrupts spatial working memory retrieval more than encoding. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 179:107396. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2021.107396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinhart, R. M. G., and Nguyen, J. A. (2019). Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 820–827. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0371-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rezayat, E., Dehaqani, M.-R. A., Clark, K., Bahmani, Z., Moore, T., and Noudoost, B. (2021). Frontotemporal coordination predicts working memory performance and its local neural signatures. Nat. Commun. 12:1103. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-21151-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, S., and Cowan, N. (2019). Flexible representations in visual working memory and interactions with long-term learning: commentary on the special issue. Br. J. Psychol. 110, 449–460. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rigotti, M., Barak, O., Warden, M. R., Wang, X. J., Daw, N. D., Miller, E. K., et al. (2013). The importance of mixed selectivity in complex cognitive tasks. Nature 497, 585–590. doi: 10.1038/nature12160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roitman, S. E., Mitropoulou, V., Keefe, R. S., Silverman, J. M., Serby, M., Harvey, P. D., et al. (2000). Visuospatial working memory in schizotypal personality disorder patients. Schizophr. Res. 41, 447–455. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00085-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romero, M. C., Davare, M., Armendariz, M., and Janssen, P. (2019). Neural effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation at the single-cell level. Nat. Commun. 10:2642. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-10638-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, N. S., Larocque, J. J., Riggall, A. C., Gosseries, O., Starrett, M. J., Meyering, E. E., et al. (2016). Reactivation of latent working memories with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Science 354, 1136–1140. doi: 10.1126/science.aah7011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruchkin, D. S., Grafman, J., Cameron, K., and Berndt, R. S. (2003). Working memory retention systems: a state of activated long-term memory. Behav. Brain Sci. 26, 709–777. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x03000165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutman, A. M., Clapp, W. C., Chadick, J. Z., and Gazzaley, A. (2010). Early top - Down control of visual processing predicts working memory performance. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1224–1234. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakurai, Y., and Takahashi, S. (2008). Dynamic synchrony of local cell assembly. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 425–440. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2008.19.6.425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salazar, R. F., Dotson, N. M., Bressler, S. L., and Gray, C. M. (2012). Content-specific fronto-parietal synchronization during visual working memory. Science 437, 1097–1101. doi: 10.1126/science.1224000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarma, A., Masse, N. Y., Wang, X.-J., and Freedman, D. J. (2015). Task-specific versus generalized mnemonic representations in parietal and prefrontal cortices. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 143–149. doi: 10.1038/nn.4168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarnthein, J., Petsche, H., Rappelsberger, P., Shaw, G. L., and Von Stein, A. (1998). Synchronization between prefrontal and posterior association cortex during human working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 7092–7096. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.12.7092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Heise, K. F., Gruber, W. R., Holz, E., Karim, A. A., et al. (2009). Brain oscillatory substrates of visual short-term memory capacity. Curr. Biol. 19, 1846–1852. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senkowski, D., and Gallinat, J. (2015). Dysfunctional prefrontal gamma-band oscillations reflect working memory and other cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 1010–1019. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shan, Y., Jia, Y., Zhong, S., Li, X., Zhao, H., Chen, J., et al. (2018). Correlations between working memory impairment and neurometabolites of prefrontal cortex and lenticular nucleus in patients with major depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 227, 236–242. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.10.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siebenhühner, F., Wang, S. H., Palva, J. M., and Palva, S. (2016). Cross-frequency synchronization connects networks of fast and slow oscillations during visual working memory maintenance. Elife 5, 15–30. doi: 10.7554/eLife.13451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, M., Warden, M. R., and Miller, E. K. (2009). Phase-dependent neuronal coding of objects in short-term memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 21341–21346. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908193106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegert, R. J., Weatherall, M., Taylor, K. D., and Abernethy, D. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of performance on simple span and more complex working memory tasks in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology 22, 450–461. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.4.450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sigurdsson, T., Stark, K. L., Karayiorgou, M., Gogos, J. A., and Gordon, J. A. (2010). Impaired hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony in a genetic mouse model of schizophrenia. Nature 464, 763–767. doi: 10.1038/nature08855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, W. (2018). Neuronal oscillations: unavoidable and useful? Eur. J. Neurosci. 48, 2389–2398. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiers, H. J., Maguire, E. A., and Burgess, N. (2001). Hippocampal amnesia. Neurocase 7, 357–382. doi: 10.1076/neur.7.5.357.16245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sreenivasan, K. K., and D’Esposito, M. (2019). The what, where and how of delay activity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 466–481. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0176-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sreeraj, V. S., Shivakumar, V., Sowmya, S., Bose, A., Nawani, H., Narayanaswamy, J. C., et al. (2019). Online theta frequency transcranial alternating current stimulation for cognitive remediation in schizophrenia: a case report and review of literature. J. ECT 35, 139–143. doi: 10.1097/YCT.0000000000000523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stokes, M. G. (2015). “Activity-silent” working memory in prefrontal cortex: a dynamic coding framework. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 394–405. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strüber, D., and Herrmann, C. S. (2020). Modulation of gamma oscillations as a possible therapeutic tool for neuropsychiatric diseases: a review and perspective. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 152, 15–25. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sunderam, S., Chernyy, N., Mason, J., Peixoto, N., Weinstein, S. L., Schiff, S. J., et al. (2006). “Seizure modulation with applied electric fields in chronically implanted animals,” in Proceedings of the 2006 International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, New York, NY, 1612–1615. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2006.259454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Supèr, H., Spekreijse, H., and Lamme, V. A. F. (2001). A neural correlate of working memory in the monkey primary visual cortex. Science 293, 120–124. doi: 10.1126/science.1060496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., and Fischer, C. (2001). Oscillatory synchrony between human extrastriate areas during visual short-term memory maintenance. J. Neurosci. 21:RC177. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-20-j0008.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tallon-Baudry, C., Mandon, S., Freiwald, W. A., and Kreiter, A. K. (2004). Oscillatory synchrony in the monkey temporal lobe correlates with performance in a visual short-term memory task. Cereb. Cortex 14, 713–720. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uhlhaas, P. J., and Singer, W. (2006). Neural synchrony in brain disorders: relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron 52, 155–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Meij, R., Kahana, M., and Maris, E. (2012). Phase-amplitude coupling in human electrocorticography is spatially distributed and phase diverse. J. Neurosci. 32, 111–123. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4816-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Kerkoerle, T., Self, M. W., and Roelfsema, P. R. (2017). Layer-specificity in the effects of attention and working memory on activity in primary visual cortex. Nat. Commun. 8:13804.

Google Scholar

Van Snellenberg, J. X., Girgis, R. R., Horga, G., van de Giessen, E., Slifstein, M., Ojeil, N., et al. (2016). Mechanisms of working memory impairment in Schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 80, 617–626. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Vugt, M. K., Schulze-Bonhage, A., Litt, B., Brandt, A., and Kahana, M. J. (2010). Hippocampal gamma oscillations increase with memory load. J. Neurosci. 30, 2694–2699. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0567-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varela, F., Lachaux, J.-P., Rodriguez, E., and Martinerie, J. (2001). The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 229–239. doi: 10.1038/35067550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vergara, J., Rivera, N., Rossi-Pool, R., and Romo, R. (2016). A neural parametric code for storing information of more than one sensory modality in working memory. Neuron 89, 54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Violante, I. R., Li, L. M., Carmichael, D. W., Lorenz, R., Leech, R., Hampshire, A., et al. (2017). Externally induced frontoparietal synchronization modulates network dynamics and enhances working memory performance. eLife 6:e22001. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, N., Ke, Y., Du, J., Liu, W., Kong, L., Zhao, X., et al. (2019). “High-Definition Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (HD-tDCS) enhances working memory training,” in Proceedings of the 2019 41st Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), Berlin, 329–332. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2019.8856976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S., Itthipuripat, S., and Ku, Y. (2019). Electrical stimulation over human posterior parietal cortex selectively enhances the capacity of visual short-term memory. J. Neurosci. 39, 528–536. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1959-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wischnewski, M., Schutter, D. J. L. G., and Nitsche, M. A. (2019). Effects of beta-tACS on corticospinal excitability: a meta-analysis. Brain Stimul. 12, 1381–1389. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.07.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, R. C., Plichta, M. M., Sambataro, F., Fallgatter, A. J., Jacob, C., Lesch, K.-P., et al. (2009). Regional brain activation changes and abnormal functional connectivity of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex during working memory processing in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2252–2266. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, G., Wang, Y., Mwansisya, T. E., Pu, W., Zhang, H., Liu, C., et al. (2014). Effective connectivity of the posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortices relates to working memory impairment in schizophrenic and bipolar patients. Schizophr. Res. 158, 85–90. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.06.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Z.-M., Bralten, J., An, L., Cao, Q.-J., Cao, X.-H., Sun, L., et al. (2017). Verbal working memory-related functional connectivity alterations in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the effects of methylphenidate. J. Psychopharmacol. 31, 1061–1069. doi: 10.1177/0269881117715607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yonelinas, A. P. (2013). The hippocampus supports high-resolution binding in the service of perception, working memory and long-term memory. Behav. Brain Res. 254, 34–44. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.05.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuste, R. (2015). From the neuron doctrine to neural networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 487–497. doi: 10.1038/nrn3962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaksas, D., Bisley, J. W., and Pasternak, T. (2001). Motion information is spatially localized in a visual working-memory task. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 912–921. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.2.912

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaksas, D., and Pasternak, T. (2006). Directional signals in the prefrontal cortex and in area MT during a working memory for visual motion task. J. Neurosci. 26, 11726–11742. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3420-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zammit, N., Falzon, O., Camilleri, K., and Muscat, R. (2018). Working memory alpha-beta band oscillatory signatures in adolescents and young adults. Eur. J. Neurosci. 48, 2527–2536. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zanos, S., Rembado, I., Chen, D., and Fetz, E. E. (2018). Phase-locked stimulation during cortical beta oscillations produces bidirectional synaptic plasticity in awake monkeys. Curr. Biol. 28, 2515–2526. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zanto, T. P., Rubens, M. T., Thangavel, A., and Gazzaley, A. (2011). Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of visual processing and working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 656–661. doi: 10.1038/nn.2773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Q., van Vugt, M., Borst, J. P., and Anderson, J. R. (2018). Mapping working memory retrieval in space and in time: a combined electroencephalography and electrocorticography approach. Neuroimage 174, 472–484.

Google Scholar

Zhao, G., Chen, F., Zhang, Q., Shen, M., and Gao, Z. (2018). Feature-based information filtering in visual working memory is impaired in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 111, 317–323. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, X., Katsuki, F., Qi, X. L., and Constantinidis, C. (2012). Neurons with inverted tuning during the delay periods of working memory tasks in the dorsal prefrontal and posterior parietal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 31–38.

Google Scholar

Zola, S. M., Squire, L. R., Teng, E., Stefanacci, L., Buffalo, E. A., and Clark, R. E. (2000). Impaired recognition memory in monkeys after damage limited to the hippocampal region. J. Neurosci. 20, 451–463. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-01-00451.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: working memory, synchrony, oscillation, brain disorders, causal manipulation

Citation: Rezayat E, Clark K, Dehaqani M-RA and Noudoost B (2022) Dependence of Working Memory on Coordinated Activity Across Brain Areas. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 15:787316. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2021.787316

Received: 30 September 2021; Accepted: 06 December 2021;
Published: 13 January 2022.

Edited by:

Greg D. Reynolds, The University of Tennessee, Knoxville, United States

Reviewed by:

Amy L. Griffin, University of Delaware, United States
Scott L. Brincat, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, United States

Copyright © 2022 Rezayat, Clark, Dehaqani and Noudoost. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Behrad Noudoost, behrad.noudoost@utah.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.