- 1Oregon Hearing Research Center, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR, United States
- 2Eaton-Peabody Laboratories, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA, United States
- 3Department of Otolaryngology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
Tinnitus and hyperacusis are life-disrupting perceptual abnormalities that are often preceded by acoustic overexposure. Animal models of overexposure have suggested a link between these phenomena and neural hyperactivity, i.e., elevated spontaneous rates (SRs) and sound-evoked responses. Prior work has focused on changes in central auditory responses, with less attention paid to the exact nature of the associated cochlear damage. The demonstration that acoustic overexposure can cause cochlear neuropathy without permanent threshold elevation suggests cochlear neuropathy per se may be a key elicitor of neural hyperactivity. We addressed this hypothesis by recording responses in the mouse inferior colliculus (IC) following a bilateral, neuropathic noise exposure. One to three weeks post-exposure, mean SRs were unchanged in mice recorded while awake, or under anesthesia. SRs were also unaffected by more intense, or unilateral exposures. These results suggest that neither neuropathy nor hair cell loss are sufficient to raise SRs in the IC, at least in 7-week-old mice, 1–3 weeks post exposure. However, it is not clear whether our mice had tinnitus. Tone-evoked rate-level functions at the CF were steeper following exposure, specifically in the region of maximal neuropathy. Furthermore, suppression driven by off-CF tones and by ipsilateral noise were reduced. Both changes were especially pronounced in neurons of awake mice. This neural hypersensitivity may manifest as behavioral hypersensitivity to sound – prior work reports that this same exposure causes elevated acoustic startle. Together, these results indicate that neuropathy may initiate a compensatory response in the central auditory system leading to the genesis of hyperacusis.
Introduction
Tinnitus, the perception of sounds in silence, is often preceded by acoustic overexposure (Eggermont and Roberts, 2004), thus neurophysiological studies of tinnitus have focused on noise-induced changes in spontaneous discharge. Noise damage typically decreases spontaneous rates (SRs) in the auditory nerve (Liberman and Kiang, 1978; Liberman and Dodds, 1984). By contrast, it often increases SRs in the central auditory pathway, including ventral and dorsal cochlear nucleus (DCN), inferior colliculus (IC), and auditory cortex (Brozoski et al., 2002; Seki and Eggermont, 2003; Bauer et al., 2008), and overexposed animals can appear to have tinnitus (Brozoski et al., 2002; Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2011). However, the relationship between SR and tinnitus is not straightforward: among identically exposed animals, not all demonstrate SR changes or tinnitus behavior. While SRs in the DCN are elevated only in animals with signs of tinnitus-like behavior (Dehmel et al., 2012; Koehler and Shore, 2013; Li et al., 2013), SRs in the IC and auditory cortex can be elevated regardless of inferred tinnitus status (Engineer et al., 2011; Coomber et al., 2014; Ropp et al., 2014).
Although threshold shift is a primary risk factor for tinnitus in humans (Roberts et al., 2010), tinnitus can occur without threshold elevation (Gu et al., 2010; Schaette and McAlpine, 2011; Gu et al., 2012) and thus without significant hair cell damage. Elevated SRs and behavioral evidence for tinnitus have also been observed in noise-exposed animals without permanent threshold shift (PTS; Bauer et al., 2008; Koehler and Shore, 2013). Moderate noise exposures that do not cause hair cell damage or PTS can destroy synapses between cochlear hair cells and auditory nerve terminals (Kujawa and Liberman, 2009). Such cochlear neuropathy does not affect audiometric thresholds, but reduces suprathreshold-evoked response amplitudes, such as the auditory brainstem response (ABR; Bharadwaj et al., 2015; Shaheen et al., 2015). Since tinnitus patients with normal thresholds have reduced ABR amplitudes (Gu et al., 2012), cochlear neuropathy may play a key role in the genesis of central spontaneous hyperactivity and tinnitus.
Hyperacusis, an intolerance to moderate-level sound, often co-occurs with tinnitus and is also often preceded by acoustic overexposure (Kreuzer et al., 2012). In humans with hyperacusis, sound-evoked fMRI responses are increased in the auditory midbrain, thalamus, and cortex (Gu et al., 2010). In animal models, acoustic overexposure can elevate sound-evoked responses in central auditory pathways (Salvi et al., 1990; Cai et al., 2009; Auerbach et al., 2014) and cause behavioral hypersensitivity to loud sounds (Hickox and Liberman, 2014; Manohar et al., 2017). Since hyperacusis can occur in humans with normal audiometric thresholds, cochlear neuropathy may also be a key catalyst of central sound-evoked hyperactivity and hyperacusis.
Prior studies of the neurophysiology of central hyperactivity did not quantify hair cells or cochlear neurons or correlate differences in peripheral damage with differences in central activity (but see Hesse et al., 2016; Asokan et al., 2018). Here, our aim was to test the idea that both spontaneous and sound-evoked hyperactivity are caused by cochlear neuropathy per se, by measuring responses in the IC of mice exposed to noise causing cochlear neuropathy with minimal hair cell loss, and to more intense noise causing both neuropathy and hair cell loss. SRs were unchanged in both groups, indicating that neither neuropathy nor hair cell loss is sufficient to increase SR in the mouse IC. However, tone-evoked rate-level functions were steeper following exposure, specifically in the region of maximal cochlear neuropathy, suggesting that neuropathy plays a role in hyperacusis.
Materials and Methods
Animals and Groups
Seven-week-old male CBA/CaJ mice were exposed to octave-band noise (8–16 kHz) for 2 h. Noise calibration to target SPL was performed immediately before each exposure session. Control mice were of the same age, gender, and strain, but were not exposed to the noise. Physiological experiments were always performed 1–3 weeks after the noise exposure. When anesthesia was used for noise exposure, it was a ketamine/xylazine mix (100/20 mg/kg, respectively, i.p.) with booster injections as needed. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary.
Two experimental groups were exposed awake and unrestrained to the 2-h noise band at 98 dB SPL, a level/duration known to produce “pure” cochlear synaptopathy in both ears, i.e., loss of cochlear synapses without any hair cell loss or PTS (Kujawa and Liberman, 2015; Shaheen et al., 2015; Suzuki et al., 2016; Valero et al., 2016). IC recordings were then made under either awake (n = 6) or anesthetized (n = 10) conditions, and results were compared to similar recordings in unexposed controls under either awake (n = 8) or anesthetized (n = 13) conditions. We used both anesthetized and awake mice. Although prior work has found noise-induced hyperactivity in the IC of anesthetized animals, anesthesia has a strong effect on IC activity, including SRs (Torterolo et al., 2002; Chung et al., 2014), and therefore could mask changes due to noise exposure. After recordings, a subset of exposed and control animals was sacrificed for histopathological analysis, to confirm the synaptopathy phenotype that has been replicated in many other studies from our group (Kujawa and Liberman, 2015; Shaheen et al., 2015; Suzuki et al., 2016; Valero et al., 2016). A third experimental group (n = 6) was exposed awake and unrestrained to the noise band at 103 dB SPL, a level/duration designed to cause synaptopathy plus significant hair cell damage and PTS. IC recordings were made from this group under awake conditions (n = 6), but none of these ears were processed histologically.
To evaluate possible differences between bilateral and unilateral noise damage, an additional three groups were exposed unilaterally while under ketamine/xylazine anesthesia using a small tweeter coupled to the ear canal via a speculum. Exposures were conducted in a warm sound-proofed room (30°C), and a stable anesthetic plane was maintained with booster injections as needed. To minimize contralateral exposure, mice were placed on their side with saline-soaked cotton in the contralateral ear canal. The three groups were exposed to the 2-h noise band at either 101 dB (n = 2), 103 dB (n = 2), or 104 dB SPL (n = 2) and then used for IC recordings under anesthetized conditions. None of these ears were processed histologically.
Preparation for IC Recordings in Anesthetized Mice
A single session was conducted in each animal, occurring 1–3 weeks post-exposure. Mice were anesthetized with a ketamine/xylazine mix (100/20 mg/kg, respectively, i.p.) with booster injections as needed. Surgical procedures and recordings were conducted in an acoustically and electrically shielded room at 30°C. Prior to surgery, DPOAEs and ABRs were measured as described below. Then, the pinnae were removed bilaterally to allow for closed-field acoustic stimulation using two custom acoustic assemblies, each containing two electrostatic drivers (CUI CDMG15008-03A) and an electret condenser microphone (Knowles FG-23329-P07). The assemblies were calibrated with a -in. Bruel and Kjaer condenser microphone and in-ear calibrations were performed at the onset of each experiment.
A small craniotomy was made over the central nucleus of the IC using a scalpel. The dura mater was left intact and covered with high-viscosity silicon oil. Recordings were made with a 16-channel, single-shank silicon probe (25 or 50 μm contact separation, 177 μm2 contact area; NeuroNexus Technologies). Electrodes were inserted dorso-ventrally, along the tonotopic axis, approximately 1 mm lateral, and 1 mm posterior to lambda, and advanced using a micropositioner (Kopf 607-C). While the central nucleus of the IC is easy to visualize in mice, electrode placement was confirmed by the tonotopic organization and sharp frequency tuning.
Preparation for IC Recording in Awake Mice
Two to five days following noise exposure, mice were anesthetized with ketamine and xylazine. The skin overlying the frontal and parietal portions of the skull was retracted, and a titanium headplate was glued to the skull using acrylic bonding material to allow for head-fixed recordings (C&B MetaBond, Parkell). Buprenorphine (0.1 mg/kg s.c.) and meloxicam (0.03 mg/kg s.c.) were given, and animals were allowed to recover for at least 48 h. Recording sessions began as soon as 1 week post-exposure and continued until 3 weeks post-exposure; each session lasted up to 3 h. One to nine (median 3) recording sessions were conducted in each mouse. At the onset of each session, mice were anesthetized with isoflurane in oxygen (2%, 2 L/min). In the first session, a craniotomy was made over the IC as in the anesthetized recordings. A ring around the craniotomy site was formed using a UV-cure resin, and the craniotomy was filled with high-viscosity silicon oil. The same craniotomy was used in subsequent sessions. Cyanoacrylate was used to glue a 1.8-mm rigid plastic tube in each ear canal. Isoflurane was discontinued, and the mouse was transferred to the recording apparatus, where it was head-fixed but free to run on a 22-cm diameter turntable beneath its feet. The ear tubes were coupled to the acoustic assemblies for closed-field sound delivery, and in-ear calibration was performed. DPOAEs were monitored throughout the session to ensure that the acoustic delivery remained constant. Animals were continuously monitored, and the session was ended for the day if they showed signs of persistent distress. Recordings were made using the same 16-channel Neuronexus probes as for experiments on anesthetized mice. At the end of each session, the mice were briefly anesthetized with isoflurane, and the ear tubes were removed. The craniotomy was cleaned and flushed with saline, then covered with a thin layer of bacitracin and sealed with UV-cure resin. Three weeks post exposure, animals were anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine and DPOAEs and ABRs were recorded. At the conclusion of these recordings, mice were sacrificed for histology.
IC Recording
Custom LabVIEW and MATLAB software controlling National Instruments 24-bit digital input/output boards generated all stimuli at a 200 kHz sampling rate. Raw signals from the electrodes were digitized at 32 bits, 24.4 kHz (RZ5 BioAmp Processor; Tucker-Davis Technologies) and stored in binary format. For awake recording, the point-by-point median across all channels was subtracted from each channel, greatly reducing motion-induced artifacts. Single units were isolated online using a sorting method based on principle components analysis (PCA) in SpikePac software (Tucker-Davis Technologies). Units were later reprocessed offline using a combination of custom MATLAB software and the Ultra Mega Sort 2000 package (Hill et al., 2011): raw waveforms were filtered both forward and backward using a fourth-order 300–5000 Hz Butterworth filter. Spikes were detected by threshold crossings above 3.5 standard deviations of the waveform (measured over intervals far removed from sound presentation). Threshold crossings occurring <0.6 ms after another spike were removed to prevent double counting due to noise. Single units were then sorted by the following: aligning spike waveforms to the peak, reducing dimensionality using PCA, overclustering using k-means (k > 32), and merging clusters based on the interface energy between clusters and violations of the absolute refractory period (Hill et al., 2011). Candidate single units were then checked for good isolation by their separation from other clusters, estimated number of sub-threshold (missed) spikes, and interspike interval distributions. The majority of channels contained zero or one well-isolated single units, but some channels contained up to two single units. In some cases, especially for awake recordings, movement of the brain relative to the skull caused single units to drift from channel to channel, at times back and forth between channels. In these cases, spikes were detected and aligned from included channels independently, waveforms from multiple channels were concatenated, and the single-channel procedure was followed beginning with PCA.
Multi-Unit Activity
During most recordings, only two to four channels contained single units. We processed the remaining multiunit activity in two ways: (1) thresholded multi-unit activity (tMUA) was computed by setting the spike threshold to 3.5 standard deviations of the neural activity (filtered 300–5000 Hz) during periods of silence presented during recording of frequency response areas (FRAs) and (2) the envelope of multi-unit activity (eMUA) was computed by band-pass filtering from 300 to 5000 Hz, computing the absolute value, and then computing the average across all repetitions of the stimulus (Supèr and Roelfsema, 2005; Kayser et al., 2007; Choi et al., 2010). In both cases, sites that had strong single-unit responses were excluded to avoid biasing the sample.
Search Stimulus
The search stimulus was a broadband noise burst presented at 60 dB SPL, alternating sides on each presentation.
Frequency Response Areas
Frequency response areas were collected by pseudorandom presentation of tone pips (50 ms on, 200 ms off, 4 ms raised cosine onset/offset ramps) to the ear contralateral to the recording site. Between two and four trials were collected per tone/level combination. Tone frequencies were of 3–70 kHz, spaced in 0.1 octave increments. The specific subset was set to cover the response range of the neurons under investigation (generally two to three octaves wide). In mice recorded under anesthesia, levels were 10–80 dB SPL in 5 dB steps, in mice recorded awake, levels were -5 to 80 dB in 5 dB steps. For both groups, a silent condition was added to the list of levels to be randomized. SR was calculated over the entire 250 ms of this these silent trials, averaged across trials and “frequencies.” Thus, the total time used to calculate SR ranged from 10 to 30 s. Discharge rates in response to tones were calculated over a time window 5–55 ms re stimulus onset, and used to construct the FRA display matrices. The excitatory region of the FRA was determined by (1) measuring the mean and standard deviation of the SR, (2) zeroing all FRA pixels <5 standard deviations above mean SR, grouping non-zero matrix cells into regions connected by edges, and finding the region with the largest average rate × (number of cells)2. This procedure successfully identified the excitatory region for the largest range of FRA shapes, and manual intervention was rarely required. Within the excitatory region, the characteristic frequency (CF) was defined as the frequency with the highest response at the lowest stimulus level, and the best frequency (BF) as the frequency producing the largest average response across all levels. The bandwidth 10 dB above threshold (BW10) was used to measure the sharpness of excitatory tuning.
The pattern of inhibition in FRAs has been used to divide IC neurons into response types V, I, and O, suggested to arise due to dominant inputs from the medial superior olive, lateral superior olive, and DCN, respectively (Ramachandran et al., 1999). Although the distribution may be continuous (Palmer et al., 2013), unit classification has revealed class-specific SR changes after noise exposure (Ropp et al., 2014). Therefore, we used FRAs to define three classes based on the bandwidth of excitation and the presence of on-CF inhibition (Egorova et al., 2001; Ma et al., 2006). “Broad” units had a V-shaped excitatory area and wide bandwidth on both low and high-frequency sides, with a total bandwidth at 10 dB above threshold (BW10) of at least one octave (Supplementary Figures S1A, S2B). “Narrow” units had an excitatory area ranging from V- to I-shaped with a particularly narrow bandwidth on the high-frequency side and a BW10 of less than one octave (Supplementary Figures S1B,C). “Non-monotonic” units also had BW10 less than one octave as well as on-CF inhibition at high levels (Supplementary Figure S1D). Inhibition was typically moderate (E/I Bal > 0), but in some cases was strong, causing the rate to CF tones to drop below SR (E/I Bal < 0; Supplementary Figure S2A; see Section “Materials and Methods”). The frequency extent of inhibition varied, causing upward-tilting, downward-tilting, and Type-O FRA shapes (Palmer et al., 2013). About 10% of units did not fit any type and were classified as “Others.” These included units that were purely inhibitory, had multiple separate excitatory areas, or had CFs more than ½ octave removed from the local multi-unit CF. The unilaterally exposed groups were too small to permit further subdivision, so they were not included in this analysis. Large threshold shifts in the 103-dB-exposed group prohibited reliable FRA classification, so these were also excluded.
Binaural-Noise Response Areas
Binaural-noise response areas were collected by pseudorandom presentation of simultaneous, dichotic noise bursts (50 ms on, 200 ms off, 4 ms raised cosine onset/offset ramps). Noise was limited from 4 to 80 kHz and shaped to be flat-spectrum by correction for the frequency response of the acoustic system. Ipsilateral and contralateral levels were randomly and independently varied, and five trials were collected per level combination. Levels were chosen to encompass the dynamic range of the neurons under investigation, in anesthetized mice this was generally 20 to 70 dB SPL, while in awake mice this was 10–70 dB SPL, both in 5 dB steps. As for the FRAs, a silent condition was added to the list of levels to be randomized, so that responses to both contralateral noise alone and ipsilateral noise alone were included in the stimulus set. Rate was averaged over a time window 5–55 ms re stimulus onset.
Binaural response properties can also be used to infer the dominant sub-collicular inputs. Canonically, neurons excited by both contralateral and ipsilateral sound (EE) receive dominant inputs from the medial superior olive, whereas neurons excited by contralateral sounds and inhibited by ipsilateral sound (EI) receive dominant inputs from the lateral superior olive, and neurons unaffected by ipsilateral sound (EO) receive dominant inputs from the cochlear nucleus (Davis et al., 1999). The majority of our recorded neurons (62%) were excited by contralateral noise and inhibited by ipsilateral noise (EI; Supplementary Figure S3A). Neurons that were inhibited by contralateral noise at low levels, but excited at higher levels were also included in this group (e.g., Supplementary Figure S3H). The strength of both ipsilateral and high-level contralateral inhibition varied substantially in this group. Consistent with the high-frequency hearing and underdeveloped medial superior olive in mouse (Irving and Harrison, 1967; Ollo and Schwartz, 1979), no EE neurons were encountered. While there were no neurons driven by ipsilateral noise alone, about 8% of neurons were facilitated by ipsilateral noise (EI/f; Supplementary Figures S3C,D; Park and Pollak, 1993; Davis et al., 1999), and another 15% were driven by contralateral noise and unaffected by ipsilateral noise (EO; Supplementary Figures S3E,F). The remaining 15% were inhibited by contralateral noise (Supplementary Figure S3G), were not driven by noise, or had more complex responses.
Fits to Rate-Level Functions
Rate-level functions at CF and BF were calculated from FRAs and fit with a four-parameter model (Sachs and Abbas, 1974; Heil et al., 2011). This model specifies firing rate r as a function of sound pressure P (in Pa) by the following equation:
where RD is the driven rate, RS is the SR, N characterizes the steepness of the curve, and L50 gives the level at 50% of the driven rate range. Non-monotonic functions were manually selected and fit using the sum of two such models, yielding seven parameters:
The slope of the rising portion of the function was used in population comparisons. Curves were fit by fminsearchbnd, a bounded version of the built-in MATLAB function fminsearch using a least-squares method. The fit was then refined by a robust procedure that iteratively refined weights of each data point before re-fitting.
DPOAEs and ABRs
For measurement of ABRs and DPOAEs, stimuli were presented unilaterally, with the mouse on its side, and the acoustic assembly just above the ear canal. After data were collected for one ear, the procedure was repeated for the other ear. Both ABRs and DPOAEs were measured once per animal. For mice undergoing IC recordings under anesthesia, ABRs and DPOAEs were measured during the same session as the IC recordings, immediately prior to the craniotomy. For mice undergoing IC recordings while awake, ABRs and DPOAEs were measured in a separate session 3 weeks post-exposure, just prior to sacrifice for histology.
Auditory brainstem responses were recorded differentially from subdermal needle electrodes with the common ground at the base of the tail. A dorsal–ventral incision was made in the pinna at the notch between the tragus and antitragus to allow direct visualization of the eardrum. For mice that had undergone IC recordings under anesthesia, the electrode montage was vertex (positive) to ipsilateral pinna, with the latter just caudal to the intertragal notch (see Shaheen et al., 2015). For those that had undergone awake recordings, the headplate prohibited placing the vertex electrode. Therefore, active electrodes were placed either (1) headplate to ipsilateral pinna or (2) contralateral pinna to ipsilateral pinna. ABR amplitudes were more variable for configuration (1); thus, results are reported for configuration (2). Pinna electrodes were placed slightly more ventral along the antitragus in order to increase the early ABR waves (pinnaV in Shaheen et al., 2015).
Responses were amplified 10,000× using two Grass P511 amplifiers with a 0.3–3 kHz passband. ABRs were evoked with 5-ms tone pips with 0.5 ms cos2 rise–fall, presented in alternating polarity at a rate of 40/s. Tone-pip frequencies were 11.3 or 32 kHz. Trials where the response amplitude exceeded 15 μV were rejected; 512 artifact-free trials of each polarity were averaged to compute ABR waveforms. Threshold was defined by visual inspection of the stacked waveforms as the lowest level at which a reproducible peak or trough appears, which usually occurs one level-step below that at which amplitude begins to grow. Wave 1 amplitude was measured peak-to-trough.
DPOAEs were recorded in the ear canal sound pressure in response to two tones, f1 and f2, each presented to separate speakers to reduce system distortion (frequency ratio f2/f1 = 1.2, and level difference L1 = L2 + 10 dB). DPOAE response was measured at 2f1 -f2 by Fourier analysis of the ear-canal sound pressure waveform. Stimulus duration was 1.6 s at each level combination (L2 varied from 20 to 80 dB SPL in 5 dB steps). Threshold was defined as the interpolated f2 level producing a DPOAE of 5 dB SPL.
Cochlear Immunostaining and Innervation Analysis
Mice were perfused intracardially with 4% paraformaldehyde. Cochleas were decalcified, dissected into half-turns, and incubated in primary antibodies: (1) mouse (IgG1) anti-CtBP2 from BD Biosciences at 1:200 and (2) mouse (IgG2) anti-GluA2 from Millipore at 1:2000. Primary incubations were followed by 60-min incubations in species-appropriate secondary antibodies. Cochlear lengths were obtained for each case, and a cochlear frequency map computed using a custom ImageJ plugin1 that translates cochlear position into frequency according to the published map for the mouse (Müller et al., 2005; Taberner and Liberman, 2005). Confocal z-stacks from each ear were obtained using a 63× glycerol-immersion objective (N.A. = 1.3) at 3.17× digital zoom on a Leica TCS SP5 confocal. Synapses in the IHC area were counted using Amira (Visage Imaging) to find the xyz coordinates of all the ribbons (CtBP2-positive puncta), and custom re-projection software was then used to assess the fraction of ribbons with closely apposed glutamate-receptor patches (GluA2 puncta).
Statistical Analysis
Statistical testing was performed in MATLAB; functions used are indicated in italics. For SRs, distributions were normal (assessed with Lilliefors test), so differences were assessed using a three-way ANOVA (anovan). Significant post hoc differences were assessed using multcompare with a Tukey–Kramer correction for multiple comparisons. Threshold, maximum driven rates, and slopes were not normally distributed, especially for multi-units. Therefore, differences were assessed with paired rank-sum tests. Six comparisons were made to test effects of exposure, and a Bonferroni–Holm correction was applied. To test effects of anesthesia, populations were pooled across exposure for each frequency bin. Therefore, these tests were corrected for three comparisons.
Results
Noise-Induced Synaptic Loss After Reversible Noise-Induced Threshold Shift
We exposed two groups of 7-week-old mice to an 8–16 kHz, 98 dB SPL noise band for 2 h, which reliably causes substantial cochlear synaptopathy with minimal hair cell damage (Shaheen et al., 2015; Valero et al., 2016). One group was then used for awake IC recordings, and one for anesthetized IC recordings. For efficiency, the time-consuming histological analysis was completed only for the awake-recording group, since this exposure has produced such highly stereotyped results in many prior studies from the Liberman lab (Shaheen et al., 2015; Valero et al., 2016); thus, only basic reconfirmation is necessary to ensure that some idiosyncratic difference in exposure technique has not altered the basic damage pattern.
As in prior work, this exposure resulted in a 40–50% loss of synapses between inner hair cells and auditory nerve fibers in the basal half of the cochlea (Figure 1A). The effect of exposure was significant as assessed by a repeated-measures ANOVA (F1,12 = 101, p < 0.001). As expected, no damage to inner hair cells was observed (data not shown), and loss of outer hair cells was confined to the basal end of the cochlea (Figure 1A). Exposure significantly reduced ABR wave I amplitudes in both anesthetized- and awake-recording groups (F1,4 = 8.8, p = 0.04; F1,21 = 9.4, p = 0.006, respectively; Figure 1B). In the anesthetized-recording group, post hoc tests indicated wave I amplitudes were significantly reduced at 32 kHz, but not at 11.3 kHz [red symbols in Figure 1, consistent with the pattern of synaptic loss (Figure 1A)]. ABR amplitudes were significantly reduced at both frequencies in the awake-recording group (green symbols in Figure 1B) although they did not have synaptic loss at 11.3 kHz (Figure 1A). Interpretation is difficult because the head plate prevented normal placement of electrodes (see Section “Materials and Methods”). Since these mice were exposed identically to those in the anesthetized-recording group, and as in prior studies (Shaheen et al., 2015; Valero et al., 2016), and because these same mice did not have synaptic loss at 11.3 kHz (Figure 1A), these results are likely in error due to variability in placement of the headplate. Consistent with the hair cell loss pattern (Figure 1A), DPOAE thresholds were normal except at 45 kHz, where they were elevated by 5–10 dB in both exposed groups (Figure 1C). DPOAE suprathreshold amplitudes were also reduced at 45 kHz (Figure 1B), consistent with the DPOAE threshold shift, and with the small but significant OHC damage in that, and more basal, regions. ABR thresholds were normal at both 11.3 and 32 kHz (Figure 1C), as expected from the DPOAE thresholds.
FIGURE 1. Summary histopathology and pathophysiology for all noise-exposure groups. Mice were awake during bilateral exposures (A–C) and anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine (K/X) during unilateral exposures (D). (A) Mean IHC synapse (top) or OHC (bottom) survival following 98 dB exposure in mice used for awake IC recordings. (B) Mean change in ABR Wave I (top) or DPOAE (bottom) amplitudes, averaged over stimulus levels from 60 to 80 dB SPL. (C) Mean ABR (top) or DPOAE (bottom) threshold shifts in mice used for IC recordings. Up arrows in (C) (bottom) indicate that at least one animal showed no response at the highest stimulus level. (D) Mean ABR (top) and DPOAE (bottom) threshold shifts following unilateral exposures. Key in (A) (bottom) applies to all other panels in (B,C). Key in (D) (bottom) also applies to (D) (top). Error bars in all panels indicate SEMs. Each group is normalized to its respective control. Asterisks indicate significant effects of exposure (∗p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.001) by Tukey–Kramer-corrected post hoc test following a repeated-measures ANOVA.
Based on the highly reproducible patterns of hair cell and neural damage, we divided the cochlear frequency space into three regions: (1) “non-neuropathic” (<16 kHz), where noise exposure did not permanently alter thresholds, hair cells or synaptic counts, (2) “neuropathic” (16–32 kHz), where exposure caused cochlear neuropathy without hair cell damage or threshold shift, and (3) “threshold-shift” (>32 kHz), where exposure caused neuropathy with some hair cell damage and PTS.
Spontaneous Rates in IC Were Unaffected by Bilateral Neuropathic Noise Exposure
We examined the central nucleus of the IC of noise-exposed mice for signs of hyperactivity via single-unit responses using single-shank silicon electrodes. In the first group of mice, responses were measured under ketamine/xylazine anesthesia, in a single session 1–3 weeks following noise exposure. SRs ranged from 0 to 50 spikes/s in both control and noise-exposed mice, but were not affected by noise-exposure in any range of CF (Figure 2A).
FIGURE 2. Noise exposure did not affect spontaneous rates (SRs) in the IC. (A,B) SRs from isolated IC neurons in control vs. exposed mice, recorded in anesthetized (A) or awake (B) preparations. Lines and shading in (A,B) indicate medians and interquartile ranges, pooled over one-octave bands. Off-axis SRs [up arrows in (B)] were 110, 131, and 104 sp/s from left to right. (C) Mean SRs binned into three CF regions, as indicated. Black double asterisks indicate significant effects of anesthesia (p < 0.001, Tukey-Kramer-corrected post hoc test following a three-way ANOVA). Keys in (A,B) also apply to (C).
Anesthesia has a strong effect on IC activity, including SRs (Torterolo et al., 2002; Chung et al., 2014). Therefore, we recorded responses from a group of awake, head-fixed mice. One to nine (median three) recording sessions were conducted in each mouse, starting from 1 week post-exposure and ending 3 weeks post-exposure. SRs ranged from 0 to 130 spikes/s and were significantly elevated relative to anesthetized mice (Figure 2B). However, there was no effect of noise exposure on SRs.
Spontaneous rate means were calculated over three CF regions and the significance of effects was evaluated using a three-way ANOVA (Figure 2C). The main effect of anesthesia was highly significant (F1,436 = 110, p < 0.001), but effects of exposure and frequency region were not (F1,436 = 0.9, p = 0.3; F2,436 = 2.6, p = 0.08). Post hoc tests did not reveal significant effects of exposure in any frequency region.
Spontaneous Rates in IC Were Also Unaffected by Unilateral or More Intense Exposures
Most studies of noise-induced SR elevation in the IC have used more intense noise exposures, causing a greater amount of cochlear pathology (ex. Mulders and Robertson, 2009). Therefore, we raised the noise exposure level to 103 dB SPL while holding all other parameters constant. At this intensity, exposure caused >40 dB of PTS across a wide range of frequencies (Figure 1C, blue diamonds). Surprisingly, SR still remained unchanged relative to controls (Figure 2A).
While elevated SRs have been observed following bilateral exposures in a number of prior studies (Ma et al., 2006; Manzoor et al., 2012, 2013; Hesse et al., 2016), the majority of studies reporting SR increases have used unilateral exposures (Bauer et al., 2008; Mulders and Robertson, 2009; Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2011; Coomber et al., 2014; Ropp et al., 2014; Vogler et al., 2014). Thus, we set out to measure SRs in three groups of unilaterally exposed mice. The exposure band and duration were held constant, but mice were exposed while under ketamine/xylazine anesthesia using a closed-field speaker. Since anesthesia often reduces body temperature, which can reduce cochlear damage, mice exposed to 101 and 103 dB did not show permanent elevation of DPOAE thresholds (Figure 1D, cyan and magenta). However, mice exposed to 104 dB exhibited up to 30 dB of PTS (orange). Once again, mean SR was unchanged relative to controls in all three groups (Figure 2A). A two-way ANOVA demonstrated no effect of exposure or frequency region (F4,264 = 0.8, p = 0.5; F2,264 = 0.3, p = 0.7). Thus, in our hands, the distribution of single-unit SRs was not obviously changed in the mouse IC following octave-band, 2-h noise exposure regardless of anesthesia, intensity or laterality.
Spontaneous Rate Was Unchanged for All Unit Types: FRA Classification
The central nucleus of the IC receives feed-forward axonal projections from nearly all brainstem auditory nuclei, and its physiological responses reflect this diversity of input (Ito et al., 2016). We used FRAs to define three classes based on the bandwidth of excitation and the presence of on-CF inhibition (see Section “Materials and Methods”; Egorova et al., 2001; Ma et al., 2006). Summed across units collected from control and 98-dB-exposed mice recorded both anesthetized and awake, the population was 55% narrow, 30% non-monotonic, 5% broad, and 10% other (Figure 3A, bottom). Averaging within each anesthesia group and frequency band, and comparing across exposure condition revealed only small differences in the proportions of different classes, and a χ2 analysis did not reveal any significant differences due to exposure [Anesthetized (<16 kHz: = 2.2, p = 0.1, 16–32 kHz: = 3.8, p = 0.05, >32 kHz: = 1.0, p = 0.3); Awake (<16 kHz: = 0.5, p = 0.5, 16–32 kHz: = 2.3, p = 0.1, >32 kHz: = 0.5, p = 0.4)] or anesthesia [Control (<16 kHz: = 2.3, p = 0.13, 16–32 kHz: = 3.2, p = 0.08, >32 kHz: = 3.7, p = 0.05)].
FIGURE 3. SRs in isolated IC neurons are unaffected by a neuropathic 98 dB noise exposure, even after segregating for response type. Three classification strategies are used: frequency response area (A), E/I combination (B), or contralateral rate-level monotonicity (C). For each strategy, the top row shows mean SRs (±SEMs), and the bottom row shows percent of population. (A) Units classified by response-area shapes – see Supplementary Figures S1, S2 for further details. “Complex or I” shows the percent of sampled neurons unclassifiable by response area. (B) Units classified by response to noise: EI, Ipsi excitatory/contra inhibitory; EO, Ipsi excitatory/contra no response; EL/f, Ipsi excitatory/contra inhibitory with binaural facilitation; Contra-I or complex, Ipsi inhibitory/contra inhibitory, Inhibitory/No Response, or complex unit types. (C) Units classified by response to contralateral noise only, in order of increasing presence of inhibition: Excitatory-monotonic, Excitatory-non-monotonic, Inhibitory-non-monotonic, and Inhibitory-monotonic. Symbol types are as described in Figure 2.
A three-way ANOVA confirmed what is visually apparent: there were no within-type effects of exposure on SRs (Figure 3A, top). Among narrow units, the effect of anesthesia was highly significant (F1,228 = 107, p < 0.001), but effects of exposure and frequency bin were not (F1,228 = 0.1, p = 0.8; F2,228 = 2.7, p = 0.07). The same was true for non-monotonic units (F1,117 = 18, p < 0.001; F1,117 = 0.3, p = 0.6; F2,117 = 0.8, p = 0.5). The number of units in the broad and other categories was too small to make a meaningful comparison of SRs.
Narrow monotonic units are typically subdivided into Type V and Type I units on the basis of the bandwidth of excitatory tuning and presence of inhibitory sidebands (Ramachandran et al., 1999). In this dataset, 75% of narrow units had inhibitory sidebands (ranging from barely detectable to substantial) and 10% had no detectable sidebands (Supplementary Figures S1B,C). The remaining 15% had SRs near zero; assessment of sidebands in these neurons would require a two-tone stimulus paradigm, which was not collected. SR was not affected by exposure within any of these subgroups (data not shown).
Spontaneous Rate Was Unchanged for All Unit Types: Binaural Noise Classification
We constructed binaural-noise response areas by playing broadband noise to both ears simultaneously, and randomly varying the level in each ear, and used them to classify unit types (see Section “Materials and Methods”). The population was 62% EI, 8% EI/f, 15% EO, and 15% II, IO, or complex. We calculated the proportions in each class within each anesthesia group and frequency band, and compared them across exposure condition (Figure 3B, bottom). A χ2 analysis for differences in the population statistics was significant in the >32 kHz anesthetized, 16–32 kHz awake, and >32 kHz awake cases ( = 5.8, p = 0.015; = 5.9, p = 0.015; = 5.0, p = 0.026, respectively). However, samples sizes were small, and after applying Yate’s correction for small samples no significant differences remained (p = 0.09; p = 0.09; p = 0.13).
There were no within-class effects of exposure on SRs (Figure 3B, top). Among EI units, the effect of anesthesia was highly significant (F1,174 = 47, p < 0.001), but effects of exposure and frequency range were not (F1,174 = 0.1, p = 0.8; F2,174 = 2.4, p = 0.09). The number of units in the other categories was too small to make a meaningful comparison of SRs.
Because the number of neurons in the EI group was so large, this class was further divided based on the shape of the rate-level functions to contralateral noise: excitatory-monotonic neurons had strictly increasing rate with increasing level, excitatory-non-monotonic neurons had functions that rose and then fell at high levels, inhibitory-non-monotonic neurons had functions that fell and then rose at high levels, and inhibitory-monotonic neurons had functions that strictly decreased. No significant exposure-related differences were found in either the proportion of neurons, or the SR, within each class (Figure 3C).
Responses evoked by binaural noise stimuli were not affected by acoustic overexposure (Supplementary Figure S4).
The Slope of Tone-Evoked Rate-vs.-Level Functions Was Elevated in Neuropathic Mice
Acoustic overexposure can enhance sound-evoked responses in the ascending auditory pathways. Prior studies have reported increased sound-evoked responses following noise exposure in the ventral cochlear nucleus (Cai et al., 2009), IC (Salvi et al., 1990), and auditory cortex (Resnik and Polley, 2017; reviewed in Auerbach et al., 2014). Increased intensity of sound-evoked EEG and fMRI signals have been reported in humans with hyperacusis, even after controlling for audiometric thresholds (Gu et al., 2010, 2012). We therefore searched for neuronal hyperactivity by quantifying responses to CF tones obtained in the measurement of FRAs. Rate-level functions were fit with the four-parameter model of Sachs and Abbas (1974), or a seven-parameter sum of two such models (see Section “Materials and Methods” and Supplementary Figure S1); the excitatory portion of this fit was then used to calculate threshold, maximum driven rate, and slope (Figure 4). In most comparisons, there was no effect of exposure. However, in the region of maximal neuropathy (16–32 kHz), slopes were elevated by 65% in awake mice (Figure 4C, p = 0.037, rank-sum test). Slopes were not elevated above 32 kHz, a region with synaptic loss along with some hair cell damage. Strikingly, slopes were not elevated under anesthesia within any frequency range, suggesting that hyperactivity may be modulated by anesthesia.
FIGURE 4. Responses to CF tones in isolated IC neurons. (A) Threshold for all neurons; (B) maximum driven rate for all neurons (maximum rate minus SR); (C) slope of the rate-level function for all neurons; (D) rate-level slopes for “narrow” or “non-monotonic” neurons only; and (E) mean rate-level functions for “narrow” or “non-monotonic” neurons only with CFs between 16 and 32 kHz. Mean values binned into three CF regions (±SEM) are shown for each parameter in (D) and in the right panels of (A–C). Units with inhibitory or complex response areas are not included. Off-axis values [up arrows in (C), center] were 64.2 and 60.9 from left to right. Lines and shading (when shown) indicate median and interquartile range, calculated over a one octave sliding window. For all panels, black asterisks indicate significant effects of anesthesia; green asterisks [(C,D) only] indicate significant effects of noise exposure: ∗p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.001 by a rank-sum test with Bonferroni–Holm correction.
Anesthesia had a strong effect on all three parameters. Thresholds were significantly elevated by anesthesia in the non-neuropathic and neuropathic regions (p < 0.001, p < 0.001, rank-sum test). As observed with SRs, maximum driven rates were reduced by anesthesia in all frequency regions (p < 0.001, p < 0.001, p < 0.001). Rate-level function slopes were also significantly reduced across all CFs (p = 0.006, p = 0.001, p < 0.001).
Separating into unit classes revealed that slope elevations in the composite means were driven by increased slopes in the rising portion of non-monotonic neurons’ rate-level functions (Figure 4D). Exposure did not have a significant effect on threshold or maximum driven rate for any response type (data not shown). However, mean rate-level functions demonstrated a trend toward elevated maximum driven rates for both narrow and non-monotonic neurons in the neuropathic region (Figure 4E). This trend was not present in the other frequency regions (data not shown).
Inhibitory Side Bands Were Reduced in Neuropathic Mice
Responses to off-CF tones were visualized by plotting the mean FRA for each group and frequency region. The most notable effect of exposure was a decrease in the suppressive sidebands in the neuropathic region (Figure 5A). This effect was quantified by plotting the driven rate vs. frequency for tones 20–30 dB above threshold. In the neuropathic region of awake mice, driven rate was elevated at frequencies above and below CF. This was apparent when pooling all neurons, but most prominent in neurons with a narrow tone-response type (Figure 5B). Notably, while mean driven rate dropped below 0 in control neurons, driven rate remained positive in exposed neurons (Figure 5B, top). Although intracellular recordings are required for confirmation, these changes suggest that exposure caused a reduction in synaptic inhibition, which is much broader than synaptic excitation in the mouse IC (Xiong et al., 2013). No significant effects were found in anesthetized mice (Figure 5B, bottom), and significant effects of exposure were not observed at other frequency regions (data not shown). To quantify impacts on frequency selectivity, we measured sharpness of tuning (Q factor) at 40 dB above threshold (Figure 5C). While there was a trend toward reduced selectivity in the neuropathic region of exposed mice, there were no significant effects of exposure after correcting for multiple comparisons.
FIGURE 5. Inhibitory side bands are reduced in neuropathic regions. (A) Mean tone-driven rates of all isolated single neurons in awake recording groups, separated into CF regions (columns) and into control, exposed, and control-exposed (rows). Gray pixels indicate that the number of units contributing to the average was too low to be reliable. (B) Mean tone-driven rate averaged at levels 20, 25, and 30 dB above threshold for narrow, non-monotonic, and all neurons with CFs between 16 and 32 kHz, as indicated. (C) Tuning sharpness (Q = bandwidth/CF) at 40 dB above threshold, averaged across all neurons. Black asterisks indicate significant effects of anesthesia; green asterisks indicate significant effects of exposure as described in Figure 4, by Tukey–Kramer-corrected post hoc test following a three-way ANOVA).
Exposure Effects on Multi-Unit Responses Were Different Than Those Seen in Single Unit
Single-unit responses in the IC are inherently variable, given the complex mixture of response types and varied input circuits. This heterogeneity in normal response profiles could hide important effects of noise exposure on stimulus coding in the IC. As an alternative to single-unit approaches, some prior studies (Hesse et al., 2016) have relied on multi-unit activity (MUA), which can be easier to measure, and, by its nature, averages response of many adjacent neurons, especially at high stimulus levels. In the IC, multiunit FRAs are usually Type V, i.e., with minimal inhibitory sidebands and monotonic rate-level functions at CF. While fine detail is obscured in MUA measures, they may be useful in evaluating gross changes in IC responses due to noise exposure.
Here, MUA was quantified in two ways: (1) by counting spikes after thresholding the electrode signal at 3.5 standard deviations of the background noise (tMUA, Figure 6A, left) and (2) by measuring the envelope of the MUA (eMUA), without any thresholding step (Figure 6A, right) (Supèr and Roelfsema, 2005; Kayser et al., 2007; Choi et al., 2010). Following neuropathic exposure, spontaneous activity was slightly (but significantly) reduced in the neuropathic region by both measures in anesthetized mice. However, in awake mice, spontaneous activity in the neuropathic region was elevated 6% following exposure in the tMUA, but reduced 10% in the eMUA. We have no clear explanation for these divergent effects, but note they are much smaller than the elevations in MUA-based spontaneous activity following exposure reported by Hesse et al., 2016, which ranged from 130 to 600%, depending on frequency region.
FIGURE 6. Multi-unit IC results do not mirror single-unit IC results. (A) Mean spontaneous activity of multi-units for three BF bins (as indicated), as estimated by eMUA (left) or by tMUA (right). Asterisks indicate significant effects of anesthesia or noise exposure, as described in Figure 4. Numbers below points indicate the number of multi-units contributing to each average. (B–D) Average multi-unit driven-rate vs. level or frequency for eMUA, as averaged over three CF regions (three columns) and for four different stimuli (four rows), as indicated. Stimuli were as follows. (B) BF tones; (C) contralateral broadband noise; (D) tones at 20, 25, and 30 dB above threshold; or (E) ipsilateral noise with concurrent 30 dB SPL contralateral noise. Asterisks indicate significant effects of anesthesia (black) or noise exposure (green and red) as described in Figure 4.
Since quantification of eMUA does not require thresholding, we evaluated the effect of neuropathic exposures on tone- and noise-driven eMUA. Responses to tones at the BF were reduced at moderate-to-high levels in the non-neuropathic region of awake mice, and in the neuropathic region of both awake and anesthetized mice (Figure 6B). This trend was visible in single units, but more robust and highly significant here. Unlike in single units, noise-driven eMUA was reduced at moderate-to-high levels in the non-neuropathic region of anesthetized mice, and in the neuropathic region of both awake and anesthetized mice (Figure 6C). These reductions might be caused by a direct effect of cochlear neuropathy, which appears to selectively destroy high-threshold, low-SR auditory nerve fibers (Furman et al., 2013).
Driven responses to tones with frequencies both above and below BF were elevated in the neuropathic region of noise exposed mice (Figure 6D). Like the on-BF changes, this effect was also visible in single units, but was more robust in the eMUA. A potentially related loss of inhibition was observed as a reduction in the suppression of eMUA by ipsilateral noise (Figure 6E). This trend was visible but not significant in single-unit responses (data not shown).
Discussion
Tinnitus and hyperacusis often occur following an episode of noise exposure, and often begin immediately post exposure (Meikle, 1997; Holgers and Pettersson, 2005; Daniel, 2007). While they frequently co-occur, the relationship between them remains unclear (Kreuzer et al., 2012). They can last indefinitely and can be elicited whether or not there is a noise-induced PTS (Axelsson and Ringdahl, 1989; Shargorodsky et al., 2010). The tinnitus percept is often tonal in nature, and, when there is PTS, the pitch often falls within the PTS region (Sereda et al., 2011, 2015; Schecklmann et al., 2012), consistent with models suggesting it arises due to compensatory homeostatic plasticity in central auditory pathways response to reduced peripheral input (Schaette and Kempter, 2012; Noreña and Farley, 2013; Auerbach et al., 2014).
Since tinnitus is the percept of sound in the absence of sound, many neurophysiological studies have looked for noise-induced changes in spontaneous activity in auditory neurons from cochlea to cortex. Although some have suggested that changes to discharge synchrony across neuronal populations or increased spontaneous bursting activity might be key to tinnitus generation (reviewed by Roberts et al., 2010), most studies have concentrated on SRs.
In the auditory nerve, SR is either unchanged or reduced by acoustic overexposure (Liberman and Kiang, 1978; Liberman and Dodds, 1984; Furman et al., 2013). Auditory nerve SR is created at the inner hair cell synapse (Rutherford and Moser, 2016); thus, loss of inner hair cells will silence all the fibers that formerly contacted them (Liberman and Kiang, 1978). Damage to, or loss of, inner hair cell stereocilia, a common result of acoustic overstimulation, causes PTS and reduces auditory nerve SR by inactivating mechanoelectric transduction channels, thereby reducing leak current, hyperpolarizing the hair cell, and reducing vesicle release at the synapse (Liberman and Dodds, 1984). The auditory nerve synapses with inner hair cells are the most vulnerable elements to noise damage (Kujawa and Liberman, 2009), and synaptopathy silences those fibers that become disconnected from their peripheral targets. Noise-induced synaptopathy preferentially affects high-threshold, low-SR auditory nerve fibers, while the surviving low-threshold, high-SR fibers exhibit essentially normal responses (Furman et al., 2013). High- and low-SR fibers differ in their cochlear nucleus innervation patterns (Liberman, 1991, 1993); thus, synaptopathy might imbalance central circuits. Because synaptopathy can be widespread in ears without hair cell loss or PTS, it might underlie the generation of noise-induced tinnitus in ears with a normal audiogram. Our major aim in the present study was to evaluate this notion.
Hyperacusis is the sensation that moderate-level sounds are intolerably loud, and the search for neurophysiological correlates have typically examined rate-level functions to CF tones. At the auditory nerve level, as for SRs, noise damage generally reduces maximum discharge rates and the slopes of the tone-evoked rate-level functions (Liberman and Kiang, 1978; Heinz and Young, 2004). Although noise damage invariably reduces SR and tone-evoked rates in the auditory periphery, it often elevates SR and tone-evoked rates in higher auditory centers (Eggermont and Roberts, 2004), presumably via re-adjustment of “central gain” (Schaette and Kempter, 2012; Noreña and Farley, 2013; Auerbach et al., 2014). Hyperactivity has been reported in the IC of several species following acoustic overexposures that were either tonal or broadband, unilateral or bilateral, and in awake or anesthetized animals (Table 1). SR elevations can be as large as 50-fold, can be observed as soon as a few hours post-exposure, and can persist for at least a year (Table 1). While exposures used in most IC studies elicited >30 dB of PTS, elevated SRs have been reported in cases with minimal PTS in both mice (Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2011; Hesse et al., 2016) and chinchillas (Bauer et al., 2008). However, the nature and degree of cochlear damage has rarely been reported, and the role of synaptopathy as a key elicitor has only recently been evaluated (Hesse et al., 2016).
Effects of Noise Exposure on Spontaneous Rate
Here, bilateral exposure to octave-band noise at 98 dB SPL did not change population measures of SR in the IC, despite substantial permanent synaptic loss throughout a large portion of the basal cochlea (Figures 1, 2). This result was replicated in recordings from both anesthetized and awake animals. Subdividing the neural population into response types did not uncover any class-specific SR changes. Thus, noise-induced synaptic loss does not appear to be sufficient to cause increased SRs in the mouse IC, at least in mice exposed at 7 weeks of age.
We increased the noise level to produce a 50 dB PTS, thereby adding significant hair cell damage to the spectrum of cochlear pathology, but SRs remained unchanged (Figures 1, 2, blue diamonds). Particularly large SR increases have been reported in IC following unilateral exposures: 10 fold in mice (Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2011) and 17- to 50-fold in guinea pigs (Mulders and Robertson, 2009, 2011). Therefore, we exposed three groups of mice unilaterally to 8–16 kHz noise at three different intensities; however, SRs were not elevated in these groups either, regardless of the amount of PTS.
In interpreting results across studies, electrode style and reliance on single unit vs. MUA must be considered. Here, we got contradictory results when assessing data from the same penetrations as single- vs. MUA. Although single-unit records are likely more reliable, referring to Table 1 shows that this distinction does not resolve the discrepancy. Similarly, some studies used micropipettes with fine tips (3 μm), while we used silicon electrodes with large contact areas (177 μm2). The smaller micropipettes may detect signals from smaller neural elements, including fibers of passage; thus, different neural populations may be sampled. However, this distinction also does not resolve the discrepancy.
The presence of anesthesia during exposure can change cochlear vulnerability (Figure 1), and might also change exposure effects on central pathways. However, SR increases have been observed in animals exposed either awake or anesthetized (see Table 1), whereas we failed to see SR increases in animals exposed either way. In anesthetized mouse IC, Hesse et al. (2016) measured multi-unit SRs and reported a twofold to sevenfold increase 4 weeks following an 8–16 kHz, bilateral, 100 dB exposure of anesthetized animals. Computing multi-unit SRs in a similar way, we observed slight decreases at 1–3 weeks post-exposure (Figure 6).
Post-exposure survival could be an important variable, as noise-induced SR changes are likely dynamic in the hours and days after damage. In our study, SRs were measured at 1–3 weeks post-exposure, a shorter time period than the other mouse studies (Table 1). In a recent study in awake mice of exactly the same noise exposure used here, SRs in primary auditory cortex were unchanged 1 day post-exposure, but elevated threefold by 4 days and back to baseline by 1 week, where they remained for next 6 weeks post exposure (Resnik and Polley, 2017). The lack of SR changes observed here in the IC at 1–3 weeks post-exposure are consistent with that pattern. Noise-induced changes in SR appear stable at longer post-exposure times. In noise-exposed guinea pigs, SRs in the IC are unchanged immediately post-exposure, but elevated by 12 h, where they remain elevated for at least 8 weeks post-exposure (Mulders and Robertson, 2009, 2011, 2013). The sources of elevated SRs may migrate centrally at longer post-exposure times: in guinea pigs, cochlear ablation <4 weeks post-exposure returns SRs in the IC to control levels, whereas ablation >8 weeks post-exposure does not (Mulders and Robertson, 2009, 2011). However, there are no studies of SR post-exposure dynamics suggesting that elevated SRs would have developed in the present study if survival had been extended beyond 3 weeks.
Age at exposure could also be an important variable, as cochlear vulnerability to noise decreases dramatically in mice between 8 and 16 weeks (Kujawa and Liberman, 2006), and adolescent central auditory pathways may react differently to noise than in the adult. In our study, mice were exposed at 7 weeks, an age we have used for many recent studies of cochlear synaptopathy, because it is after the onset of sexual maturity (∼4–5 weeks), but before the period where vulnerability is changing rapidly with age (thereby introducing a potential source of variability). As seen in Table 1, this is a younger age than that in the other mouse studies, and, although cross-species age comparison is difficult, may also represent a relatively younger age than in all other studies summarized there. Changes in excitatory/inhibitory balance that must underlie noise-induced hyperactivity may be more effectively rebalanced in the young brain than in the adult. It is interesting, in this regard, that tinnitus is much less common in younger people than in adults (Martinez et al., 2015).
Another intriguing clue to the discrepancy between our results and prior studies is that our control SRs were significantly higher than in other mouse studies, e.g., 2.5 times higher than others in anesthetized mice (Ma et al., 2006; Hesse et al., 2016) and 6× higher than others in awake mice (Longenecker and Galazyuk, 2011). This, in turn, suggests a difference in pre-exposure excitatory/inhibitory balance in the IC, which is interesting given reports that the emergence of noise-induced SR elevations and tinnitus can also depend on pre-exposure state. Given identical noise exposure, gerbils with low pre-exposure spontaneous and sound-evoked neural activity in the auditory cortex and brainstem develop “tinnitus” and cortical SR elevations, while those with higher pre-exposure activity do not (Ahlf et al., 2012; Tziridis et al., 2015). The cause of the differences in pre-exposure state between the two groups is unclear, but may be critically important in understanding the genesis of tinnitus.
Effects of Noise Exposure on Sound-Evoked Responses
Here, we saw no significant noise-induced effects on the maximum tone-evoked rates of IC neurons, however the slopes of rate-level functions to CF tones were elevated in a subset of neurons (Figures 4, 5). This effect was seen only in neurons with CFs in the affected region (16–32 kHz), strongly suggesting it might be caused by the cochlear synaptopathy per se. The effect was also seen only in units with non-monotonic rate-level functions, suggesting that only a subset of the central circuitry appears to compensate for a reduced peripheral drive by increasing its overall gain. In the face of reduced peripheral input, the balance of excitation and inhibition may be adjusted to maintain detectability. Since non-monotonic neurons receive substantial on-CF inhibition, the shape of their rate-level functions may be particularly sensitive to this balance. Changes to the excitatory/inhibitory balance were also observed in response to off-CF tones (Figure 5) and to binaural noise (Figure 6). In the chinchilla IC, elevated evoked rates were observed in a larger proportion of non-monotonic neurons than monotonic neurons immediately following exposure to a high-intensity, above-CF tone (Wang et al., 1996). The authors suggest that this difference could be due to a reduction of lateral inhibition from the exposed region. Ketamine, an NDMA receptor agonist, reduces excitatory drive, which could mask changes to the excitatory/inhibitory balance and may be the reason that slopes, in our study, were unaffected by noise exposure in anesthetized mice (Figure 5).
In mice exposed to the same traumatic noise used here, both SR and tone rate-level slopes were transiently increased in auditory cortical neurons 3–4 days post exposure, recovering to baseline by 1 week (Resnik and Polley, 2017). Again using the same noise exposure, calcium imaging of cortico-collicular neurons revealed increased rate-level slopes 2 days post exposure that remained elevated to at least 2 weeks (Asokan et al., 2018). Here, we did not track neuronal responsiveness at post-exposure times <1 week, when larger effects might have been seen. More extensive and long-lasting changes to the excitatory/inhibitory balance were observed following near-complete cochlear synaptopathy by cochlear administration of a Na/K pump blocker (ouabain): despite a massive loss of peripheral input, tone rate-level slopes recovered in the IC and were elevated in the auditory cortex by 30 days post-treatment (Chambers et al., 2016). Clearly, the nature and extent of central hyperactivity following peripheral lesion is a complex function of the lesion severity, the region of the auditory pathway under study, the post-exposure time and the baseline state at the time of exposure (Table 1).
Tinnitus, Hyperacusis, and Central Gain
A number of tests are used to infer the presence of tinnitus in animals. One is based on pre-pulse inhibition (PPI) of the acoustic startle reflex, where the “prepulse” is a silent gap in an otherwise continuous background noise. The idea is that a tinnitus percept makes the noise gap less salient and reduces gap-induced PPI of startle (Turner et al., 2006). In a prior study of mice, evaluated 1–10 weeks after exposure to the same traumatic noise used here, animals showed reduction of gap-induced PPI when the background was narrow-band noise centered in the synaptopathic region (Hickox and Liberman, 2014). This might suggest a tonal tinnitus near 32 kHz. However, the effect was seen only with no delay between gap offset and startle onset, not with an 80-ms gap-startle delay. This dependence on stimulus timing casts doubt as to whether the tinnitus was really “filling the gap” and thus on the relevance of the gap-startle test to tinnitus. It also leaves open the question of whether this synaptopathic exposure produces tinnitus in mice.
Studies of SR in the DCN and thalamus report elevations only in animals with reduced gap-induced PPI (Dehmel et al., 2012; Li et al., 2013; Kalappa et al., 2014), while SRs in the IC and auditory cortex are elevated regardless of the results of a gap-induced PPI assay (Engineer et al., 2011; Coomber et al., 2014; Ropp et al., 2014). Elevated SRs have been reported in the DCN following synaptopathic noise exposures that cause no PTS in chinchillas (Bauer et al., 2008) and guinea pigs (Koehler and Shore, 2013). However, based on either gap-induced PPI (Koehler and Shore, 2013) or an operant conditioning test of tone perception (Bauer et al., 2008), only some exposed animals showed tinnitus. Together, these results suggest that synaptopathy alone is not sufficient to cause either elevated SRs or tinnitus. However, since all the studies in Table 1 likely cause significant synaptopathy, it remains possible that synaptopathy is a key factor in the generation of noise-induced tinnitus and SR elevation. Whether the reductions in ascending auditory drive from the periphery are translated into chronic hyperactivity in higher auditory centers likely depends on the intersubject differences in circuit plasticity within the central circuits themselves.
In a prior study, mice exposed to the same noise used here showed significant hypersensitivity to moderate-level sound, as seen in decreased thresholds and increased amplitudes of the acoustic startle reflex, as measured without PPI (Hickox and Liberman, 2014). Since the IC is a dominant contributor to these responses (Fendt et al., 2001), increased IC rate-level slopes may be key to the increased startle responses associated with cochlear synaptopathy, which in turn may be related to the decreased noise-level tolerance in humans with acoustic overexposure. If true, regeneration of the lost synaptic connections in the periphery, as has been accomplished via local cochlear deliver of neurotrophins in both mouse and guinea pig (Wan et al., 2014; Sly et al., 2016; Suzuki et al., 2016; Chen et al., 2018), might return the sound-evoked IC responses to normal. Of course, the efficacy of such a treatment in reversing the apparent changes in central gain might well depend on the duration of the loss of ascending drive to the central auditory pathways.
Author Contributions
LAS designed the experiments, collected and analyzed data, and wrote and edited the manuscript. MCL designed the experiments, and wrote and edited the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by grants from the NIDCD: R01 DC00188 and T32 DC 00038.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank Leslie Liberman for expert assistance in cochlear immunostaining. This work appeared as a preprint on the bioRxiv server (Shaheen and Liberman, 2018).
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnsys.2018.00059/full#supplementary-material
FIGURE S1 | Exemplars of four IC unit types based on frequency response areas. (A–D) Right column shows frequency response areas, color indicates firing rate, normalized to maximum rate. Left column shows mean rate-vs.-level functions (±SEMs) to CF tones (black) with red line showing best-fit functions with 10, 50, and 90% of the excitatory driven rate range (blue arrow) indicated by red circles. Inhibitory driven rate is indicated by green arrow.
FIGURE S2 | Metrics used for unit typing based on frequency response areas. (A) Excitatory/Inhibitory balance, the difference between excitatory and inhibitory driven rates (see Supplementary Figure S1) normalized by their sum. (B) The bandwidth of the excitatory response at 10 dB above threshold.
FIGURE S3 | Unit types based on binaural-noise response areas, with normalized rate (to maximum rate) for 8 control exemplars. (A) Excitatory/Inhibitory (EI). (B) Excitatory/Inhibitory (EI), contra-non-monotonic. (C,D) Excitatory/Inhibitory with ipsilateral facilitation (EI/f), contra-non-monotonic. (E) Excitatory/No Response (EO). (F) Excitatory/No Response (EO), contra-non-monotonic. (G) Inhibitory/Inhibitory (II). (H) Inhibitory/Inhibitory (II), contra-non-monotonic.
FIGURE S4 | IC Single-unit responses to noise were unchanged following neuropathic damage. Mean threshold (A), maximum driven rate (B), and slope of the rate-level functions (C) for contralateral broadband noise, extracted from the binaural noise response maps. Black asterisks indicate significant effects of anesthesia as described in Figure 4. There were no significant effects of exposure. No effects of exposure on spontaneous or sound-evoked rates were revealed by classifying neurons by tone-, binaural-noise, or contralateral-noise response types (data not shown).
Footnotes
- ^ http://www.masseyeandear.org/research/otolaryngology/investigators/laboratories/eaton-peabody-laboratories/epl-histology-resources/
References
Ahlf, S., Tziridis, K., Korn, S., Strohmeyer, I., and Schulze, H. (2012). Predisposition for and prevention of subjective tinnitus development. PLoS One 7:e44519. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044519
Asokan, M. M., Williamson, R. S., Hancock, K. E., and Polley, D. B. (2018). Sensory overamplification in layer 5 auditory corticofugal projection neurons following cochlear nerve synaptic damage. Nat. Commun. 9:2468. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-04852-y
Auerbach, B. D., Rodrigues, P. V., and Salvi, R. J. (2014). Central gain control in tinnitus and hyperacusis. Neuro Otol. 5:206. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2014.00206
Axelsson, A., and Ringdahl, A. (1989). Tinnitus—a study of its prevalence and characteristics. Br. J. Audiol. 23, 53–62. doi: 10.3109/03005368909077819
Bauer, C. A., Turner, J. G., Caspary, D. M., Myers, K. S., and Brozoski, T. J. (2008). Tinnitus and inferior colliculus activity in chinchillas related to three distinct patterns of cochlear trauma. J. Neurosci. Res. 86, 2564–2578. doi: 10.1002/jnr.21699
Bharadwaj, H. M., Masud, S., Mehraei, G., Verhulst, S., and Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (2015). Individual differences reveal correlates of hidden hearing deficits. J. Neurosci. 35, 2161–2172. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3915-14.2015
Brozoski, T. J., Bauer, C. A., and Caspary, D. M. (2002). Elevated fusiform cell activity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of chinchillas with psychophysical evidence of tinnitus. J. Neurosci. 22, 2383–2390. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-06-02383.2002
Cai, S., Ma, W.-L., and Young, E. (2009). Encoding intensity in ventral cochlear nucleus following acoustic trauma: implications for loudness recruitment. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 10, 5–22. doi: 10.1007/s10162-008-0142-y
Chambers, A. R., Resnik, J., Yuan, Y., Whitton, J. P., Edge, A. S., Liberman, M. C., et al. (2016). Central gain restores auditory processing following near-complete cochlear denervation. Neuron 89, 867–879. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.041
Chen, H., Xing, Y., Xia, L., Chen, Z., Yin, S., and Wang, J. (2018). AAV-mediated NT-3 overexpression protects cochleae against noise-induced synaptopathy. Gene Ther. 25, 251–259. doi: 10.1038/s41434-018-0012-0
Choi, Y. S., Koenig, M. A., Jia, X., and Thakor, N. V. (2010). Quantifying time-varying multiunit neural activity using entropy-based measures. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 57, 2771–2777. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2010.2049266
Chung, Y., Hancock, K. E., Nam, S.-I., and Delgutte, B. (2014). Coding of electric pulse trains presented through cochlear implants in the auditory midbrain of awake rabbit: comparison with anesthetized preparations. J. Neurosci. 34, 218–231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2084-13.2014
Coomber, B., Berger, J. I., Kowalkowski, V. L., Shackleton, T. M., Palmer, A. R., and Wallace, M. N. (2014). Neural changes accompanying tinnitus following unilateral acoustic trauma in the guinea pig. Eur. J. Neurosci. 40, 2427–2441. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12580
Daniel, E. (2007). Noise and hearing loss: a review. J. Sch. Health 77, 225–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2007.00197.x
Davis, K. A., Ramachandran, R., and May, B. J. (1999). Single-unit responses in the inferior colliculus of decerebrate cats, I. I. sensitivity to interaural level differences. J. Neurophysiol. 82, 164–175. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.1.164
Dehmel, S., Pradhan, S., Koehler, S., Bledsoe, S., and Shore, S. (2012). Noise overexposure alters long-term somatosensory-auditory processing in the dorsal cochlear nucleus-possible basis for tinnitus-related hyperactivity? J. Neurosci. 32, 1660–1671. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4608-11.2012
Dong, S., Mulders, W. H. A. M., Rodger, J., Woo, S., and Robertson, D. (2010). Acoustic trauma evokes hyperactivity and changes in gene expression in guinea-pig auditory brainstem. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 1616–1628. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07183.x
Eggermont, J. J., and Roberts, L. E. (2004). The neuroscience of tinnitus. Trends Neurosci. 27, 676–682. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.08.010
Egorova, M., Ehret, G., Vartanian, I., and Esser, K.-H. (2001). Frequency response areas of neurons in the mouse inferior colliculus. I. Threshold and tuning characteristics. Exp. Brain Res. 140, 145–161. doi: 10.1007/s002210100786
Engineer, N. D., Riley, J. R., Seale, J. D., Vrana, W. A., Shetake, J. A., Sudanagunta, S. P., et al. (2011). Reversing pathological neural activity using targeted plasticity. Nature 470, 101–104. doi: 10.1038/nature09656
Fendt, M., Li, L., and Yeomans, J. S. (2001). Brain stem circuits mediating prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex. Psychopharmacology 156, 216–224. doi: 10.1007/s002130100794
Furman, A. C., Kujawa, S. G., and Liberman, M. C. (2013). Noise-induced cochlear neuropathy is selective for fibers with low spontaneous rates. J. Neurophysiol. 110, 577–586. doi: 10.1152/jn.00164.2013
Gu, J. W., Halpin, C. F., Nam, E.-C., Levine, R. A., and Melcher, J. R. (2010). Tinnitus, diminished sound-level tolerance, and elevated auditory activity in humans with clinically normal hearing sensitivity. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 3361–3370. doi: 10.1152/jn.00226.2010
Gu, J. W., Herrmann, B. S., Levine, R. A., and Melcher, J. R. (2012). Brainstem auditory evoked potentials suggest a role for the ventral cochlear nucleus in tinnitus. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 13, 819–833. doi: 10.1007/s10162-012-0344-1
Heeringa, A. N., and van Dijk, P. (2014). The dissimilar time course of temporary threshold shifts and reduction of inhibition in the inferior colliculus following intense sound exposure. Hear. Res. 312, 38–47. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2014.03.004
Heil, P., Neubauer, H., and Irvine, D. R. F. (2011). An improved model for the rate–level functions of auditory-nerve fibers. J. Neurosci. 31, 15424–15437. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1638-11.2011
Heinz, M. G., and Young, E. D. (2004). Response growth with sound level in auditory-nerve fibers after noise-induced hearing loss. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 784–795. doi: 10.1152/jn.00776.2003
Hesse, L. L., Bakay, W. M. H., Ong, H. C., Anderson, L. A., Ashmore, J., McAlpine, D., et al. (2016). Non-monotonic relation between noise exposure severity and neuronal hyperactivity in the auditory midbrain. Front. Neurol. 7:133. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00133
Hickox, A. E., and Liberman, M. C. (2014). Is noise-induced cochlear neuropathy key to the generation of hyperacusis or tinnitus? J. Neurophysiol. 111, 552–564. doi: 10.1152/jn.00184.2013
Hill, D. N., Mehta, S. B., and Kleinfeld, D. (2011). Quality metrics to accompany spike sorting of extracellular signals. J. Neurosci. 31, 8699–8705. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0971-11.2011
Holgers, K., and Pettersson, B. (2005). Noise exposure and subjective hearing symptoms among school children in Sweden. Noise Health 7, 27–37. doi: 10.4103/1463-1741.31635
Irving, R., and Harrison, J. M. (1967). The superior olivary complex and audition: a comparative study. J. Comp. Neurol. 130, 77–86. doi: 10.1002/cne.901300105
Ito, T., Bishop, D. C., and Oliver, D. L. (2016). Functional organization of the local circuit in the inferior colliculus. Anat. Sci. Int. 91, 22–34. doi: 10.1007/s12565-015-0308-8
Kalappa, B. I., Brozoski, T. J., Turner, J. G., and Caspary, D. M. (2014). Single unit hyperactivity and bursting in the auditory thalamus of awake rats directly correlates with behavioural evidence of tinnitus. J. Physiol. 592, 5065–5078. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.278572
Kayser, C., Petkov, C. I., and Logothetis, N. K. (2007). Tuning to sound frequency in auditory field potentials. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 1806–1809. doi: 10.1152/jn.00358.2007
Koehler, S. D., and Shore, S. E. (2013). Stimulus timing-dependent plasticity in dorsal cochlear nucleus is altered in tinnitus. J. Neurosci. 33, 19647–19656. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2788-13.2013
Kreuzer, P. M., Landgrebe, M., Schecklmann, M., Staudinger, S., and Langguth, B. (2012). Trauma-associated tinnitus: audiological, demographic and clinical characteristics. PLoS One 7:e45599. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045599
Kujawa, S. G., and Liberman, M. C. (2006). Acceleration of age-related hearing loss by early noise exposure: evidence of a misspent youth. J. Neurosci. 26, 2115–2123. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4985-05.2006
Kujawa, S. G., and Liberman, M. C. (2009). Adding insult to injury: cochlear nerve degeneration after “temporary” noise-induced hearing loss. J. Neurosci. 29, 14077–14085. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2845-09.2009
Kujawa, S. G., and Liberman, M. C. (2015). Synaptopathy in the noise-exposed and aging cochlea: primary neural degeneration in acquired sensorineural hearing loss. Hear. Res. 330, 191–199. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2015.02.009
Li, S., Choi, V., and Tzounopoulos, T. (2013). Pathogenic plasticity of Kv7.2/3 channel activity is essential for the induction of tinnitus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 9980–9985. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302770110
Liberman, M. C. (1991). Central projections of auditory-nerve fibers of differing spontaneous rate. I. Anteroventral cochlear nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 313, 240–258. doi: 10.1002/cne.903130205
Liberman, M. C. (1993). Central projections of auditory nerve fibers of differing spontaneous rate, II: posteroventral and dorsal cochlear nuclei. J. Comp. Neurol. 327, 17–36. doi: 10.1002/cne.903270103
Liberman, M. C., and Dodds, L. W. (1984). Single-neuron labeling and chronic cochlear pathology. II. Stereocilia damage and alterations of spontaneous discharge rates. Hear. Res. 16, 43–53. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(84)90024-8
Liberman, M. C., and Kiang, N. Y. (1978). Acoustic trauma in cats. Cochlear pathology and auditory-nerve activity. Acta Otolaryngol. Suppl. 358, 1–63.
Longenecker, R. J., and Galazyuk, A. V. (2011). Development of tinnitus in CBA/CaJ mice following sound exposure. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 12, 647–658. doi: 10.1007/s10162-011-0276-1
Ma, W.-L. D., Hidaka, H., and May, B. J. (2006). Spontaneous activity in the inferior colliculus of CBA/J mice after manipulations that induce tinnitus. Hear. Res. 212, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2005.10.003
Manohar, S., Spoth, J., Radziwon, K., Auerbach, B. D., and Salvi, R. (2017). Noise-induced hearing loss induces loudness intolerance in a rat Active Sound Avoidance Paradigm (ASAP). Hear. Res. 353, 197–203. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2017.07.001
Manzoor, N. F., Gao, Y., Licari, F., and Kaltenbach, J. A. (2013). Comparison and contrast of noise-induced hyperactivity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus and inferior colliculus. Hear. Res. 295, 114–123. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2012.04.003
Manzoor, N. F., Licari, F. G., Klapchar, M., Elkin, R. L., Gao, Y., Chen, G., et al. (2012). Noise-induced hyperactivity in the inferior colliculus: its relationship with hyperactivity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 976–988. doi: 10.1152/jn.00833.2011
Martinez, C., Wallenhorst, C., McFerran, D., and Hall, D. A. (2015). Incidence rates of clinically significant tinnitus: 10-year trend from a cohort study in England. Ear Hear. 36, e69–e75. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000121
Meikle, M. B. (1997). Electronic access to tinnitus data: the oregon tinnitus data archive. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 117, 698–700. doi: 10.1016/S0194-5998(97)70055-X
Mulders, W. H., and Robertson, D. (2009). Hyperactivity in the auditory midbrain after acoustic trauma: dependence on cochlear activity. Neuroscience 164, 733–746. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.08.036
Mulders, W. H., and Robertson, D. (2011). Progressive centralization of midbrain hyperactivity after acoustic trauma. Neuroscience 192, 753–760. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.06.046
Mulders, W. H., and Robertson, D. (2013). Development of hyperactivity after acoustic trauma in the guinea pig inferior colliculus. Hear. Res. 298, 104–108. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2012.12.008
Müller, M., Hünerbein, K., von Hoidis, S., and Smolders, J. W. T. (2005). A physiological place–frequency map of the cochlea in the CBA/J mouse. Hear. Res. 202, 63–73. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2004.08.011
Noreña, A. J., and Farley, B. J. (2013). Tinnitus-related neural activity: theories of generation, propagation, and centralization. Hear. Res. 295, 161–171. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2012.09.010
Ollo, C., and Schwartz, I. R. (1979). The superior olivary complex in C57BL/6 mice. Am. J. Anat. 155, 349–373. doi: 10.1002/aja.1001550306
Palmer, A. R., Shackleton, T. M., Sumner, C. J., Zobay, O., and Rees, A. (2013). Classification of frequency response areas in the inferior colliculus reveals continua not discrete classes. J. Physiol. 591, 4003–4025. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2013.255943
Park, T. J., and Pollak, G. D. (1993). GABA shapes sensitivity to interaural intensity disparities in the mustache bat’s inferior colliculus: implications for encoding sound location. J. Neurosci. 13, 2050–2067. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-05-02050.1993
Ramachandran, R., Davis, K. A., and May, B. J. (1999). Single-Unit responses in the inferior colliculus of decerebrate cats, I. Classification based on frequency response maps. J. Neurophysiol. 82, 152–163. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.1.152
Resnik, J., and Polley, D. B. (2017). Fast-spiking GABA circuit dynamics in the auditory cortex predict recovery of sensory processing following peripheral nerve damage. eLife 6:e21452. doi: 10.7554/eLife.21452
Roberts, L. E., Eggermont, J. J., Caspary, D. M., Shore, S. E., Melcher, J. R., and Kaltenbach, J. A. (2010). Ringing ears: the neuroscience of tinnitus. J. Neurosci. 30, 14972–14979. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4028-10.2010
Ropp, T.-J. F., Tiedemann, K. L., Young, E. D., and May, B. J. (2014). Effects of unilateral acoustic trauma on tinnitus-related spontaneous activity in the inferior colliculus. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 15, 1007–1022. doi: 10.1007/s10162-014-0488-2
Rutherford, M. A., and Moser, T. (2016). “The ribbon synapse between type I spiral ganglion neurons and inner hair cells,” in The Primary Auditory Neurons of the Mammalian Cochlea, eds A. Dabdoub, B. Fritzsch, A. N. Popper, and R. R. Fay (New York, NY: Springer), 117–156.
Sachs, M. B., and Abbas, P. J. (1974). Rate versus level functions for auditory-nerve fibers in cats: tone-burst stimuli. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 56, 1835–1847. doi: 10.1121/1.1903521
Salvi, R. J., Saunders, S. S., Gratton, M. A., Arehole, S., and Powers, N. (1990). Enhanced evoked response amplitudes in the inferior colliculus of the chinchilla following acoustic trauma. Hear. Res. 50, 245–257. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(90)90049-U
Schaette, R., and Kempter, R. (2012). Computational models of neurophysiological correlates of tinnitus. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 6:34. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2012.00034
Schaette, R., and McAlpine, D. (2011). Tinnitus with a normal audiogram: physiological evidence for hidden hearing loss and computational model. J. Neurosci. 31, 13452–13457. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2156-11.2011
Schecklmann, M., Vielsmeier, V., Steffens, T., Landgrebe, M., Langguth, B., and Kleinjung, T. (2012). Relationship between audiometric slope and tinnitus pitch in tinnitus patients: insights into the mechanisms of tinnitus generation. PLoS One 7:e34878. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034878
Seki, S., and Eggermont, J. J. (2003). Changes in spontaneous firing rate and neural synchrony in cat primary auditory cortex after localized tone-induced hearing loss. Hear. Res. 180, 28–38. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(03)00074-1
Sereda, M., Edmondson-Jones, M., and Hall, D. A. (2015). Relationship between tinnitus pitch and edge of hearing loss in individuals with a narrow tinnitus bandwidth. Int. J. Audiol. 54, 249–256. doi: 10.3109/14992027.2014.979373
Sereda, M., Hall, D. A., Bosnyak, D. J., Edmondson-Jones, M., Roberts, L. E., Adjamian, P., et al. (2011). Re-examining the relationship between audiometric profile and tinnitus pitch. Int. J. Audiol. 50, 303–312. doi: 10.3109/14992027.2010.551221
Shaheen, L., and Liberman, M. C. (2018). Effects of cochlear synaptopathy on spontaneous and sound-evoked activity in the mouse inferior colliculus. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/381087
Shaheen, L. A., Valero, M. D., and Liberman, M. C. (2015). Towards a diagnosis of cochlear neuropathy with envelope following responses. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 16, 727–745. doi: 10.1007/s10162-015-0539-3
Shargorodsky, J., Curhan, G. C., and Farwell, W. R. (2010). Prevalence and characteristics of tinnitus among US Adults. Am. J. Med. 123, 711–718. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2010.02.015
Sly, D. J., Campbell, L., Uschakov, A., Saief, S. T., Lam, M., and O’Leary, S. J. (2016). Applying neurotrophins to the round window rescues auditory function and reduces inner hair cell synaptopathy after noise-induced hearing loss. Otol. Neurotol. Off. Publ. Am. Otol. Soc. Am. Neurotol. Soc. Eur. Acad. Otol. Neurotol. 37, 1223–1230. doi: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000001191
Supèr, H., and Roelfsema, P. R. (2005). Chronic multiunit recordings in behaving animals: advantages and limitations. Prog. Brain Res. 147, 263–282. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(04)47020-4
Suzuki, J., Corfas, G., and Liberman, M. C. (2016). Round-window delivery of neurotrophin 3 regenerates cochlear synapses after acoustic overexposure. Sci. Rep. 6:24907. doi: 10.1038/srep24907
Taberner, A. M., and Liberman, M. C. (2005). Response properties of single auditory nerve fibers in the mouse. J. Neurophysiol. 93, 557–569. doi: 10.1152/jn.00574.2004
Torterolo, P., Falconi, A., Morales-Cobas, G., and Velluti, R. A. (2002). Inferior colliculus unitary activity in wakefulness, sleep and under barbiturates. Brain Res. 935, 9–15. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02235-7
Turner, J. G., Brozoski, T. J., Bauer, C. A., Parrish, J. L., Myers, K., Hughes, L. F., et al. (2006). Gap detection deficits in rats with tinnitus: a potential novel screening tool. Behav. Neurosci. 120, 188–195. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.1.188
Tziridis, K., Ahlf, S., Jeschke, M., Happel, M. F. K., Ohl, F. W., and Schulze, H. (2015). Noise trauma induced neural plasticity throughout the auditory system of Mongolian gerbils: differences between tinnitus developing and non-developing animals. Front. Neurol. 6:22. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00022
Valero, M. D., Hancock, K. E., and Liberman, M. C. (2016). The middle ear muscle reflex in the diagnosis of cochlear neuropathy. Hear. Res. 332, 29–38. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2015.11.005
Vogler, D. P., Robertson, D., and Mulders, W. H. (2014). Hyperactivity following unilateral hearing loss in characterized cells in the inferior colliculus. Neuroscience 265, 28–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.01.017
Wan, G., Gómez-Casati, M. E., Gigliello, A. R., Liberman, M. C., and Corfas, G. (2014). Neurotrophin-3 regulates ribbon synapse density in the cochlea and induces synapse regeneration after acoustic trauma. eLife 3:e03564. doi: 10.7554/eLife.03564
Wang, J., Salvi, R. J., and Powers, N. (1996). Plasticity of response properties of inferior colliculus neurons following acute cochlear damage. J. Neurophysiol. 75, 171–183. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.1.171
Keywords: cochlear synaptopathy, tinnitus, hyperacusis, noise exposure, inferior colliculus
Citation: Shaheen LA and Liberman MC (2018) Cochlear Synaptopathy Changes Sound-Evoked Activity Without Changing Spontaneous Discharge in the Mouse Inferior Colliculus. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 12:59. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2018.00059
Received: 04 September 2018; Accepted: 26 October 2018;
Published: 03 December 2018.
Edited by:
Preston E. Garraghty, Indiana University Bloomington, United StatesReviewed by:
Todd M. Mowery, New York University, United StatesAndrej Kral, Hannover Medical School, Germany
Copyright © 2018 Shaheen and Liberman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Luke A. Shaheen, shaheenl@ohsu.edu