Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 17 June 2014
This article is part of the Research Topic Distributed networks: new outlooks on cerebellar function. View all 16 articles

The cerebellum for jocks and nerds alike

  • Department of Neuroscience, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA

Historically the cerebellum has been implicated in the control of movement. However, the cerebellum's role in non-motor functions, including cognitive and emotional processes, has also received increasing attention. Starting from the premise that the uniform architecture of the cerebellum underlies a common mode of information processing, this review examines recent electrophysiological findings on the motor signals encoded in the cerebellar cortex and then relates these signals to observations in the non-motor domain. Simple spike firing of individual Purkinje cells encodes performance errors, both predicting upcoming errors as well as providing feedback about those errors. Further, this dual temporal encoding of prediction and feedback involves a change in the sign of the simple spike modulation. Therefore, Purkinje cell simple spike firing both predicts and responds to feedback about a specific parameter, consistent with computing sensory prediction errors in which the predictions about the consequences of a motor command are compared with the feedback resulting from the motor command execution. These new findings are in contrast with the historical view that complex spikes encode errors. Evaluation of the kinematic coding in the simple spike discharge shows the same dual temporal encoding, suggesting this is a common mode of signal processing in the cerebellar cortex. Decoding analyses show the considerable accuracy of the predictions provided by Purkinje cells across a range of times. Further, individual Purkinje cells encode linearly and independently a multitude of signals, both kinematic and performance errors. Therefore, the cerebellar cortex's capacity to make associations across different sensory, motor and non-motor signals is large. The results from studying how Purkinje cells encode movement signals suggest that the cerebellar cortex circuitry can support associative learning, sequencing, working memory, and forward internal models in non-motor domains.

Introduction

The role of the cerebellum in the nervous system remains controversial. Traditionally, cerebellar function has been viewed in the context of motor control, given the well-established fact that cerebellar insults result in movement deficits. Different aspects of the cerebellum's involvement in motor control have generated multiple hypotheses regarding cerebellar function including movement timing (Braitenberg and Atwood, 1958; Keele and Ivry, 1990; Welsh et al., 1995; O'Reilly et al., 2008), error detection and correction (Oscarsson, 1980), motor learning (Marr, 1969; Albus, 1971; Gilbert and Thach, 1977; Ito, 2002), and providing internal models (Wolpert et al., 1998; Kawato, 1999; Imamizu et al., 2000; Morton and Bastian, 2006; Shadmehr et al., 2010).

More recently, evidence for cerebellar involvement in non-motor processes such as cognition, emotions and social interaction has accumulated at a rapid pace. The findings include the rapid expansion of the cerebellar hemispheres in primates (Leiner et al., 1986, 1989) with development of projections between the cerebellum and non-motor cortical areas (Schmahmann and Pandya, 1989, 1991, 1997; Middleton and Strick, 1994, 2001; Kelly and Strick, 2003), cerebellar activation related to cognitive behaviors (Petersen et al., 1988; Kim et al., 1994; Hayter et al., 2007), cognitive and emotional dysfunction associated with cerebellar lesions/disease (Fiez et al., 1992; Schmahmann, 2004; Burk, 2007), and the influence on cognitive processes by manipulating cerebellar excitability (Ferrucci et al., 2008; Pope and Miall, 2012; Boehringer et al., 2013). These contributions to non-motor behaviors raise the question of whether the cerebellum performs specific computations/functions in different domains or performs a common process across all domains. Based on its stereotypical architecture, a plausible and parsimonious hypothesis is that the cerebellum performs a uniform process in both motor and non-motor processes (Schmahmann, 2000, 2010; Ramnani, 2006; Thach, 2007; Ito, 2008). An important implication of this hypothesis is that understanding cerebellar information processing in the motor domain can illuminate the contributions of the cerebellum in non-motor domains.

Working from the common processing viewpoint, this review argues that the present state of our understanding of the cerebellum's role in motor behavior can shed light on non-motor functions. Several authors have taken a similar perspective (Ito, 2008; Imamizu and Kawato, 2009; Pezzulo, 2011; Pezzulo et al., 2012). An advantage of applying a motor control view to non-motor processing is that subjects, including non-human primates, can be required to perform extremely demanding motoric tasks in highly controlled environments. Despite task complexity, motor behavior can be described and quantified by well-defined measures, allowing intimate and unambiguous access to cerebellar signals. Therefore, this review evaluates the motor signals encoded by cerebellar neurons, including the existence of a new class of signals in Purkinje cell simple spike activity related to performance errors, as well as a dual encoding mechanism for motor parameters (Hewitt et al., 2011; Popa et al., 2012). Together, these signals could provide the neural substrate for computing sensory prediction errors postulated by internal model hypotheses (Wolpert and Ghahramani, 2000; Mazzoni and Krakauer, 2006; Shadmehr et al., 2010). We suggest these new findings regarding simple spike signaling in motor behaviors provide insights into cerebellar function that could help understanding cerebellar involvement in non-motor domains.

Cerebellum and Motor Domain

Kinematic Signals

Numerous studies have documented that simple spike activity encodes kinematic variables, including position, velocity, speed and acceleration that describe body part motions without consideration of their causes such as forces or joint torques. This common encoding of kinematics is true for different effectors and motor behaviors. In the intermediate zone and its neighboring lateral zones surrounding the primary fissure, Purkinje cells encode position, direction, amplitude, velocity and speed of arm movements (Thach, 1970; Harvey et al., 1977; Mano and Yamamoto, 1980; Marple-Horvat and Stein, 1987; Fortier et al., 1989; Fu et al., 1997; Coltz et al., 1999; Roitman et al., 2005; Pasalar et al., 2006). These regions in the monkey are analogous to the regions in the human cerebellum engaged during limb movements (Stoodley and Schmahmann, 2009; Timmann et al., 2009). In the floccular complex, Purkinje cells encode eye position, velocity and acceleration during smooth pursuit and ocular following (Stone and Lisberger, 1990; Shidara et al., 1993; Gomi et al., 1998; Medina and Lisberger, 2009). In the posterior vermis, similar kinematic encoding of the eye movements is present for saccades and smooth pursuit (Thier et al., 2002; Dash et al., 2013). An important issue in interpreting these results is the possible confound between effector kinematics and kinetics (Shidara et al., 1993; Ebner et al., 2011). However, a study designed to eliminate this confound demonstrated that Purkinje cells do not encode kinetics or muscle activity during manual tracking (Pasalar et al., 2006). We conclude that the representations of kinematics in the simple spike activity are unambiguous and ubiquitous, suggesting that the cerebellar cortex performs common processing for very different movements and effectors.

However, other confounds present in some experimental paradigms have introduced ambiguity in the results. For example, saccadic eye movements, circular tracking, and center-out reach all introduce high degrees of correlation among kinematic parameters (Paninski et al., 2004; Hewitt et al., 2011). Further, most paradigms do not provide a uniform or complete coverage of the work space of kinematic variables. Another confound in assessing if cell firing leads kinematics, required of forward internal hypotheses (Miall and Wolpert, 1996; Bastian, 2006; Shadmehr et al., 2010), is the predictability found in many tasks. Task predictability introduces the possibility that leading simple spike activity could be due to expectations related to upcoming trials rather than predicting the consequences of the motor command.

A random tracking task, briefly described in Figures 1A,B, eliminates or reduces these experimental problems and allows for a more systematic evaluation of how arm kinematics are encoded (Paninski et al., 2004). Therefore, we evaluated Purkinje cell firing during random tracking and verified that position, velocity and speed of the limb (denoted in Figure 1B by the C subscripted variables) were statistically independent (Hewitt et al., 2011). For a large majority of Purkinje cells, simple spike firing is modulated in relation to these kinematic parameters. The firing of individual Purkinje cells was characterized using lagged linear regressions of the simple spike firing with kinematics, identifying significant correlations and the timing of the strongest correlations (lead and/or lag). A similar approach has been used by a number of cerebellar investigators (Shidara et al., 1993; Gomi et al., 1998; Medina and Lisberger, 2009). Comparing the regression results based on individual parameters with those based on multiple parameters shows that signals encoding single parameters are mutually independent. Velocity is the dominant parameter, followed by position and then speed (Hewitt et al., 2011; Popa et al., 2012). The lead/lag values, the time intervals by which the neural activity leads (negative values) or lags (positive values) motor behavior, show a negative bias in which the simple spike firing tends to lead motor behavior, as observed in other studies (Marple-Horvat and Stein, 1987; Coltz et al., 1999; Roitman et al., 2005; Medina and Lisberger, 2009). Given the unpredictability of random tracking, this observation strongly supports the assumption that a majority of Purkinje cells predict the kinematic consequences of motor commands. However, the wide distribution of leads and lags, including both negative and positive values, also suggests the presence of both predictive and feedback signals (Marple-Horvat and Stein, 1987; Fu et al., 1997; Roitman et al., 2005). Remarkably, using the coefficients determined during random tracking allowed an accurate reconstruction of the simple spike activity recorded during different tasks, including circular tracking and center-out reach (Hewitt et al., 2011), strongly suggesting that the representations of arm kinematics are task-independent. This implies that a single global limb model is used widely as opposed to requiring large numbers of internal models for specific movements (Wolpert and Kawato, 1998; Imamizu et al., 2003, 2007).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Purkinje cell simple spike modulation in relation to task errors and hand kinematics during random tracking. (A) Using a robot manipulandum, moving in an horizontal plane, monkeys track a randomly moving target on a vertical screen, by keeping the cross-shaped cursor within the target (3 cm in diameter). Excursions of the cursor outside target area longer than 500 ms result in trial abort. (B) Geometry of the motor parameters. Target (gray circle) moves on a random trajectory (blue trace). Cursor (black cross) maps the hand movement (red trace). Hand kinematic parameters are position (Xc and Yc), velocity (VXc and VYc) and speed (magnitude of the hand velocity vector). Performance error parameters are position error (XE and YE) and RE (magnitude of the position error vector) and position director error (PDE—the difference between the direction of the position error vector and direction of hand movement). (C–E) Simple spike modulation for an example Purkinje cell is plotted in relation to performance errors: XE and YE (C), RE (D), and PDE (E). In (C) the firing rate is color coded relative to overall mean firing and in relation to the target (white circle). Plots of the simple spike firing with each error parameter based on the optimal τ obtained from the lagged regressions. (A) is reproduced from Hewitt et al. (2011) and (B–E) from Popa et al. (2012) with permission.

It is possible that the temporal relationships between cerebellar signals and motor behavior reflect hard-wired circuitry properties like synaptic delays and conduction times in which the distribution of lead/lag populations would be normal, centered on well-defined time values. However, the distribution of the lead/lag values of the cerebellar signals during random tracking is extremely broad and quasi-uniform (Hewitt et al., 2011). This observation suggests that the temporal properties of the cerebellar signals are the results of computational processes reflecting the temporal constrains and requirements necessary to execute motor behaviors. For example, as internal model predictions are expected in various motor sequences involved in implementation of motor behavior, the predictions need to be “broadcasted” over a wide range of time intervals. The temporal properties of the kinematic representation are specific to different structures and regions. For example, in the primary motor cortex the distribution of the kinematic leads/lags during a continuous spiral tracing experiment is uni-modal and centered around a 100 ms lead, as determined from signals conveying very accurate representations of the trajectory, while premotor cortex leads/lags are distributed bi-modally around 250 and 0 ms (Moran and Schwartz, 1999). Also, the timing of the signals in the motor cortices are dependent on trajectory curvature (Moran and Schwartz, 1999), while the cerebellar representations are independent of the curvature (Hewitt et al., 2011). These findings highlight fundamental functional differences in kinematic signaling in the cerebellum vs. the motor cortical areas.

Error Processing in the Cerebellum—Complex Spikes or Simple Spikes?

Error processing has been a long-standing hypothesis of cerebellar function, (Oscarsson, 1980) and there is a long history of studies focused on identifying error-related signals in cerebellar activity. The dominant hypothesis is that the error signals are encoded by the complex spike discharge of Purkinje cells (Oscarsson, 1980; Ito, 2000). An error encoding role of complex spikes has been proposed, not only in the motor domain, but also in the cerebellum's role in non-motor behaviors (Ito, 2008; Schmahmann, 2010; Koziol et al., 2012; Yamazaki and Nagao, 2012). Observations favoring this hypothesis in the motor domain are the complex spike modulation occurring with retinal-slip (Graf et al., 1988; Barmack and Shojaku, 1995; Kobayashi et al., 1998) and induced saccadic errors during eye movements (Soetedjo et al., 2008). Also, complex spike discharge modulates with reach end point errors (Kitazawa et al., 1998), learning a predictable target redirection during smooth pursuit (Medina and Lisberger, 2008), redirection of reaching (Kim et al., 1987), responding to unexpected loads (Gilbert and Thach, 1977), and adaptation to visuomotor transformations (Ojakangas and Ebner, 1994). However, many other experiments found no clear relationship between motor errors and complex spike discharge. Complex spike modulation could not be related to direction or speed errors during center-out reaching (Ebner et al., 2002) nor with eye movement errors during saccade and smooth pursuit learning (Catz et al., 2005; Dash et al., 2010). Also, perturbations and performance errors during reaching in cats failed to evoke responses in inferior olive neurons, the origin of the climbing fiber projection (Horn et al., 1996). An intriguing observation is that complex spike modulation in the oculomotor vermis occurs late in eye movement adaptation and persists after learning has stabilized (Catz et al., 2005; Dash et al., 2010; Prsa and Thier, 2011), which is inconsistent with the traditional error signal hypothesis. Additionally, complex spike error signals occur in a small fraction of trials and are evident only after extensive averaging (Ojakangas and Ebner, 1994; Kitazawa et al., 1998). Also, the very low complex spike firing frequency provides a limited bandwidth to encode the continuous error signals that occur during movements (Ebner et al., 2011).

An open question is whether simple spike discharge encodes errors. Until recently there was limited information on the presence of error signals in the simple spike discharge. In a reaching task, the simple spike activity was modulated with trial success or failure (Greger and Norris, 2005). During manual tracking, simple spike discharge was correlated with direction and speed errors (Roitman et al., 2009), however, the interpretation was confounded because the error parameters were not statistically independent from the kinematics. Instructive signals in the simple spike firing contribute to cerebellar-dependent learning in the vestibulo-ocular reflex (Ke et al., 2009), also suggesting the presence of error signals.

The random tracking paradigm has numerous advantages for addressing these questions about simple spike error encoding. Random tracking involves a high degree of difficulty and the monkeys make frequent excursions outside the target area that require correction in 500 ms to avoid a trial abort (Hewitt et al., 2011). As a result, the task requires continuous evaluation of motor performance and implementing corrective movements to compensate for errors. Performance errors, defined as the divergence between the current movement goal and the consequence of the motor commands, were characterized based on the relative movement between the target center and the hand-controlled cursor. The performance errors evaluated included the cursor position relative to the target center (XE and YE), the distance between cursor and target center (RE) and angular distance from the direction necessary to move from the current position to the target center (PDE) (Popa et al., 2012). These defined performance errors, depicted in Figure 1B, assume that the target center is the current movement goal. Accordingly, the probability density functions of the kinematics and performance measures show the animals strive to keep the cursor in the center of the target and prefer to move toward the target center. It is interesting to note that the error parameters, although related to arm movement, are independent of the kinematic variables, thus forming a novel, non-kinematic class of motor variables related to task execution.

We found that simple spike firing robustly modulates with all four error parameters. As shown for an example Purkinje cell in Figure 1, the simple spike activity increases with both XE and YE, resulting in a planar pattern characterized by increased firing in the upper right quadrant and decreased firing in the lower left quadrant of the circular target (Figure 1C). Firing increases linearly with RE (Figure 1D) and modulates with PDE (Figure 1E), demonstrating that individual Purkinje cells can simultaneously encode a complex representation of performance errors. The frequency of significant error-related modulation was extremely high, with over 90% of Purkinje cells modulated with respect to XE, YE, and RE and over 80% with PDE. In contrast with the view that error coding is relegated to the complex spike discharge, simple spike firing carries a wealth of information about performance errors.

Dual Temporal Encoding of Errors

An intriguing aspect of the simple spike modulation with these error parameters is the temporal properties. A natural assumption would be that Purkinje cell firing either leads or lags the behavior by a single, constant time interval at which the firing pattern best correlates with the behavioral parameter. However, examination of the simple spike modulation at different leads and lags reveals a different story. In the example shown in Figure 2, maps of the simple spike firing relative to XE and YE show a modulation pattern characterized by high firing in the lower right quadrant at −480 to −400 ms (i.e., leading the position error) that fades as the time shift approaches 0 ms. Therefore, firing precedes the position errors by 400 ms and suggests simple spike encoding predicts the sensory consequences of the motor command. However, a new modulation pattern emerges at a lag of approximately 100 ms, with high firing at the target edge, except for the lower right quadrant of the error space. This new modulation pattern then fades as the lags approach 500 ms. Therefore, the simple spike firing also lags position error by 100 ms suggesting that firing is modulated by sensory feedback. Interestingly, the modulation patterns at different leads and lags are complementary: the region of high firing at lead time (−400 ms) coincides with the region of low firing at lag time (100 ms). This example shows that encoding of error parameters by individual cells can include both a prediction of the sensory consequences of motor commands as well as sensory feedback.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Time course of the simple spike modulation with performance errors. Each plot is the simple spike firing in relation to XE and YE as a function of time (τ) from an example Purkinje cell. Negative τ represents the firing leading the error signals. As in Figure 1, simple spike firing rate is color coded relative to overall mean firing and in relation to the target (white circle).

The analyses designed to quantify and characterize the encoding of the error variables across the population of Purkinje cells had to clear two hurdles. The first was to capture the complex temporal properties described above. To accomplish this, we used repeated linear regressions of the instantaneous firing against the error parameters at all lags to characterize the temporal relationships between Purkinje cell firing and behavioral variables. The second hurdle was to ensure that the encoding is not due to interactions with other variables, such as kinematics. Here we used linear regressions for each error parameter based on residual firing that was obtained by eliminating the firing variability associated with known variables such as kinematics and the other error parameters (Popa et al., 2012). These analyses yield, as functions of the lead or lag (τ), measures of the goodness of fit [the coefficient of determination (R2)] and firing sensitivity [the regression coefficients (β)] for each parameter. Figure 3 exemplifies the R2 and β profiles computed only for XE (A,B, respectively) and YE (D,E, respectively) although the cell (the same as in Figure 1) significantly encodes all errors (XE, YE, RE, and PDE). Both R2 profiles are bi-modal (Figures 3A,D), with two local maxima. Each R2 profile has a peak at negative time values (XE at −400 ms and YE at −300 ms) and a peak at positive time values (XE at 200 ms and YE at 250 ms). Our interpretation of these observations is that XE and YE are dually encoded by predictive and feedback-related signals in the simple spike discharge of a single cell. Importantly, the discharge sensitivity changes sign for both β profiles, with negative values for the predictive representations and positive values for the feedback representations (Figures 3B,E), showing opposing modulations for the predictive and feedback signals encoding the same behavioral parameter. Dual temporal encoding is common. It was observed in 72% of Purkinje cells and 74% of dually encoded parameters show opposing modulations between predictive and feedback signals (Popa et al., 2012). Interestingly, a similar dual encoding mechanism was discovered when monkeys perform a rule processing task where the signals between prefrontal and parietal cortices exhibit dual timing at 50 and 150 ms with anti-correlated modulation (Crowe et al., 2013).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Dual temporal encoding of error signals. (A,D) R2 as a function of lead/lag (τ) for position error parameters (XE and YE, respectively) from the Purkinje cell shown in Figure 1. (B,E) Regression coefficients for XE (βXE) and YE (βYE) as a function of τ. Arrows denote the times of the significant R2 peaks and error bars the confidence intervals of βXE and βYE at the times of the R2 maxima [arrows in (A,D), respectively]. Note change in the sign of regression coefficient showing the reversal in the firing sensitivity at predictive versus feedback timing. (C,F) Plot of the decoded upcoming XE and YE (mean ± SD) versus observed based on the population of Purkinje cells. Slope of decoded estimate versus the observed is 0.99 (ρ = 0.95, p < 0.001) for XE and 0.98 (ρ = 0.83, p < 0.001) for YE showing the accuracy of the prediction. Decoding estimates were from 25 repetitions of the off-line decoding algorithm (see Popa et al., 2012). Plots are reproduced from Popa et al. (2012) with permission.

The presence and ubiquity of the dual error signal representations can be interpreted in the context of internal models. Many researchers have postulated that the cerebellum acts as a forward internal model that predicts the sensory consequences of a motor command (Robinson, 1975; Miall and Wolpert, 1996; Wolpert and Ghahramani, 2000; Bastian, 2006; Shadmehr et al., 2010). Integral to implementing a forward internal model is comparing the prediction of the sensory consequences with the actual sensory feedback to compute sensory prediction errors. Sensory prediction errors are the critical signals that drive adaptation of both eye and limb movements (Wallman and Fuchs, 1998; Noto and Robinson, 2001; Mazzoni and Krakauer, 2006; Shadmehr et al., 2010). Functional imaging and patient studies suggest the cerebellum is involved in error processing, including sensory prediction errors (Diedrichsen et al., 2005; Morton and Bastian, 2006; Tseng et al., 2007; Xu-Wilson et al., 2009; Izawa et al., 2012).

Decoding analysis allows one to test whether the simple spike error signals in a population of Purkinje cells contain sufficient information to understand the behavior. Figure 3C,F show the results for decoding the predictions for XE and YE, respectively. Decoded values have highly linear correlations with the observed values for all error variables. Moreover, for XE, YE, and PDE the slope is very close to unity, showing that the Purkinje cell population provides a remarkably accurate prediction of the upcoming errors. As we have noted (Popa et al., 2012), there is some degradation of the decoding accuracy toward the boundaries of the error space that reflects either the relative scarcity of data points or that the predictions are confined to the space relevant for successfully performing the task.

We hypothesized that the dual encoding of performance errors provides the neural substrate needed to generate sensory prediction errors. For the majority of Purkinje cells, the prediction and feedback error signals have opposing effects on simple spike modulations, precisely as required to compute the difference between the predicted and the actual feedback. Dual encoding could result in reduced simple spike sensitivity to self-generated sensory information whereby the presence of both the predicted consequences of the motor commands and sensory feedback signals act to cancel each other. Consistent with this interpretation, Purkinje cells show greater sensitivity to passive self-motion, driven by sensory feedback than to active, self-generated motion driven by both sensory and internal feedback (Brooks and Cullen, 2013). Also, the long term decrease in sensitivity to motor errors observed with PET imaging during adaptation to constant force fields (Nezafat et al., 2001) could be due to an improving match between internally generated predictions and sensory feedback. A similar reduction in the cerebellar BOLD response occurs during a cognitive task in which subjects learn first-order rules (Balsters and Ramnani, 2011), presumably due to a comparison in the cerebellar cortex between the predicted and perceptual consequences of mental manipulations. A study of fear conditioning found that cerebellar activation decreased during the conditioning phase while either unexpected application or omission of the noxious stimuli resulted in increased cerebellar activation (Ploghaus et al., 2000). This result is consistent with acquiring a dual representation of the stimulus application, resulting in cancellation of predictive and feedback signals when the prediction is correct and increased activity when there is prediction error. Furthermore, cerebellar activation during a letter manipulation task is consistent with encoding task execution errors in a non-motor task (Marvel and Desmond, 2012). Although the hypothesis that individual Purkinje cells compute directly sensory prediction errors by subtracting the predictive and feedback signals is very seductive, the fact that the two signals are separated in time requires further investigation. It is possible that this computation is performed downstream, for example, in the cerebellar nuclei. It is also possible that for relatively slow changing signals the dual encoding could approximate the sensory prediction error.

Similar cancellation of self-generated sensory signals occurs in cerebellum-like structures (Bell et al., 2008; Sawtell and Bell, 2008; Requarth and Sawtell, 2011). This suppression is due to anti-Hebbian plasticity at the parallel fiber-principle neuron synapse that sculpts the response to the efferent copy into a negative image of the response to the sensory input (Bell et al., 1997; Han et al., 2000; Requarth and Sawtell, 2011). This mechanism is predicated on the appropriate convergence of sensory inputs and an efference copy of the motor command on the principal neurons (Sawtell, 2010). A comparable convergence of proprioceptive and cortico-pontine inputs has been reported in the mouse cerebellar cortex (Huang et al., 2013).

To test whether dual encoding is a general principle of cerebellar representations, we re-examined the simple spike firing relationship with kinematics using the analytical methods described above, regressing the appropriate residuals in which variability associated with all other motor parameters was removed against individual kinematic variables (Popa, 2013). Similar to the error parameters described earlier, a majority of Purkinje cells also encode kinematics with dual signals, one related to an internal prediction and one related to sensory feedback. There is increasing evidence that the time delays required to create opposing signals might be generated by the mossy fiber-granule cell circuitry in the mammalian cerebellum. Long term plasticity at the mossy fiber—granule cell synapse can delay incoming signals for up to 100 ms, or even longer when larger sections of the circuitry are entrained (D'Angelo, 2011). Similar delays are generated in cerebellar-like structures involving the mossy fibers, granule cells and unipolar brush cells (Kennedy et al., 2014). Together, these findings suggest that the leads/lags found for performance errors and kinematic signals are determined, at least in part, by computations occurring in the cerebellar cortex, resulting in specific temporal alignments of cerebellar signals. We hypothesize that dual temporal encoding is a general property of cerebellar cortical information processing and also likely plays a central role in non-motor cerebellar functions.

Both motor coordination and motor sequences require interactions between different components (e.g., effectors, muscles, etc.) activated at different times and, therefore, controlling the individual components requires motor predictions across a spectrum of leads. Successful population decoding of the error predictions combines individual Purkinje cell signals at all possible lead times, with values between -500 and 0 ms. In other words, the cerebellar cortex unfurls the predictions throughout a long time window. Similarly, the simple spike signals that lag kinematics or errors occur across this rather protracted time course. One possible interpretation is that this provides a mechanism for the cerebellum's role in coordination among effectors (Thach et al., 1992; van Donkelaar and Lee, 1994; Bastian et al., 1996; Serrien and Wiesendanger, 2000; Miall et al., 2001) and in movement sequences (Braitenberg et al., 1997; Doyon et al., 1997; Molinari and Petrosini, 1997; Molinari et al., 1997, 2008; Nixon and Passingham, 2000). Under this assumption, cerebellar cortical output integrates the predicted motor command outcomes at specific lead times with appropriately matched feedback signals to generate the required coordination among effectors needed to accomplish the movement goal while simultaneously updating the motor controller so that the next motor command can be generated. It has been hypothesized that the cerebellum fulfills a similar function in detecting sequences in the verbal, spatial and cognitive domains (Molinari et al., 2008). The long and quasi-uniform lead and lag times found in the simple spike discharge provides a neural substrate for monitoring and controlling sequences. For example, cerebellar damage impairs sequencing of cards depicting brief stories regardless of whether verbal, spatial, or behavioral strategies are used (Leggio et al., 2008). An imaging study based on letter manipulations showed an interesting difference in cerebellar activation depending on the cognitive process engaged (Marvel and Desmond, 2012). The presentation of a sequence of letters, without processing requirements, induces a fast, transient activation, consistent with the response to tightly packed input signals. However, when processing instructions are added, the activation becomes sustained over a longer time interval. This temporal expansion is consistent with the temporal unfurling of the cerebellar representations observed during random tracking.

Another interesting implication for the capacity of Purkinje cells to provide predictions and feedback at a wide range of times is in working memory. Numerous functional imaging studies demonstrate cerebellar activation associated with the working memory system (Chen and Desmond, 2005; Hautzel et al., 2009; Marvel and Desmond, 2010). Given that the storage capacity of working memory is limited to between four and seven items (Miller, 1956; Luck and Vogel, 1997), the changes in its content associated with shifting attention focus occurs every 200–500 ms (Muller et al., 1998; Woodman and Luck, 1999). Having both prediction and feedback signals over comparable time horizons, the cerebellar cortex may facilitate novel associations between past and current working memory content and among different classes of information.

Linear Integration of Kinematics and Error Signals

The presence of both error and kinematic signals in the Purkinje cell simple spike discharge raises the question of whether the cells are functionally segregated into populations that preferentially encode one class of signals over the other. Using multi-linear models that included (1) all variables from each class, (2) position, velocity, and speed kinematics, and (3) position, radial, and direction errors, we showed that the average R2 for kinematics and error models are comparable. As detailed above, accurate decoding of the upcoming behavior can be achieved for both errors and kinematics. Therefore, encoding of these two classes of variables is equally robust. For single cells, the distribution of R2 values reveals a strong linear positive correlation between errors and kinematics and there is no evidence of segregation into subpopulations. The representations of single parameters are additive, as the sum of the R2 profiles for the individual parameters closely matches the R2 profile of both error and kinematic multi-linear models (Popa et al., 2012; Popa, 2013). These observations reinforce the concept of signal independence and supports the hypothesis that Purkinje cells linearly integrate parallel fiber input (Walter and Khodakhah, 2006, 2009). Therefore, kinematic and error signals are highly integrated at the single neuron and population levels.

The integration of the kinematic and error signals strongly suggests that Purkinje cells favor complexity. There are approximately 200,000 parallel fiber-Purkinje cell synapses (Napper and Harvey, 1988), while less than 200 active synapses are required to drive simple spike discharge (Isope and Barbour, 2002), suggesting a very high theoretical bandwidth. During random tracking, we evaluated nine behavioral parameters, five kinematic and four error signals. Each parameter could be dually encoded, resulting in 18 independent signals in the simple spike discharge. Out of these 18 possible signals, on average, individual Purkinje cells encode 10 different signals simultaneously (Popa, 2013). Therefore, each Purkinje cell carries a very rich representation of the motor behavior suggesting that an important aspect of cerebellar function is associating different signals, possibly of different modalities.

The number of signals identified in individual cell simple spike discharges is small in comparison to the theoretical bandwidth, suggesting the possibility that the same cell could encode diverse representations in different contexts. For example, one cell could use a common representation of an effector with multiple performance error signals that differ across tasks. As reviewed above, functional imaging studies show that the cerebellum encodes errors as well as kinematics. Although clearly not at the level of single cells, imaging suggests the cerebellum is activated in relation to non-motor functions including language, spatial processing, working memory, executive function, and emotional processing (for review see Stoodley, 2012). These activations particularly engaged the postero-lateral regions and are consistent with the integration of many classes of signals within the same areas. Association of information across modalities has been central to several theories of the cerebellum's role in cognition (Drepper et al., 1999; Timmann et al., 2002, 2010; Molinari et al., 2008).

The high number of independent signals present in each Purkinje cell discharge and the additive property of these signals suggest that the cerebellar function includes an associative aspect. As others have proposed, it is possible that Purkinje cells use plasticity mechanisms to select only the relevant signals from the high number of possible parallel fiber inputs to each neuron, and these relevant signals maintain consistent relationships to form a representation of the motor behavior (Marr, 1969; Albus, 1971). Associative learning in the cerebellum is not restricted to the motor domain (for review see Timmann et al., 2010). For example, the cerebellum is implicated in fear learning (Sacchetti et al., 2004) and cognitive associative learning (Drepper et al., 1999; Timmann et al., 2002). Making the required association between the relevant afferent signals would utilize the Purkinje cell's capacity to integrate large numbers of signals. Interestingly, the cerebellum appears to acquire an internal model related to the application of noxious stimuli but fails to respond when innocuous stimuli are applied (Ploghaus et al., 2000), suggesting that the cerebellum evaluates the relevance of inputs and disregards the irrelevant ones.

Implications for Internal Models of Cognitive Processes

Understanding cerebellar involvement in non-motor domains faces numerous challenges ranging from imprecise definitions and imperfect models of cognitive behavior to indirect investigation methods (Koziol et al., 2012; Buckner, 2013). Fortunately, the remarkable uniformity of the cerebellar cortex architecture (Eccles et al., 1967; Ito, 1984; Ramnani, 2006) suggests that the cerebellum should perform the same signal processing across motor and non-motor domains (Ramnani, 2006; Thach, 2007; Ito, 2008; Schmahmann, 2010). Under this hypothesis, insights into cerebellar processes provided by single cell studies of motor behavior in well controlled experimental conditions and within a rigorous theoretical framework may help illuminate non-motor aspects of cerebellar function.

Throughout this review we have noted how the information processing properties of cerebellar neurons might contribute to non-motor functions. In this final section we explore more fully the concept of internal models in motor and non-motor functions of the cerebellum. Several investigators have proposed that the internal model hypotheses can be used to understand cerebellar involvement in non-motor domains (Ito, 2008; Imamizu and Kawato, 2009; Koziol et al., 2012). In support of the hypothesis that forward internal model processes are common across function domains is the observation that the information provided by an internal model of the hand during visually guided tracking is used in a visual discrimination task (Stanley and Miall, 2009) showing that signals involved in motor control are used in non-motor behaviors. It also been hypothesized that forward internal models acquired in the motor domain, interacting with the mirror system in the cerebral cortex, facilitate action understanding (Caligiore et al., 2013), the capability to asses mental states such as goals and intentions underlying actions performed by different subjects. Several functional imaging studies found specific cerebellar activation in response to noun-verb associations (Petersen et al., 1988) or to word completion tasks (Desmond and Fiez, 1998), suggesting activation of a forward internal model that provides lexical predictions. Strongly supporting this hypothesis is the observation that disruption of cerebellar function by repeated transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces language predictive performance (Lesage et al., 2012). Under the assumption that cerebellar processing in the motor domain translates to non-motor functions, we consider the implications of our random tracking results to understanding the cerebellar contribution to cognitive processes in the context of forward internal models. The classical version of the forward internal model assumes that the cerebellum provides a model of the effector that predicts the sensory consequences of the motor commands (Robinson, 1975; Miall and Wolpert, 1996; Bastian, 2006; Shadmehr et al., 2010). In this framework, the internal model updating is driven by the motor error signals, more specifically sensory prediction errors (Morton and Bastian, 2006; Tseng et al., 2007; Xu-Wilson et al., 2009; Shadmehr et al., 2010; Izawa et al., 2012). As detailed above, the historical emphasis has been that the error signals are conveyed solely by the climbing fibers and complex spikes. Likewise, it has been assumed that complex spikes provide error signals in non-motor behaviors (Ito, 2008; Schmahmann, 2010; Koziol et al., 2012; Yamazaki and Nagao, 2012). It has also been proposed that the cerebellum uses the same processes performed by a forward internal model on copies of the mental models encoded in the parietal cortex which are then manipulated by the commands issued by the prefrontal cortex, while the inferior olive encodes the cognitive errors (Ito, 2008). Recently described projections between the cerebral cortex and cerebellum provide the required pathways to support the hypothesis that the cerebellar cortex acquires and manipulates copies of the mental models (Schmahmann and Pandya, 1989, 1991, 1997; Middleton and Strick, 1994, 2001; Kelly and Strick, 2003). However, the existence of pathways necessary to support the hypothesis that the inferior olive encodes cognitive error signals remain tentative at best (Ito, 2008). For example, principal olive, the subdivision of the inferior olive that provides the climbing fiber projections to cerebellar cortical regions connected to associative areas of the cerebral cortex, does not receive projections from the cerebral cortex (Schmahmann, 2010). Further, as discussed above, the validity of the concept that complex spikes signal motor errors is being questioned.

Our recent findings describing performance error signals in the simple spike activity show that the signals required by the forward internal model theories are conveyed by parallel fiber inputs, thus eliminating the idea that climbing fibers are the sole pathway for conveying error signals (Ito, 2008). This would strengthen the hypothesis that forward internal model processes could be replicated in the cognitive domain by emphasizing the established projections between cerebellum and prefrontal and parietal cortices and reducing the relevance on inferior olive input. However, this would also require a reassessment of the role of complex spikes in the cerebellar function. For example, recent optogenetic experiments show that simple spike activation induces a complex spike response (Chaumont et al., 2013; Witter et al., 2013). This suggests that the complex spike activity, modulated by the response of Purkinje cells to parallel fiber inputs, engage specific plasticity mechanisms (Ito et al., 1982; Marquez-Ruiz and Cheron, 2012) that refine suboptimal outputs of the cerebellar cortex.

Complex goal-directed behavior, such as visually guided, random tracking, raises the question of how a motor command is selected and implemented under very stringent temporal constraints as the monkey has only 500 ms to recover from excursions outside the target. Within such a tight temporal budget it is unlikely that a classical process of error perception and action selection, which requires ~400 ms, can be implemented (Madl et al., 2011). One accepted solution to this problem is that the cerebellum acts as a forward internal model that predicts the future state of the effector (Ito, 2008; Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008). Nevertheless, the selection of the motor command cannot be completed based only on effector state predictions, as the action selection is also dependent on performance information. This information is presumably processed in the classical sequence of perception-selection-execution. However, our demonstration of the integration of kinematic and performance error predictions shows that the cerebellar cortex provides predictions of the consequences of the motor command both in terms of future states of the effector and the usefulness toward meeting the current movement goal. We suggest that the cerebellar cortex predictions provide the information needed to complete action selection with little reliance on sensory feedback.

First, it is interesting to explore the implications of cerebellar prediction of both kinematic and performance states for the process of action selection that involves interactions among widely distributed cortical areas and also subcortical structures including the cerebellum and basal ganglia (Cisek and Kalaska, 2010). Action selection appears to be heavily dependent on perception (Ledberg et al., 2007) and therefore, may be too slow for the implementation and control of fast, ongoing behaviors, such as random tracking. Decision making may be adequate to control the slower components of the behavior, such as the decision to start a new trial or to abandon an ongoing trial. However, once the decision to engage in a trial is made, the selection and implementation of the on-going movements required to complete the behavior needs to be controlled by faster mechanisms (Shadmehr et al., 2010). Faster control could be achieved by acquiring a forward internal model of the behavior that combines signals describing the effector response and the task-specific performance and then learning to predict the consequences of the specified actions. Therefore, action bias might be viewed as a staged process in which a slower, perception-driven component are under prefrontal and basal ganglia control, while the cerebellum controls a faster, experience-dependent and perception-independent component.

Second, it is interesting to examine the cerebellar internal model of thought process hypothesis (Ito, 2008) in an expanded version of the forward internal model framework. It had been suggested that when the cerebellum acquires a forward internal model of a cognitive problem, by performing the manipulations required to solve the problem on cerebellar copies of the mental models eliminates the perception of thinking about the problem, thus generating an “intuition” (Ito, 2008). However, this model leaves unanswered the problem of recognizing the solution. It is possible to perform the evaluation of the intermediate results under conscious control, which would eliminate the “unexpected” quality associated with intuition. An internal model that includes not only copies of the mental models manipulated in the process, but also copies of cognitive error representations, could allow the evaluation of the results under cerebellar control.

Extending the internal forward model hypothesis to both predictions about the effectors and performance could have another implication. Presumably, experience-driven internal models are acquired by consistently activating and associating specific cerebellar inputs and the required plasticity mechanisms. Under normal conditions the consistency of inputs would be only related to repeated consideration of the same problem and, therefore, the resulting internal models could be refined to provide an optimal solution. However, if unrelated inputs are consistently activated, for example in pathological conditions, this could bias the internal forward model. In this condition, the subject would perceive a suboptimal result as the correct solution.

Next Steps

Extending cerebellar function into non-motor processing has raised a host of intriguing questions about the computations performed by the cerebellar circuitry and how these computations support processes such as cognition, working memory, and language processing. While patient studies, non-invasive stimulation techniques and functional imaging in human subjects are powerful tools to test hypotheses on the role of the cerebellum in non-motor functions and have already provided considerable insights, single unit electrophysiological studies are clearly needed to gain a greater understanding of how the cerebellum participates in and contributes to higher cortical functions at the neuronal level. Electrophysiological studies would use non-human primates because these subjects are capable of the types of complex, non-motor behaviors needed to address these questions. Furthermore, non-human primates have the required cerebellar-cortical connectivity that underpins the cerebellum's role in non-motor behaviors. Finally, single cell recordings are the only present day technique that can answer the question central to this review, which is “Are the computations performed by the cerebellar cortex similar in the motor and non-motor domains?”

Author Contributions

Angela L. Hewitt, Laurentiu S. Popa, and Timothy J. Ebner all contributed to the design, analysis, and interpretation of the work. Angela L. Hewitt had primary responsibility for data acquisition. All authors contributed to the drafting, giving final approval and agreeing to be held accountable for all aspects of the work.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Michael McPhee for generating graphics and Kris Bettin and Lauren Skalicky for preparation of the manuscript. Supported in part by NIH grants R01 NS18338, R01 F31 NS071686, and T32 GM008244.

References

Albus, J. S. (1971). A theory of cerebellar function. Math. Biosci. 10, 25–61. doi: 10.1016/0025-5564(71)90051-4

CrossRef Full Text

Balsters, J. H., and Ramnani, N. (2011). Cerebellar plasticity and the automation of first-order rules. J. Neurosci. 31, 2305–2312. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4358-10.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barmack, N. H., and Shojaku, H. (1995). Vestibular and visual climbing fiber signals evoked in the uvula-nodulus of the rabbit cerebellum by natural stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 74, 2573–2589.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bastian, A. J. (2006). Learning to predict the future: the cerebellum adapts feedforward movement control. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 645–649. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.08.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bastian, A. J., Martin, T. A., Keating, J. G., and Thach, W. T. (1996). Cerebellar ataxia: abnormal control of interaction torques across multiple joints. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 492–509.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bell, C. C., Han, V., and Sawtell, N. B. (2008). Cerebellum-like structures and their implications for cerebellar function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 1–24. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094225

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bell, C. C., Han, V. Z., Sugawara, Y., and Grant, K. (1997). Synaptic plasticity in a cerebellum-like structure depends on temporal order. Nature 387, 278–281. doi: 10.1038/387278a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boehringer, A., Macher, K., Dukart, J., Villringer, A., and Pleger, B. (2013). Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation modulates verbal working memory. Brain Stimul. 6, 649–653. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.10.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Braitenberg, V., and Atwood, R. P. (1958). Morphological observations on the cerebellar cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 109, 1–33. doi: 10.1002/cne.901090102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Braitenberg, V., Heck, D., and Sultan, F. (1997). The detection and generation of sequences as a key to cerebellar function: experiments and theory. Behav. Brain Sci. 20, 229–245. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X9700143X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brooks, J. X., and Cullen, K. E. (2013). The primate cerebellum selectively encodes unexpected self-motion. Curr. Biol. 23, 947–955. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.029

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buckner, R. L. (2013). The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron 80, 807–815. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.044

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burk, K. (2007). Cognition in hereditary ataxia. Cerebellum 6, 280–286. doi: 10.1080/14734220601115924

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Caligiore, D., Pezzulo, G., Miall, R. C., and Baldassarre, G. (2013). The contribution of brain sub-cortical loops in the expression and acquisition of action understanding abilities. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2504–2515. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Catz, N., Dicke, P. W., and Thier, P. (2005). Cerebellar complex spike firing is suitable to induce as well as to stabilize motor learning. Curr. Biol. 15, 2179–2189. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.11.037

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chaumont, J., Guyon, N., Valera, A. M., Dugue, G. P., Popa, D., Marcaggi, P., et al. (2013). Clusters of cerebellar Purkinje cells control their afferent climbing fiber discharge. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 16223–16228. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302310110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chen, S. H., and Desmond, J. E. (2005). Cerebrocerebellar networks during articulatory rehearsal and verbal working memory tasks. Neuroimage 24, 332–338. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cisek, P., and Kalaska, J. F. (2010). Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 33, 269–298. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135409

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Coltz, J. D., Johnson, M. T., and Ebner, T. J. (1999). Cerebellar Purkinje cell simple spike discharge encodes movement velocity in primates during visuomotor arm tracking. J. Neurosci. 19, 1782–1803.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Crowe, D. A., Goodwin, S. J., Blackman, R. K., Sakellaridi, S., Sponheim, S. R., MacDonald, A. W. 3rd., et al. (2013). Prefrontal neurons transmit signals to parietal neurons that reflect executive control of cognition. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1484–1491. doi: 10.1038/nn.3509

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

D'Angelo, E. (2011). Neural circuits of the cerebellum: hypothesis for function. J. Integr. Neurosci. 10, 317–352. doi: 10.1142/S0219635211002762

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dash, S., Catz, N., Dicke, P. W., and Thier, P. (2010). Specific vermal complex spike responses build up during the course of smooth-pursuit adaptation, paralleling the decrease of performance error. Exp. Brain Res. 205, 41–55. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2331-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dash, S., Dicke, P. W., and Thier, P. (2013). A vermal Purkinje cell simple spike population response encodes the changes in eye movement kinematics due to smooth pursuit adaptation. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:3. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Desmond, J. E., and Fiez, J. A. (1998). Neuroimaging studies of the cerebellum: language, learning and memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 355–362. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01211-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Diedrichsen, J., Hashambhoy, Y., Rane, T., and Shadmehr, R. (2005). Neural correlates of reach errors. J. Neurosci. 25, 9919–9931. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1874-05.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Doyon, J., Gaudreau, D., Laforce, R. Jr. Castonguay, M., Bedard, P. J., Bedard, F., et al. (1997). Role of the striatum, cerebellum, and frontal lobes in the learning of a visuomotor sequence. Brain Cogn. 34, 218–245. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1997.0899

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Drepper, J., Timmann, D., Kolb, F. P., and Diener, H. C. (1999). Non-motor associative learning in patients with isolated degenerative cerebellar disease. Brain 122(Pt 1), 87–97. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.1.87

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ebner, T. J., Hewitt, A., and Popa, L. S. (2011). What features of movements are encoded in the discharge of cerebellar neurons during limb movements? Cerebellum 10, 683–693. doi: 10.1007/s12311-010-0243-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ebner, T. J., Johnson, M. T., Roitman, A., and Fu, Q. (2002). What do complex spikes signal about limb movements? Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 978, 205–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb07568.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eccles, J. C., Ito, M., and Szentagothai, J. (1967). The Cerebellum as a Neuronal Machine. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Ferrucci, R., Marceglia, S., Vergari, M., Cogiamanian, F., Mrakic-Sposta, S., Mameli, F., et al. (2008). Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation impairs the practice-dependent proficiency increase in working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1687–1697. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20112

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fiez, J. A., Petersen, S. E., Cheney, M. K., and Raichle, M. E. (1992). Impaired non-motor learning and error detection associated with cerebellar damage. A single case study. Brain 115(Pt 1), 155–178. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.1.155

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fortier, P. A., Kalaska, J. F., and Smith, A. M. (1989). Cerebellar neuronal activity related to whole-arm reaching movements in the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 62, 198–211.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Fu, Q. G., Flament, D., Coltz, J. D., and Ebner, T. J. (1997). Relationship of cerebellar Purkinje cell simple spike discharge to movement kinematics in the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 78, 478–491.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gilbert, P. F., and Thach, W. T. (1977). Purkinje cell activity during motor learning. Brain Res. 128, 309–328. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90997-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gomi, H., Shidara, M., Takemura, A., Inoue, Y., Kawano, K., and Kawato, M. (1998). Temporal firing patterns of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar ventral paraflocculus during ocular following responses in monkeys I. Simple spikes. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 818–831.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Graf, W., Simpson, J. I., and Leonard, C. S. (1988). Spatial organization of visual messages of the rabbit's cerebellar flocculus. II. Complex and simple spike responses of Purkinje cells. J. Neurophysiol. 60, 2091–2121.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Greger, B., and Norris, S. (2005). Simple spike firing in the posterior lateral cerebellar cortex of Macaque Mulatta was correlated with success-failure during a visually guided reaching task. Exp. Brain Res. 167, 660–665. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0155-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Han, V. Z., Grant, K., and Bell, C. C. (2000). Reversible associative depression and nonassociative potentiation at a parallel fiber synapse. Neuron 27, 611–622. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00070-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harvey, R. J., Porter, R., and Rawson, J. A. (1977). The natural discharges of Purkinje cells in paravermal regions of lobules V and VI of the monkey's cerebellum. J. Physiol. 271, 515–536.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hautzel, H., Mottaghy, F. M., Specht, K., Muller, H. W., and Krause, B. J. (2009). Evidence of a modality-dependent role of the cerebellum in working memory? An fMRI study comparing verbal and abstract n-back tasks. Neuroimage 47, 2073–2082. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hayter, A. L., Langdon, D. W., and Ramnani, N. (2007). Cerebellar contributions to working memory. Neuroimage 36, 943–954. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hewitt, A., Popa, L. S., Pasalar, S., Hendrix, C. M., and Ebner, T. J. (2011). Representation of limb kinematics in Purkinje cell simple spike discharge is conserved across multiple tasks. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 2232–2247. doi: 10.1152/jn.00886.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Horn, K. M., van Kan, P. L., and Gibson, A. R. (1996). Reduction of rostral dorsal accessory olive responses during reaching. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 4140–4151.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Huang, C. C., Sugino, K., Shima, Y., Guo, C., Bai, S., Mensh, B. D., et al. (2013). Convergence of pontine and proprioceptive streams onto multimodal cerebellar granule cells. Elife 2:e00400. doi: 10.7554/eLife.00400

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Imamizu, H., Higuchi, S., Toda, A., and Kawato, M. (2007). Reorganization of brain activity for multiple internal models after short but intensive training. Cortex 43, 338–349. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70459-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Imamizu, H., and Kawato, M. (2009). Brain mechanisms for predictive control by switching internal models: implications for higher-order cognitive functions. Psychol. Res. 73, 527–544. doi: 10.1007/s00426-009-0235-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Imamizu, H., Kuroda, T., Miyauchi, S., Yoshioka, T., and Kawato, M. (2003). Modular organization of internal models of tools in the human cerebellum. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 5461–5466. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0835746100

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Imamizu, H., Miyauchi, S., Tamada, T., Sasaki, Y., Takino, R., Putz, B., et al. (2000). Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool. Nature 403, 192–195. doi: 10.1038/35003194

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Isope, P., and Barbour, B. (2002). Properties of unitary granule cell–>Purkinje cell synapses in adult rat cerebellar slices. J. Neurosci. 22, 9668–9678.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Ito, M. (1984). The Cerebellum and Neural Control. New York, NY: Raven Press.

Ito, M. (2000). Mechanisms of motor learning in the cerebellum. Brain Res. 886, 237–245. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)03142-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ito, M. (2002). Historical review of the significance of the cerebellum and the role of Purkinje cells in motor learning. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 978, 273–288. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb07574.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ito, M. (2008). Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 304–313. doi: 10.1038/nrn2332

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ito, M., Sakurai, M., and Tongroach, P. (1982). Climbing fibre induced depression of both mossy fibre responsiveness and glutamate sensitivity of cerebellar Purkinje cells. J. Physiol. 324, 113–134.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Izawa, J., Criscimagna-Hemminger, S. E., and Shadmehr, R. (2012). Cerebellar contributions to reach adaptation and learning sensory consequences of action. J. Neurosci. 32, 4230–4239. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6353-11.2012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kawato, M. (1999). Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9, 718–727. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00028-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ke, M. C., Guo, C. C., and Raymond, J. L. (2009). Elimination of climbing fiber instructive signals during motor learning. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1171–1179. doi: 10.1038/nn.2366

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keele, S. W., and Ivry, R. (1990). Does the cerebellum provide a common computation for diverse tasks? A timing hypothesis. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 608, 179–207. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb48897.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kelly, R. M., and Strick, P. L. (2003). Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. J. Neurosci. 23, 8432–8444.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kennedy, A., Wayne, G., Kaifosh, P., Alvina, K., Abbott, L. F., and Sawtell, N. B. (2014). A temporal basis for predicting the sensory consequences of motor commands in an electric fish. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 416–422. doi: 10.1038/nn.3650

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, J. H., Wang, J. J., and Ebner, T. J. (1987). Climbing fiber afferent modulation during treadmill locomotion in the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 57, 787–802.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kim, S. G., Ugurbil, K., and Strick, P. L. (1994). Activation of a cerebellar output nucleus during cognitive processing. Science 265, 949–951. doi: 10.1126/science.8052851

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kitazawa, S., Kimura, T., and Yin, P. B. (1998). Cerebellar complex spikes encode both destinations and errors in arm movements. Nature 392, 494–497. doi: 10.1038/33141

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kobayashi, Y., Kawano, K., Takemura, A., Inoue, Y., Kitama, T., Gomi, H., et al. (1998). Temporal firing patterns of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar ventral paraflocculus during ocular following responses in monkeys II. Complex spikes. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 832–848.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Koziol, L. F., Budding, D. E., and Chidekel, D. (2012). From movement to thought: executive function, embodied cognition, and the cerebellum. Cerebellum 11, 505–525. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0321-y

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ledberg, A., Bressler, S. L., Ding, M., Coppola, R., and Nakamura, R. (2007). Large-scale visuomotor integration in the cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 17, 44–62. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj123

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leggio, M. G., Tedesco, A. M., Chiricozzi, F. R., Clausi, S., Orsini, A., and Molinari, M. (2008). Cognitive sequencing impairment in patients with focal or atrophic cerebellar damage. Brain 131, 1332–1343. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leiner, H. C., Leiner, A. L., and Dow, R. S. (1986). Does the cerebellum contribute to mental skills? Behav. Neurosci. 100, 443–454. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.100.4.443

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leiner, H. C., Leiner, A. L., and Dow, R. S. (1989). Reappraising the cerebellum: what does the hindbrain contribute to the forebrain? Behav. Neurosci. 103, 998–1008. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.103.5.998

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lesage, E., Morgan, B. E., Olson, A. C., Meyer, A. S., and Miall, R. C. (2012). Cerebellar rTMS disrupts predictive language processing. Curr. Biol. 22, R794–R795. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Luck, S. J., and Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature 390, 279–281. doi: 10.1038/36846

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Madl, T., Baars, B. J., and Franklin, S. (2011). The timing of the cognitive cycle. PLoS ONE 6:e14803. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014803

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mano, N., and Yamamoto, K. (1980). Simple-spike activity of cerebellar Purkinje cells related to visually guided wrist tracking movement in the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 43, 713–728.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Marple-Horvat, D. E., and Stein, J. F. (1987). Cerebellar neuronal activity related to arm movements in trained rhesus monkeys. J. Physiol. 394, 351–366.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Marquez-Ruiz, J., and Cheron, G. (2012). Sensory stimulation-dependent plasticity in the cerebellar cortex of alert mice. PLoS ONE 7:e36184. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036184

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marr, D. (1969). A theory of cerebellar cortex. J. Physiol. 202, 437–470.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Marvel, C. L., and Desmond, J. E. (2010). Functional topography of the cerebellum in verbal working memory. Neuropsychol. Rev. 20, 271–279. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9137-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marvel, C. L., and Desmond, J. E. (2012). From storage to manipulation: how the neural correlates of verbal working memory reflect varying demands on inner speech. Brain Lang. 120, 42–51. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.08.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mazzoni, P., and Krakauer, J. W. (2006). An implicit plan overrides an explicit strategy during visuomotor adaptation. J. Neurosci. 26, 3642–3645. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5317-05.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Medina, J. F., and Lisberger, S. G. (2008). Links from complex spikes to local plasticity and motor learning in the cerebellum of awake-behaving monkeys. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 1185–1192. doi: 10.1038/nn.2197

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Medina, J. F., and Lisberger, S. G. (2009). Encoding and decoding of learned smooth pursuit eye movements in the floccular complex of the monkey cerebellum. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 2039–2054. doi: 10.1152/jn.00075.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miall, R. C., Reckess, G. Z., and Imamizu, H. (2001). The cerebellum coordinates eye and hand tracking movements. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 638–644. doi: 10.1038/88465

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miall, R. C., and Wolpert, D. M. (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw. 9, 1265–1279. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(96)00035-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Middleton, F. A., and Strick, P. L. (1994). Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in higher cognitive function. Science 266, 458–461. doi: 10.1126/science.7939688

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Middleton, F. A., and Strick, P. L. (2001). Cerebellar projections to the prefrontal cortex of the primate. J. Neurosci. 21, 700–712.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol. Rev. 63, 81–97. doi: 10.1037/h0043158

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Molinari, M., Chiricozzi, F. R., Clausi, S., Tedesco, A. M., De, L. M., and Leggio, M. G. (2008). Cerebellum and detection of sequences, from perception to cognition. Cerebellum 7, 611–615. doi: 10.1007/s12311-008-0060-x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Molinari, M., Leggio, M. G., Solida, A., Ciorra, R., Misciagna, S., Silveri, M. C., et al. (1997). Cerebellum and procedural learning: evidence from focal cerebellar lesions. Brain 120(Pt 10), 1753–1762. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.10.1753

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Molinari, M., and Petrosini, L. (1997). Is sequence in/sequence out a cerebellar mode of operation in cognition too? Behav. Brain Sci. 20, 259–260. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X97391432

CrossRef Full Text

Moran, D. W., and Schwartz, A. B. (1999). Motor cortical activity during drawing movements: population representation during spiral tracing. J. Neurophysiol. 82, 2693–2704.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Morton, S. M., and Bastian, A. J. (2006). Cerebellar contributions to locomotor adaptations during splitbelt treadmill walking. J. Neurosci. 26, 9107–9116. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2622-06.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muller, M. M., Teder-Salejarvi, W., and Hillyard, S. A. (1998). The time course of cortical facilitation during cued shifts of spatial attention. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 631–634. doi: 10.1038/2865

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Napper, R. M., and Harvey, R. J. (1988). Number of parallel fiber synapses on an individual Purkinje cell in the cerebellum of the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 274, 168–177. doi: 10.1002/cne.902740204

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nezafat, R., Shadmehr, R., and Holcomb, H. H. (2001). Long-term adaptation to dynamics of reaching movements: a PET study. Exp. Brain Res. 140, 66–76. doi: 10.1007/s002210100787

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nixon, P. D., and Passingham, R. E. (2000). The cerebellum and cognition: cerebellar lesions impair sequence learning but not conditional visuomotor learning in monkeys. Neuropsychologia 38, 1054–1072. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00138-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Noto, C. T., and Robinson, F. R. (2001). Visual error is the stimulus for saccade gain adaptation. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 12, 301–305. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00062-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ojakangas, C. L., and Ebner, T. J. (1994). Purkinje cell complex spike activity during voluntary motor learning: relationship to kinematics. J. Neurophysiol. 72, 2617–2630.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

O'Reilly, J. X., Mesulam, M. M., and Nobre, A. C. (2008). The cerebellum predicts the timing of perceptual events. J. Neurosci. 28, 2252–2260. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2742-07.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Oscarsson, O. (1980). “Functional organization of olivary projection to the cerebellar anterior lobe,” in The Inferior Olivary Nucleus: Anatomy and Physiology, ed J. Courville (New York, NY: Raven), 279–290.

Paninski, L., Fellows, M. R., Hatsopoulos, N. G., and Donoghue, J. P. (2004). Spatiotemporal tuning of motor cortical neurons for hand position and velocity. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 515–532. doi: 10.1152/jn.00587.2002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pasalar, S., Roitman, A. V., Durfee, W. K., and Ebner, T. J. (2006). Force field effects on cerebellar Purkinje cell discharge with implications for internal models. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1404–1411. doi: 10.1038/nn1783

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M., and Raichle, M. E. (1988). Positron emission tomographic studies of the processing of single words. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 154–170.

Pezzulo, G. (2011). Grounding procedural and declarative knowledge in sensorimotor anticipation. Mind Lang. 26, 78–114. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01411.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L. W., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M. H., McRae, K., and Spivey, M. J. (2012). Computational grounded cognition: a new alliance between grounded cognition and computational modeling. Front. Psychol. 3:612. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00612

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ploghaus, A., Tracey, I., Clare, S., Gati, J. S., Rawlins, J. N., and Matthews, P. M. (2000). Learning about pain: the neural substrate of the prediction error for aversive events. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 9281–9286. doi: 10.1073/pnas.160266497

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Popa, L. S. (2013). Computational Studies of Cerebellar Cortical Circuitry. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota.

Popa, L. S., Hewitt, A. L., and Ebner, T. J. (2012). Predictive and feedback performance errors are signaled in the simple spike discharge of individual Purkinje cells. J. Neurosci. 32, 15345–15358. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2151-12.2012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pope, P. A., and Miall, R. C. (2012). Task-specific facilitation of cognition by cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the cerebellum. Brain Stimul. 5, 84–94. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Prsa, M., and Thier, P. (2011). The role of the cerebellum in saccadic adaptation as a window into neural mechanisms of motor learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 2114–2128. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07693.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ramnani, N. (2006). The primate cortico-cerebellar system: anatomy and function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 511–522. doi: 10.1038/nrn1953

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Requarth, T., and Sawtell, N. B. (2011). Neural mechanisms for filtering self-generated sensory signals in cerebellum-like circuits. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 21, 602–608. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Robinson, D. A. (1975). “Oculomotor control signals,” in Basic Mechanisms of Ocular Motility and their Clinical Implications, eds P. Bachyrita and G. Lennerstrand (Oxford: Pergamon), 337–374.

Roitman, A. V., Pasalar, S., and Ebner, T. J. (2009). Single trial coupling of Purkinje cell activity to speed and error signals during circular manual tracking. Exp. Brain Res. 192, 241–251. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1580-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roitman, A. V., Pasalar, S., Johnson, M. T., and Ebner, T. J. (2005). Position, direction of movement, and speed tuning of cerebellar Purkinje cells during circular manual tracking in monkey. J. Neurosci. 25, 9244–9257. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1886-05.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sacchetti, B., Scelfo, B., Tempia, F., and Strata, P. (2004). Long-term synaptic changes induced in the cerebellar cortex by fear conditioning. Neuron 42, 973–982. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.05.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sawtell, N. B. (2010). Multimodal integration in granule cells as a basis for associative plasticity and sensory prediction in a cerebellum-like circuit. Neuron 66, 573–584. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sawtell, N. B., and Bell, C. C. (2008). Adaptive processing in electrosensory systems: links to cerebellar plasticity and learning. J. Physiol. Paris 102, 223–232. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.10.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schmahmann, J. D. (2000). The role of the cerebellum in affect and psychosis. J. Neurolinguistics 13, 189–214. doi: 10.1016/S0911-6044(00)00011-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schmahmann, J. D. (2004). Disorders of the cerebellum: ataxia, dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 16, 367–378. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16.3.367

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schmahmann, J. D. (2010). The role of the cerebellum in cognition and emotion: personal reflections since 1982 on the dysmetria of thought hypothesis, and its historical evolution from theory to therapy. Neuropsychol. Rev. 20, 236–260. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9142-x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schmahmann, J. D., and Pandya, D. N. (1989). Anatomical investigation of projections to the basis pontis from posterior parietal association cortices in rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 289, 53–73. doi: 10.1002/cne.902890105

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schmahmann, J. D., and Pandya, D. N. (1991). Projections to the basis pontis from the superior temporal sulcus and superior temporal region in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 308, 224–248. doi: 10.1002/cne.903080209

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schmahmann, J. D., and Pandya, D. N. (1997). Anatomic organization of the basilar pontine projections from prefrontal cortices in rhesus monkey. J. Neurosci. 17, 438–458.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Serrien, D. J., and Wiesendanger, M. (2000). Temporal control of a bimanual task in patients with cerebellar dysfunction. Neuropsychologia 38, 558–565. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00116-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shadmehr, R., and Krakauer, J. W. (2008). A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Exp. Brain Res. 185, 359–381. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1280-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shadmehr, R., Smith, M. A., and Krakauer, J. W. (2010). Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 33, 89–108. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153135

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shidara, M., Kawano, K., Gomi, H., and Kawato, M. (1993). Inverse-dynamics model eye movement control by Purkinje cells in the cerebellum. Nature 365, 50–52. doi: 10.1038/365050a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Soetedjo, R., Kojima, Y., and Fuchs, A. F. (2008). Complex spike activity in the oculomotor vermis of the cerebellum: a vectorial error signal for saccade motor learning? J. Neurophysiol. 100, 1949–1966. doi: 10.1152/jn.90526.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stanley, J., and Miall, R. C. (2009). Using predictive motor control processes in a cognitive task: behavioral and neuroanatomical perspectives. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 629, 337–354. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-77064-2_17

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stone, L. S., and Lisberger, S. G. (1990). Visual responses of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar flocculus during smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkeys. I. Simple spikes. J. Neurophysiol. 63, 1241–1261.

Stoodley, C. J. (2012). The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional imaging studies. Cerebellum 11, 352–365. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0260-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stoodley, C. J., and Schmahmann, J. D. (2009). Functional topography in the human cerebellum: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage 44, 489–501. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.039

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thach, W. T. (1970). Discharge of cerebellar neurons related to two maintained postures and two prompt movements. II. Purkinje cell output and input. J. Neurophysiol. 33, 537–547.

Thach, W. T. (2007). On the mechanism of cerebellar contributions to cognition. Cerebellum 6, 163–167. doi: 10.1080/14734220701373530

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thach, W. T., Goodkin, H. P., and Keating, J. G. (1992). The cerebellum and the adaptive coordination of movement. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 403–442. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.15.030192.002155

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thier, P., Dicke, P. W., Haas, R., Thielert, C. D., and Catz, N. (2002). The role of the oculomotor vermis in the control of saccadic eye movements. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 978, 50–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb07555.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Timmann, D., Drepper, J., Frings, M., Maschke, M., Richter, S., Gerwig, M., et al. (2010). The human cerebellum contributes to motor, emotional and cognitive associative learning. A review. Cortex 46, 845–857. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.06.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Timmann, D., Drepper, J., Maschke, M., Kolb, F. P., Boring, D., Thilmann, A. F., et al. (2002). Motor deficits cannot explain impaired cognitive associative learning in cerebellar patients. Neuropsychologia 40, 788–800. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00181-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Timmann, D., Konczak, J., Ilg, W., Donchin, O., Hermsdorfer, J., Gizewski, E. R., et al. (2009). Current advances in lesion-symptom mapping of the human cerebellum. Neuroscience 162, 836–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tseng, Y. W., Diedrichsen, J., Krakauer, J. W., Shadmehr, R., and Bastian, A. J. (2007). Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 54–62. doi: 10.1152/jn.00266.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Donkelaar, P., and Lee, R. G. (1994). Interactions between the eye and hand motor systems: disruptions due to cerebellar dysfunction. J. Neurophysiol. 72, 1674–1685.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Wallman, J., and Fuchs, A. F. (1998). Saccadic gain modification: visual error drives motor adaptation. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 2405–2416.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Walter, J. T., and Khodakhah, K. (2006). The linear computational algorithm of cerebellar Purkinje cells. J. Neurosci. 26, 12861–12872. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4507-05.2006

CrossRef Full Text

Walter, J. T., and Khodakhah, K. (2009). The advantages of linear information processing for cerebellar computation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 4471–4476. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812348106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Welsh, J. P., Lang, E. J., Suglhara, I., and Llinas, R. (1995). Dynamic organization of motor control within the olivocerebellar system. Nature 374, 453–457. doi: 10.1038/374453a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Witter, L., Canto, C. B., Hoogland, T. M., de Gruijl, J. R., and De Zeeuw, C. I. (2013). Strength and timing of motor responses mediated by rebound firing in the cerebellar nuclei after Purkinje cell activation. Front. Neural Circuits 7:133. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00133

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wolpert, D. M., and Ghahramani, Z. (2000). Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nat. Neurosci. 3(Suppl.), 1212–1217. doi: 10.1038/81497

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wolpert, D. M., and Kawato, M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Netw. 11, 1317–1329. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(98)00066-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wolpert, D. M., Miall, R. C., and Kawato, M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 338–347. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01221-2

CrossRef Full Text

Woodman, G. F., and Luck, S. J. (1999). Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search. Nature 400, 867–869. doi: 10.1038/23698

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Xu-Wilson, M., Chen-Harris, H., Zee, D. S., and Shadmehr, R. (2009). Cerebellar contributions to adaptive control of saccades in humans. J. Neurosci. 29, 12930–12939. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3115-09.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yamazaki, T., and Nagao, S. (2012). A computational mechanism for unified gain and timing control in the cerebellum. PLoS ONE 7:e33319. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033319

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: performance errors, sensory prediction errors, internal models, Purkinje cells, cognition

Citation: Popa LS, Hewitt AL and Ebner TJ (2014) The cerebellum for jocks and nerds alike. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:113. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00113

Received: 28 February 2014; Accepted: 27 May 2014;
Published online: 17 June 2014.

Edited by:

Richard Apps, University of Bristol, UK

Reviewed by:

Yosef Yarom, Hebrew University, Israel
Henrik Jörntell, Lund University, Sweden

Copyright © 2014 Popa, Hewitt and Ebner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Timothy J. Ebner, Department of Neuroscience, University of Minnesota, Lions Research Building, Room 421, 2001 Sixth Street S.E., Minneapolis, MN 55455, USA e-mail: ebner001@umn.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.