- 1Department of Research and Science Education, Phipps Conservatory and Botanical Gardens, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
- 2School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, United States
- 3Department of Earth System Science and the Woods Institute for the Environment, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States
The COVID-19 outbreak drastically altered the behaviors of millions of Americans in 2020, including behaviors that contribute to carbon emissions. As many Americans stayed home midyear, environmental groups noted the decrease in driving and transportation-related pollution, theorizing that the pandemic could have a positive impact on the environment by decreasing individuals' carbon emissions. However, it is dubious that individuals will behave in a more eco-friendly manner under the uncertain and stressful conditions of a global pandemic simply because they are more likely to be confined to their homes. We examined sustainability behaviors in 2018 and in the early pandemic in 2020 among a sample of members of a U.S., botanical garden. We surveyed members in May–July 2018, asking whether they had or had not done 11 sustainability behaviors (e.g., used alternative transportation, took shorter showers) in the past month. We resurveyed members about their engagement in those behaviors in April 2020 as well as to recall their engagement in those behaviors pre-pandemic in February 2020. We examined differences in self-reported behaviors among respondents who had taken both the May–July 2018 and April 2020 surveys (matched group n = 227), and then among respondents who had taken either the May–July (n = 1057) or the April 2020 survey (n = 881), but not both. Respondents in the matched group were more likely to report recycling, reducing red meat consumption, eating a plant-based diet, and reducing food waste in April 2020 compared to May–July 2018; they were less likely to compost, check the air in their tires, and use a smart thermostat. However, these differences also emerged when examining recalled behavior in February 2020, suggesting that matched group respondents' self-reports may reflect changes in behavior over time rather than due to the pandemic. The unmatched group was more likely to reduce food waste but less likely to use alternative transportation to commute, check the air in their tires for fuel efficiency, and recycle in April 2020 compared to May–July 2018. Thus, few changes in sustainability behaviors can be attributed to the pandemic, but those that do involve personal travel or home confinement.
Introduction
The global outbreak of COVID-19 drastically impacted the lives of Americans in 2020. Starting in March of 2020, as state and local authorities imposed lockdowns and stay-at-home orders to mitigate the spread of the deadly virus, many people were confined to their homes, lost their jobs, and experienced stress about the uncertainty of the pandemic. The lockdown measures touched nearly all facets of public and private life for the world's people, as well as countries' economies and the global environment.
In terms of environmental impact, there is some evidence for the contribution of lock-down measures to improved environmental outcomes, at least in the initial stages of the pandemic. Scientists observed large drop-offs in air pollutant levels during the initial lockdown phases of March and April 2020 (Berman and Ebisu, 2020; Muhammad et al., 2020), reduced demand for and use of fossil fuels (Wang et al., 2020), and enhanced wildlife activity (Manenti et al., 2020). Indeed, one study estimated that, as of May 2020, new emissions of global greenhouse gases had been reduced by 2.5 Gt, which the authors attributed to worldwide decreases in production and consumption (Lenzen et al., 2020). Another estimated an 8.8% reduction in CO2 emissions in the first half of 2020 compared to the same time period in 2019 (Liu et al., 2020). Additionally, the popular media initially extolled the benefits of the pandemic for the environment, calling them the “silver lining” of the shutdowns (Cripe, 2020; Rochard, 2020; Walt, 2020).
An assumption of the pandemic-induced environmental benefits narrative is that these large-scale benefits are partially driven by individual behaviors—that people in lockdown, or with reduced socialization and mobilization due to the pandemic, have changed their consumption or carbon-emitting habits en masse. There is some evidence to support this pandemic-induced reduction in carbon emissions. For example, one survey of U.K. adults in April of 2020 found that food shoppers reported fewer shopping trips, increased preparation of home-cooked meals, and decreased food waste (Roberts and Downing, 2020). In the transportation sector, large decreases in traffic volume were documented in the initial months of the pandemic (Clark, 2020; Hudda et al., 2020) as the number of daily vehicle-miles decreased (Dutzik, 2020; Stavrinos et al., 2020). Additionally, residential sector emissions reductions were estimated to account for 3% of the global decrease in daily CO2 emissions (Liu et al., 2020). We note that while most estimates of greenhouse gas emission reductions due to the pandemic include a mix of commercial or industrial sector estimates (e.g., steel production, infrastructure) as well as indices of individual behaviors (e.g., residential electricity use), there has not been a thorough comparison of individual-level versus commercial-level contributions to pandemic-linked environmental benefits.
However, there is also evidence that conditions specific to the pandemic are fostering more wasteful or higher carbon behaviors. In attempts to reduce contact with potentially virus-laden products, many people increased their consumption of single-use plastics (Patrício Silva et al., 2021), such as in food takeout containers. Market research firms have noted sharp pandemic-linked increases in online shopping activity (Rattner, 2020) that would be associated with increases in plastic and cardboard packaging as well as use of fossil fuels for transportation linked to e-commerce. Additionally, plastic and other single-use waste from disposable personal protective equipment (PPE) in the medical sector has increased (Rizan et al., 2021) along with waste associated with disposable face masks worn by members of the general public (Fadare and Okoffo, 2020; Sangkham, 2020).
In addition, it is possible that the pandemic's influence on sustainability behaviors is more strongly seen in specific populations. For instance, the pandemic and its associated economic downturn had disproportionate negative impacts on poorer Americans. Researchers found that while private sector employment decreased by 22% in the first months of the pandemic, the impact was greater among low income workers, who saw their employment decline by 35% compared to 9% for high income workers (Cajner et al., 2020). Additionally, the kinds of jobs that can be done from home (thus not requiring a carbon-intensive commute) are linked to education and income such that high income, well-educated people are more likely to be able to work from home during the pandemic (Baker, 2020; Hoenig and Wenz, 2020). Thus, to the extent that enacting sustainability behaviors requires financial resources or status-based privileges, those with higher incomes would be expected to enact more of those behaviors than low income workers. Alternately, some sustainability behaviors, such as reducing food waste or saving water, are associated with saving money. We might expect that people with lower incomes or more financial instability would enact these sustainability behaviors, to the extent that those behaviors are cost-saving.
One drawback to most of this research on specific individual sustainability behaviors early in the pandemic is that it lacks strong reference groups against which to measure changes in behavior. Some surveys rely on self-reports that explicitly ask respondents to describe their current pandemic behavior in contrast with pre-pandemic onset behavior (e.g., Roberts and Downing, 2020), and these retrospective comparisons might not be accurate as they depend on respondents being able to accurately recall past behavior. In addition, some studies report changes in large-scale trends of consumer behaviors but lack detailed reporting of behavioral shifts at an individual level. Studies that link individuals' behaviors over time are needed to rectify issues of retrospective self-report.
In order to overcome these deficiencies in methodologies to better understand the environmental impact of the pandemic, we need to better document how the pandemic altered sustainability behaviors that are linked to greenhouse gas emissions as well as waste. Accounting for changes in individuals' sustainability behaviors pre-pandemic to early pandemic will allow a more comprehensive understanding of the role that households play in characterizing the environmental impact of the pandemic.
The current study presents a glimpse into sustainability behavior changes in the early pandemic in a sample of botanical garden members in Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania. This sample is comprised of people who are environmentally engaged and have more education than the general population (Drummond et al., 2020), and thus are an ideal sample to examine for changes in sustainability behaviors, as they likely had higher baseline engagement in eco-friendly actions. Due to the unprecedented nature of the pandemic and the exploratory nature of this work, we ask the following research question in lieu of establishing directional hypotheses: Are environmentally engaged individuals more likely, less likely, or equally likely to report engaging in various sustainability behaviors after the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic? In general, environmental beliefs tend to predict pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors (Gadenne et al., 2011), but this relation is highly dependent on the specific environmental beliefs and behaviors in question. For example, one longitudinal study of Americans found that those who had the strongest beliefs in and were most concerned about climate change were least likely to report engaging in individual sustainability behaviors but more likely to support policies fighting climate change (Hall et al., 2018). How these relations might play out in the unprecedented context of a global pandemic remains to be seen, as it is possible that a positive relation between concern about climate change and pro-environmental behaviors could be nullified by pandemic concerns. That is, a desire to act in environmentally friendly ways could be superseded by the more immediate demands of surviving a global pandemic. Interestingly, whereas recent research has highlighted the importance of future orientation in predicting pro-environmental behaviors (Beiser-McGrath and Huber, 2018), some have speculated that the trauma of the pandemic could disrupt positive expectations for the future (Holman and Grisham, 2020), which would decrease motivation to act sustainably. However, a recent longitudinal study of U.K. adults showed no change in climate concern from 2019 to June 2020, indicating that concern for climate change is robust to the assumed insecurities of the pandemic (Evensen et al., 2021). To the extent that climate concerns motivate sustainability behaviors, this would indicate that sustainability behaviors remained constant through the pandemic. Alternatively, it is possible that those with high concern for climate change increase their sustainable behaviors given coverage of the pandemic as environmentally beneficial. In this case, a social identity as an environmentalist motivates behaviors that are in line with the expectations of others (which would be that the pandemic is allowing for nature to heal, etc.), as social identification can influence behavior via the desire to act in accordance with group goals (Cialdini, 2003; Oyserman et al., 2007). Thus, this study aims to determine if environmentally-conscious people did, in fact, engage in more eco-friendly behaviors due to the pandemic.
Data from this project comes from two waves of surveys of those botanical garden members—the first administered in the summer of 2018 and the second in the summer of 2020. This novel study examines changes in self-reported completion of 11 sustainability behaviors. We examine these behaviors in two ways—first with a longitudinal design that assessed changes in the same sample of respondents over 2 years, and second with a cross-sectional design that examined differences between two samples of respondents surveyed pre-pandemic and in the early pandemic. This multi-method approach allows us to verify the robustness of any changes in sustainability behaviors.
Methods
Participants
Members of a botanical garden were twice surveyed about their attitudes and behaviors regarding environmental issues. Between May and July of 2018, a survey was sent to 21,763 members of Phipps Conservatory and Botanical Gardens and 1,284 responses were recorded (response rate: 6%). Between June and August of 2020, a similar survey was sent to 30,480 Phipps members and 1,108 responses were recorded (response rate: 4%). Of these two waves of survey respondents, 227 respondents took both surveys, resulting in the matched sample. Removing the matched sample respondents from the pre-pandemic and early pandemic samples results in a pre-pandemic unmatched sample size of 1,057 and an early pandemic unmatched sample size of 881 respondents. See Table 1 for comparisons of these samples in terms of self-reported demographic characteristics.
Compared to the 2019 demographics of the local population of residents within the county in which the botanical garden is located, our sample is more likely to be female (77.1% May–July 2018 unmatched sample; 77.3% April 2020 unmatched sample; 72.5% matched sample vs. 51.6% locally) and have at least a Bachelor's degree (83.9% May–July 2018 unmatched sample; 84.9% April 2020 unmatched sample; 92.4% matched sample vs. 42.9% locally) (U.S. Census Bureau, 2021).
Procedure and Design
Botanical garden members were sent a link to the survey via emails sent to the member listserv of Phipps. Detailed response rate information for 2018 are reported in previous reporting on these data in Drummond et al. (2020). In this 2018 survey, respondents provided information on their current sustainability behaviors. In the 2020 survey, a total of 30,480 households were contacted, out of which 1,587 (5%) households accessed the link to the survey. Of those that accessed the survey, 11 did not give their consent to participate in research and were not allowed to continue to the survey and 468 did not complete the survey, leaving a final sample of 1,108 with an overall response rate of 4%. In this 2020 survey, respondents provided information on their current sustainability behaviors as well as recalled information about their pre-pandemic sustainability behaviors.
Terminology and Timeline
See Figure 1 for a timeline of the survey distributions and associated terminology. For ease of reporting, we will refer to the pre-pandemic time, assessed by the first survey, as “May–July 2018.” We will refer to the recalled pre-pandemic time, assessed by the second survey, as “February 2020.” Finally, we will refer to the early pandemic time, also assessed by the second survey, as “April 2020.”
Survey Measures
Sustainability Behaviors
In the pre-pandemic survey, participants were presented with 11 individual actions and with the following description: “Below is a list of actions people can take to reduce a household's impacts on the environment. Please indicate which actions you personally took in the past month.” The actions were drawn from prior research on environmental behavior (Gardner and Stern, 2010; Truelove and Parks, 2012) and estimates of greenhouse gas emissions (Hawken, 2017) and are as follows: used public transportation, biked, or walked to work instead of driving; used energy efficient lightbulbs such as CFLs or LEDs; recycled; taken shorter showers; driven a hybrid or electric vehicle; reduced red meat consumption; eaten a more plant-based diet; reduced food waste; composted waste; checked the air in your tires to ensure fuel efficiency; used a smart thermostat; installed or used low-flow shower heads or faucets. Participants could respond “Yes,” “No,” or “Not applicable.”
In the 2020 survey, participants were presented with the same 11 behaviors and response options as the May–July 2018 survey in two response contexts. First, participants were asked to think back to their lives before the COVID-19 outbreak (the survey instructions for this section included the line “For many people, this will mean thinking about February 2020”) and were instructed to indicate which behaviors they had done in February of 2020. We note that these behaviors are recalled pre-pandemic behaviors and will be denoted by “February 2020.” Then participants were asked to think about their household's behaviors during the COVID-19 outbreak (the survey instructions for this section included the line “For many people, this will mean thinking about April 2020”) and were instructed to indicate which behaviors they had done in April of 2020. We will refer to these contemporaneous reports of behaviors as “April 2020.”
Statistical Methods
We will present two parallel sets of analyses comparing May–July 2018 sustainability behaviors to April 2020 sustainability behaviors using two samples—a matched group and an unmatched group. For the purposes of this analysis, all “Not applicable” responses are treated as missing; “No” responses were coded as 0 and “Yes” responses as 1. For the matched group comparisons, we use McNemar's test to test for differences in the marginal probabilities of responses changing at two time points; first comparing May–July 2018 responses to February 2020 responses and second comparing May–July 2018 responses to April 2020 responses. These analyses test for differences between changes from “No” to “Yes” responses compared to “Yes” to “No” responses. We use the McNemar's test with a continuity correction when a cell count in the contingency table is <5 and correct for multiple comparisons with the discrete Bonferroni-Holm multiplicity adjustment. For discordant pairs of cells (i.e., those changing their response over time from “No” to “Yes” or from “Yes” to “No”) that are low-occurrence (<25), we do not perform McNemar's test due to the increased Type I error rate (Agresti, 2014; Fagerland et al., 2014). For the unmatched group comparisons, we use chi-squared tests to test for differences in the expected frequencies and the observed frequencies of the “No/Yes” responses May–July 2018 and April 2020, correcting for multiple comparisons with the Bonferroni adjusted alpha level of 0.0045 (0.05/11) per test.
Results
Matched Group Analysis
The ratios of response changes, or discordant cells, for the matched sample can be found in Table 2.
Used Public Transportation, Biked, or Walked to Work Instead of Driving
Due to a low count in the discordant pair, McNemar's test was not performed comparing May–July 2018 to February 2020. The proportion of people reporting having done this action changed from May–July 2018 to April 2020, χ2 (1, N = 79) = 7.22, p = 0.007 with continuity correction, such that the proportion of people responding “No” May–July 2018 and then “Yes” April 2020 (0.00%) is smaller than the proportion of people responding “Yes” May–July 2018 and then “No” April 2020 (65.91%). However, we note that an especially high proportion of respondents in the early pandemic selected “Not applicable” to this item-−58.6% compared to less than 17% for all other sustainability behaviors. Thus, we conducted an exploratory, post-hoc analysis to directly account for the “Not applicable” responses in the early pandemic. We tested for changes in paired responses in a 3 ×3 matrix of responses in 2018 (“Yes,” “No,” or “Not applicable”) and in April 2020 (“Yes,” “No,” or “Not applicable”) using the Stuart Maxwell test and found a significant difference, χ2 (2, N = 227) = 62.10, p < 0.001 post-hoc pairwise comparisons revealed a significant increase from those who selected “Yes” in May–July 2018 to “Not applicable” in April 2020 (adjusted χ2 p < 0.001), for those who selected “No” in May–July 2018 to “Not applicable” in April 2020 (adjusted χ2 p = 0.0024), and for those who selected “Yes” in May–July 2018 to “No” in April 2020 (adjusted χ2 p < 0.001).
Used Energy Efficient Lightbulbs
Due to a low count in the discordant pair, McNemar's test was not performed comparing May–July 2018 to February 2020. Similarly, due to a low count in the discordant pair, McNemar's test was not performed comparing May–July 2018 to April 2020.
Recycled
Due to a low count in the discordant pair, McNemar's test was not performed comparing May–July 2018 to February 2020. Similarly, due to a low count in the discordant pair, McNemar's test was not performed comparing May–July 2018 to April 2020.
Taken Shorter Showers
The proportion of people reporting having done this action did not change significantly from May–July 2018 to February 2020, χ2 (1, N = 223) = 4.37, p = 0.037, as this comparison did not survive the discrete Bonferroni-Holmes multiplicity adjustment correction with alpha set at 0.0125. In addition, the proportion of people reporting having done this action did not change from May–July 2018 to April 2020, χ2 (1, N = 220) = 4.12, p = 0.042, as this comparison did not survive the discrete Bonferroni-Holmes multiplicity adjustment correction with alpha set at 0.0167.
Driven a Hybrid or Electric Vehicle
Due to a low count in the discordant pair, McNemar's test was not performed comparing May–July 2018 to February 2020. Similarly, due to a low count in the discordant pair, McNemar's test was not performed comparing May–July 2018 to April 2020.
Reduced Red Meat Consumption
The proportion of people reporting having done this action changed from May–July 2018 to February 2020, χ2 (1, N = 211) = 47.65, p < 0.001, such that the proportion of people responding “No” May–July 2018 and then “Yes” February 2020 (29.82%) is larger than the proportion of people responding “Yes” May–July 2018 and then “No” February 2020 (15.58%). In addition, the proportion of people reporting having done this action changed from May–July 2018 to April 2020, χ2 (1, N = 210) = 36.10, p < 0.001, such that the proportion of people responding “No” May–July 2018 and then “Yes” April 2020 (27.59%) is larger than the proportion of people responding “Yes” May–July 2018 and then “No” April 2020 (22.37%).
Eaten a More Plant-Based Diet
The proportion of people reporting having done this action changed from May–July 2018 to February 2020, χ2 (1, N = 222) = 42.36, p < 0.001, such that the proportion of people responding “No” May–July 2018 and then “Yes” February 2020 (16.95%) is larger than the proportion of people responding “Yes” May–July 2018 and then “No” February 2020 (15.34%). In addition, the proportion of people reporting having done this action changed from May–July 2018 to April 2020, χ2 (1, N = 223) = 42.55, p < 0.001, such that the proportion of people responding “No” May–July 2018 and then “Yes” April 2020 (21.67%) is larger than the proportion of people responding “Yes” May–July 2018 and then “No” April 2020 (17.18%).
Reduced Food Waste
The proportion of people reporting having done this action changed from May–July 2018 to February 2020, χ2 (1, N = 222) = 114.89, p < 0.001, such that the proportion of people responding “No” May–July 2018 and then “Yes” February 2020 (53.66%) is larger than the proportion of people responding “Yes” May–July 2018 and then “No” February 2020 (9.39%). In addition, the proportion of people reporting having done this action changed from May–July 2018 to April 2020, χ2 (1, N = 223) = 111.36, p < 0.001, such that the proportion of people responding “No” May–July 2018 and then “Yes” April 2020 (57.14%) is larger than the proportion of people responding “Yes” May–July 2018 and then “No” April 2020 (12.71%).
Composted
The proportion of people reporting having done this action did not change from May–July 2018 to February 2020, χ2 (1, N = 216) = 3.27, p = 0.07, as this comparison did not survive the discrete Bonferroni-Holmes multiplicity adjustment correction with alpha set at 0.025. In addition, the proportion of people reporting having done this action did not change from May–July 2018 to April 2020, χ2 (1, N = 214) = 4.37, p = 0.037, as this comparison did not survive the discrete Bonferroni-Holmes multiplicity adjustment correction with alpha set at 0.01.
Checked Air in Tires to Ensure Fuel Efficiency
The proportion of people reporting having done this action changed from May–July 2018 to February 2020, χ2 (1, N = 214) = 31.53, p < 0.001, such that the proportion of people responding “No” May–July 2018 and then “Yes” February 2020 (36.00%) is larger than the proportion of people responding “Yes” May–July 2018 and then “No” February 2020 (12.95%). In addition, the proportion of people reporting having done this action changed from May–July 2018 to April 2020, χ2 (1, N = 201) = 9.31, p = 0.002, such that the proportion of people responding “No” May–July 2018 and then “Yes” April 2020 (23.61%) is smaller than the proportion of people responding “Yes” May–July 2018 and then “No” April 2020 (28.68%).
Used a Smart Thermostat
The proportion of people reporting having done this action changed from May–July 2018 to February 2020, χ2 (1, N = 212) = 8.00, p = 0.0046, such that the proportion of people responding “No” May–July 2018 and then “Yes” February 2020 (22.05%) is smaller than the proportion of people responding “Yes” May–July 2018 and then “No” February 2020 (25.88%). In addition, the proportion of people reporting having done this action changed from May–July 2018 to April 2020, χ2 (1, N = 211) = 10.19, p = 0.0014, such that the proportion of people responding “No” May–July 2018 and then “Yes” April 2020 (18.25%) is smaller than the proportion of people responding “Yes” May–July 2018 and then “No” April 2020 (27.06%).
Unmatched Group Analysis
Raw counts of responses for the unmatched sample, as well as p-values from the chi-squared tests, can be found in Table 3.
Used Public Transportation, Biked, or Walked to Work Instead of Driving
The proportion of people reporting having done this action differed by time, χ2 (1, N = 1176) = 55.74, p < 0.001. Respondents were less likely to say that they had used alternative transportation to get to work in the past month in April 2020 (23.00%) than in May–July 2018 (45.07%). We additionally explored the selection of “NA” as an additional response option for this sustainability behavior, as we did for the matched group. In this case, the proportion of responses differed by time, χ2 (2, N = 1176) = 155.69, p < 0.001. A series of post-hoc tests revealed that the proportion of people responding “Yes” differed by time such that fewer people responded “Yes” in April 2020 compared to May–July 2018 (p < 0.001), and the proportion of people responding “NA” differed by time such that more people responded “NA” in April 2020 (n = 455) compared to May–July 2018 (n = 307; p < 0.001).
Used Energy Efficient Lightbulbs
The proportion of people reporting having done this action did not differ by time, χ2 (1, N = 1,911) = 7.80, p = 0.005, as the test did not survive the Bonferroni correction.
Recycled
The proportion of people reporting having done this action differed by time, χ2 (1, N = 1,914) = 10.68, p = 0.001. Respondents were less likely to say that they had recycled in the past month in April 2020 (92.82%) than in May–July 2018 (96.29%).
Taken Shorter Showers
The proportion of people reporting having done this action did not differ by time, χ2 (1, N = 1,906) = 6.02, p = 0.014, as the test did not survive the Bonferroni correction.
Driven a Hybrid or Electric Vehicle
The proportion of people reporting having done this action did not differ by time, χ2 (1, N = 1,743) = 5.17, p = 0.023, as the test did not survive the Bonferroni correction.
Reduced Red Meat Consumption
The proportion of people reporting having done this action did not differ by time, χ2 (1, N = 1,860) = 0.44, p = 0.51.
Eaten a More Plant-Based Diet
The proportion of people reporting having done this action did not differ by time, χ2 (1, N = 1,902) = 0.74, p = 0.39.
Reduced Food Waste
The proportion of people reporting having done this action differed by time, χ2 (1, N = 1,914) = 9.27, p = 0.002. Respondents were more likely to say that they had reduced food waste in the past month in April 2020 (82.16%) than in May–July 2018 (76.36%).
Composted
The proportion of people reporting having done this action did not differ by time, χ2 (1, N = 1,877) = 0.44, p = 0.36.
Checked the Air in Your Tires to Ensure Fuel Efficiency
The proportion of people reporting having done this action differed by time, χ2 (1, N = 1,833) = 20.38, p < 0.001. Respondents were less likely to say that they had checked the air in their tires in the past month in April 2020 (52.07%) than in May–July 2018 (62.67%).
Used a Smart Thermostat
The proportion of people reporting having done this action did not differ by time, χ2 (1, N = 1,835) = 0.01, p = 0.92.
Discussion
In a novel natural experiment, this research tracked changes in botanical garden members' self-reports of engaging in a variety of sustainability behaviors before and after the onset of the global COVID-19 pandemic. We tracked these changes in two ways. First, we identified a matched group of garden members who had responded to both a 2018 and a 2020 survey and we compared their responses over time. Second, we defined two unmatched groups of respondents who had either taken the 2018 or the 2020 survey, but not both, and compared their responses as independent samples.
Across both of our matched and unmatched samples, we find no evidence of a widespread shift in sustainability behaviors after the onset of the pandemic in either direction. While the small (n = 227) group of matched sample respondents were more likely to report engaging in sustainability behaviors after the onset of the pandemic than 2 years prior, this shift appears to have occurred prior to the onset of the pandemic, as respondents also were more likely to report engaging in these behaviors in February of 2020. Specifically, respondents were more likely to report that they reduced their red meat consumption and food waste, ate a more plant-based diet, and used a smart thermostat in both February 2020 and April 2020 compared to 2018. That these changes could be measured in February 2020 suggests that the pandemic, which began to impact most Americans in March 2020, is not a causal factor driving behavioral change.
This pattern of matched group respondents being more likely to report engaging in sustainability behaviors in both February and April of 2020 compared to 2018 reflects broad changes over time toward more sustainable behaviors that, while not pandemic-related, could be attributed to the composition of the matched group. This is a sample of 227 members of a botanical garden who responded twice, nearly 2 years apart, to environmental behavior surveys sent by that botanical garden. These respondents represent just 1.04% of the total botanical garden members contacted in 2018 and 0.74% of the total contacted in 2020. That is, the matched sample respondents represent a minority of all respondents who, based on their demonstrated reliability in taking multiple surveys from the botanical garden over time, might reasonably be viewed as categorically different from one-time respondents in a way that is related to their increased likelihood of engaging in sustainability behaviors over time. Indeed, the response patterns from this group may be a result of self-selection bias, whereby respondents from the first survey who wish to report on their more sustainable behaviors are more likely to participate in the second survey, but those who would not have more sustainable behaviors to report would be less likely to participate a second time. We additionally note that a larger proportion of the matched sample has a graduate or professional degree than both unmatched groups (Table 1).
We did find changes in two sustainability behaviors involving personal travel both pre-pandemic and in the early pandemic. First, post-hoc tests revealed that the matched group respondents selected “Not applicable” for using alternative transportation to get to work in the early pandemic when they had previously selected “Yes” in 2018. This shift could be explained by the fact that many people in the early pandemic switched to remote work, negating the need for a commute at all, or lost their jobs. This finding, paired with their decreased likelihood to check the air in their tires to ensure fuel efficiency in the early pandemic (the only behavior that showed an early pandemic effect), suggests that the pandemic reduced driving time and driving-associated behaviors.
The findings from our unmatched samples demonstrate the complexities of pandemic life for sustainable lifestyles. Compared to pre-pandemic respondents, respondents in the early stages of the pandemic were more likely to engage in one sustainability behavior—reducing food waste. However, these respondents were also less likely to engage in three sustainability behaviors: recycling, using alternative transportation to get to work, and checking the air in their tires, compared to pre-early pandemic respondents. Instead of a widespread shift toward or away from sustainable behaviors, these findings reflect a more nuanced view of how the realities of the pandemic, and its accompanying effects on employment and leisure, have downstream consequences for sustainability behaviors.
The increased likelihood of reducing food waste is consistent with other studies on the impact of the pandemic on food usage (Rodgers et al., 2021), and reveals a potential environmental benefit of the pandemic. Reducing individuals' food waste at the point of consumption has been identified as a top priority in creating a sustainable food system that will ultimately reduce global greenhouse gas emissions (Hawken, 2017; Willett et al., 2019). While it is possible that respondents in the early pandemic reduced their food waste in the interest of making more environmentally-friendly choices, the fact that this was the sole sustainability behavior that increased suggests that another explanation is needed. First, in the early weeks of nationwide shutdowns in the U.S., there were food shortages across the meat and dairy sectors (Peel, 2021; Weersink et al., 2021) and increases in panic-buying and stockpiling behavior among food shoppers (Keane and Neal, 2021). Thus, this scarcity, or perceived scarcity, of food may have caused respondents to reduce food waste as a means to maximize their food supply. Additionally, people may have been motivated to fully exhaust their pantries before risking exposure to the virus by venturing out to the grocery store. A final possibility is that, with pandemic-related losses in income, respondents were reducing food waste as a cost-saving measure.
The finding that unmatched group respondents were less likely to report recycling in the early pandemic compared to pre-pandemic demonstrates a potential environmental disadvantage of the pandemic. Recycling is a commonly cited example of an action that reduces one's carbon footprint (Attari et al., 2010, 2016), and it is frequently recommended in environmental literature despite having a low impact on reducing carbon emissions (Wynes and Nicholas, 2017). A multitude of individual-level factors have been positively linked to recycling behaviors (e.g., self-efficacy or pro-environmental values; Geiger et al., 2019), but these factors are likely to be relatively stable in our sample population of botanical garden members. For example, our measure of concern for climate change did not differ between the unmatched groups, indicating that the pandemic did not impact climate change perceptions. Thus, it is unlikely that the pandemic reduced recycling by negatively impacting perceptions of the environment or recycling attitudes; instead, it is likely that the contextual or incidental factors related to the pandemic were the cause. Recycling is less likely to occur in households that lack a recycling bin (Geiger et al., 2019), and with people spending more time at home with fewer opportunities to recycle in public places due to the pandemic, those without established household recycling habits were perhaps less likely to encounter opportunities to recycle. However, we note that the recycling rates for both pre-pandemic and pandemic-era groups were high, and thus we caution against interpreting a decline in recycling from 96 to 92% as a particularly dire environmental consequence of the pandemic.
The findings that relate to transportation (i.e., taking alternative transportation to work and checking the air in your tires to ensure fuel efficiency) from the unmatched group comparisons reveal the extent to which the pandemic has influenced personal travel patterns. First, as with the matched group, unmatched group respondents may be less likely to report taking alternative transportation to work because they are working remotely and thus do not need to travel in any capacity to their workplace, or because they have experienced pandemic-related job loss and have no workplace that would necessitate a commute. Thus, this decreased likelihood of using alternative transportation, while on its face seems to imply an increased reliance on traditional, high-carbon commuting, may in fact represent a decline in all commuting behaviors, which is itself a sustainability behavior on par with using alternative transportation. Unfortunately, this research cannot definitively say whether this is the case, as we did not ask for the reason behind such responses. However, pandemic-related decreases in use of public transportation specifically (Ahangari et al., 2020; Teixeira and Lopes, 2020) and mobility generally (Warren and Skillman, 2020) are well-documented and fit with our findings. Similarly, unmatched group respondents' decreased likelihood of checking the air in their tires to ensure fuel efficiency may on its face seem like a decrease in sustainable behavior, but in actuality may reflect a decrease in personal vehicle usage.
This pattern of results from the unmatched groups may reflect more net-positive environmental benefits after pandemic onset than at first glance. First, unmatched respondents in the early pandemic were more likely to reduce food waste and possibly less likely to use personal vehicles than pre-pandemic respondents, which would reflect behaviors that are commonly cited as effective for reducing individual greenhouse gas emissions (Hawken, 2017). Additionally, while the reduction in recycling behavior is potentially vexing, we note that it is a small effect among people who were likely at ceiling for the behavior. Paired with the fact that individual recycling is a relatively low-impact sustainability behavior in terms of its carbon-reduction potential (Wynes and Nicholas, 2017), we cannot conclude that our sample's decrease in recycling behavior would have a large negative environmental impact. Thus, the change in sustainability behaviors of the unmatched sample are, on balance, likely to be modestly positive.
However, there is reason to be skeptical that these changes in sustainability behaviors will outlive the pandemic era. In countries where the recovery from the pandemic has begun, such as China, there has been an increase in domestic travel for leisure and work approaching pre-pandemic levels (Chen et al., 2020). Indeed, even in the U.S. there is emerging evidence of recoveries in traffic volume, such as in usage of toll roads (Conduent Business Services, 2020) and increased number of driver-reported trips (CNN Business, 2021). Some researchers, while acknowledging that nearly half of the global decrease in daily carbon emissions comes from the transportation sector, predict that the effect will be temporary as it does not reflect underlying changes in transport systems (Le Quéré et al., 2020).
Reductions in food waste could potentially have a long-lasting effect. Many have noted an aversion to food waste both culturally and individually after crises such as the Great Depression (Poppendieck, 1986). With the COVID-19 pandemic in particular, a recent study found that those experiencing pandemic-related unemployment spent less on food and had less confidence in their ability to afford food than those not experiencing unemployment (Restrepo et al., 2021), which could increase or perpetuate food waste reduction behaviors, as food waste minimization has been linked to a desire to not waste money (Graham-Rowe et al., 2014). To the extent that the pandemic imparts a lasting belief in frugality on those who lived through it, the transition to food waste reduction behaviors should be stable. However, to the extent that food waste reduction is linked to food or income insecurity, one would predict that this behavior would not continue post-pandemic when unemployment rates decline. Similarly, one might anticipate that a return to business-as-usual for our sample would result in a return to formerly high levels of recycling behaviors as well.
While these results are modestly positive, we recognize a primary limitation to this work is a potential lack of generalizability to the wider U.S. and international populations. The uniqueness of our sample of botanical garden members meant that they were ideal for studying changes in sustainability behaviors because they were more likely to be doing those behaviors pre-pandemic, but it also means that translating our effects to a less environmentally-engaged population is difficult. We also acknowledge that our sample could be at ceiling for these behaviors, and thus it would be fruitful for future studies to examine sustainability behavior change among those with low or average baseline sustainability behavior engagement. Additionally, our sample had higher education, and likely higher income, than a representative U.S. sample. This likely resulted in a sample that was economically secure and stable during the pandemic, so future research should clarify the role that income and financial security played in pandemic-related changes in sustainability behaviors. Future research could also examine engagement in sustainability behaviors not studied here, like reducing consumption of dairy products (Kause et al., 2019), purchasing energy-efficient household appliances, or insulating and weatherizing their home (Stern et al., 2016).
Despite the lack of certainty regarding the continuation of these sustainability behaviors, we can draw some general conclusions about the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on sustainability behaviors in an environmentally engaged sample. First, we find no evidence of changes in most of the sustainability behaviors measured that can be attributed to the pandemic. Second, many of the observed changes seem to center around decreases in personal travel behaviors that are probably linked to early pandemic stay-at-home orders. Finally, increases in reducing food waste and decreases in recycling were found, but further research into behaviors occurring beyond the early pandemic stage are needed to discern if these changes are long-lasting.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Michigan Institutional Review Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
CD, RP, GW-P, and SS contributed to the design and implementation of the surveys upon which this study is based and provided feedback on the manuscript. KM, CD, GW-P, and SS contributed to the conceptual design of the study. CD organized and maintained the data. KM performed the statistical analyses and wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Agresti, A. (2014). Categorical Data Analysis. Hoboken: Wiley. Available online at: https://books.google.com/books?hl=enandlr=andid=hpEzw4T0sPUCandoi=fndandpg=PR7andots=nsBh9n9wO3andsig=sBE5TY6uNsSIgTYyHQjenLDRdx4 (accessed December 10, 2020).
Ahangari, S., Chavis, C., and Jeihani, M. (2020). Public transit ridership analysis during the COVID-19 pandemic. MedRxiv [preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2020.10.25.20219105
Attari, S. Z., DeKay, M. L., Davidson, C. I., and De Bruin, W. B. (2010). Public perceptions of energy consumption and savings. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 107, 16054–16059. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001509107
Attari, S. Z., Krantz, D. H., and Weber, E. U. (2016). Energy conservation goals: What people adopt, what they recommend, and why. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 11, 342–351.
Baker, M. (2020). Characterizing occupations that cannot work from home: a means to identify susceptible worker groups during the COVID-19 pandemic. Am. J. Public Health. 110, 1126–1135. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2020.305738
Beiser-McGrath, L. F., and Huber, R. A. (2018). Assessing the relative importance of psychological and demographic factors for predicting climate and environmental attitudes. Climatic Change 149, 335–347. doi: 10.1007/s10584-018-2260-9
Berman, J. D., and Ebisu, K. (2020). Changes in U.S. air pollution during the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci. Total Environ. 739:139864. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.139864
Cajner, T., Crane, L., Decker, R. A., Grigsby, J., Hamins-Puertolas, A., Hurst, E., et al. (2020). The U.S. labor market during the beginning of the pandemic recession (NBER Working Paper No. w27159). National Bureau of Economic Research. Available online at: https://www.nber.org/papers/w27159 (accessed December 16, 2020).
Chen, G., Enger, W., Saxon, S., and Yu, J. (2020). What can Other Countries Learn From China's Travel and Tourism Recovery During the COVID-19 Crisis? Rosemont, IL: McKinsey and Company. Available online at: https://www.mckinsey.com/industries/travel-logistics-and-infrastructure/our-insights/what-can-other-countries-learn-from-chinas-travel-recovery-path# (accessed December 15, 2020).
Cialdini, R. B. (2003). Crafting normative messages to protect the environment. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 12, 105–109. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01242
Clark, S. (2020). The traffic data that shows the road into—and out of—Covid-19 lockdown. The Guardian. Available online at: https://www.theguardian.com/world/ng-interactive/2020/apr/27/the-traffic-data-that-shows-the-road-into-and-out-of-covid-19-lockdown (accessed November 11, 2020).
CNN Business (2021). US economic Recovery After the Coronavirus Pandemic and Recession. Available online at: https://www.cnn.com/business/us-economic-recovery-coronavirus (accessed February 22, 2021).
Conduent Business Services (2020). Traffic on major U.S. toll road systems achieves strong recovery toward pre-COVID-19 levels. GlobeNewswire. Available online at: https://www.globenewswire.com/news-release/2020/12/17/2147000/0/en/Traffic-on-Major-U-S-Toll-Road-Systems-Achieves-Strong-Recovery-Toward-Pre-COVID-19-Levels.html (accessed December 15, 2020).
Cripe, T. (2020). Is there a silver lining of the COVID-19 pandemic? Environmental experts say maybe. Utah State University Today. https://www.usu.edu/today/story/is-there-a-silver-lining-of-the-covid-19-pandemic-environmental-experts-say-maybe (accessed December 16, 2020).
Drummond, C., States, S. L., and Wong-Parodi, G. (2020). Factors associated with the adoption of renewable energy amongst botanical garden members. Environ. Res. Commun. 2:051005. doi: 10.1088/2515-7620/ab8a70
Dutzik, T. (2020). America on pause: Vehicle travel during COVID-19 and what comes next. U.S. Public Interest Research Group. Available online at: https://uspirg.org/blogs/blog/usp/america-pause-vehicle-travel-during-covid-19-and-what-comes-next (accessed November 13, 2020).
Evensen, D., Whitmarsh, L., Bartie, P., Devine-Wright, P., Dickie, J., Varley, A., et al. (2021). Effect of “finite pool of worry” and COVID-19 on UK climate change perceptions. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 118:3. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2018936118
Fadare, O. O., and Okoffo, E. D. (2020). Covid-19 face masks: a potential source of microplastic fibers in the environment. Sci. Total Environ. 737:140279. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.140279
Fagerland, M. W., Lydersen, S., and Laake, P. (2014). Recommended tests and confidence intervals for paired binomial proportions. Stat. Med. 33, 2850–2875. doi: 10.1002/sim.6148
Gadenne, D., Sharma, B., Kerr, D., and Smith, T. (2011). The influence of consumers' environmental beliefs and attitudes on energy saving behaviours. Energy Policy 39, 7684–7694. doi: 10.1016/j.enpol.2011.09.002
Gardner, G. T., and Stern, P. C. (2010). The short list: The most effective actions U.S. households can take to curb climate change. Environ. Sci. Policy Sustain. Develop. 50, 12–25. doi: 10.3200/ENVT.50.5.12-25
Geiger, J., Steg, L., van der Werff, E., and Unal, A. (2019). A meta-analysis of factors related to recycling. J. Environ. Psychol. 64:78–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.05.004
Graham-Rowe, E., Jessop, D. C., and Sparks, P. (2014). Identifying motivations and barriers to minimising household food waste. Resourc. Conserv. Recycl. 84, 15–23. doi: 10.1016/j.resconrec.2013.12.005
Hall, M. P., Lewis, N. A., and Ellsworth, P. C. (2018). Believing in climate change, but not behaving sustainably: evidence from a one-year longitudinal study. J. Environ. Psychol. 56, 55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.03.001
Hawken, P. (2017). Drawdown: The Most Comprehensive Plan Ever Proposed to Reverse Global Warming. London: Penguin Books.
Hoenig, K., and Wenz, S. E. (2020). Education, health behavior, and working conditions during the pandemic: evidence from a German sample. Euro. Soc. 20, 1–14. doi: 10.1080/14616696.2020.1824004
Holman, E. A., and Grisham, E. L. (2020). When time falls apart: the public health implications of distorted time perception in the age of COVID-19. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy 12:S63–S65. doi: 10.1037/tra0000756
Hudda, N., Simon, M. C., Patton, A. P., and Durant, J. L. (2020). Reductions in traffic-related black carbon and ultrafine particle number concentrations in an urban neighborhood during the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci. Total Environ. 742:140931. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.140931
Kause, A., de Bruin, W. B., Millward-Hopkins, J., and Olsson, H. (2019). Public perceptions of how to reduce carbon footprints of consumer food choices. Environ. Res. Lett. 14:114005. doi: 10.1088/1748-9326/ab465d
Keane, M., and Neal, T. (2021). Consumer panic in the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Econ. 220, 86–105. doi: 10.1016/j.jeconom.2020.07.045
Le Quéré, C., Jackson, R. B., Jones, M. W., Smith, A. J. P., Abernethy, S., Andrew, R. M., et al. (2020). Temporary reduction in daily global CO2 emissions during the COVID-19 forced confinement. Nat. Clim. Chang. 10, 647–653. doi: 10.1038/s41558-020-0797-x
Lenzen, M., Li, M., Malik, A., Pomponi, F., Sun, Y.-Y., Wiedmann, T., et al. (2020). Global socio-economic losses and environmental gains from the Coronavirus pandemic. PLoS ONE 15:e0235654. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0235654
Liu, Z., Ciais, P., Deng, Z., Lei, R., Davis, S. J., Feng, S., et al. (2020). Near-real-time monitoring of global CO2 emissions reveals the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic. Nat. Commun. 11, 1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-20254-5
Manenti, R., Mori, E., Di Canio, V., Mercurio, S., Picone, M., Caffi, M., et al. (2020). The good, the bad and the ugly of COVID-19 lockdown effects on wildlife conservation: Insights from the first European locked down country. Biol. Conserv. 249:108728. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2020.108728
Muhammad, S., Long, X., and Salman, M. (2020). COVID-19 pandemic and environmental pollution: A blessing in disguise? Sci. Total Environ. 728:138820. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.138820
Oyserman, D., Fryberg, S. A., and Yoder, N. (2007). Identity-based motivation and health. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 1011–1027. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.1011
Patrício Silva, A. L., Prata, J. C., Walker, T. R., Duarte, A. C., Ouyang, W., Barcelò, D., et al. (2021). Increased plastic pollution due to COVID-19 pandemic: challenges and recommendations. Chem. Eng. J. 405:126683. doi: 10.1016/j.cej.2020.126683
Peel, D. (2021). Beef supply chains and the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States. Anim. Front. 11, 33–38. doi: 10.1093/af/vfaa054
Poppendieck, J. (1986). Breadlines Knee-deep in Wheat: Food Assistance in the Great Depression. California: University of California Press.
Rattner, N. (2020). Coronavirus: What Americans are buying online while in quarantine. CNBC. Available online at: https://www.cnbc.com/2020/04/19/coronavirus-what-americans-are-buying-online-while-in-quarantine.html (accessed December 16, 2020).
Restrepo, B. J., Rabbitt, M. P., and Gregory, C. A. (2021). The effect of unemployment on food spending and adequacy: Evidence from coronavirus-induced firm closures. Appl. Econ. Perspect. Policy 43, 185–204. doi: 10.1002/aepp.13143
Rizan, C., Reed, M., and Bhutta, M. F. (2021). Environmental impact of personal protective equipment distributed for use by health and social care services in England in the first six months of the COVID-19 pandemic. J. R. Soc. Med. 114, 250–263. doi: 10.1177/01410768211001583
Roberts, M., and Downing, P. (2020). Citizen responses to the COVID-19 lockdown—food purchasing, management, and waste. waste and resources action programme. Available online at: https://bfff.co.uk/wp-content/uploads/2020/05/Wrap-Citizen-responses-to-the-Covid-19-lockdown.pdf (accessed December 15, 2020).
Rochard, V. (2020). Silver lining of pandemic shows it's possible to solve climate change. SAP Brandvoice, Forbes.com. Available online at: https://www.forbes.com/sites/sap/2020/04/22/environmental-benefit-covid-19/?sh=~6f090a4428c2 (accessed December 16, 2020).
Rodgers, R. F., Lombardo, C., Cerolini, S., Franko, D. L., Omori, M., Linardon, J., et al. (2021). Waste not and stay at home” evidence of decreased food waste during the COVID-19 pandemic from the U.S. and Italy. Appetite 160:105110. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105110
Sangkham, S. (2020). Face mask and medical waste disposal during the novel COVID-19 pandemic in Asia. Case Stud. Chem. Environ. Eng. 2:100052. doi: 10.1016/j.cscee.2020.100052
Stavrinos, D., McManus, B., Mrug, S., He, H., Gresham, B., Albright, M. G., et al. (2020). Adolescent driving behavior before and during restrictions related to COVID-19. Acc. Anal. Prevent. 144:105686. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2020.105686
Stern, P. C., Janda, K. B., Brown, M. A., Steg, L., Vine, E. L., and Lutzenhiser, L. (2016). Opportunities and insights for reducing fossil fuel consumption by households and organizations. Nat. Energy 1, 1–6. doi: 10.1038/nenergy.2016.43
Teixeira, J., and Lopes, M. (2020). The link between bike sharing and subway use during the COVID-19 pandemic: the case-study of New York's Citi Bike. Transport. Res. Interdisciplin. Perspect. 6:100166. doi: 10.1016/j.trip.2020.100166
Truelove, H. B., and Parks, C. (2012). Perceptions of behaviors that cause and mitigate global warming and intentions to perform these behaviors. J. Environ. Psychol. 32, 246–259. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp,0.2012.04.002
U.S. Census Bureau (2021). Explore Census Data. Available online at: https://www.census.gov/data/tables.html (accessed January 7, 2021).
Walt, S. (2020). The Coronavirus Pandemic's 5 Benefits for the World. ForeignPolicy.com. Available online at: https://foreignpolicy.com/2020/05/26/coronavirus-pandemic-silver-linings-climate-change/ (accessed December 16, 2020).
Wang, Q., Lu, M., Bai, Z., and Wang, K. (2020). Coronavirus pandemic reduced China's CO2 emissions in short-term, while stimulus packages may lead to emissions growth in medium- and long-term. Appl. Energy 278:115735. doi: 10.1016/j.apenergy.2020.115735
Warren, M. S., and Skillman, S. W. (2020). Mobility Changes in Response to COVID-19. arXiv preprint arXiv:2003.14228.
Weersink, A., von Massow, M., Bannon, N., Ifft, J., Maples, J., McEwan, K., et al. (2021). COVID-19 and the agri-food system in the United States and Canada. Agric. Syst. 188:103039. doi: 10.1016/j.agsy.2020.103039
Willett, W., Rockström, J., Loken, B., Springmann, M., Lang, T., Vermeulen, S., et al. (2019). Food in the anthropocene: the EAT-Lancet commission on healthy diets from sustainable food systems. Lancet 393, 447–492. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)31788-4
Keywords: pro-environment behavior, sustainability behavior, COVID-19, eco-friendly behavior, individual behavior
Citation: Mascatelli K, Drummond Otten C, Piacentini RV, Wong-Parodi G and States SL (2021) Comparisons of Sustainability Behaviors Pre- and Early Pandemic Among Botanical Garden Members. Front. Sustain. Cities 3:707380. doi: 10.3389/frsc.2021.707380
Received: 09 May 2021; Accepted: 12 November 2021;
Published: 07 December 2021.
Edited by:
Surendra Sarsaiya, Zunyi Medical University, ChinaReviewed by:
Archana Jain, Zunyi Medical University, ChinaRanjan Singh, Avadh University, India
Puja Ray, Presidency University, India
Ravindra Prasad Aharwal, Raja Bhoj Government College, India
Copyright © 2021 Mascatelli, Drummond Otten, Piacentini, Wong-Parodi and States. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Sarah L. States, c3N0YXRlcyYjeDAwMDQwO3BoaXBwcy5jb25zZXJ2YXRvcnkub3Jn