- Department of Management, School of Business and Law, University of Agder, Kristiansand, Norway
Social identity and social capital are critical to human well-being and sustainable development. However, existing research on sustainable behavior typically treats these factors merely as taken-for-granted preconditions to environmental protection. This paper argues that they can also be direct outcomes of environment-oriented efforts, thus becoming drivers of sustainable societies. As part of a larger research project on household recycling and waste behavior, the authors assess and compare perceived social identity and social capital of residents in a multi-family residential dwelling in Sweden before (N = 66) and after (N = 18) exposure to a social norm-based intervention delivered through a smartphone app. Reported levels of social identity and social capital were higher after (vs. before) the intervention, both compared to participants' own previous measurement and a control group. Together, the current research offers a novel perspective on technology-enabled social norms. Specifically, such norms may not only create sustainable responses immediately, but may also play a pivotal role in shaping more sustainable communities in the long run. Future studies are needed to elucidate the specific psychological mechanisms driving the observed effects and shed further light on whether, when, and why social norms may foster sustainable responses with a clear social connotation.
Introduction
The sustainability literature has long been focused on environmental aspects. More holistic sustainability approaches capturing and including a social dimension of sustainability have only recently been introduced (Beske-Janssen et al., 2015), with frameworks such as the planetary boundaries (Rockström et al., 2009; Steffen et al., 2015) being expanded through the social “doughnut model” (Raworth, 2017) and methods such as life cycle assessments (LCAs) (Heiskanen, 2002) being further developed into social LCAs (Benoît et al., 2010; Guinée et al., 2011). At the same time, sustainability problems such as climate change present a social dilemma (i.e., benefits of action are shared collectively while costs often operate at the individual level) which may only be understood and resolved through closer investigation of social aspects such as human cooperation (Boon-Falleur et al., 2022). Two important constructs with demonstrated downstream effects on a wide range of outcomes linked to collaboration and compliance are social identity and social capital (Lochner et al., 1999; Selman, 2001; Barnes et al., 2020; Van Bavel et al., 2022) as described in further detail below and in the remainder of this article.
The Role of Social Identity and Social Capital in Sustainable Development
Social identity is understood as the degree to which an individual identifies as a member of a given social group, while social capital refers to an individual's network and ties to friends, neighbors, or acquaintances (Tajfel, 1974; Forrest and Kearns, 2001; Uzzell et al., 2002). While related, both constructs are independent: An individual may identify strongly as a FC Barcelona fan1 without knowing many others in that group, while students may have close ties with their classmates but identify only loosely with the overall university's student body. The factors of social identity and social capital are basic human needs (Maslow, 1981) and present widely agreed upon developmental targets (UNGA, 2015), highly correlated with socio-economic welfare (Easterly et al., 2006) and considered crucial for continued growth (OECD, 2011). Communities rich in social capital have been shown to be more resilient in moments of crisis (Gutiérrez et al., 2011), enable long-term sustainable use of collectively-managed natural resources (Woolcock, 2001; Pretty, 2003), and have the potential to avoid the “tragedy of the commons” (Ostrom, 1990; Dietz et al., 2017). On the more negative end, social capital (or rather, its gradual disintegration) has also gained renewed attention in the public discussion as inequalities, populist political views, hate crimes, loneliness among vulnerable groups, and premature deaths – referred to as “deaths of despair” – are on the rise (Case and Deaton, 2020).
Although social capital and social identity are not completely absent in the existing sustainability literature, they are often implied merely as preconditions, as in studies using social normative influence to foster environmentally beneficial behaviors (Allcott, 2011; Loschelder et al., 2019) that assume a certain degree of social identity in the invoked reference group. However, a more prominent, explicit focus on social identity and social capital in sustainability research may also provide relevant insights not just into their roles in supporting environment-oriented interventions, but as direct outcomes of such interventions and thus drivers of sustainable societies in their own right.
Conceptual Framework
Social Norm-Based Interventions and Their Influence on Social Identity and Social Capital
Making social norms salient by, for instance, providing information about how others are behaving or which behaviors others approve or disapprove of, is an effective way to influence behaviors (Cialdini et al., 1990; Schultz, 1999). The influence of the social norm is moderated by who these “others” are (Schultz et al., 2008; Mertens and Schultz, 2021; Otterbring, 2021), meaning that it is paramount to carefully select a reference group that is comparable (Festinger, 1954) and important (Tajfel, 1974) to the influence targets. Studies using social normative influence thus typically select a reference group that shares a social identity with the influence targets (e.g., other FC Barcelona fans) and make the desired behaviors of this reference group salient (e.g., using public transport to get to the stadium). In this sense, the common social identity is used as a means to an end (i.e., to increase the influence of the social norm; cf. Chou and Nordgren, 2017). However, social norm messages do not only make the norm salient – they also make the reference group salient. Although this relationship is understudied in the literature, it appears reasonable that this salience should be associated with an increased identification of the influence targets with the reference group and hence a strengthened social identity.
Connecting this general line of logic with the literature on social capital (e.g., Forrest and Kearns, 2001; Pretty, 2003; Ntontis et al., 2020), it can further be assumed that a strengthened social identity should be positively associated with social capital: people feeling connected to their neighbors through a shared social identity should be more open to making new ties with members of this social group, thus creating a stronger network and increasing social capital. Therefore, a behavioral intervention based on social norms should also increase the social capital within the reference group of the norm.
Study Setting and Hypotheses
The current research examines to what extent social identity and social capital can be outcomes of – rather than just taken-for-granted preconditions for – a sustainability-oriented behavioral intervention. Specifically, the authors report the results of a longitudinal study employing novel ‘smart’ waste disposal infrastructure and a purpose-built smartphone app to investigate whether a digitally administered social norm-based intervention with the primary goal of improving household recycling rates could also increase social capital (measured by the number of neighbors residents know by name) and social identity (measured by a reported sense of place identity and affinity with the neighbors) among residents in a Swedish multi-residential family dwelling. As the first prediction, the social norm-based intervention is expected to increase identification with the invoked reference group and thus strengthen the social identity of the influence targets. Moreover, as a second prediction, this increased social identity should arguably make influence targets more likely to form new connections with members of the invoked social group, thus increasing social capital. Accordingly, the authors hypothesize:
H1: Intervention participants will report a stronger sense of social identity both after (vs. before) the intervention and when compared with control participants.
H2: Intervention participants will report knowing a larger number of members of their social group both after (vs. before) the intervention and when compared with control participants.
Methodology
The study was connected to a larger research project that collected objective waste data on the household level in a multi-family residential setting and investigated the impact of social norm feedback on households' waste and recycling behavior in this context. While the results of that overarching project in terms of changed waste and recycling behaviors are reported elsewhere (Siepelmeyer et al., 2022), the current article investigates the impacts of the social norm-based behavioral intervention on social identity and social capital (H1-H2).
Participants and Descriptive Details
All 153 households living in three apartment buildings forming one building block in the novel “smart-city” (Shahrokni et al., 2015) district “Stockholm Royal Seaport” in Sweden were potential participants in the overarching research project, as they all disposed their waste through “smart” waste chutes equipped with novel technology that objectively quantified waste amounts and recycling levels for each household. These residents were invited to download a smartphone app giving them access to statistics and social comparison feedback about their household's waste and recycling behavior (i.e., a social norm-based intervention). The app was available for Apple and Android users and required an activation code which residents received on a postcard or could request upon opening the app. In addition to the postcards, posters in public areas of the apartment buildings were used to advertise the smartphone app and its functions to all residents and elicit participation. To create realistic conditions comparable to those faced by commercial apps and hence ensure high ecological validity (Carbon, 2020; Gidlöf et al., 2021), residents chose freely whether they wanted to download the app and were not assigned to user or non-user conditions. Out of the 153 households who could have potentially installed the smartphone app and thus be exposed to the social norm intervention, 66 residents (from 59 different households) decided to do so.
Over the course of 12 months, app users were exposed to social comparison feedback in the app (i.e., the social norm intervention), which used neighbors in the same building block as a reference group and provided waste statistics and comparisons within this context. For example, a graph displayed a household's amount of unsorted waste in comparison to the waste amount of the top 10% of neighbors (i.e., the 10% of neighboring households with the lowest amount of unsorted waste) and the average amount of unsorted waste among the neighbors (Siepelmeyer et al., 2022). The smartphone app was also used to distribute electronic surveys to its users. Baseline levels of social identity and social capital were measured with a survey that app users had to fill out upon first opening the app and before being able to use any of its functions (i.e., prior to the intervention), followed by a second measurement through a voluntary follow-up survey distributed 9 months later.
All residents who downloaded the app were automatically enrolled as participants in this study, which resulted in 66 participants during the first wave of data collection, followed by 18 participants who subsequently replied to the follow-up survey. As such, the study used a longitudinal approach.
To rule out a selection bias account, and hence counter the possibility that participants with a certain demographic profile could have been more prone to complete the follow-up survey, the authors tested for demographic differences in age, number of adults in the household, and number of children in the household between intervention participants (who replied to the survey both before and after the intervention; N = 18) and control participants (who solely replied to the pre-intervention survey; N = 48). A lack of demographic differences between intervention and control participants would strengthen the confidence that any possible differences on the focal dependent variables would be due to the intervention rather than any of these potential confounding variables. As expected, three independent samples t-tests found no differences on any of these demographic variables between the groups (all ps ≥ 0.30). Table 1 presents descriptive details across both groups of participants on these variables.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics, including means (and standard deviations) on demographic variables.
Procedure
Participants initially received an electronic survey through the app they had installed for the purpose of the overarching research project and replied to a set of items linked to their neighborhood. Four items were used to measure social identity: participants' stated a) affinity with the neighbors living in the same building; b) affinity with the neighbors living in the same block; c) neighborhood trust; and d) sense of residence identity. The items were averaged to form a composite index of social identity, which was found to be reliable both at the first wave of data collection (α = 0.76) and during the follow-up survey (α = 0.72). Furthermore, to measure social capital in the area, participants indicated how many of their neighbors they knew on a continuous measure without fixed response alternatives. Scale properties were varied for the social identity and social capital measures to reduce the impact of common method bias (Podsakoff et al., 2012; Gasiorowska et al., 2022; Otterbring and Folwarczny, 2022). All items are clearly stated in Table 2 below, including information about scale formats, prior research using similar measures, and – when applicable – reliability estimates in terms of Cronbach's α. Note that reliability estimates cannot be provided for social capital, given that this construct was captured through a single-item measure. Such single-item measures are valid if they represent clear and unambiguous constructs, as in the current case (Bergkvist and Rossiter, 2007).
To boost statistical power, and given the formal predictions (as per H1-H2) combined with the small sample size in the longitudinal analyses (cf. Shavit et al., 2021; Skarin et al., 2021), the authors relied on one-tailed tests, as recommended by several scholars (e.g., Jones, 1954; Cho and Abe, 2013; Lakens et al., 2018; Otterbring et al., 2021).
Results
An independent samples t-test found no significant difference on the social identity index prior to the intervention [t(64) = 0.39, p = 0.70] among participants who did (M = 4.88, SD = 0.99) vs. did not (M = 4.76, SD = 1.17) reply to the second-wave survey. Similarly, there was no significant difference in the number of neighbors known prior to the intervention [t(64) = 1.04, p = 0.30] among participants who did (M = 5.39, SD = 5.44) vs. did not (M = 4.17, SD = 3.72) reply to the follow-up survey. These findings are important as they further indicate, on top of the lack of demographic differences between intervention and control participants (as reported in the section “Participants and Descriptive Details”), that there was no evident selection bias, indicating that both groups of participants were comparable before the intervention took place. Therefore, the authors proceed as follows in the main analyses. First, comparisons are made between participants who replied to the follow-up survey after the intervention with participants who only replied to the initial survey before the intervention, with the latter group of participants serving as a control. Second, these analyses are complemented with longitudinal alternatives among the subset of participants who replied to the survey both before and after the intervention.
Intervention vs. Control Participants
In partial support of H1, an independent samples t-test on the social identity index revealed that participants in the intervention group (M = 5.24, SD = 0.96) reported a marginally stronger sense of social identity linked to their neighborhood compared to participants in the control group [M = 4.76, SD = 1.17; t(64) = 1.55, p = 0.06, η2 = 0.04], although the p-value was slightly above conventional levels of statistical significance but with a moderate or typical effect size according to current conventions in psychological science (e.g., Richard et al., 2003; Gignac and Szodorai, 2016; Funder and Ozer, 2019).
A similar analysis on the number of neighbors known by name (i.e., social capital) showed that participants in the intervention group (M = 9.00, SD = 11.41) reported knowing a significantly larger number of neighbors than those in the control group [M = 4.17, SD = 3.72; t(64) = 2.61, p = 0.005, η2 = 0.10], with a moderate-to-large effect size. In this analysis, however, there was one evident outlier in the intervention group, whose reported number of known neighbors (i.e., 50) was more than 3.29 standard deviations beyond the mean of the intervention group. Excluding this outlier did not change the nature or significance of the results, as intervention participants (M = 6.59, SD = 5.21) still reported knowing a significantly larger number of neighbors than control participants [M = 4.17, SD = 3.72; t(63) = 2.07, p = 0.02, η2 = 0.06]. Because non-parametric tests are less sensitive to extreme values than their parametric counterparts given their rank-based nature, the authors also conducted a Mann-Whitney U test on the entire sample, including the outlier, which again revealed a significant difference between the groups (U = 293.00, Z = 2.04, p = 0.02). Indeed, intervention participants (Mdn = 5) reported knowing a larger number of neighbors than control participants (Mdn = 4). Taken together, this provides converging evidence for H2.
Figure 1 depicts the results on the dependent variables for control participants (Control) and intervention participants, with the means of intervention participants presented across both waves of data collection (Pre-Intervention and Post-Intervention, respectively). To enhance readability, the y-axis has been truncated for the social identity index.
Figure 1. Mean differences on social identity (left) and social capital (right) between control participants and intervention participants pre- and post-intervention.
Intervention Participants Before vs. After Intervention
In further support of H1, a paired-samples t-test on the social identity index found that participants in the intervention group reported a stronger sense of social identity linked to their neighborhood after (M = 5.24, SD = 0.96) rather than before the intervention [M = 4.88, SD = 0.99; t(17) = 1.72, p = 0.05, η2 = 0.15]; see the left-hand side of Figure 1.
Similarly, and in line with H2, participants in the intervention group reported knowing a significantly larger number of neighbors after (M = 9.00, SD = 11.41) as opposed to before the intervention [M = 5.39, SD = 5.44; t(17) = 2.16, p = 0.02, η2 = 0.22]; see the right-hand side of Figure 1. Again, excluding the outlier from this analysis did not change the nature or significance of the results [t(16) = 3.17, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.39], as participants reported knowing a larger number of neighbors after (M = 6.59, SD = 5.21) rather than before (M = 5.39, SD = 5.44) the intervention. Also, the non-parametric version of the paired samples t-test (i.e., the Wilcoxon Signed Rank test), including the outlier, yet again revealed that these findings were robust (Z = 2.94, p = 0.002), with participants reporting knowing a larger number of neighbors after (Mdn = 5) relative to before (Mdn = 4) the intervention. In fact, only 2 of the 18 participants in the intervention group reported knowing fewer neighbors after (vs. before) the intervention, whereas 12 reported knowing more, with the remaining 4 stating familiarity with an equal number of neighbors pre- and post-intervention. As such, H2 was strongly supported.
Discussion
The present longitudinal study applied a novel, explicit focus on social identity and social capital as direct outcomes of, rather than taken-for-granted preconditions for, a social norm-based behavioral intervention. Results demonstrate that an intervention primarily designed to increase environmentally beneficial behaviors also increases social identity and social capital among the influence targets. These findings have two important implications.
First, the results indicate that social norm-based interventions can indeed have a positive influence on social aspects of sustainability, even when the intervention itself is focused on sustainability aspects associated with environmental conservation. The possibility of creating sustainable “win-win outcomes” presents a novel perspective on social norm-based interventions. In particular, the current research suggests that these interventions may play a pivotal role in supporting sustainable development efforts, considering their wide applicability in research and practice.
Second, as increased social identity and social capital may bring about additional benefits, such as stronger behavioral influence of social norms and greater community resilience, social norm-based interventions could ideally be a starting point for an almost self-reinforcing sustainability focus, potentially with far-reaching future-focused benefits. Combined with the opportunity of distributing such interventions through novel technologies (e.g., smartphone apps), this is a relevant insight for the growing stream of transformative service research, as a potential strategy to support and sustain societal and consumer well-being (Otterbring, 2017; Field et al., 2021; Ostrom et al., 2021).
Limitations and Future Research
The current work offers a novel perspective on the role of technology-induced social norms in supporting societal sustainable development, providing several interesting avenues for future research. Admittedly, however, the study is not without limitations. For example, while the longitudinal sample size is comparable with other similar studies (e.g., Skarin et al., 2021; Shavit et al., 2021), the relatively few data points in the second wave of data collection limit the ability to identify small and moderate effect sizes with sufficient statistical power. Unfortunately, the small sample size in the second wave of data collection was impossible to fully circumvent, as participants were free to choose whether to complete the follow-up survey. Still, considering that the surveys were distributed several months apart and that the response rate during the first wave of data collection was not substantially larger than that of the second wave of data collection, the results should reasonably have some predictive validity. Indeed, of the 153 households who could download the app and reply to the initial survey, 59 households (38.56%) decided to do so, corresponding to 66 residents as some households had more than one adult who replied to the initial survey. Subsequently, 18 of those 66 residents (27.27%) also completed the follow-up survey, with these response rates being common in studies relying on similar designs (Kirkwood and Walton, 2014; Siegrist et al., 2015). Nevertheless, future research should include more households to overcome the limitation associated with small sample sizes and preferably conduct a priori power analyses to ascertain that the effect sizes of interest can be detected with sufficient statistical power given the set target sample size (Lakens, 2022). Moreover, as no mediating or moderating variables were documented in the current study, scholars addressing similar topics should optimally try to elucidate the specific psychological mechanisms driving the obtained effects, while simultaneously examining the replicability, generalizability, and boundary conditions of the current results. For example, several studies have shown that certain individual-differences factors may shape individuals' sustainability-related responses (Luchs and Mooradian, 2012; Folwarczny and Otterbring, 2021; Vizcaíno et al., 2021) as well as the extent to which they are influenced by social norms (Cialdini et al., 1999; Perugini and Gallucci, 2001; Stankov, 2011). Thus, future research should preferably include measures related to, for example, people's personality traits, cultural values, and the importance put on environmental protection to shed further light on whether, when, and why social norms may foster sustainable responses with a clear social connotation. As such, while this empirical investigation constitutes the first preliminary step for examining the theorizing proposed herein, more research is needed to gain a deeper understanding of the studied outcomes and build a nomological network of cumulative evidence.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
HS collected and curated the data for analysis and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. TO performed the statistical analysis and wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to conception and design of the study, contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This project was supported by a grant from the Swedish Energy Agency.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Footnotes
1. ^An anonymous reviewer requested us to clarify the meaning of this abbreviation. In the current context, FC refers to a football/soccer club (as in “Futbol Club Barcelona”).
References
Allcott, H. (2011). Social norms and energy conservation. J. Public Econ. 95, 1082–1095. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2011.03.003
Barnes, M. L., Wang, P., Cinner, J. E., Graham, N. A. J., Guerrero, A. M., Jasny, L., et al. (2020). Social determinants of adaptive and transformative responses to climate change. Nat. Clim. Chang. 10, 823–828. doi: 10.1038/s41558-020-0871-4
Benoît, C., Norris, G. A., Valdivia, S., Ciroth, A., Moberg, A., Bos, U., et al. (2010). The guidelines for social life cycle assessment of products: just in time! Int. J. Life Cycle Assessment 15, 156–163. doi: 10.1007/s11367-009-0147-8
Bergkvist, L., and Rossiter, J. R. (2007). The predictive validity of multiple-item versus single-item measures of the same constructs. J. Market. Res. 44, 175–184. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.44.2.175
Beske-Janssen, P., Johnson, M. P., and Schaltegger, S. (2015). 20 years of performance measurement in sustainable supply chain management – what has been achieved? Supply Chain Manage. 20, 664–680. doi: 10.1108/SCM-06-2015-0216
Boon-Falleur, M., Grandin, A., Baumard, N., and Chevallier, C. (2022). Leveraging social cognition to promote effective climate change mitigation. Nat. Clim. Chang. 12, 332–338. doi: 10.1038/s41558-022-01312-w
Carbon, C.-C. (2020). Ecological art experience: how we can gain experimental control while preserving ecologically valid settings and contexts. Front. Psychol. 11, e800. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00800
Case, A., and Deaton, A. (2020). Deaths of Despair and the Future of Capitalism. Princeton: Princeton University Press. doi: 10.1515/9780691217062
Cho, H.-C., and Abe, S. (2013). Is two-tailed testing for directional research hypotheses tests legitimate? J. Bus. Res. 66, 1261–1266. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2012.02.023
Chou, E. Y., and Nordgren, L. F. (2017). Safety in numbers: why the mere physical presence of others affects risk-taking behaviors. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 30, 671–682. doi: 10.1002/bdm.1959
Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., and Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 1015–1026. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015
Cialdini, R. B., Wosinska, W., Barrett, D. W., Butner, J., and Gornik-Durose, M. (1999). Compliance with a request in two cultures: the differential influence of social proof and commitment/consistency on collectivists and individualists. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 25, 1242–1253. doi: 10.1177/0146167299258006
Dietz, T., Ostrom, E., and Stern, P. C. (2017). The struggle to govern the commons. Int. Environ. Governance 302, 53–57. doi: 10.4324/9781315092546-4
Easterly, W., Ritzen, J., and Woolcock, M. (2006). Social Cohesion, institutions, and growth. Econ. Politics 18, 103–120. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0343.2006.00165.x
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Hum. Relat. 7, 117–140. doi: 10.1177/001872675400700202
Field, J. M., Fotheringham, D., Subramony, M., Gustafsson, A., Ostrom, A. L., Lemon, K. N., et al. (2021). Service research priorities: designing sustainable service ecosystems. J. Serv. Res. 24, 462–479. doi: 10.1177/10946705211031302
Folwarczny, M., and Otterbring, T. (2021). Secure and sustainable but not as prominent among the ambivalent: Attachment style and proenvironmental consumption. Pers. Individ. Dif. 183:111154. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111154
Forrest, R., and Kearns, A. (2001). Social cohesion, social capital and the neighbourhood. Urban Studies 38, 2125–2143. doi: 10.1080/00420980120087081
Funder, D. C., and Ozer, D. J. (2019). Evaluating effect size in psychological research: sense and nonsense. Adv. Methods Practices Psychol. Sci. 2, 156–168. doi: 10.1177/2515245919847202
Gasiorowska, A., Folwarczny, M., and Otterbring, T. (2022). Anxious and status signaling: Examining the link between attachment style and status consumption and the mediating role of materialistic values. Pers. Individ. Dif. 190:111503. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2022.111503
Gidlöf, K., Lahm, E. S., Wallin, A., and Otterbring, T. (2021). Eco depletion: The impact of hunger on prosociality by means of environmentally friendly attitudes and behavior. J. Retail. Consumer Serv. 62:102654. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2021.102654
Gignac, G. E., and Szodorai, E. T. (2016). Effect size guidelines for individual differences researchers. Pers. Individ. Dif. 102, 74–78. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.069
Guinée, J. B., Heijungs, R., Huppes, G., Zamagni, A., Masoni, P., Buonamici, R., et al. (2011). Life cycle assessment: past, present, and future. Environ. Sci. Technol. 45, 90–96. doi: 10.1021/es101316v
Gutiérrez, N. L., Hilborn, R., and Defeo, O. (2011). Leadership, social capital and incentives promote successful fisheries. Nature 470, 386–389. doi: 10.1038/nature09689
Heiskanen, E. (2002). The institutional logic of life cycle thinking. J. Clean. Prod. 10, 427–437. doi: 10.1016/S0959-6526(02)00014-8
Jones, L. V. (1954). A rejoinder on one-tailed tests. Psychol. Bull. 51, 585–586. doi: 10.1037/h0055313
Kirkwood, J., and Walton, S. (2014). How green is green? Ecopreneurs balancing environmental concerns and business goals. Austr. J. Environ. Manage. 21, 37–51. doi: 10.1080/14486563.2014.880384
Lakens, D. (2022). Sample size justification. Collabra: Psychol. 8, 1–32. doi: 10.1525/collabra.33267
Lakens, D., Scheel, A. M., and Isager, P. M. (2018). Equivalence testing for psychological research: a tutorial. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 1, 259–269. doi: 10.1177/2515245918770963
Lochner, K., Kawachi, I., and Kennedy, B. P. (1999). Social capital: a guide to its measurement. Health Place 5, 259–270. doi: 10.1016/S1353-8292(99)00016-7
Loschelder, D. D., Siepelmeyer, H., Fischer, D., and Rubel, J. A. (2019). Dynamic norms drive sustainable consumption: Norm-based nudging helps café customers to avoid disposable to-go-cups. J. Econ. Psychol. 75:102146. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2019.02.002
Luchs, M. G., and Mooradian, T. A. (2012). Sex, personality, and sustainable consumer behaviour: elucidating the gender effect. J. Consumer Policy 35, 127–144. doi: 10.1007/s10603-011-9179-0
Mertens, S. N., and Schultz, P. W. (2021). Referent group specificity: Optimizing normative feedback to increase residential recycling. J. Environ. Psychol. 73:101541. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101541
Ntontis, E., Drury, J., Amlôt, R., Rubin, G. J., and Williams, R. (2020). What lies beyond social capital? The role of social psychology in building community resilience to climate change. Traumatology 26, 253–265. doi: 10.1037/trm0000221
Ostrom, A. L., Field, J. M., Fotheringham, D., Subramony, M., Gustafsson, A., Lemon, K. N., et al. (2021). Service research priorities: managing and delivering service in turbulent times. J. Serv. Res. 24, 329–353. doi: 10.1177/10946705211021915
Ostrom, E. (1990). Governing the Commons: The Evolution of Institutions for Collective Action. Cambridge: Cambridge university press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511807763
Otterbring, T. (2017). Smile for a while: the effect of employee-displayed smiling on customer affect and satisfaction. J. Serv. Manage. 28, 284–304. doi: 10.1108/JOSM-11-2015-0372
Otterbring, T. (2021). Peer presence promotes popular choices: A “Spicy” field study on social influence and brand choice. J. Retail. Consumer Serv. 61:102594. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2021.102594
Otterbring, T., and Folwarczny, M. (2022). Firstborns buy better for the greater good: Birth order differences in green consumption values. Personal. Individual Differ. 186:111353. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111353
Otterbring, T., Wu, F., and Kristensson, P. (2021). Too close for comfort? The impact of salesperson-customer proximity on consumers' purchase behavior. Psychol. Market. 38, 1576–1590. doi: 10.1002/mar.21519
Perugini, M., and Gallucci, M. (2001). Individual differences and social norms: the distinction between reciprocators and prosocials. Euro. J. Personal. 15(1_suppl), S19–S35. doi: 10.1002/per.419
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2012). Sources of method bias in social science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 539–569. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100452
Postmes, T., Haslam, S. A., and Jans, L. (2013). A single-item measure of social identification: Reliability, validity, and utility. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 52, 597–617. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12006
Pretty, J. (2003). Social capital and the collective management of resources. Science 302, 1912–1914. doi: 10.1126/science.1090847
Raworth, K. (2017). A Doughnut for the Anthropocene: humanity's compass in the 21st century. Lancet Planetary Health 1, e48–e49. doi: 10.1016/S2542-5196(17)30028-1
Richard, F. D., Bond, C. F., and Stokes-Zoota, J. J. (2003). One hundred years of social psychology quantitatively described. Rev. Gene. Psychol. 7, 331–363. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.7.4.331
Rockström, J., Steffen, W., Noone, K., Persson, Å., Chapin, F. S. I., Lambin, E., et al. (2009). Planetary boundaries: exploring the safe operating space for humanity. Ecol. Soc. 14:art32. doi: 10.5751/ES-03180-140232
Schultz, P. W. (1999). Changing behavior with normative feedback interventions: a field experiment on curbside recycling. Basic Appl. Soc. Psych. 21, 25–36. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp2101_3
Schultz, P. W., Khazian, A. M., and Zaleski, A. C. (2008). Using normative social influence to promote conservation among hotel guests. Soc. Influence 3, 4–23. doi: 10.1080/15534510701755614
Selman, P. (2001). Social capital, sustainability and environmental planning. Plan. Theory Practice 2, 13–30. doi: 10.1080/14649350122850
Shahrokni, H., Årman, L., Lazarevic, D., Nilsson, A., and Brandt, N. (2015). Implementing smart urban metabolism in the Stockholm Royal Seaport: Smart city SRS. J. Indus. Ecol. 19, 917–929. doi: 10.1111/jiec.12308
Shavit, Y., Roth, Y., and Teodorescu, K. (2021). Promoting healthy eating behaviors by incentivizing exploration of healthy alternatives. Front. Nutr. 8, 1–8. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2021.658793
Siegrist, M., Visschers, V. H. M., and Hartmann, C. (2015). Factors influencing changes in sustainability perception of various food behaviors: Results of a longitudinal study. Food Qual. Prefer. 46, 33–39. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.07.006
Siepelmeyer, H., Otterbring, T., and Mäkivierikko, A. (2022). Social Comparison Feedback and Household Waste Behavior: Combining App-Based Interventions, Granular Longitudinal Waste Data and Real-Life Conditions. IAREP Conference.
Skarin, F., Wästlund, E., and Gustafsson, H. (2021). Maintaining or losing intervention-induced health-related behavior change. A mixed methods field study. Front. Psychol. 12, e688192. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.688192
Stankov, L. (2011). Individual, country and societal cluster differences on measures of personality, attitudes, values, and social norms. Learn. Individ. Differ. 21, 55–66. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.09.002
Steffen, W., Richardson, K., Rockström, J., Cornell, S. E., Fetzer, I., Bennett, E. M., et al. (2015). Planetary boundaries: Guiding human development on a changing planet. Science 347:6223. doi: 10.1126/science.1259855
Tajfel, H. (1974). Social identity and intergroup behaviour. Soc. Sci. Inform. 13, 65–93. doi: 10.1177/053901847401300204
UNGA. (2015). Transforming Our World: The 2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development. Available online at: https://sustainabledevelopment.un.org/content/documents/21252030%20Agenda%20for%20Sustainable%20Development%20web.pdf
Uzzell, D., Pol, E., and Badenas, D. (2002). Place identification, social cohesion, and environmental sustainability. Environ. Behav. 34, 26–53. doi: 10.1177/0013916502034001003
Van Bavel, J. J., Cichocka, A., Capraro, V., Sjåstad, H., Nezlek, J. B., Pavlovi,ć, T., et al. (2022). National identity predicts public health support during a global pandemic. Nat. Commun. 13:517. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-27668-9
Vizcaíno, F. V., Cardenas, J. J., and Cardenas, M. (2021). A look at the social entrepreneur: the effects of resilience and power distance personality traits on consumers' perceptions of corporate social sustainability. Int. Entrepreneurship Manage. J. 17, 83–103. doi: 10.1007/s11365-019-00626-0
Keywords: social norms, new technology, sustainability, social identity, social capital, transformative consumer research
Citation: Siepelmeyer H and Otterbring T (2022) Citizen Coherence and Cultivated Cleanliness: Using Technology-Induced Social Norms to Strengthen Sustainable Household Bonds. Front. Sustain. 3:899938. doi: 10.3389/frsus.2022.899938
Received: 19 March 2022; Accepted: 19 April 2022;
Published: 12 May 2022.
Edited by:
Myriam Ertz, Université du Québec à Chicoutimi, CanadaReviewed by:
Urvashi Tandon, Chitkara University, IndiaShouheng Sun, University of Science and Technology Beijing, China
Copyright © 2022 Siepelmeyer and Otterbring. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Henrik Siepelmeyer, aGVucmlrLnNpZXBlbG1leWVyJiN4MDAwNDA7dWlhLm5v