Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Soc. Psychol., 05 January 2024
Sec. Attitudes, Social Justice and Political Psychology

Grand challenge: social psychology without hubris

  • Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, United States

In this editorial, the Founding Field Chief Editor of Frontiers in Social Psychology expresses several ideas about the past, present, and possible future of social psychology, seeking to explain we need social psychology, why we need a new journal in social psychology, and what kind of journal in social psychology we need. The Editor argues for a rich, humanistic, interdisciplinary, philosophically informed social psychology devoted to addressing social problems in the illustrious traditions of John Dewey, Kurt Lewin, Gordon Allport, Muzafer Sherif, Solomon Asch, Morton Deutsch, and others. He suggests that disciplinary “crises” of practicality, historicity, and replicability may be more interconnected than is generally recognized. The Editor advocates a non-hubristic, theory-driven, multi-leveled analysis of human behavior that attends to both subjective and objective aspects of social, cultural, economic, and political contexts. Editorial priorities of the new journal include scientific rigor, social relevance, and intellectual humility.

“We are not called upon to be either boasters or sentimentalists regarding the possibilities of our science… But we are entitled in our daily work to be sustained by the conviction that we are not working in indifference to or at cross purposes with the practical strivings of a common humanity.”

(Dewey, 1917, p. 277)

Introduction

In this editorial, I take the opportunity to convey some of my own ideas about the past, present, and possible future of social psychology, with the ulterior motive of explaining why I think we will always need social psychology, why we need a new journal in social psychology now, and what kind of journal in social psychology it is I think we need. My history of the field is necessarily brief, highly selective, and admittedly impressionistic, if not downright idiosyncratic. I do not claim that my perspective on the first century and a quarter of social psychology is comprehensive or complete, nor do I expect it to be universally accepted. I am simply offering my take on where we have been, where we went wrong, and where I would like to see us go next.

Fortunately, there are many intellectually satisfying histories of social psychology available—especially the ways in which it grew out of nineteenth century philosophy in Europe and North America—written by others more qualified than I (Allport, 1968; Cartwright, 1979; Collier et al., 1991; Farr, 1996; Jones, 1998; Jahoda, 2007; Ross et al., 2010; Dovidio et al., 2012; Hilton, 2012; Kashima and Gelfand, 2012; Kruglanski and Stroebe, 2012; Reis, 2019). There are also valuable autobiographical recollections offered by some of the field's most prolific contributors over the last half-century or more (e.g., Festinger, 1980; Bruner, 1983; Heider, 1983; Deutsch, 1999; McGuire, 1999; Rodrigues and Levine, 1999; Kelman, 2004; Moscovici and Marková, 2006; Aronson, 2010; Kassin, 2022). Scientific and professional developments in Asian, African, and Latin American social psychology have received much less historical attention over the years, but hopefully this is finally changing (Kruglanski and Stroebe, 2012, p. 10–14), for the social psychology we envision is one that knows no geographical boundaries.

In the early twentieth century, the great philosopher, psychologist, and educator Dewey (1917) remarked that social science was roughly three centuries behind physical science (p. 275). Even today, the feeling persists that social psychology is a young field. It is hard to say whether we are catching up—or whether we should even be comparing ourselves to physics in the first place, although one of the founders of modern social psychology, Kurt Lewin, encouraged such comparisons. Some—like Smith (1976), Sherif (1977), Nisbett (1990), and Rozin (2001)—have argued that we should cease defining ourselves in relation to the natural and physical sciences.

Sherif (1977), for instance, remarked that too often we behave like “dazzled copycats:” “The physical sciences are psychology's rich relatives… We strive to be like them as they are now—mature and established,” but we are ignorant of the “tribulations that [they] went through at the hands of the Establishment of their times” (p. 376). Asch (1987) lamented that “Because physicists cannot speak with stars or electronic currents, psychologists have often been hesitant to speak to their human participants” (p. xv). Smith (1976), too, argued it is a mistake to be guided exclusively “by aspirations and strategies borrowed from the inanimate sciences” (p. 442). My own view is that physics envy is but one manifestation of a scientistic outlook shared by far too many social psychologists.1

We should recognize, for instance, that not every important issue is an empirical one, and we should do a better job of distinguishing between empirical and non-empirical questions (e.g., Wallach and Wallach, 1994, 1998a,b, 2001a,b; Gigerenzer, 1998). Moreover, we need not distance ourselves from the humanities and other social sciences; we would do well to embrace what we have in common with other areas of scholarly inquiry, including those that are not amenable to the experimental method. Smith (1976) would point out that we do have something important to add: “With the humanistic scholar and the artist, the social psychologist might also hope to … participate in societal feedback processes that make it less likely that future realities will be humanly disastrous, more likely that they will be humanly fulfilling” (p. 442; see also McGuire, 1973).

Most would agree that social psychology, despite its promising nature, has endured a fairly turbulent childhood and adolescence. Its practitioners have endured a string of crises and have suffered, in my view, for its most hubristic excesses. Arguably these include the drawing of universalistic, sometimes grandiose conclusions about “human nature” based on underdeveloped, unsystematic theorizing; a fetish for flashy, surprising, or newsworthy effects; reliance upon statistically underpowered studies involving a culturally narrow, parochial data base; flawed research designs, poor measurement techniques, and unacknowledged threats to internal, external, and construct validity; and the opportunistic manipulation of dubious statistical procedures to draw desired conclusions2 (inter alia, Henrich et al., 2010; Fiedler, 2011; Simmons et al., 2011; Nosek et al., 2012; Open Science Collaboration, 2015; Spellman, 2015; Bless and Burger, 2016; Finkel et al., 2017; Gray, 2017; Giner-Sorolla, 2019; Muthukrishna and Henrich, 2019; Blake and Gangestad, 2020; Fabrigar et al., 2020; Berkman and Wilson, 2021; Gelman and Vazire, 2021; Grahek et al., 2021). Still, hope is not lost. Many adults succeed in overcoming adversity experienced early in life (Masten and Reed, 2002; Cutuli et al., 2020).

Pettigrew (2018), for one, believes that the field has improved greatly by responding to so-called “crises.” And, as Jones (1998) noted years ago, “The future of social psychology is assured” because of “the vital importance of its subject matter” and “its unique conceptual and methodological strengths,” which, under the best of circumstances, “permit the identification of underlying processes in everyday social life” (p. 49). We need social psychology.

In what follows, I first highlight what I see as some of the most valuable themes and enduring challenges of social psychology—its legacy and the things we should seek to preserve and tackle going forward. I find it extremely useful to return to the roots of social psychology—especially the wisdom and leadership of John Dewey, Kurt Lewin, Gordon Allport, Muzafer Sherif, Solomon Asch, Morton Deutsch, and others—because I often feel that we have strayed too far from their visions of a rich, humanistic, interdisciplinary, philosophically informed social psychology that is devoted to diagnosing and ameliorating serious social problems. It is a troubling sign of scientistic hubris that we ignore our past—or treat it sentimentally but not seriously—and assume that we have little or nothing to learn from those who made their contributions in the last century (Billig, 2015; Colucci and Colombo, 2018).

After revisiting the early history of social psychology, I make a few brief comments about the so-called “crises” of social psychology—including the “practicality crisis,” the “historicism crisis,” and the “replication crisis”—and suggest that they may be more connected to one another than is commonly assumed. The fact that so few social psychologists appear to have noticed any connections is another sign that we are ignoring—rather than learning from—our shared history. I humbly submit that there are some extremely important lessons we could learn from the past and potential gains to realize by embracing a more realistic understanding of our particular brand of social science. Finally, I outline ambitious plans to launch a new journal in an unsettled landscape marked by ongoing controversies and conflicts in social psychology and a rapidly changing market for open-access academic publishing.

The need for social psychology

In the English-speaking world, we often assume that social psychology began in 1908 with the publication of two major textbooks—one by an American sociologist, E. A. Ross, and the other by a British psychologist, William McDougall. The fact is that both were beaten to the punch by a few continental Europeans, including an Italian author, Paola Orano, who published Psicologia Sociale in 1902 (Jahoda, 2007; Kruglanski and Stroebe, 2012). It is convenient for us now to forget all about Orano, who supported the fascist regime in Italy and was a close collaborator of Mussolini (Doise, 1986; Sensales, 2020). But perhaps it behooves us to bear in mind that social psychology—like virtually all other scientific and technological innovations—can be used for good or evil, and it is a constant part of our ethical mission to distinguish clearly between the two.

In celebrating the first 25 years of the American Psychological Association, Dewey (1917) paid appropriate tribute to pioneers such as William James, Jean-Gabriel De Tarde, James Mark Baldwin, E.A. Ross, William McDougall, Edward Thorndike, Graham Wallas, and Wilhelm Wundt. But he also urged humility: “No science has so much cause to be humble about its actual achievements as has social science, including social psychology” (p. 275). He asked rhetorically: “How often have we been invited to build up our social, political, and ethical explanations in terms of some single and supposedly dominant mental constituent!” (p. 268).

Dewey's (1917) article, entitled “The Need for Social Psychology,” was full of complaints about the status quo. Among other things, he lamented the “backwardness of social psychology” (p. 271), the fact that it often served as “a bulwark of conservatism”—a way of looking behind rather than ahead. For example, he bristled at psychological approaches that relied too heavily on innate tendencies in human behavior, to the exclusion of socialization processes. Dewey worried that social scientists were not only getting things wrong but justifying the existing social system:

The most powerful apologetics for any arrangement or institution is the conception that it is an inevitable result of fixed conditions of human nature. Consequently... an antecedently given mind [is] appealed to in justification of the established order as to the family, the school, the government, industry, commerce and every other institution (p. 273).

Dewey objected to “attitudes of polite aloofness [and] condescending justification as to social institutions” (p. 274), which he felt were all-too common among psychologists of his generation. He was a restless progressive, an educational innovator, and a champion of liberal democracy. The social psychology he “needed” was one that would pave the way for a better, freer, and more just society (Colucci and Colombo, 2018).

More than a century ago, Dewey (1917) argued for a “new point of view” that treated the “social mind” as “an offspring of the life of association, intercourse, transmission, and accumulation” and “social facts as the material of an experimental science, where the problem is that of modifying belief and desire … by enacting specific changes in the social environment” (p. 273–274). He envisioned a science that would demonstrate clearly how objective social forces shape the mental states of individuals and groups and, consequently, their behavior. Because of his unwavering commitment to democratic progress, Dewey saw personal and social change—and, indeed, human agency—as fundamental to the emerging science of social psychology, a dynamic field that would “show man how his mind is to take part in giving these changes one direction rather than another” (Dewey, 1917, p. 274). There was an ethical dimension to Dewey's vision of social psychology that is woefully lacking in contemporary contributions. Authors these days often make ostentatious, albeit unconvincing displays of “value neutrality,” presumably in an effort—largely in vain, as it turns out—to avoid pernicious accusations of “ideological bias” that are themselves ideologically motivated in obvious ways (e.g., Duarte et al., 2015).

The social psychology Dewey envisioned was ambitious but not hubristic. Befitting a “pragmatist” or an “instrumentalist” (his preferred term), Dewey felt that the scientific method was supremely useful but fallible. In Human Nature and Conduct, he noted that the scientific revolution “began with recognition that every natural object... is to be known only by experimental inquiries which will exhibit a multitude of complicated, obscure and minute relationships.” He went on: “The case is not otherwise with ideals of justice or peace or human brotherhood, or equality, or order. Like thunderbolts and tubercular disease and the rainbow they can be known only by extensive and minute observation of consequences incurred in action” (Dewey, 1930, p. 56–57). The fact that science is the most reliable form of epistemic inquiry available to us does not mean that it is free from human limitations. On the contrary, we should always treat the fruits of our investigation—in natural science no less than social science—as provisional and subject to revision (McGuire, 2013).

More than anyone else in the early twentieth century, it was Kurt Lewin who most definitively answered Dewey's call for a dynamic social psychology emphasizing freedom, social justice, and democratic self-determination. Both sought to identify ideal but attainable conditions for human agency and progressive change in mental life and social affairs. In the preface to a posthumous collection of Lewin's essays, Gordon Allport (Allport, 1948) wrote:

There is a striking kinship between the work of Kurt Lewin and the work of John Dewey. Both agree that democracy must be learned anew in each generation, and that is a far more difficult form of social structure to attain and to maintain than is autocracy. Both see the intimate dependence of democracy upon social science. Without knowledge of, and obedience to, the laws of human nature in group settings, democracy cannot succeed. And without freedom for research and theory as provided only in a democratic environment social science will surely fail. (p. xi)

Dewey and Lewin shared many things, including a philosophical education, a commitment to the experimental method, and an interdisciplinary outlook. They valued “action research,” that is, problem-oriented collaborations between researchers and practitioners with the aim of producing “context-bound, values-based knowledge and solutions from their public inquiries into system problems” (Dickens and Watkins, 1999, p. 186). According to Colucci and Colombo (2018), Dewey and Lewin also shared the normative goal of “emancipatory social relevance,” that is, the pursuit of knowledge that would serve not to control others but to help “individuals to obtain self-enlightenment about their societal and social dependencies” (Teo, 1998, p. 240; see also Tolman, 2009). It should go without saying that pursuing emancipatory social relevance does not mean one is entitled to distort the truth or prioritize one's values over the facts; on the contrary, we assume that truth itself serves an emancipatory function and is essential to the unmasking of bogus ideologies.

Part of Lewin's extraordinary success as the founder of modern social psychology is attributable to his own personality and leadership style. His biographer, Marrow (1969, p. ix–xv), wrote that Lewin “lived psychology—not only for his love of science but also for his devotion to mankind” and believed that “his life as a scientist must be integrated with his life as a citizen:” “Unlike the typical German professor, he was a natural, spontaneous democrat” (p. xi). Lewin's legacy of training and pedagogy is unparalleled. Many of his former students became illustrious figures who spread the message of social psychology throughout Europe and the US for decades. At the same time, contemporary appreciations of Dewey and Lewin tend toward the superficial (Billig, 2015; Colucci and Colombo, 2018), and social psychologists today would do well to ground their research much more firmly in their tradition, which was “optimistic and constructive,” focusing on the complex “relationship between theory and practice” with a clear emphasis on “the issue of social change” and the goal of “improving society by spreading democracy and ‘resolving' or at least addressing social conflict” (Colucci and Colombo, 2018, p. 32).

Two (or more) crises in social psychology

Opinions differ greatly about the nature and substance of the first so-called “crisis” in social psychology (e.g., McGuire, 1973; Elms, 1975; Secord, 1976; Sherif, 1977; Minton, 1984; Blank, 1988; Jackson, 1988; Greenwood, 1989; Parker, 1989; Wallach and Wallach, 1994; Jost and Kruglanski, 2002; Pettigrew, 2018). Some point to Ring's (1967) critique of his contemporaries for abandoning Lewin's humanistic, action-oriented vision of a scientific discipline that would “advance the cause of human welfare” (p. 113) in favor of “clever experimentation on exotic topics with a zany manipulation” (p. 117). Only 30 years after Lewin's death, Ring felt that social psychology had already become frivolous: “Whoever can conduct the most contrived, flamboyant, and mirth-producing experiments receives the highest score on the kudometer” (p. 117). The worry that social psychologists have abandoned their charter to use scientific methods to address serious social problems has recurred throughout the years (Helmreich, 1975; Smith, 1976; Minton, 1984; Giner-Sorolla, 2019; Berkman and Wilson, 2021; Moghaddam, 2023; Power et al., 2023).

Dissatisfaction with the field emerged also in Europe, where prominent voices such as Moscovici (1972) and Tajfel (1972) decried the alleged individualism of North American social psychology (as did Steiner, 1974) and the neglect of social-contextual influences on human behavior. Some of these concerns were amplified by Gergen (1973), who claimed that:

The continued attempt to build general laws of social behavior seems misdirected, and the associated belief that knowledge of social interaction can be accumulated in a manner similar to the natural sciences appears unjustified. In essence, the study of social psychology is primarily an historical undertaking. We are essentially engaged in a systematic account of contemporary affairs. We utilize scientific methodology, but the results are not scientific principles in the traditional sense (p. 316–317).

Ultimately, Gergen's (1991) “social constructionism” took a radical, relativistic turn. He embraced the skeptical conclusion that scientific statements “are simply optional myths on all fours with religious or political dogmas and ideologies” (Smith, 1994, p. 408).

Unsurprisingly, most social psychologists rejected the above critiques, especially that of Gergen (1973, 1991), as far too sweeping, even nihilistic. In the eyes of many, it was a postmodernist call to dispose of the baby with the bathwater (Schlenker, 1974; Deutsch, 1976; Greenwald, 1976; Manis, 1976; Shaw and Costanzo, 1982; Zajonc, 1989; Jones, 1998). Even M. Brewster Smith, who had welcomed Gergen's earlier critique (Smith, 1976), objected to the abandonment of a reasonably modest conception of social psychological science “as an evidential, public, self-critical social enterprise, an enterprise that has successfully sought progressively more adequate and comprehensive understanding of the phenomena in its domain—an enterprise committed to an ideal of truth, the approach to which can be evaluated pragmatically” (Smith, 1994, p. 408).

The sheer extremity of the social constructionist critique was, perhaps, unfortunate, insofar as some specific points were well-taken (Cronbach, 1975; Deutsch, 1976; Secord, 1976; Smith, 1976; Sherif, 1977; Converse, 1986; Blank, 1988; Greenwood, 1989; Campbell and Russo, 1999; Jost and Kruglanski, 2002; Sullivan, 2020; Power et al., 2023). Chief among these was the conclusion that human social behavior is linked to concrete historical, social, cultural, political, and economic contexts (inter alia, Moscovici, 1972; Tajfel, 1972; Geertz, 1973; Hardin and Higgins, 1996; Rozin, 2001; Nisbett and Masuda, 2003; Markus, 2005; Roberts, 2007; Kay and Zanna, 2009; Kashima and Gelfand, 2012; Bar-Tal, 2013; Kay and Eibach, 2013; Greenfield, 2017; Salter et al., 2018; Muthukrishna and Henrich, 2019; Jost, 2020; Napier et al., 2020; Banaji et al., 2021; Essien et al., 2021; Pettigrew, 2021; Cikara et al., 2022; Goudarzi et al., 2022; Najdowski and Goff, 2022; Rivera Pichardo et al., 2022; Liaquat et al., 2023; Kitayama and Salvador, 2024). Indeed, the same argument was made convincingly by Dewey (1917) and is perfectly compatible with Lewin's (1947) dynamic field theory (e.g., Minton, 1984). To my mind, recognizing the importance of social-contextual factors does not mean that social psychologists should cease striving for generalizability as a scientific goal. On the contrary, we should seek to develop a comprehensive, systematic understanding of social situations, fields, environments, episodes, or contexts and to identify the most important underlying dimensions—such as different types of social structures, roles, resources, and stratification systems—to explain variability in social contexts (e.g., Milgram, 1965; Frederiksen, 1972; Forgas, 1979; Argyle et al., 1981; Magnusson, 1981; Saucier et al., 2007; Reis, 2008; Yang et al., 2009; Foa and Foa, 2012; Uskul and Oishi, 2020).

Perhaps Gergen's (1973) biggest mistake was assuming that science itself is incapable of modeling dynamic processes such as those characterized by temporal or historical change. Many social psychologists objected that his ahistorical view of the natural sciences was a caricature. Manis (1976) noted that we would not “ask a meteorologist to predict tomorrow's weather from a general forecasting system, while denying him vital information concerning air pressure, winds, neighboring weather patterns, etc., that would presumably serve as important variables in the application of his model” (p. 429). Smith (1976) added: “There are natural sciences that are explicitly historical: thus paleontology and historical geology” (p. 440). Converse (1986) wrote: “if being ‘firmly wedded to historical circumstance' is sufficient to disqualify the investigation as ‘science,' then we must hastily, if perhaps apologetically, inform geology it is not, and has never been, a science” (p. 45). Moreover, “the study of biology is every bit as ‘firmly wedded to historical circumstance' as is geology or planetary science” (p. 51). Converse pointed out that Lewin's “situation” term is, in fact, vastly more complex than is often recognized, encompassing as it does, “social structure, political and economic institutions, cultural values, and historical sequencing” (p. 58; see also Minton, 1984). Because of this complexity, Converse conjectured, social psychology might take 500 years “to match the accomplishment of the first 50 years of physics” (p. 48).

With regard to the “practicality crisis” (Ring, 1967; Helmreich, 1975; see also Elms, 1975; Smith, 1976; Minton, 1984), Deutsch (1976), one of Lewin's protégés, counseled modesty and patience about what social psychology can accomplish:

Social psychology, per se, does not and cannot provide the solutions to war, poverty, racism, sexism, crime, or any social problem. This is not to say that social psychology is unable to make important contributions to the understanding of these problems. But it is to say that the gap between what we can do, even at our best, and the elimination of these problems is very large. When these problems take on an urgent character, the gap is experienced as an acute crisis but the gap is chronic—it is an inevitable frustration of being a socially concerned social psychologist. We have our role to play in helping to bring about progressive social change but it is only a small role in a complex drama with many actors. Recognition of the inherent limits of our part would not reduce the gap but it might alleviate some of our frustration. (p. 135)

Deutsch's position should not be confused with apathy or, as Lewin put it, “highbrow aversion or... a fear of social problems” (Lewin, 1947, p. 169). Rather, like Dewey (1917), Deutsch took the long view in advocating for an action-oriented, humanistic, non-hubristic social psychology, recognizing that many of the problems we seek to address are multiply determined and resistant to social change for reasons that are beyond our control.

Social psychology's second major crisis period, which arose roughly 40 years after the first, is often referred to as the “replication crisis.” Ostensibly, it had nothing to do with the earlier crisis (or crises). The presenting problem was that attempts to reproduce the results of large numbers of social psychology studies in the published literature “failed” roughly half of the time, stoking anxiety that the field as a whole suffered from “selective reporting, selective analysis, and insufficient specification of the conditions necessary or sufficient to obtain the results” (Open Science Collaboration, 2015, p. 943; see also Spellman, 2015). While many scholars blamed statistical improprieties (e.g., Simmons et al., 2011), others pointed to deficiencies in theory, that is, the “lack of a cumulative theoretical framework or frameworks” (Muthukrishna and Henrich, 2019, p. 221; see also Gintis, 2007). No doubt both contributed to the problem, and there are probably other causes as well, including widespread naïveté about philosophy of science (e.g., see Cacioppo et al., 2004, for a useful primer with implications for social psychology).

McGuire (2013), who lived through social psychology's first crisis, anticipated at least some aspects of the second. He took aim at psychologists' uncritical acceptance of logical empiricism, as exemplified by Karl Popper, arguing that “one is led to absurdity in thinking of the empirical work as testing one's hypotheses rather than developing them by making clear to ourselves and others the implicit limitations of austerely stated laws” (p. 420). From the perspective of McGuire's (1989) perspectivist meta-theory, the goal of research in social psychology is not merely to reject “false positives” nor to provide “confirmatory” evidence that enables one's pet hypotheses to live and fight another day, but to identify the boundary conditions under which a given theoretical proposition may—and may not—be treated as a useful, valid generalization. Among other things, this approach requires a deep appreciation of the context-embeddedness of social psychological phenomena.

By chasing flashy results, ignoring earlier critiques about the context-dependence of social psychological observations, and stubbornly pushing universalistic theories of human nature assuming “a genetic line possessed by all species”—all the while ignoring “a cumulative cultural line unique to us” (Muthukrishna and Henrich, 2019, p. 223) and failing to develop sophisticated, dynamic, integrative theories that incorporate “social structure, political and economic institutions, cultural values, and historical sequencing” (Converse, 1986, p. 58)—social psychologists may have been setting themselves up for a fall. We now know that studies on topics that are understood to be “contextually sensitive (varying in time, culture, or location)” (Van Bavel et al., 2016, p. 6,454)—such as specific cultural stereotypes—are less easily replicated than studies on other topics (Klein et al., 2012; Gilbert et al., 2016; Greenfield, 2017; Noah et al., 2018; Pettigrew, 2018). This suggests, at least to me, that there is a connection between the first crisis in social psychology and the second.

To take an obvious example, one could be deeply dismayed—on short-sighted, scientistic grounds—that this month's Gallup Poll “failed to replicate” President Biden's approval ratings from only 1 month ago. However, this would be silly, insofar as everyone assumes that public opinion about the president is the kind of thing that is likely to change from 1 month to the next. So, the question I have is, why do (or did) so many social psychologists assume that most or all of the phenomena that interest them are static rather than dynamic—the kinds of things that should not be expected to change over time (or from one place to the next)? Historical permanence was not assumed by Dewey, Lewin, Allport, Sherif, or other founders of social psychology. Nor was cultural universality. How and why, then, did contemporary social psychologists drift so far from their roots?

My own impression, as someone who was trained in the U.S. in the early 1990's, is that for many years social psychologists were encouraged to speak and write about their work in hubristic terms. It is conceivable that, as the neoliberal age unfolded, professional and self-promotional (i.e., marketing) considerations of a personal and/or collective (i.e., disciplinary) nature came to eclipse attention to and awareness of philosophical and scientific realities. This problem is by no means confined to social psychology. I suspect it is a prevalent consequence of university incentive structures (e.g., Nosek et al., 2012), which focus increasingly on individual achievements and the rewarding of “academic stars.” In social psychology, leaders of the field—presumably under pressure from employers as well as governmental and non-governmental funding sources—may have responded defensively rather than constructively to the first “crisis,” fighting off accusations that the discipline was “unscientific” by overcompensating and redoubling efforts to conform to an unrealistic image of the natural sciences. This, in turn, may have led many otherwise reasonable scholars to make exaggerated claims about the generality and universality (e.g., timelessness) of their “effects.”3

In this way, it seems to me that social psychologists were taught to be scientistic rather than scientific. For decades, “basic” research on (presumably) domain-general processes was at all times favored over “applied” research that might address some specific social problem (Helmreich, 1975). Simple, declarative sentences about general, if not universal or absolute, statements of psychological fact were (and often still are) selected as titles for articles and conference presentations. These hubristic titles practically dare other researchers to contradict them, initiating a scramble to discover minor exceptions that add little in the way of theory-building. The discussion sections of articles published in flagship journals invariably claimed that some fundamental cognitive or motivational process had been successfully isolated in the laboratory. Results were seldom, if ever, seen as influenced by decisions researchers made about how to operationalize or measure their variables—or the fact that only a few carefully chosen stimuli had been selected to represent a wide array of human experiences (e.g., Yarkoni, 2022). If there was another theory that could (also) explain the findings, the article was apt to be rejected, and the authors knew it. Thus, they were obliged to argue that no rival theory could possibly explain the totality of their observations. If they wanted to be tenured or promoted, many social psychologists probably felt they had little choice but to exaggerate the importance, universality, and generalizability of their work.

If those days of hubris are finally over, and—because past failures can no longer be denied or dismissed—intellectual humility and scientific modesty are finally “in” (Teo, 2019; Hoekstra and Vazire, 2021; Power et al., 2023), then I, for one, am glad. However, I worry that by placing all of the blame for the “replication crisis” on statistical problems—and ignoring the serious conceptual, theoretical, and meta-theoretical issues—the next generation of social psychologists may be setting themselves up for another fall. I agree that it is a terrific idea for researchers to think through all aspects of their research program from the outset (if possible). However, the idea that “preregistration” of one's hypotheses is a panacea against the scourge of false positives strikes me as hubristic.4 So is the notion that a conclusion is more trustworthy, in a scientific sense, because someone happened to put their educated guesses up on a website. I am especially skeptical that publicly proclaiming a few (relatively safe) predictions and interpreting all results only in light of what was anticipated is, in itself, likely to produce any real scientific breakthroughs. I also see other problems with what is confidently declared to be “best practices” in psychological science these days. Frequently one is invited to review a manuscript in which 40% or 50% or 60% of the participants have been dropped because the authors adhered rigidly to an unfortunate set of exclusion criteria that seemed reasonable to them some weeks or months before. Robustness tests make more sense to use than turning science into a betting contest.

To be clear, I think it is wonderful that our research community is constantly trying to make empirical observations more informative and trustworthy. And I agree with Pettigrew (2018) that many methodological innovations, such as multilevel modeling, are strengthening our ability to illuminate sources of social-contextual variability in human behavior, thereby linking micro, meso, and macro levels of analysis in social psychology. At the same time, we need to recognize and accept what my statistics professor, Abelson (1995) knew, namely that scientific work is itself a rhetorical endeavor—an attempt to persuade people of something they do not already know based on logic and evidence.

Theory is paramount, and there is a huge role for creativity in the development and empirical assessment of social psychological argumentation (Nisbett, 1990; McGuire, 1999). We ought to give researchers credit for generating new and interesting ideas—whether they bear out empirically or not, encouraging them to take risks and be creative, rather than requiring authors to commit themselves to (and “defend”) any specific empirical outcome. Let us acknowledge explicitly that theory development is and should be a dynamic process rather than a static one, and revising theories in light of accumulating evidence is not something to be hidden or ashamed of; it is a valuable part of the scientific process (e.g., Trope, 2004).

Any given social psychological study—because of choices made by human beings about which variables to emphasize and how to operationalize them, how to implement experimental manipulations and procedures, how to collect, code, and analyze the data, and so on—is either rhetorically convincing to experts in the field or not (Abelson, 1995). That is, every decision made about study design, measurement, data analysis, and interpretation is one that should be justified on grounds that are sensible and compelling to other researchers. In this endeavor, theory is critically important (Becker and McClintock, 1972; Shaw and Costanzo, 1982; Kruglanski, 2001, 2004; Trope, 2004; Van Lange et al., 2012; Gray, 2017), because all properly scientific disputes are about whether various patterns of data strengthen or weaken belief in some proposition or set of related propositions about the state of the world. The answers we arrive at in science are nothing like the absurd one imagined in The Hitchhikers' Guide to the Galaxy, when a supercomputer states that the answer to a question about the meaning of life is “42.” The answers we seek are not just numbers; they are arguments that satisfy our curiosity, at least for now.

New frontiers in social psychology

How, then, can a new journal help us overcome and learn from past mistakes and move forward in a determined, more successful fashion on all these fronts? Certainly, no single journal will be able to “fix” the various deficiencies in the theory, meta-theory, and practice of social psychology that led to the field's major crises. Nor can it cure the historical amnesia that, in my view, inhibits us from learning (enough) about the past. However, my editorial team and I are offering an alternative platform that researchers can use to counter broader professional trends toward scientific hubris and help to reestablish a shared sense of social psychology's historical mission.

We sincerely hope that you will enjoy reading Frontiers in Social Psychology. It is a brand-new open access journal that aims to establish itself as one of the leading publication outlets of conceptual, theoretical, methodological, and empirical work in social psychology. Please note that we value qualitative as well as quantitative methods, and we seek to promote innovative methodological techniques that advance our understanding of the human situation. We prize theoretical development and sophisticated, properly nuanced, and socially contextualized efforts at hypothesis confrontation (not merely confirmation). Save the bluster, just tell us what you think you have or have not found and why, and why you think our readers should care.

We will publish a wide range of article types, including brief research reports and more lengthy research articles, hypothesis and theory statements, opinion and perspective pieces, general commentaries, methodological contributions and data reports, mini reviews and more systematic review articles, case reports and clinical trials, policy briefs and extended discussions (see https://www.frontiersin.org/journals/social-psychology/for-authors/article-types). This tremendous diversity of “deliverables” parallels our pluralistic (but not relativistic) view of academic scholarship. As should be clear from this editorial, we do not assume that “one size fits all” or that a bright line separates facts from values, data from interpretation, or truth from deeply considered opinion.

In my work as Field Chief Editor, I am supported by an esteemed, generous, democratic, insightful, and non-hubristic group of Specialty Chief Editors. We are launching this journal in the fall of 2023 with four Specialty Areas: (1) Attitudes, Social Justice, and Political Psychology, led by Richard Eibach of the University of Waterloo in Canada; (2) Intergroup Relations and Group Processes, led by Kimberly Rios of the University of Illinois in Urbana-Champaign; (3) Gender, Sexuality, and Relationships, led by Rachel Calogero of the University of Western Ontario, Canada, and Maria Giuseppina Pacilli of the University of Perugia in Italy; and (4) Computational Social Psychology, led by Michal Kosinski of Stanford University. We worked hard on recruiting an internationally recognized and ethnolinguistically, geographically, culturally, and methodologically diverse board of Associate and Review Editors, people who are also generous and accomplished. Our work, please remember, is an act of community service.

Unfortunately, we have learned that many potential authors are skeptical of the Frontiers model of scientific publishing. We share many of your concerns and are seeking to learn from past problems with other journals, including runaway AI. For better or worse, the financial burden of academic publishing is in the process of being shifted away from (a) university libraries and other institutions that (for decades) paid handsomely for journal subscriptions to (b) an open-access model in which fees are paid instead by authors and those who support their work, including granting agencies and, again, universities as employers of those who produce the vast majority of social scientific output.

One perfectly understandable concern is that scholars in developing countries will be unable to afford to pay publication fees. We hasten to mention that the publisher has institutional agreements with over 250 institutions and a Fee Support Program for researchers who lack access to research funding. Fortunately, our impression is that other Frontiers journals have featured more articles from authors working in East Asia and the Global South than we are accustomed to seeing in the pages of “traditional” or “mainstream” journals in social and personality psychology, which have been strongly influenced by reputational familiarity and other institutional prestige cues.

In a relatively short time, Frontiers journals have risen aggressively and impressively through the ranks of scientific publishing. On average, their journals rank at the 72nd percentile (out of 99) in terms of Impact Factors, and some are as high as the 91st percentile. In terms of scientific publishers, they currently rank as #3 in the world (averaging five citations per article)—behind the American Chemical Society and the Royal Society of Chemistry, but ahead of Elsevier, PLoS, Springer Nature, Wiley, Oxford, Cambridge, Sage, Taylor & Francis, De Gruyter and other publishers. In 2022 alone, Frontiers articles were viewed and downloaded 646 million times all over the world.

In any case, all of us—authors, editors, reviewers, and readers—find ourselves struggling to adapt to this brave new world of academic publishing, which brings new levels of transparency and accountability. Peer reviews, which were once guarded secretly, are now made public, at least for articles accepted for publication. Although we cannot promise perfection, my editorial team and I will do our best to build a new, open-access repository of social psychological contributions of which the entire research community can be proud for years to come.

Concluding remarks

The need for social psychology, it seems to me, is universal, even if the specific topics, problems, methods, and solutions we favor are not. For better or worse, we cannot stand outside of history—or culture or politics or economics. But we can make truly valuable contributions to understanding how these forces shape and are shaped by individuals and social groups in our own times and places, and perhaps—especially if we are willing and able to collaborate with fellow travelers in other disciplines—understanding the structures, functions, and dynamics of social and psychological systems more generally. It is still within the purview of our science to understand how, as Sherif (1977) put it, “the human psychological system [is] transformed during development and concrete interchanges with the social environment, as it shapes up into a human individual in a given culture with its particular living conditions and social arrangements of class, gender, occupation, etc. with their associated value orientations” (p. 376–377). According to no less an authority than Pettigrew (2018), researchers are closer than ever to fulfilling the promise of “contextual social psychology” by quantitatively modeling both subjective and objective aspects of the social context and documenting their effects on the thoughts, feelings, and actions of individuals and groups.

Like John Dewey, Kurt Lewin, Gordon Allport, Muzafer Sherif, Solomon Asch, Morton Deutsch, and many others they inspired—we keep the faith that a dynamic, context-sensitive social psychology can make a genuine difference in people's lives and point the way toward a better and more just future for humanity. At Frontiers in Social Psychology, we would like to see studies that are theory-driven and methodologically sound, communicated in ways that are accessible, constructive, reasonable, and persuasive. Many of the articles will also be problem-focused, that is, potentially valuable to society. Whether our activity is supported by public or private contributions (or both), we owe it to society at large to produce work that addresses the needs and interests of the many, especially those who are presently underserved, and that promotes their welfare. Our editorial priorities, therefore, are scientific rigor and social relevance, along with intellectual humility.

Concerning the last of these, we may still have something to learn from Dewey (1917), who abhorred “pretending [that] a scientific treatment starts from any other than a pluralistic basis” and disregarding “the complexity and specific variety of the factors of human nature, each operating in response to its own highly specific stimulus, and each subject to almost infinite shadings and modulations as it enters into combination and competition with others” (p. 269). We should not deceive ourselves into thinking that the behaviors we observe are simpler or more general, let alone immutable or inevitable, than they really are. At Frontiers in Social Psychology, we seek to promote research that is ethically grounded, methodologically sound (that is, convincing to experts), and both theoretically and practically ambitious. It should be offered modestly, without intellectual grandiosity, pomposity, or dismissiveness toward the valid contributions of others, including those working in neighboring disciplines or subdisciplines. If you share this vision of what social psychology can and should be, please, please join us.

Author's note

I thank Rachel Calogero, Richard P. Eibach, Curtis D. Hardin, Orsolya Hunyady, Lawrence J. Jost, Michal Kosinski, Maria Giuseppina Pacilli, Séamus Power, Kimbery Rios, and two anonymous reviewers for extremely helpful and constructive comments on earlier drafts of this editorial.

Author contributions

JJ: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author JJ declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^According to Williams (2015, p. 6–7), there are four major tenets of scientism, which are clearly distinguishable from mere adherence to the scientific method. They are: (1) “Only certifiably scientific knowledge counts as real knowledge. All else is mere opinion or nonsense.” (2) “The methods and assumptions underlying the natural sciences including epistemological and metaphysical doctrines, are appropriate for all sciences, including, prominently, the human and social sciences.” (3) “Scientism exudes and promotes an exaggerated confidence in science (i.e., natural science in all its avatars) to produce knowledge and solve the problems facing humanity. Such confidence would be reasonable only if the world, in every aspect of reality, were of a nature that lends itself to study by a science conceived and constructed as our natural science is.” (4) “Scientism assumes, and requires a naturalist, materialist, rather than mechanistic metaphysics. To understand fundamental aspects of humanity is to explain them in terms of things outside of them, and to explain them in that way is to destroy [or reduce] them.”

2. ^Here I am not referring to cases of outright fraud, of which several have come to light, but to manipulating the data in ways that were once considered normatively appropriate but that inflated error rates and distorted scientific conclusions (e.g., Simmons et al., 2011).

3. ^It is interesting to consider whether these scholars believed their own overconfident rhetoric (possibly succumbing to self-deception) or merely exaggerated the conclusiveness of their results because of professional incentives to “sell” their work. In any case, it is safe to say that social norms developed in many areas of academia to favor hubristic forms of self-promotion over more modest forms of communication (e.g., Hoekstra and Vazire, 2021).

4. ^I do not mean to suggest that all proponents of scientific preregistration believe that this practice, in and of itself, will solve all of the field's problems. However, too many authors seem to be treating it as a sine qua non of good scientific research without thinking deeply about the theoretical foundations of hypotheses that are or are not preregistered. Some also treat a preregistration “badge” as an invitation to exaggerate the importance and generalizability of their contribution.

References

Abelson, R. P. (1995). Statistics as Principled Argument. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Allport, G. W. (1948). “Foreword,” in Resolving Social Conflicts: Selected Papers on Group Dynamics by Kurt Lewin, ed G. W. Lewin (New York, NY: Harper & Brothers).

Google Scholar

Allport, G. W. (1968). “The historical background of modern social psychology,” in Handbook of Social Psychology, Vol. 1, G. Lindzey (Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley), 3–56.

Google Scholar

Argyle, M., Furnham, A., and Graham, J. A. (1981). Social Situations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Aronson, E. (2010). Not by Chance Alone: My Life as a Social Psychologist. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Asch, S. (1987). Social Psychology. New York, NY: Oxford.

Google Scholar

Banaji, M. R., Fiske, S. T., and Massey, D. S. (2021). Systemic racism: individuals and interactions, institutions and society. Cogn. Res. 6, 1–21. doi: 10.1186/s41235-021-00349-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Tal, D. (2013). Intractable Conflicts: Socio-Psychological Foundations and Dynamics. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Becker, G., and McClintock, C. G. (1972). “Scientific theory and social psychology,” in Experimental Social Psychology, ed C. G. McClintock (New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston), 5–20.

Google Scholar

Berkman, E. T., and Wilson, S. M. (2021). So useful as a good theory? The practicality crisis in (social) psychological theory. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 864–874. doi: 10.1177/1745691620969650

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Billig, M. (2015). The myth of Kurt Lewin and the rhetoric of collective memory in social psychology textbooks. Theory Psychol. 25, 703–718. doi: 10.1177/0959354315594255

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blake, K. R., and Gangestad, S. (2020). On attenuated interactions, measurement error, and statistical power: guidelines for social and personality psychologists. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 46, 1702–1711. doi: 10.1177/0146167220913363

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blank, T. O. (1988). Reflections on Gergen's “Social psychology as history” in perspective. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 14, 651–663. doi: 10.1177/0146167288144001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bless, H., and Burger, A. M. (2016). A closer look at social psychologists' silver bullet: inevitable and evitable side effects of the experimental approach. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 11, 296–308. doi: 10.1177/1745691615621278

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruner, J. (1983). In Search of Mind: Essays in Autobiography. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., Semin, G. R., and Berntson, G. G. (2004). Realism, instrumentalism, and scientific symbiosis: psychological theory as a search for truth and the discovery of solutions. Am. Psychologist 59, 214–223. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.4.214

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, D. T., and Russo, M. J. (1999). Social Experimentation. London: Sage.

Google Scholar

Cartwright, D. (1979). Contemporary social psychology in historical perspective. Soc. Psychol. Q. 42, 82–93.

Google Scholar

Cikara, M., Martinez, J. E., and Lewis Jr, N. A. (2022). Moving beyond social categories by incorporating context in social psychological theory. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 1, 537–549. doi: 10.1038/s44159-022-00079-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Collier, G., Minton, H. L., and Reynolds, G. (1991). Currents of Thought in American Social Psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Colucci, F. P., and Colombo, M. (2018). Dewey and Lewin: a neglected relationship and its current relevance to psychology. Theory Psychol. 28, 20–37. doi: 10.1177/0959354317740229

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Converse, P. E. (1986). “Generalization and the social psychology of ‘other worlds.”' in Metatheory in Social Science: Pluralisms and Subjectivities, eds D. W. Fiske and R. Shweder (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press), 42–60.

Google Scholar

Cronbach, L. J. (1975). Beyond the two disciplines of scientific psychology. Am. Psychologist 30, 116–127. doi: 10.1037/h0076829

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cutuli, J. J., Herbers, J. E., Masten, A. S., and Reed, M. G. J. (2020). “Resilience in development,” in The Oxford Handbook of Positive Psychology, eds C. R. Snyder, L. M. Edwards, S. J. Lopez, and S. C. Marques (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 171–188.

Google Scholar

Deutsch, M. (1976). Theorizing in social psychology. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 2, 134–141. doi: 10.1177/014616727600200214

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deutsch, M. (1999). “A personal perspective on the development of social psychology in the twentieth century,” in Reflections on 100 Years of Experimental Social Psychology, eds A. Rodrigues and R. V. Levine (New York, NY: Basic Books), 1–34.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dewey, J. (1917). The need for social psychology. Psychol. Rev. 24, 266–277. doi: 10.1037/h0071213

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewey, J. (1930). Human Nature and Conduct: An Introduction to Social Psychology. New York, NY: The Modern Library.

Google Scholar

Dickens, L., and Watkins, K. (1999). Action research: rethinking Lewin. Manag. Learn. 30, 127–140. doi: 10.1177/1350507699302002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doise, W. (1986). “Mass psychology, social psychology, and the politics of Mussolini,” in Changing Conceptions of Crowd Mind and Behavior, eds C. Graumann and S. Moscovici (New York, NY: Springer), 69–82.

Google Scholar

Dovidio, J. F., Newheiser, A. K., and Leyens, J. P. (2012). “A history of intergroup relations research,” in Handbook of the History of Social Psychology, eds A. W. Kruglanski and W. Stroebe (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 407–429.

Google Scholar

Duarte, J. L., Crawford, J. T., Stern, C., Haidt, J., Jussim, L., and Tetlock, P. E. (2015). Political diversity will improve social psychological science. Behav. Brain Sci. 38, e130. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14000430

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Elms, A. C. (1975). The crisis of confidence in social psychology. Am. Psychologist 30, 967–976. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.30.10.967

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Essien, I., Calanchini, J., and Degner, J. (2021). Moderators of intergroup evaluation in disadvantaged groups: a comprehensive test of predictions from system justification theory. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 120, 1204–1230. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000302

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., and Petty, R. E. (2020). A validity-based framework for understanding replication in psychology. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 24, 316–344. doi: 10.1177/1088868320931366

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farr, R. M. (1996). The Roots of Modern Social Psychology. Oxford: Blackwell.

Google Scholar

Festinger, L. (1980). Retrospections on Social Psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Fiedler, K. (2011). Voodoo correlations are everywhere-not only in neuroscience. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 163–171. doi: 10.1177/1745691611400237

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Finkel, E. J., Eastwick, P. W., and Reis, H. T. (2017). Replicability and other features of a high-quality science: toward a balanced and empirical approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 113, 244–253. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000075

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foa, E. B., and Foa, U. G. (2012). “Resource theory of social exchange,” in Handbook of Social Resource Theory: Theoretical Extensions, Empirical Insights, and Social Applications, eds K. Törnblom and A. Kazemi (New York, NY: Springer), 15–32.

Google Scholar

Forgas, J. P. (1979). Social Episodes: The Study of Interaction Routines. London: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Frederiksen, N. (1972). Toward a taxonomy of situations. Am. Psychologist 27, 114–123. doi: 10.1037/h0032705

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Geertz, C. (1973). The Interpretation of Cultures. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Gelman, A., and Vazire, S. (2021). Why did it take so many decades for the behavioral sciences to develop a sense of crisis around methodology and replication? J. Methods Measur. Soc. Sci. 12, 37–41. doi: 10.2458/jmmss.3062

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gergen, K. J. (1973). Social psychology as history. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 26, 309–320. doi: 10.1037/h0034436

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gergen, K. J. (1991). The Saturated Self: Dilemmas of Identity in Contemporary Life. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Gigerenzer, G. (1998). Surrogates for theories. Theory Psychol. 8, 195–204. doi: 10.1177/0959354398082006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, D. T., King, G., Pettigrew, S., and Wilson, T. D. (2016). Comment on “estimating the reproducibility of psychological science.” Science 351, 1037. doi: 10.1126/science.aad9163

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Giner-Sorolla, R. (2019). From crisis of evidence to a “crisis” of relevance? Incentive-based answers for social psychology's perennial relevance worries. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 30, 1–38. doi: 10.1080/10463283.2018.1542902

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gintis, H. (2007). A framework for the unification of the behavioral sciences. Behav. Brain Sci. 30, 1–61. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07000581

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goudarzi, S., Badaan, V., and Knowles, E. D. (2022). Neoliberalism and the ideological construction of equity beliefs. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 17, 1431–1451. doi: 10.1177/17456916211053311

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grahek, I., Schaller, M., and Tackett, J. L. (2021). Anatomy of a psychological theory: integrating construct-validation and computational-modeling methods to advance theorizing. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 803–815. doi: 10.1177/1745691620966794

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, K. (2017). How to map theory: reliable methods are fruitless without rigorous theory. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 731–741. doi: 10.1177/1745691617691949

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenfield, P. M. (2017). Cultural change over time: why replicability should not be the gold standard in psychological science. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 762–771. doi: 10.1177/1745691617707314

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G. (1976). Transhistorical lawfulness of behavior: a comment on two papers. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 2, 391–391. doi: 10.1177/014616727600200411

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwood, J. D. (1989). Explanation and Experiment in Social Psychological Science: Realism and the Social Constitution of Action. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Hardin, C., and Higgins, E. T. (1996). “Shared reality: how social verification makes the subjective objective,” in Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior, Vol. 3, eds R. M. Sorrentino and E. T. Higgins (New York, NY: Guilford), 28–84.

Google Scholar

Heider, F. (1983). The Life of a Psychologist: An Autobiography. Lawrence, KS: University of Kansas Press.

Google Scholar

Helmreich, R. (1975). Applied social psychology: the unfulfilled promise. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 1, 548–560. doi: 10.1177/014616727500100402

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61–83. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilton, D. (2012). “The emergence of cognitive social psychology: a historical analysis,” in Handbook of the History of Social Psychology, eds A. W. Kruglanski and W. Stroebe (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 45–79.

Google Scholar

Hoekstra, R., and Vazire, S. (2021). Aspiring to greater intellectual humility in science. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 1602–1607. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01203-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, J. M. (1988). Social Psychology, Past and Present. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Jahoda, G. (2007). A History of Social Psychology. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Jones, E. E. (1998). “Major developments in social psychology during the past five decades,” in Handbook of Social Psychology, 4th edn, Vol. 1, eds D. T. Gilbert, S. Fiske, and G. Lindzey (New York, NY: McGraw-Hill), 3–57.

Google Scholar

Jost, J. T. (2020). A Theory of System Justification. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Jost, J. T., and Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). The estrangement of social constructionism and experimental social psychology: history of the rift and prospects for reconciliation. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6, 168–187. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0603_1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kashima, Y., and Gelfand, M. J. (2012). “A history of culture in psychology,” in Handbook of the History of Social Psychology, eds A. W. Kruglanski and W. Stroebe (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 499–520.

Google Scholar

Kassin, S. (2022). Pillars of Social Psychology: Stories and Retrospectives. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Kay, A. C., and Eibach, R. P. (2013). Compensatory control and its implications for ideological extremism. J. Soc. Iss. 69, 564–585. doi: 10.1111/josi.12029

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kay, A. C., and Zanna, M. P. (2009). “A contextual analysis of the system justification motive and its societal consequences,” in Social and Psychological Bases of Ideology and System Justification, eds J. T. Jost, A. C. Kay, and H. Thorisdottir (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 158–181.

Google Scholar

Kelman, H. C. (2004). “Continuity and change: my life as a social psychologist,” in The Social Psychology of Group Identity and Social Conflict: Theory, Application, and Practice, eds A. H. Eagly, R. M. Baron, and V. L. Hamilton (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 233–275.

Google Scholar

Kitayama, S., and Salvador, C. E. (2024). Cultural psychology: beyond east and west. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 75, 63333. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-021723-063333

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, O., Doyen, S., Leys, C., Magalhães de Saldanha da Gama, P. A., Miller, S., Questienne, L., et al. (2012). Low hopes, high expectations: expectancy effects and the replicability of behavioral experiments. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 572–584. doi: 10.1177/1745691612463704

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruglanski, A. W. (2001). That “vision thing”: the state of theory in social and personality psychology at the edge of the new millennium. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 80, 871–875. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.871

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruglanski, A. W. (2004). The quest for gist: on challenges of going abstract in social and personality psychology. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 8, 156–163. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0802_9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruglanski, A. W., and Stroebe, W. (2012). “The making of social psychology,” in Handbook of the History of Social Psychology, eds A. W. Kruglanski and W. Stroebe (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 499–520.

Google Scholar

Lewin, K. (1947). Field Theory in Social Science. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

Google Scholar

Liaquat, U., Jost, J. T., and Balcetis, E. (2023). System justification motivation as a source of backlash against equality-promoting policies-and what to do about it. Soc. Iss. Pol. Rev. 17, 131–154. doi: 10.1111/sipr.12093

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnusson, D. (1981). Toward a Psychology of Situations: An Interactional Perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Manis, M. (1976). Is social psychology really different? Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 2, 428–437. doi: 10.1177/014616727600200417

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Markus, H. R. (2005). On telling less than we can know: the too tacit wisdom of social psychology. Psychol. Inq. 16, 180–184. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1604_08

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marrow, A. J. (1969). The Practical Theorist: The Life of Kurt Lewin. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Masten, A. S., and Reed, M. G. J. (2002). “Resilience in development,” in Handbook of Positive Psychology, eds C. R. Snyder and S. J. Lopez (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 74–88.

Google Scholar

McGuire, W. J. (1973). The yin and yang of progress in social psychology: seven koan. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 26, 446–456. doi: 10.1037/h0034345

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGuire, W. J. (1989). “A perspectivist approach to the strategic planning of programmatic scientific research,” in Psychological of Science: Contributions to Metascience, eds B. Gholson, W. R. Shadish, Jr, R. A. Neimeyer, and A. C. Houts (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 214–245.

Google Scholar

McGuire, W. J. (1999). Constructing Social Psychology: Creative and Critical Processes. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

McGuire, W. J. (2013). An additional future for psychological science. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 415–423. doi: 10.1177/1745691613491270

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Milgram, S. (1965). Some conditions of obedience and disobedience to authority. Hum. Relat. 18, 57–76. doi: 10.1177/001872676501800105

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Minton, H. L. (1984). J.F. Brown's social psychology of the 1930s: a historical antecedent to the contemporary crisis in social psychology. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 10, 31–42. doi: 10.1177/0146167284101003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moghaddam, F. M. (2023). How Psychologists Failed: We Neglected the Poor and Minorities, Favored the Rich and Privileged, and Got Science Wrong. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Moscovici, S. (1972). “Society and theory in social psychology,” in The Context of Social Psychology: A Critical Assessment, eds J. Israel and H. Tajfel (London: Academic Press), 17–68.

Google Scholar

Moscovici, S., and Marková, I. (2006). The Making of Modern Social Psychology: The Hidden Story of How an International Social Science Was Created. Malden, MA: Polity.

Google Scholar

Muthukrishna, M., and Henrich, J. (2019). A problem in theory. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 221–229. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0522-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Najdowski, C. J., and Goff, P. A. (2022). Toward a psychological science of abolition democracy: insights for improving theory and research on race and public safety. Soc. Iss. Pol. Rev. 16, 33–78. doi: 10.1111/sipr.12083

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Napier, J. L., Suppes, A., and Bettinsoli, M. L. (2020). Denial of gender discrimination is associated with better subjective well-being among women: a system justification account. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 50, 1191–1209. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2702

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbett, R. E. (1990). The anticreativity letters: advice from a senior tempter to a junior tempter. Am. Psychologist 45, 1078–1082. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.45.9.1078

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbett, R. E., and Masuda, T. (2003). Culture and point of view. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 100, 11163–11170. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1934527100

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Noah, T., Schul, Y., and Mayo, R. (2018). When both the original study and its failed replication are correct: feeling observed eliminates the facial-feedback effect. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 114, 657. doi: 10.1037/pspa0000121

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nosek, B. A., Spies, J. R., and Motyl, M. (2012). Scientific utopia: II. Restructuring incentives and practices to promote truth over publishability. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 615–631. doi: 10.1177/1745691612459058

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Open Science Collaboration (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science 349, aac4716. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4716

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, I. (1989). The Crisis in Modern Social Psychology-and How to End It. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Pettigrew, T. F. (2018). The emergence of contextual social psychology. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 44, 963–971. doi: 10.1177/0146167218756033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pettigrew, T. F. (2021). Contextual Social Psychology: Reanalyzing Prejudice, Voting, and Intergroup Contact. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Power, S. A., Zittoun, T., Akkerman, S., Wagoner, B., Cabra, M., Cornish, F., et al. (2023). Social psychology of and for world-making. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2023, 10888683221145756. doi: 10.1177/10888683221145756

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reis, H. T. (2008). Reinvigorating the concept of situation in social psychology. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 12, 311–329. doi: 10.1177/1088868308321721

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reis, H. T. (2019). “A brief history of social psychology,” in Advanced Social Psychology: The State of the Science, 2nd Edn, eds E. J. Finkel and R. Baumesiter (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 9–38.

Google Scholar

Ring, K. (1967). Experimental social psychology: some sober questions about some frivolous values. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 3, 113–123. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(67)90016-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivera Pichardo, E. J., Jost, J. T., and Benet-Martínez, V. (2022). Internalization of inferiority and colonial system justification: the case of Puerto Rico. J. Soc. Iss. 78, 79–106. doi: 10.1111/josi.12437

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, B. W. (2007). Contextualizing personality psychology. J. Personal. 75, 1071–1082. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00467.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodrigues, A., and Levine, R. V. (1999). Reflections on 100 Years of Experimental Social Psychology. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Ross, L., Lepper, M., and Ward, A. (2010). “History of social psychology: insights, challenges, and contributions to theory and application,” in Handbook of Social Psychology, 5th Edn, Vol. 1, eds S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, and G. Lindzey (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley), 3–50.

Google Scholar

Rozin, P. (2001). Social psychology and science: some lessons from Solomon Asch. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 5, 2–14. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0501_1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salter, P. S., Adams, G., and Perez, M. J. (2018). Racism in the structure of everyday worlds: a cultural-psychological perspective. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 27, 150–155. doi: 10.1177/0963721417724239

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saucier, G., Bel-Bahar, T., and Fernandez, C. (2007). What modifies the expression of personality tendencies? Defining basic domains of situation variables. J. Personal. 75, 479–504. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00446.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlenker, B. R. (1974). Social psychology and science. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 29, 1–15. doi: 10.1037/h0035668

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Secord, P. F. (1976). Transhistorical and transcultural theory. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 2, 418–420. doi: 10.1177/014616727600200415

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sensales, G. (2020). Italian Social Psychologies and Fascist Regimes: History of a Collective Removal. Oxford: Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology.

Google Scholar

Shaw, M. E., and Costanzo, P. R. (1982). Theories of Social Psychology, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Sherif, M. (1977). Crisis in social psychology: some remarks towards breaking through the crisis. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 3, 368–382. doi: 10.1177/014616727700300305

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., and Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1359–1366. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417632

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, M. B. (1976). Social psychology, science, and history: so what? Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 2, 438–444. doi: 10.1177/014616727600200418

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, M. B. (1994). Selfhood at risk: postmodern perils and the perils of postmodernism. Am. Psychologist 49, 405–411. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.49.5.405

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spellman, B. A. (2015). A short (personal) future history of revolution 2.0. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 886–899. doi: 10.1177/1745691615609918

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiner, I. D. (1974). Whatever happened to the group in social psychology? J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 10, 94–108. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(74)90058-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, D. (2020). Social psychological theory as history: outlining the critical-historical approach to theory. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 24, 78–99. doi: 10.1177/1088868319883174

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tajfel, H. (1972). “Experiments in a vacuum,” in The Context of Social Psychology: A Critical Assessment, eds J. Israel and H. Tajfel (London: Academic Press), 69–122.

Google Scholar

Teo, T. (1998). Klaus Holzkamp and the rise and decline of German critical psychology. Hist. Psychol. 1, 235–253. doi: 10.1037/1093-4510.1.3.235

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Teo, T. (2019). “Academic subjectivity, idols, and the vicissitudes of virtues in science: epistemic modesty versus epistemic grandiosity,” in Psychological Studies of Science and Technology. Palgrave Studies in the Theory and History of Psychology, eds K. C. O'Doherty, L. M. Osbeck, E. Schraube, and J. Yen (London: Palgrave Macmillan), 31–48.

Google Scholar

Tolman, C. W. (2009). Holzkamp's critical psychology as a science from the standpoint of the human subject. Theory Psychol. 19, 149–160. doi: 10.1177/0959354309103535

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trope, Y. (2004). Theory in social psychology: seeing the forest and the trees. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 8, 193–200. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0802_13

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Uskul, A. K., and Oishi, S. (2020). What is socio-ecological psychology? Curr. Opin. Psychol. 32, 181–184. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.01.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Bavel, J. J., Mende-Siedlecki, P., Brady, W. J., and Reinero, D. (2016). Contextual sensitivity in scientific reproducibility. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 113, 6454–6459. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1521897113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Lange, P. A., Kruglanski, A. W., and Higgins, E. T. (2012). “Theories of social psychology: an introduction,” in Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology, Vol. 1, eds P. A. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, and E. T. Higgins (London: Sage), 1–8.

Google Scholar

Wallach, L., and Wallach, M. A. (1994). Gergen versus the mainstream: are hypotheses in social psychology subject to empirical test? J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 67, 233–242. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.233

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallach, L., and Wallach, M. A. (2001a). Experiments in social psychology: science or self-deception? Theory Psychol. 11, 451–473. doi: 10.1177/0959354301114001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallach, L., and Wallach, M. A. (2001b). A response on concepts, laws and measurement in social psychology. Theory Psychol. 11, 489–494. doi: 10.1177/0959354301114004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallach, M. A., and Wallach, L. (1998a). When experiments serve little purpose: misguided research in mainstream psychology. Theory Psychol. 8, 183–194. doi: 10.1177/0959354398082005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallach, M. A., and Wallach, L. (1998b). Of surrogacy, circularity, causality and near-tautologies: a response. Theory Psychol. 8, 213–217. doi: 10.1177/0959354398082008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, R. N. (2015). “Introduction,” in Scientism: The New Orthodoxy, eds R. N. Williams and D. N. Robinson (London: Bloomsbury), 1–21.

Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Read, S. J., and Miller, L. C. (2009). The concept of situations. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 3, 1018–1037. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00236.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yarkoni, T. (2022). The generalizability crisis. Behav. Brain Sci. 45, E1. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X20001685

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zajonc, R. B. (1989). Styles of explanation in social psychology. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 19, 345–368. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420190503

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social psychology, social science, history, culture, politics, theory, evidence

Citation: Jost JT (2024) Grand challenge: social psychology without hubris. Front. Soc. Psychol. 1:1283272. doi: 10.3389/frsps.2023.1283272

Received: 25 August 2023; Accepted: 04 December 2023;
Published: 05 January 2024.

Edited by:

Natalie Shook, University of Connecticut, United States

Reviewed by:

Miguel Moya, University of Granada, Spain
Matthew Hibbing, University of California, Merced, United States

Copyright © 2024 Jost. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: John T. Jost, john.jost@nyu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.