- 1Virtual Human Interaction Lab, Department of Communication, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States
- 2College of Nursing, Department of Computer Science, Institute for Simulation & Training (Synthetic Reality Lab), University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL, United States
Social presence, or the feeling of being there with a “real” person, is a crucial component of interactions that take place in virtual reality. This paper reviews the concept, antecedents, and implications of social presence, with a focus on the literature regarding the predictors of social presence. The article begins by exploring the concept of social presence, distinguishing it from two other dimensions of presence—telepresence and self-presence. After establishing the definition of social presence, the article offers a systematic review of 233 separate findings identified from 152 studies that investigate the factors (i.e., immersive qualities, contextual differences, and individual psychological traits) that predict social presence. Finally, the paper discusses the implications of heightened social presence and when it does and does not enhance one's experience in a virtual environment.
Introduction
Since its conceptualization, virtual reality (VR) has been touted as a novel communication medium that would radically change the way people interact with each other (Biocca and Levy, 2013). William Gibson famously described cyberspace as a “consensual hallucination experienced daily by billions of legitimate operators, in every nation” (Gibson, 1984, p. 51), portraying the social nature of “stepping through a barrier” (Slater and Wilbur, 1997, p. 2) into the virtual environment. More recently, VR pioneer Jaron Lanier, expressed his hope that VR would lead to new and exciting forms of communication (Lanier, 2017).
Despite the conceptualization of VR as a social medium wherein individuals could co-exist and interact with each other (Biocca and Levy, 2013), much of the early research on VR technology focused on single-user head-mounted display (HMD) systems that typically were not available outside of the laboratory. In more recent years, however, VR technology has rapidly made its transition from lab to home in various forms. This increased accessibility of VR technology has fueled a renewed interest in the social applications of VR, which is exemplified by the launch of multiple platforms including AltSpace VR, Facebook Spaces, High Fidelity, Normal VR, Oculus Medium, Rec Room, Sansar, and VR Chat.
One of the primary attractions of VR is purported to be the level of social presence it affords in comparison to other forms of technology-mediated communication. Social presence refers to the subjective experience of being present with a “real” person and having access to his or her thoughts and emotions (Biocca, 1997); as such, one of the primary goals of networked communication systems is to offer higher levels of social presence (Biocca and Harms, 2002). Earlier forms of text-based computer-mediated communication (CMC) offered a limited amount of verbal and nonverbal information, which subsequently reduced the level of social presence people could feel within a set amount of time. Recent advancements in technology, however, have made media far more immersive than the past; in contrast to earlier forms of CMC, wherein individuals could only use text-based cues to express themselves, VR systems have the capacity to offer a wide array of social cues through visual, audio, haptic, and—to a lesser extent—olfactory information. It is therefore necessary to understand how different technological features influence perceptions of social presence to inform the design of VR platforms.
Researchers have also found that social presence can be influenced by contextual and individual factors that impact perceptions of the psychological distance between interactants (e.g., Siriaraya and Ang, 2012; Kang and Gratch, 2014; Verhagen et al., 2014). Studies conducted by these researchers show that the communication context as well as the individual traits of the interactants can influence perceptions of social presence. One of the most significant contributions of this line of research is that it sheds light on when increasing immersion is (and is not) necessary in order to induce stronger feelings of social presence. In a similar vein, these studies can inform both academic and applied researchers on how to maximize the amount of social presence one can feel within a given virtual environment.
To understand the concept, antecedents, and implications of social presence, we will first define two key concepts of the current paper, namely immersion and presence. Then we will offer a brief description of two separate dimensions of presence—telepresence and self-presence—to distinguish them from social presence. The remainder of the paper will focus on synthesizing the research on the antecedents of social presence to explore what does (and does not) impact perceptions of social presence.
Immersion and the Dimensions of Presence
While some researchers use the terms “immersion” and “presence” interchangeably, distinguishing the two concepts allows for a better understanding of the difference between the technological qualities and psychological experiences afforded by mediated communication. Immersion can be defined as a medium's technological capacity to generate realistic experiences that can remove people from their physical reality (Slater and Wilbur, 1997). When defined in this way, immersion can be objectively measured by the technological affordances of a medium. Media are more immersive when they can deliver “an inclusive, extensive, surrounding and vivid illusion of reality to the senses of a human participant” (Slater and Wilbur, 1997, p. 604). Features such as audio and visual quality, frame rate, stereoscopy, and field of view can impact the extent to which a system is immersive (Welch et al., 1996; Johnson and Stewart, 1999; Skalski and Whitbred, 2010; Cummings and Bailenson, 2016).
In contrast to immersion, presence is the subjective experience of actually being in the mediated virtual environment (Slater and Wilbur, 1997; Witmer and Singer, 1998; Walther and Parks, 2002). As presence is needed for people to fully experience a virtual environment, it has been the focus of both applied and academic work on virtual reality (Cummings et al., 2012). Presence can be further divided into three distinct subcategories: telepresence (spatial presence), self-presence, and social presence (Lee, 2004).
Telepresence can be defined as “the extent to which one feels present in the mediated environment, rather than in the immediate physical environment” (Steuer, 1992, p. 75). This dimension of presence relates strongly to how vividly the user experiences the environmental and spatial properties of the mediated environment. When the perception of telepresence is strong, people should no longer be aware that their experiences are being mediated through technology (Lombard and Ditton, 1997).
In contrast to telepresence, self-presence is the extent to which the “virtual self is experienced as the actual self” (Aymerich-Franch et al., 2012, p. 1). This dimension of presence differs from telepresence, as it is not related to how vividly one experiences his or her surroundings, but rather, how connected one feels to his or her virtual body, emotions, or identity (Ratan and Hasler, 2009).
Finally, social presence or co-presence, refers to the “sense of being with another” (Biocca et al., 2003, p. 456) and is dependent on the ease with which one perceives to have “the access to the intelligence, intentions, and sensory impressions of another” (Biocca, 1997, p. 22). The concept was first introduced as a theoretical framework to understand the interactions that took place on different forms of media (Short et al., 1976). Social presence differs from both telepresence and self-presence, as it requires a co-present entity that appears to be sentient. Social presence is an integral part of virtual environments that mediate people; without social presence, the mediated other is merely experienced as an artificial entity and not a social being (Lee et al., 2006a).
The Evolution of Social Presence
Social presence was first conceptualized by Short et al. (1976) and was defined as the salience of the interactants and their interpersonal relationship during a mediated conversation. According to Short et al. (1976), intimacy and immediacy are the two core components of social presence. These two concepts are closely related to each other; intimacy refers to the feeling of connectedness that communicators feel during an interaction, while immediacy is the psychological distance between the communicators. Both intimacy and immediacy are determined by verbal and nonverbal cues such as facial expressions, vocal cues, gestures, and physical appearance (Gunawardena and Zittle, 1997). Short and colleagues argued that some media were more capable at delivering these cues, while others were not, emphasizing that social presence was a “quality of the medium itself” (Short et al., 1976, p. 65).
The view that social presence is technologically determined was also echoed by early CMC researchers who endorsed the cues-filtered-out perspective (see Walther and Parks, 2002 for review). For example, media richness theory (Daft and Lengel, 1986) claimed that different media varied in their ability to reproduce “rich” social information (e.g., immediate feedback, language variety, personalization, number of cues), thereby making some media more appropriate than others for certain tasks. Put otherwise, certain media are inherently superior to others in achieving a specific communication goal. While some researchers have since rejected this technology-driven conceptualization of social presence (e.g., Walther, 1992), others continue to examine whether people feel different levels of social presence when interacting via a specific medium compared to another. Studies that focus on how the modality or specific technological affordances of a medium (e.g., immersive features) impact social presence are based on the assumption that certain affordances of a medium can increase or decrease social presence when all other circumstances are equal (e.g., Axelsson et al., 2001; Moreno and Mayer, 2004; Zhan and Mei, 2013).
In contrast to these medium-centric views of social presence, Walther (1992) argued that individuals are capable of adapting to different communication media, and can thus achieve their communication goals accordingly. From this perspective, the experience of social presence is highly contingent on the interactants, rather than the medium itself. This view is known as social information processing theory (SIPT). According to this theory, communication environments that offer fewer verbal and/or nonverbal cues (e.g., text-based CMC) can produce equal levels of intimacy as face-to-face (FtF) communication, although it may take more time. Walther (1996) later expanded this theory to posit that people who communicate via text-based CMC platforms could, in some cases, achieve even higher levels of social presence than FtF interactants by carefully selecting which facets of themselves they wish to reveal (i.e., hyperpersonal model of communication). Subsequent studies have since shown that individuals adopt different strategies to convey socioemotional cues on platforms with relatively limited verbal and nonverbal cues (e.g., Ramirez et al., 2002; Antheunis et al., 2010).
While both SIPT and the hyperpersonal model posit that technology does not solely determine the level of social presence a medium can afford, it is important to note that neither perspective denies the inherent differences between media. When individuals are only given limited communication options (e.g., short timespan, specific task type, etc.), it is probable that the technological features of the environment will influence the level of social presence a person feels. At the same time, however, this perspective offers a more nuanced view of social presence; while the immersive qualities (i.e., computer system's technological capacity to deliver a vivid experience) can impact social presence, individual communication strategies as well as contextual differences have a significant effect on social presence.
Why Does Social Presence Matter?
While both telepresence and self-presence have received academic focus, social presence has been considered to be particularly important within virtual environments with social actors (regardless of whether they are controlled by actual people or computer algorithms). This is due to the impact of social presence on social influence. Studies have shown that social presence is associated with a variety of positive communication outcomes, such as persuasion and attraction (e.g., Fogg and Tseng, 1999; Lee et al., 2006a). For example, Hassanein and Head (2007) found that social presence was positively associated with trust, enjoyment, and perceived usefulness of an online shopping website, which led to greater purchase intentions. Another study wherein social presence was operationalized to focus on the extent to which participants felt like they were together with their partner similarly found that social presence predicted attraction toward a physically embodied agent (i.e., robot; Lee et al., 2006a).
Antecedents of Social Presence
Because social presence often predicts positive communication outcomes, both academic researchers and practitioners have displayed a great interest in studying the factors that increase social presence. By reviewing 233 separate findings identified from 152 studies, we found that researchers have most often explored the influence of immersive qualities, contextual differences, and individual psychological traits on social presence (see Table 1). However, to the best of our knowledge, little effort has been made to synthesize the findings of these studies (for an exception, see Cummings and Wertz, 2018). Consequently, it is difficult to have a holistic understanding of which features are the most influential in predicting social presence. This paper attempts to overcome this shortcoming by offering a systematic review of the extant literature on the immersive, contextual, and psychological features that impact perceived social presence. The results, details, and general information of the studies that were reviewed are available in Tables 1–3.
Table 3. Summary of publication impact factora, sample size, and number of citationsb of reviewed studies.
Materials and Method
To collect studies that focused on the antecedents of social presence, we directly reviewed the archives of academic journals with a focus on virtual environments including Computers in Human Behavior; Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking; Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication; Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments; Frontiers in Robotics and AI, and conference proceedings from the International Society for Presence Researchers (ISPR) Conference and the IEEE Conference on Virtual Reality. We chose these outlets by selecting and expanding upon the outlets chosen in a recent meta-analysis on presence conducted by Cummings and Bailenson (2016). Based on concepts and terms that co-occurred frequently according to the subjective judgment of the researcher, we also conducted keyword searches in the EBSCO Host Communication & Mass Media databases, PsycNET, the Temple University ISPR Telepresence Literature Refshare database, and Google Scholar. Search terms included a combination of terms related to social presence, such as “social presence,” “co-presence,” “social richness,” “computers as social actors,” “virtual reality,” “virtual environments,” and “immersion” in addition to predictors that we identified during our search including “modality,” “HMD,” “realism,” “stereoscopy,” “haptics,” “audio,” “display,” “tracking,” “gender,” “agency,” and “proximity.”
Once the candidate studies were identified, we selected studies that (1) used at least one self-report measure of social presence (or the synonymous concept of co-presence); if social presence and co-presence were measured separately, we considered both measures in our review, (2) included experimental manipulations and/or questionnaire items (e.g., personality, gender, etc.) that were used to assess the predictors of social presence, and (3) conducted quantitative analyses to determine whether a predictor significantly influenced perceptions of social presence.
Studies that measured social presence with related but distinct constructs (e.g., interactivity, positive affect, social influence, telepresence, interpersonal attraction, electronic propinquity) were not included, as they do not uniquely measure the extent to which one feels as if she is present with a sentient being. Similarly, concepts that share theoretical similarities with, but do not uniquely measure social presence, were excluded. One significant concept that was not included due to this criterion was plausibility illusion (Slater, 2009). Plausibility illusion refers to the credibility of the events that are unfolding in the virtual environment. According to Slater (2009), plausibility illusion is orthogonal to the “sense of being there,” which is conceptualized as place illusion. Plausibility illusion shares similarities with social presence as it captures the extent to which the user feels that he or she is interacting with an actual social being (Biocca et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2006a). However, plausibility illusion also includes dimensions other than social presence because the concept simultaneously captures the credibility of various aspects of a virtual scenario, not just the virtual human (Slater et al., 2010).
Finally, while we are aware of the strengths of behavioral and physiological measures and limitations of self-report measures (Slater, 2004; Friedman et al., 2006), we did not include studies that exclusively used behavioral and/or physiological measures of social presence to reduce variance and maximize internal validity when comparing study findings. The criteria used to select studies were adapted from Cummings and Bailenson (2016) to fit the current context. Based on this process (Figure 1), we were able to identify 152 studies with 233 separate findings regarding the factors that can predict social presence. When discussing the results, we assumed that the findings of the studies were true and correct.
Figure 1. Flow chart of study identification. *Social presence is not a dependent variable: 27; No quantitative self-report measure of social presence: 24; Review article: 9; General presence is measured: 6; Work-in-progress: 3; Measure not reported: 1; Conference presentation of published article: 5.
Findings: What Predicts Social Presence?
Considering that social presence was initially considered to be an inherent quality of a communication medium (Short et al., 1976), it is natural that a significant body of research explored how modality influences social presence. For similar reasons, the technological affordances that enable the reproduction of various social cues (e.g., presence of a visual representation, haptic feedback, etc.) have received considerable attention as potential antecedents of social presence. However, while earlier studies on the predictors of social presence focused almost entirely on immersive qualities, more recent studies also consider the impact of contextual and individual factors, perhaps as an acknowledgment of social presence as a subjective experience. The following sections will thus categorize and discuss the predictors of social presence using three overarching categories that emerged while conducting the systematic review: immersive qualities, contextual properties, and individual traits.
Immersive Qualities and Social Presence
General Modality
Much of the earlier social presence research focused on how modalities with varying levels of immersion afford different levels of presence. It is important to note that while research on general modality does offer insight into how certain technological features (e.g., depth cues, display, stereoscopy) might influence social presence, it compares media that vary across multiple features, which makes it difficult to isolate the affordance(s) that influenced perceptions of social presence. This camp of research is well-aligned with the traditions of social presence theory (Short et al., 1976) and media richness theory (Daft and Lengel, 1986) in that they are grounded on the assumption that the technological qualities of a medium afford different levels of social presence. In their meta-analysis on the impact of immersion on telepresence, Cummings and Bailenson (2016) similarly found that general modality (e.g., comparing an HMD with head-tracking to a desktop computer) was one of the most frequently studied predictors of telepresence.
As can be seen in Table 1, research on the impact of modality on social presence to date most often compares (1) CMC with FtF communication, (2) text-based CMC with other forms of audiovisual modalities, and (3) immersive virtual environments with non-immersive virtual environments. Although it is less common, a small number of studies also compare different types of virtual environments (e.g., Heldal et al., 2005; Johnsen and Lok, 2008).
Because FtF interaction is considered to be the gold-standard for social presence (Biocca et al., 2001), a considerable amount of research compares FtF communication with CMC to determine how successful a given system is at establishing a social presence. Most of these studies found that communicators experience lower levels of social presence during CMC compared to FtF conversations. For example, Cortese and Seo (2012) found that CMC participants felt less social presence than FtF participants while they were discussing issues mentioned in a news article for 20 min. More specifically, the researchers operationalized social presence to assess both how sociable their partner was and how “co-located” they felt with their partner, and found that FtF communicators experienced higher levels of social presence compared to their CMC counterparts. Similar results were found in online learning contexts (Zhan and Mei, 2013) and decision-making scenarios (Biocca et al., 2001; Alge et al., 2003). One exception to this trend was Francescato et al. (2006) study, which found no differences in perceived social presence between students who completed a seminar series online, compared to those who completed the same seminar face-to-face. It is important to note, however, that participants completed the seminar series over a period of 2 months. This extended experiment period may be why the authors did not find a difference between CMC and FtF conditions. Just as Walther (1992) found that granting additional time to CMC interactants led to equally desirable communication outcomes as their FtF counterparts, the 2-month period employed by Francescato et al. (2006) may have been sufficient for both groups of participants to adapt their communication strategies to the given platforms and attain similar levels of social presence.
Studies that compared text-based CMC with more vivid forms of communication modalities (e.g., audio, video, avatar) also found that participants felt the lowest level of social presence when communicating via text-based CMC compared to “richer” forms of media, when given the same amount of time (e.g., Bente et al., 2008; Appel et al., 2012; Kim et al., 2013b). For example, Bente et al. (2008) measured how much social presence participants felt while selecting the best job candidate out of a pool of six applicants in a text chat, audio, audio with video, or avatar communication platform. They found that participants in the text chat condition felt significantly less social presence when compared with participants who communicated via other modalities. Similarly, studies that compared text-based CMC with modalities that offered audiovisual cues, such as videoconferencing (Sallnäs, 2005; Kim et al., 2014), avatar-mediated communication, and audio communication (Kim et al., 2013b) generally found that text-based CMC elicits lower social presence than modalities that offer additional audiovisual cues.
While audio and video modalities appear to have a clear advantage over text-based CMC, the strength of audiovisual modalities over audio-only modalities is less clear. Of the nine studies identified in Table 1 (de Greef and Ijsselsteijn, 2001; Yoo and Alavi, 2001; Sallnäs, 2005, Study 1 & 2; Bailenson et al., 2006; Bente et al., 2008; Homer et al., 2008; de Greef, 2014; Gimpel et al., 2016) that offered a comparison between audio-only and audio-video modalities, only four found that the addition of video increased perceptions of social presence (de Greef and Ijsselsteijn, 2001; Yoo and Alavi, 2001; de Greef, 2014; Gimpel et al., 2016). While the sample size is small (n = 9), these results suggest that linear increments of immersion do not necessarily lead to corresponding increases in social presence. Considering that two of the studies (de Greef and Ijsselsteijn, 2001; de Greef, 2014) that did find that adding video increased social presence required participants to complete a visual task, while the studies that did not find differences between the audio-only and audio-video conditions provided participants with tasks that had a weaker visual component (e.g., decision-making task, interview task), it is possible that the nature of the task moderates the benefits of adding video to audio. Table 2 shows details of these studies.
A small number of studies (e.g., Steed et al., 1999; Slater et al., 2000; Moreno and Mayer, 2004) have also compared immersive virtual platforms (e.g., HMD, cave automatic virtual environment; CAVE) with non-immersive ones (e.g., Desktop). While the literature shows a general consensus that immersive virtual environments are more likely to generate greater feelings of telepresence compared to non-immersive virtual platforms (Cummings and Bailenson, 2016), this does not appear to be the case for social presence. Among the 10 studies that we identified, only two studies found significant differences in social presence between an immersive platform and a non-immersive one (Schroeder et al., 2001; Heldal et al., 2005). These results, coupled with the fact that the addition of video does not consistently increase one's sense of social presence, suggest that once a threshold is met, increasing the immersive quality of a modality does not automatically lead to increased social presence. As such, it may be both theoretically and practically important to isolate features and explore the extent to which each feature does (or does not) contribute to increasing social presence to further understand the dimensions of immersion that affect social presence.
Visual Representation
One of the unique features that influence social presence in virtual environments is the visual representation of the communication partner. Studies that focus on visual representations explore how the appearance of the partner in virtual reality influences one's sense of social presence. These studies generally manipulate (1) the presence or absence of a visual representation and (2) the visual realism of the virtual representation. Visual realism consists of photographic, anthropomorphic, and behavioral (or communicative) realism (Harris et al., 2009). Photographic and anthropomorphic realism both pertain to the appearance of the virtual representation; the former assesses how “realistic” it appears, while the latter refers to how “humanlike” it is. In contrast, behavioral realism is defined as the extent to which the virtual representation behaves in the way an actual person would behave (e.g., blink naturally, shift positions, “breathe,” etc.).
While there are a few exceptions (e.g., Qiu and Benbasat, 2005; Kim and Sundar, 2012), most of the current evidence indicates that people feel higher levels of social presence when there is a visual representation available, as can be noted in Table 1. For example, participants who were able to see their partner's avatar reported higher levels of social presence compared to those who spoke with an “invisible” partner after they shopped for clothes together in a virtual shopping mall (Kim et al., 2013b). Another study (Feng et al., 2016) similarly found that participants felt greater social presence toward online support-seekers who provided a profile picture compared to those who did not. Furthermore, participants were more likely to give responses that reflected an awareness of and adaptation to the support-seeker and his/her context (person-centeredness) when there was a profile picture available, an effect that was partially mediated by social presence.
In addition to the impact of providing a visual representation, studies have also examined how the extent to which a visual representation behaves like an actual person (i.e., behavioral realism) affects social presence. Behavioral realism can be operationalized by the complete absence or presence of nonverbal behavior (animations) or how much the virtual human's nonverbal behavior is consistent with actual humans (e.g., presence or absence of eye gaze). Studies generally show that behavioral realism is a powerful predictor of perceived social presence. These positive effects are most consistently found when the avatar's or agent's behavior indicates awareness of their communication partner's presence (e.g., mutual gaze, nodding at appropriate times, blushing). For example, von der Pütten et al. (2010) found that participants felt higher levels of social presence when they interacted with a computerized agent (Rapport Agent) that displayed appropriate feedback behavior by nodding its head compared to one that did not. Similarly, Pan et al. (2008) found that participants felt the highest level of social presence when a virtual agent blushed strongly (whole-face blush) after making a mistake during a presentation. Participants also felt higher levels of social presence when their communication partner maintained longer mutual eye contact with them compared to when he or she did not (Bente et al., 2008, Study 1); when the duration of the mutual eye gaze was too long (which is behaviorally unrealistic), however, participants responded negatively (Bente et al., 2008, Study 2). The significance of behavioral realism in fostering a sense of social presence may also explain why previous studies failed to find a positive association between the use immersive avatar-mediated VR systems and social presence. More specifically, the lack of significant results may have been due to the fairly limited level of behavioral realism afforded by older platforms.
In contrast to the relatively consistent effects of behavioral realism on social presence, studies on the impact of photographic and anthropomorphic realism reveal mixed results. While some studies show an increase in social presence when the visual representation is more photographically or anthropomorphically realistic (e.g., Kang and Watt, 2013), others report no differences (e.g., Bailenson et al., 2001; Bente et al., 2008) or even a reduction in social presence (e.g., Nowak and Biocca, 2003). The inconsistency in these results may be explained by three factors. First, photographic realism may simply not be the most crucial component of social presence. As Blascovich et al. (2002) and Nass et al. (1994) argue, the appearance of the visual representation might simply be less important than behavioral social cues. Second, the social presence questionnaires used may not have been sensitive enough to capture the subtle differences caused by variations in the appearance of the virtual human. Finally, these inconsistent effects may be explained by the varying levels of behavioral realism in each study. Studies that manipulate both the appearance and behavior of the visual representation show strong support for consistency effects (Garau et al., 2003; Bailenson et al., 2005). That is, participants feel greater social presence when the level of behavioral realism is consistent with the level of photographic realism. Garau et al. (2003) found, for example, that while increasing the level of photographic realism did not have a main effect on social presence, participants felt higher levels of social presence when they interacted with an avatar high in photographic realism compared to one low in photographic realism when the avatar displayed realistic eye gaze behavior (i.e., high behavioral realism). The opposite effect was found for avatars low in behavioral realism. In a separate study, Bailenson et al. (2005) also noted that the consistency between behavioral and photographic realism positively predicts social presence.
To summarize, the current literature offers evidence that (1) the presence of a visual representation and (2) a more behaviorally realistic visual representation enhance social presence. In contrast, while both photographic and anthropomorphic realism can enhance perceptions of social presence, this effect appears to be contingent on certain boundary conditions, including consistency with the level of behavioral realism.
Interactivity
While real-time virtual communication between actual people is usually characterized by high levels of interactivity, the level of interactivity afforded by a computerized agent can vary. As such, studies that explored the impact of interactivity on social presence generally looked into how an agent's interactivity influences social presence. Considering that social presence is dependent on how strongly one feels that he or she is talking with an intelligent being that is aware of his or her presence (Biocca, 1997), it is unsurprising that the extant research, albeit with some boundary conditions, offers robust evidence that interactivity is positively associated with social presence. In their study on social agents, for example, Skalski and Tamborini (2007) invited participants to listen to a health message on blood pressure. They found that participants who were given the opportunity to interact with the agent by letting it know the order in which they wished to receive the health information felt higher levels of social presence compared to participants who did not have this opportunity. Fortin and Dholakia (2005) similarly found positive effects of interactivity, although their results were qualified by participants' need for cognition (NFC); participants high in NFC showed a linear increase in social presence as the level of interactivity increased, while those low in NFC exhibited a ceiling effect wherein social presence increased between low and medium levels of interactivity, but plateaued for medium and high levels of interactivity.
Haptic Feedback
Due to the significance of touch in physical interactions, a lot of effort has been—and continues to be—made to introduce interpersonal touch through haptic devices in virtual environments. The current review identified haptic feedback as one of the most commonly studied immersive qualities that influence social presence, apart from visual representation and interactivity. With the exception of one study (Sallnäs et al., 2000), all of the 10 studies that we identified found a positive relationship between haptic feedback and perceptions of social presence (Table 1). For example, participants felt higher levels of social presence when they received haptic feedback as they lifted a (virtual) box with a partner compared to when such feedback was not available (Kim et al., 2004). One thing to note is that, as Table 2 shows, most of the studies on haptic feedback reviewed in the present paper required participants to jointly manipulate an object (e.g., move blocks together, play air hockey). As such, the tasks themselves may have been biased to amplify the positive effects of haptic feedback compared to tasks that require less “manual” collaboration.
Depth Cues (Stereoscopy and Motion Parallax)
Stereoscopic displays create the illusion of depth by providing slightly different images to each eye. Motion parallax is a monocular depth cue wherein people perceive objects closer to them to be moving at a faster rate than objects a further distance. The studies that were identified in the present paper (Mühlbach et al., 1995; Takatalo et al., 2011; Kim et al., 2012; Ahn et al., 2014) suggest that the inclusion of depth cues increase social presence. In one study, for example, college freshmen viewed a virtual character (computerized agent) as it gave a 5-min news presentation about the school that they would be attending in either a stereoscopic or monoscopic display (Ahn et al., 2014). The researchers found that stereoscopy significantly increased perceptions of being together with the virtual character. Mühlbach et al. (1995) similarly found that participants felt greater social presence when they engaged in a video conferencing session using a stereoscopic display compared to a monoscopic one. Although the researchers of this study used “telepresence” to describe their outcome variable, the measures that they used (“It was as if we were all in the same room” and “It was like a real face-to-face meeting”) reflected social presence, rather than telepresence. While these studies point to a positive relationship between stereoscopy and social presence, more research is needed to support this hypothesis.
Audio Quality
Research that investigated the impact of audio quality on social presence generally focused on how altering the number of sound channels influences perceptions of social presence. Surprisingly, we were unable to identify studies that addressed the impact of audio disturbances such as noise or dropout on social presence. While we only found three studies that manipulated audio quality, all of these studies found that improving audio quality leads to an increased sense of social presence. For example, Skalski and Whitbred (2010) conducted a study wherein participants were assigned to play a first-person shooter video game with either a 6-channel (Dolby 5.1) or 2-channel (Dolby Stereo) sound system. They found that the high audio-quality participants felt higher levels of social richness (i.e., social presence) than their low audio-quality counterparts. The authors also manipulated image quality, but no interaction effects were found between image and audio quality. In perhaps one of the earliest studies of social presence, Christie (1974) conducted a study wherein 36 businessmen discussed an important business topic in groups of six, and found that participants reported higher levels of social presence for the multi-speaker phone system than for the standard or high-fidelity speakerphone.
Display
A small number of studies also manipulated features of the display itself, namely image definition and display size, to examine their influence on social presence. The results of these studies yield mixed results. While two studies (Bracken, 2005; Ahn et al., 2014) found that more immersive displays (i.e., higher definition, larger screen size) led to higher social presence, two others (Skalski and Whitbred, 2010; James et al., 2011) were unable to find a significant effect of display on social presence. As such, more research is needed to understand when and how display qualities influence social presence.
Contextual Properties and Social Presence
As mentioned above, recent studies have begun to expand research on the predictors of social presence from immersive qualities to contextual and individual properties. This shift in the landscape may, in part, be attributed to the fact that social presence is a subjective experience that is influenced by both the perceived physical and psychological distance between the interactants, not solely the technological qualities of a medium. As such, both contextual and individual factors that contribute to how familiar or close a virtual human feels may have an influence on social presence above and beyond immersion. The following sections will describe antecedents of social presence that are not associated with objective immersive qualities, but contextual and individual qualities that impact one's subjective perceptions of being together with another person.
Application of Social Psychology: Personality/Traits of Virtual Human
Multiple studies have applied well-established findings from social psychology for positive interpersonal evaluations (e.g., similarity attraction, social penetration theory, social identity theory, preference for consistency, etc.) to technology-mediated contexts to explore their relevance in interpersonal perceptions (e.g., Reeves and Nass, 1996; Jin, 2012; Verhagen et al., 2014). This line of research has found that most interpersonal dynamics that can be found in FtF contexts can be replicated in virtual environments with both agents and avatars. For example, Qiu and Benbasat (2010) found that participants were more likely to feel higher levels of social presence when they interacted with a virtual product recommendation agent whose appearance matched their ethnicity than one that did not, replicating findings based on social identity theory (Tajfel, 1979). In another study (Kang and Gratch, 2014), participants perceived more social presence when their virtual counselor (computerized agent) disclosed more personal information about itself, which offers support for Altman and Taylor's (1973) self-disclosure theory. Similarly, participants felt higher levels of social presence when their partner's virtual representation was similar to his or her actual physical appearance (Jin, 2012), which resonates with findings regarding preference for consistency (Festinger, 1962). These studies underscore the fact that social presence is not only influenced by immersive qualities that can objectively provide richer social cues, but also by psychological processes that allow individuals to interpret the available social cues in more positive (or negative) ways.
Agency
Differences in agency occur depending on whether or not the virtual human is controlled by an actual human (i.e., avatar) or a computerized algorithm (i.e., agent). Studies that explore the impact of (perceived) agency on social presence generally introduce the virtual human as an actual person or a computerized character prior to the interaction. Approximately half of the studies surveyed in this paper found that people felt higher levels of social presence when the virtual human was thought to be controlled by an actual person rather than by a computer program. For example, participants felt greater social presence when they believed that the Rapport Agent they were interacting with was a real person compared to when they thought it was an artificial intelligence (Appel et al., 2012). These results are in line with Blascovich et al. (2002) model of social influence, which posits that avatars require a lower threshold of realism than agents to yield social influence. While they did not explore the impact of agency on social presence using a questionnaire, another study showed that participants showed higher physiological arousal while playing a computer game when they thought their opponent was an avatar compared to when they thought it was an agent (Lim and Reeves, 2010). These findings echo a meta-analysis conducted by Fox et al. (2015) that found that avatars generally elicit greater social influence than agents.
The remaining half of the studies, however, suggests that participants perceive similar levels of social presence for both agents and avatars (Nowak and Biocca, 2003; von der Pütten et al., 2010; Dalzel-Job, 2014, Study 2; Kothgassner et al., 2014, 2017; Felnhofer et al., 2018). Considering the fact that the majority of the studies published prior to 2010 found a positive relationship between agency and social presence (4 out of 5 studies), while only a small number of the studies published after 2010 did (2 out of 7 studies), it is possible that users have started to develop different expectations regarding how an avatar (vs. agent) should behave and/or look in virtual environments, and that deviations from these media expectations can lead to less social presence or doubt of the veridicality of the experimental manipulation, regardless of purported agency.
Physical Proximity
The present paper also identified five studies that explored the impact of absolute physical distance between interactants on feelings of social presence (e.g., Gajadhar et al., 2008; Croes et al., 2016; Järvelä et al., 2016). These studies consistently show a positive relationship between physical proximity and perceptions of social presence. To explore the impact of physical proximity on social presence, this line of research compared the social presence of participants who had completed an activity in the same room to those who had completed the same activity in different rooms. Of note is that participants who were in the same room were often able to see each other during the interaction, while those that were placed in separate locations remained visually anonymous. As such, it is difficult to determine if the purported effects of physical proximity were driven by physical closeness, visual (non)anonymity, or both. Only two of these studies (Hatta and Ken-ichi, 2008; Croes et al., 2016) were able to effectively separate the effects of visual anonymity from physical co-location. Croes et al. (2016) study found that both physical co-location and visibility (non-anonymity) separately and positively predicted social presence. Hatta and Ken-ichi (2008) found an interaction between physical proximity and visibility, such that while physical closeness did lead to higher levels of social presence for visually anonymous partners, this effect did not persist when partners could see each other. In light of these findings, it is possible to conjecture that the positive association between social presence and physical proximity found in the remaining studies stemmed from a combination of physical co-location and visibility. In sum, there is cogent evidence that physical closeness with the interaction target contributes to perceived psychological distance and social presence, but it is likely that this effect will be influenced by the visibility of the virtual partner.
Task Type
Four studies (Kim et al., 2013a; de Greef, 2014; Herrewijn and Poels, 2015; Wu et al., 2015) explored the influence of task type on perceived social presence. In one study (Kim et al., 2013a), participants either took care of or were taken care of by a robot. The researchers found that participant felt higher levels of social presence when the robot was the caregiver, compared to when they were asked to take care of the robot. In another study, participants felt lower levels of social presence when they were asked to observe their partner play a multiplayer game compared to when their partner observed them or when they played the game together with their partner (Herrewijn and Poels, 2015). While it is difficult to draw definitive conclusions from these studies due to the small sample size, they suggest that tasks that encourage self-directed attention (i.e., encourage the virtual human to focus on the participant) may increase social presence. Just as nonverbal cues that implied the virtual human's awareness of the participant increased social presence (e.g., Bente et al., 2008; Shahid et al., 2012), people may feel higher levels of social presence when the given task requires the virtual human to pay attention to and accommodate their behavior. More details about the tasks are given in Table 2.
Social Cues About the Presence of Others
More recent studies (Choi and Kwak, 2017, Study 2; Lee and Nass, 2004; Lee et al., 2005; Kim and Sundar, 2014; Daher et al., 2016; Kim, 2016; Robb et al., 2016) have examined how the number of people or the mere presence of another person who is aware of the virtual environment (i.e., social cues) impacts feelings of social presence. In general, these studies show that seeing other people who share or interact with the same virtual environment as the user increases social presence. For example, Daher et al. (2016) found that being exposed to a conversation between a virtual human and a real person prior to the study increased feelings of social presence for the participant after interacting with the same virtual human. Choi and Kwak (2017, Study 2) found that participants felt a stronger sense of social presence when they were communicating with multiple remote partners via a telepresence robot compared to a single remote partner. These results are in line with the findings of Kim (2016) and Lee and Nass (2004), who also found that multiple virtual communicators increase feelings of social presence. In contrast to these findings, Robb et al. (2016) found that having a human teammate did not appear to increase the perceived social presence of a virtual medical practitioner. Overall, however, the majority of the research suggests that being in a context wherein individuals are exposed to cues that indicate a social context (e.g., conversation, partner, group, etc.) can lead to heightened levels of social presence. Considering the non-significant findings of Robb et al. (2016) and the relatively small number of studies, however, more research is needed to conclusively understand the implications of co-present others on social presence.
Identity Cues
Finally, studies have also explored the provision of identity cues (e.g., name, portrait picture) as a contextual factor that influences social presence, and found that increasing the number of identity cues enhances feelings of social presence (Li et al., 2015; Feng et al., 2016; Choi and Kwak, 2017; Schumann et al., 2017). Given the fact that social presence is contingent on the extent to which an individual feels that he or she is in the presence of a “real person,” it is natural that providing participants with cues that offer insight into the “true” identity of their virtual partner(s) enhances social presence.
Individual Differences and Social Presence
Demographic Characteristics: Gender and Age
As can be noted in Table 1, two of the most commonly examined individual differences in relation to social presence are the gender and age of the user. Most of the studies that explored the relationship between users' demographic variables and social presence did not specifically focus on these demographic variables, but included them as covariates or control variables in their analyses. In terms of gender, the majority of the surveyed studies found that females experience higher levels of social presence compared to males (e.g., Giannopoulos et al., 2008; Johnson, 2011). Age, in contrast, does not appear to have a strong association with social presence. The age range of the seven studies that explored the relationship between age and social presence are as follows: Cho et al. (2015): 21–44, Felnhofer et al. (2014): range not reported (M = 23.34, SD = 2.73), Hauber et al. (2005): 19–63, Kim et al. (2004): not reported, Lim and Richardson (2016): 24–58, Richardson and Swan (2003): 19–63, Siriaraya and Ang (2012): 22–80. Five of these found no significant relationship between age and social presence. However, considering the fact that the remaining two studies (Siriaraya and Ang, 2012; Felnhofer et al., 2014) both found that older participants tended to experience lower levels of social presence, it may be worth exploring if factors such as familiarity with a given technology or openness to new experiences influence perceptions of social presence.
Psychological Traits
As can be noted in Table 1, more recent research explored the impact of psychological traits on social presence (e.g., Giannopoulos et al., 2008; Jin, 2010; Cortese and Seo, 2012; Kim et al., 2013a). These studies either looked at the impact of an individual's (1) propensity to become immersed in a virtual environment (e.g., immersive tendency, transportability) or (2) attitudes toward social interactions (e.g., communication apprehension, interdependent construal, extraversion, need to belong) on social presence. These studies showed that people who have stronger immersive tendencies are also more likely to experience stronger social presence. For instance, (Kim et al., 2013a) found that participants who were higher in immersive tendency were more likely to feel stronger social presence when interacting with a social robot. More interestingly, studies also found that individuals who value or enjoy social interactions experience higher levels of social presence. Jin (2010), for example, found that individuals who had interdependent self-construals experienced stronger social presence. There are two non-exclusive explanations for these findings; first, individuals who have positive attitudes toward social interactions may have a stronger desire to feel social presence and thus try harder to gratify this motivation during a virtual interaction. Second, people who are less socially oriented may lack the ability to adequately attend to the social information at hand (Cortese and Seo, 2012), and consequently experience lower levels of social presence than their more socially oriented counterparts even with the same amount of social cues. Overall, these studies highlight the importance of considering individual differences when examining features in a virtual environment that might influence social presence.
Discussion
Thus far, the present paper defined social presence and explored the technological, contextual, and individual qualities that can influence perceptions of social presence. Overall, we found that immersion and context have a positive effect on social presence, although there do appear to be ceiling effects and boundary conditions. While demographic information, and psychological traits associated with positive attitudes toward social interactions also tended to increase participants' feelings of social presence, the effects of demographic characteristics were less conclusive. Although we interpreted null findings to indicate the absence of a significant effect, it is important to note that several of the studies were conducted on a small number of participants (see Table 3). As such, some of the non-significant results can also be interpreted as inconclusive findings, and thus merit further research. As mentioned before, while earlier studies on the predictors of social presence focused primarily on the impact of immersive features, a growing number of researchers have begun to consider contextual and individual features as factors that can increase or decrease feelings of social presence (Figure 2).
Figure 2. Proportion of studies that examine immersive, contextual, and individual predictors of social presence.
One caveat to the present review is that social presence was operationalized in a number of different ways depending on the study (see Table 2). Considering the fact that questionnaire wording can influence responses (Borgers et al., 2004), it is possible that the use of different measures may account, at least in part, for why the same feature predicted social presence in some cases, but not in others. As social presence is often measured in different contexts (e.g., human-agent interaction, human-human interaction, etc.), some diversity in measures is inevitable (Biocca et al., 2003). However, more effort is needed to build a “foundation for theory and measure of social presence with greater explanatory and predictive power” (Biocca et al., 2003, p. 474).
While the variability of sample size per predictor necessitates caution in interpreting our results, we found that depth cues, audio quality, haptic feedback, and interactivity often had positive effects on social presence (Figure 3). In contrast, there influence of general modality, visual representation, and display were somewhat weaker. Among contextual factors, physical proximity, identity cues, and the personality/traits of the virtual human were often significant predictors of social presence. Somewhat surprisingly, the effects of agency were less conclusive (Figure 4). In terms of demographic factors, neither age nor gender appeared to have a clear effect on social presence. In contrast, certain psychological traits (e.g., transportability, extraversion, need to belong) tended to predict social presence. However, as much of the available research focuses on a select number of predictors such as general modality, visual representations, and personality/traits of the virtual other, more studies are needed before we can draw concrete conclusions about the impact of certain features (Figure 5).
Figure 3. Effects of immersive features on social presence. *Other refers to moderated or non-linear results.
Figure 4. Effects of contextual features on social presence. *Other refers to moderated or non-linear results.
One interesting point to note is that the majority of the studies identified in this paper frame social presence as an “absolute good.” Social presence is often used to assess how “successful” a given communication system is at emulating the gold-standard of FtF communication (e.g., Biocca et al., 2001; Hauber et al., 2005). In addition, researchers frequently hypothesize that increasing the salience of the mediated communication partner will naturally lead to more positive social outcomes (e.g., Fogg and Tseng, 1999; Hassanein and Head, 2007). While there is a wealth of research that supports this claim, this approach misleads researchers to neglect the fact that social presence may not always yield positive outcomes. This is an important issue to consider when designing communication systems; more social presence may not always be better (Allmendinger, 2010). Therefore, it is necessary to consider the characteristics of the communicator as well as the context in order to leverage the unique possibility of offering varying levels of social presence within virtual environments.
Attempts to increase social presence may lead to negative communication outcomes when the communicator is a person who feels discomfort during social interactions. Individuals who have high social anxiety or communication apprehension are generally uncomfortable in the presence of people. As such, these individuals prefer to withdraw from social situations and stay in the background, rather than engaging in the conversation (Allmendinger, 2010; Cortese and Seo, 2012). Consequently, they may feel more comfortable when the social presence of their communication partners is low, rather than when it is high. The fact that individuals who do not value or enjoy social interactions (e.g., shy, high communication apprehension, weaker need to belong, etc.) feel less social presence than their more social counterparts (Giannopoulos et al., 2008; Jin, 2010; Cortese and Seo, 2012; Kim et al., 2013a) offers some empirical evidence that socially withdrawn individuals may be less motivated to attend to social cues that enhance social presence. Directly germane to this hypothesis, studies (Joinson, 2004; Hertel et al., 2008; Hammick and Lee, 2014) consistently show that less socially oriented individuals prefer interacting through a medium that is considered to be “leaner” (e.g., text-based CMC), while more socially oriented individuals prefer to interact via a “richer” modality (e.g., FtF). Similarly, Poeschl (2017) found that perceiving the virtual audience to be more socially present tended to lead to a worse speech-giving performance.
The desirability of social presence may also differ depending on the interaction context. Studies suggest that higher levels of social presence are more beneficial in equivocal contexts wherein there is no “correct” outcome, such as negotiations (Daft and Lengel, 1986; Garau et al., 2003). In contrast, it is possible that people will prefer lower levels of social presence when they are feeling more vulnerable; the success of text-based counseling and support systems (Dinakar et al., 2015) lends some support to this conjecture. Taken together, these studies suggest that attempting to increase social presence may not have uniformly positive results; rather, special attention should be paid to the communication preferences and goals of the interactants.
In addition to individual traits, social presence may have differential communication outcomes depending on one's attitude toward his or her communication partner. That is, while increasing the salience of a neutral or likable communication partner may increase positive social outcomes, enhancing the social presence of a disliked communication partner might lead to less desirable results. As Lee and Shin (2012) argue, increased social presence of a disliked target can escalate the negative thoughts associated with him or her, which may in turn amplify prior attitudes toward the target. The fact that gamers felt more hostile and were more verbally aggressive toward their opponent when they experienced stronger levels of presence during a violent game (Nowak et al., 2008) offers some support to this hypothesis. Lee and Shin (2012) also found that while higher social presence of a high-profile politician led to stronger agreement with his opinions when participants liked him, this was not the case when participants did not have positive pre-dispositions toward him.
Considering these boundary conditions of the benefits of increased social presence, researchers should focus not only on the predictors of social presence, but also the interpersonal outcomes of enhanced social presence. Doing so will offer a more holistic view of social presence that will allow for a better understanding of when it is (and is not) desirable for a virtual environment to adopt immersive and contextual qualities that will increase social presence.
Limitations
There are several limitations in the current study. First the research that was reviewed often used different measures of social presence, which limits their comparability. It is thus important for researchers to note the different measures used (available in Table 2), to contextualize the findings of each study that was reviewed. We chose not to conduct a quantitative meta-analysis due to the variability of measures, as we believed this approach would lead to the exclusion of a number of important studies.
In addition, we did not assess the quality of each study that was included in this review; rather, we assumed that the findings of each study were true and correct. However, we included the publication outlet, number of citations, and the impact factor of the publication outlet (when available) in addition to the number of participants in Table 3. While these factors are not definitive criteria in determining the quality of a study, we hope that they will help readers better understand the nature of each study.
Another limitation of the present study is that we were unable to include concepts that share theoretical similarities with social presence. While this decision was made to achieve a higher level of internal validity, it led to the exclusion of research on important concepts, one of which is plausibility illusion (Slater, 2009; Slater et al., 2010). Plausibility illusion research significantly contributes to understanding when and how people respond to virtual others as “real” people, as it encapsulates the extent to which one feels as if the depicted events are actually occurring. In contrast to “the sense of being there” (i.e., place illusion), which tends to be contingent on the technological characteristics of the environment, Plausibility illusion concerns the credibility of a scenario, and thus is not dependent on the sensory capabilities of a virtual environment (Slater and Sanchez-Vives, 2016; Gonzalez-Franco and Peck, 2018). Although plausibility illusion is not identical to the concept of social presence (see Methods section), it can inform social presence researchers on why higher levels of immersion do not universally lead to higher levels of social presence. More importantly, plausibility illusion research can offer insight into when and how non-technological factors (e.g., mimicry, task type, etc.) influence the believability of the virtual human. In one study on bystander effects in a virtual bar, for example, participants reported contextual factors (e.g., appearance of bar, responsiveness of other characters, believability of dialogue with victim) as issues that brought them out of the virtual experience (“breaks in presence” Slater and Steed, 2000; Slater et al., 2013). Researchers have also explored the impact of the personality of the virtual human (Pan et al., 2015), level of coherence to the user's expectations (Skarbez et al., 2017), and the physicality of the virtual human (Chuah et al., 2013) on plausibility illusion.
Our study also did not explore the how the actual agency of the target influences social presence; while we did review studies that examined how agency affects social presence, they addressed perceived, rather than actual agency. While manipulating perceived agency does maximize internal validity, it reduces some of the external validity, given that avatars and agents are likely to behave differently outside of the laboratory. Although this is beyond the scope of the present study, we have included a column in Table 2 that notes whether or not the evaluation target in each study was an actual person or a computer algorithm.
Finally, the present study did not address potential moderators that could influence the impact of each feature on social presence. As we discussed above, both individual and contextual factors may moderate the findings of our systematic review. Future studies would benefit from exploring potential moderators and their relative effects.
Conclusion and Future Directions
Despite its potential drawbacks, social presence is a critical experience within networked environments. While increased social presence may not always lead to positive results, multiple studies show that the vivid perceptions of another person often lead to greater enjoyment and social influence in neutral and positive contexts (e.g., Fogg and Tseng, 1999; Hassanein and Head, 2007). Hence, a considerable amount of scholarly efforts have been made to identify factors that increase feelings of social presence, as we have found in the present paper. By reviewing these studies, we were able to identify immersive, contextual, and individual qualities that impact perceptions of social presence.
It is important to note, however, that due to the period during which they were conducted, many of these studies employed limited technology, and thus do not address the implications of recent technological advancements. For example, many VR systems now offer inverse kinematics, the prediction of joint movements based on the position(s) of a limited set of trackers. Considering the fact that both gesture and posture have a significant influence on person perception in FtF contexts (Riggio and Friedman, 1986), it is possible that this added layer of technology in CMC will impact experiences of social presence. However, this possibility has not been fully explored within the current social presence literature. Similarly, studies have also failed to explore the implications of rendering expressions that are driven by facial motion tracking data, another recent technological development. These research questions are important both from a theoretical and applied standpoint. From a theoretical point of view, these questions offer insight into the social cues that are necessary to induce feelings of a “social being,” or what it means for a virtual entity to “appear human.” In addition, these questions allow us to explore how immersive VR systems that support unprecedentedly high levels of behavioral realism influence social presence. From an applied point of view, this research will allow system designers to understand how to allocate resources when developing networking systems.
Future studies on social presence would also benefit from considering plausibility illusion research when formulating hypotheses. In addition, more empirical research is needed on the theoretical similarities and differences between social presence and plausibility illusion. For example, while there is evidence that the personality of the virtual human (e.g., friendliness, empathy, etc.) influences social presence, it is less probable that these features will influence plausibility illusion, or how believable they find the virtual human's behavior to be. Lending some support to this conjecture, Pan et al. (2015) found that the shyness of a virtual human did not influence perceptions of plausibility illusion. In contrast, it is reasonable to conjecture that behavioral realism will positively influence both social presence and plausibility illusion. This line of research can aid in creating a more cohesive theoretical framework for presence and its components, fostering fruitful intra- and inter-disciplinary discussions between VR researchers.
In addition, future studies should offer a more holistic view of social presence by considering the different dimensions that impact social presence. Just as studies found that increasing the behavioral realism of a virtual human that had low photographic realism did not lead to increased social presence (Garau et al., 2003), it would be beneficial to consider boundary conditions (e.g., contextual, individual) of the findings available in the current literature. One understudied, but important, boundary condition is the relationship between the conversation partners. Given that technological features such as audio delays differentially influence communication outcomes depending on the relationship between the partners (Koudenburg et al., 2014), this avenue of research may help researchers and practitioners understand how to design social VR systems when individuals are already acquainted with each other. Furthermore, considering that multiple studies reviewed in this paper show that increasing immersive qualities does not linearly increase social presence (e.g., Moreno and Mayer, 2004; Sallnäs, 2005; Homer et al., 2008), it would be critical to understand if, and if so when, there is a ceiling effect of immersion on social presence.
Lanier (2014) noted that a good VR system should be “good enough to fool you, to engage your whole body, to include others as avatars with you in there, to be usable in the long term, and giving you enough to do to outlast the first few demos” (p. xiii). However, he cautions that such high quality VR is still only available at a limited number of places. With the popularization of VR at the horizon, it is essential for both academic and industrial researchers to increase their understanding of what helps create the sense of being there with other people in this space of “consensual hallucination” (Gibson, 1984, p. 51).
Author Contributions
CO, JB, and GW contributed to the conception and design of the study. CO organized and reviewed the database of social presence studies. CO wrote the first draft of the manuscript. CO, JB, and GW contributed to the manuscript revision, read and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This project was funded in part from an academic research gift from Google. We also acknowledge support from the Office of Naval Research (ONR), code 30, under Dr. Peter Squire, Program Manager (ONR award N00014-17-1-2927) and the National Science Foundation (NSF) under grant IIS-1800961.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We acknowledge Florida Hospital for their support of GW via their Endowed Chair in Healthcare Simulation. We would also like to thank Ryan Burns, Jim Cummings, and Tobin Asher for their valuable input regarding this project.
References
Ahn, D., Seo, Y., Kim, M., Kwon, J. H., Jung, Y., Ahn, J., et al. (2014). The effects of actual human size display and stereoscopic presentation on users' sense of being together with and of psychological immersion in a virtual character. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 17, 483–487. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0455
Alge, B. J., Wiethoff, C., and Klein, H. J. (2003). When does the medium matter? Knowledge-building experiences and opportunities in decision-making teams. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Proces. 91, 26–37. doi: 10.1016/S0749-5978(02)00524-1
Alghamdi, M., Regenbrecht, H., Hoermann, S., Langlotz, T., and Aldridge, C. (2016). “Social presence and mode of videocommunication in a collaborative virtual environment,” in Proceedings of the 20th Pacific Asia Conference on Information Systems. (Chiayi: AISeL).
Allmendinger, K. (2010). Social presence in synchronous virtual learning situations: the role of nonverbal signals displayed by avatars. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 22, 41–56. doi: 10.1007/s10648-010-9117-8
Al-Natour, S., Benbasat, I., and Cenfetelli, R. (2011). The adoption of online shopping assistants: Perceived similarity as an antecedent to evaluative beliefs. J. Assoc. Inform. Syst. 12, 347–374. doi: 10.17705/1jais.00267
Altman, I., and Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social Penetration: The Development of Interpersonal Relationships. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Antheunis, M. L., Valkenburg, P. M., and Peter, J. (2010). Getting acquainted through social network sites: testing a model of online uncertainty reduction and social attraction. Comput. Human Behav. 26, 100–109. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2009.07.005
Appel, J., von der Pütten, A., Krämer, N. C., and Gratch, J. (2012). Does humanity matter? analyzing the importance of social cues and perceived agency of a computer system for the emergence of social reactions during human-computer interaction. Adv. Hum. Computer Inter. 13:324694. doi: 10.1155/2012/324694
Axelsson, A. S., Abelin, Å., Heldal, I., Schroeder, R., and Wideström, J. (2001). Cubes in the cube: a comparison of a puzzle-solving task in a virtual and a real environment. CyberPsychol. Behav. 4, 279–286. doi: 10.1089/109493101300117956
Aymerich-Franch, L., Karutz, C., and Bailenson, J. N. (2012). “Effects of facial and voice similarity on presence in a public speaking virtual environment,” in Proceedings of the International Society for Presence Research Annual Conference (Philadelphia, PA).
Bailenson, J. N., Blascovich, J., Beall, A. C., and Loomis, J. M. (2001). Equilibrium theory revisited: mutual gaze and personal space in virtual environments. Pres. Teleoperat.Virtu. Environ. 10, 583–598. doi: 10.1162/105474601753272844
Bailenson, J. N., Blascovich, J., Beall, A. C., and Loomis, J. M. (2003). Interpersonal distance in immersive virtual environments. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 819–833. doi: 10.1177/0146167203029007002
Bailenson, J. N., Swinth, K. R., Hoyt, C. L., Persky, S., Dimov, A., and Blascovich, J. (2005). The independent and interactive effects of embodied-agent appearance and behavior on self-report, cognitive, and behavioral markers of copresence in immersive virtual environments. Pres. Teleoperat.Virtu. Environ. 14, 379–393. doi: 10.1162/105474605774785235
Bailenson, J. N., and Yee, N. (2005). Digital chameleons automatic assimilation of nonverbal gestures in immersive virtual environments. Psychol. Sci. 16, 814–819. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01619.x
Bailenson, J. N., Yee, N., Merget, D., and Schroeder, R. (2006). The effect of behavioral realism and form realism of real-time avatar faces on verbal disclosure, nonverbal disclosure, emotion recognition, and copresence in dyadic interaction. Pres. Teleoperat.Virtu. Environ. 15, 359–372. doi: 10.1162/pres.15.4.359
Basdogan, C., Ho, C. H., Srinivasan, M. A., and Slater, M. (2000). An experimental study on the role of touch in shared virtual environments. ACM Trans. Comp, Hum. Interact., 7, 443–460. doi: 10.1145/365058.365082
Bente, G., Eschenburg, F., and Aelker, L. (2007). “Effects of simulated gaze on social presence, person perception and personality attribution in avatar-mediated communication,” in Proceedings of the 10th Annual International Workshop on Presence (Barcelona).
Bente, G., Rüggenberg, S., Krämer, N. C., and Eschenburg, F. (2008). Avatar-mediated networking: increasing social presence and interpersonal trust in net-based collaborations. Hum. Commun. Res. 34, 287–318. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2008.00322.x
Biocca, F. (1997). The cyborg's dilemma: progressive embodiment in virtual environments. J. Comp. Mediated Commun. 3:JCMC324. doi: 10.1111/j.1083-6101.1997.tb00070.x
Biocca, F., and Harms, C. (2002). “Defining and measuring social presence: Contribution to the networked minds theory and measure,” in Proceedings of the 10th Annual International Workshop on Presence (Porto).
Biocca, F., Harms, C., and Burgoon, J. K. (2003). Toward a more robust theory and measure of social presence: review and suggested criteria. Pres. Teleoperat.Virtu. Environ. 12, 456–480. doi: 10.1162/105474603322761270
Biocca, F., Harms, C., and Gregg, J. (2001). “The networked minds measure of social presence: pilot test of the factor structure and concurrent validity,” in Proceedings of the 4th Annual International Workshop on Presence (Philadelphia, PA).
Biocca, F., and Levy, M. R. (eds.). (2013). Communication in the Age of Virtual Reality. London: Routledge.
Blascovich, J., Loomis, J., Beall, A. C., Swinth, K. R., Hoyt, C. L., and Bailenson, J. N. (2002). Immersive virtual environment technology as a methodological tool for social psychology. Psychol. Inq. 13, 103–124. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1302_01
Borgers, N., Sikkel, D., and Hox, J. (2004). Response effects in surveys on children and adolescents: The effect of number of response options, negative wording, and neutral mid-point. Quality Quantity 38, 17–33. doi: 10.1023/B:QUQU.0000013236.29205.a6
Bouchard, S., Bernier, F., Boivin, E., Dumoulin, S., Laforest, M., Guitard, T, et al. (2013). Empathy toward virtual humans depicting a known or unknown person expressing pain. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 16, 61–71. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.1571
Bracken, C. C. (2005). Presence and image quality: the case of high-definition television. Media Psychol. 7, 191–205. doi: 10.1207/S1532785XMEP0702_4
Casanueva, J. S., and Blake, E. H. (2001). “The effects of avatars on co-presence in a collaborative virtual environment,”in Annual Conference of the South African Institute of Computer Scientists and Information Technologists (SAICSIT2001) (Pretoria).
Chellali, A., Dumas, C., and Milleville-Pennel, I. (2011). Influences of haptic communication on a shared manual task. Interact. Comput. 23, 317–328. doi: 10.1016/j.intcom.2011.05.002
Cho, Y. H., Yim, S. Y., and Paik, S. (2015). Physical and social presence in 3D virtual role-play for pre-service teachers. Internet Higher Educ. 25, 70–77. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2015.01.002
Choi, J. J., and Kwak, S. S. (2017). Who is this? identity and presence in robot-mediated communication. Cogn. Syst. Res. 43, 174–189. doi: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2016.07.006
Choi, Y. K., Miracle, G. E., and Biocca, F. (2001). The effects of anthropomorphic agents on advertising effectiveness and the mediating role of presence. J. Interact. Advert. 2, 19–32. doi: 10.1080/15252019.2001.10722055
Christie, B. (1974). Perceived usefulness of person-person telecommunications media as a function of the intended application. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 4, 366–368. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420040307
Chuah, J. H., Robb, A., White, C., Wendling, A., Lampotang, S., Kopper, R., et al. (2013). Exploring agent physicality and social presence for medical team training. Pre. Teleoperat. Virtual Environ. 22, 141–170. doi: 10.1162/PRES_a_00145
Clayes, E. L., and Anderson, A. H. (2007). Real faces and robot faces: the effects of representation on computer-mediated communication. Int. J. Hum. Comp. Studies, 65, 480–496. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2006.10.005
Cortese, J., and Seo, M. (2012). The role of social presence in opinion expression during FtF and CMC discussions. Commun. Res. Rep. 29, 44–53. doi: 10.1080/08824096.2011.639913
Croes, E. A., Antheunis, M. L., Schouten, A. P., and Krahmer, E. J. (2016). Teasing apart the effect of visibility and physical co-presence to examine the effect of CMC on interpersonal attraction. Comput. Human Behav. 55, 468–476. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.09.037
Cuddetta, M., Francescato, D., Porcelli, R., and Renzi, P. (2003). Scala di Presenza Sociale. Unpublished scale. Department of Psychology, University of Rome La Sapienza. Rome.
Cummings, J. J., and Bailenson, J. N. (2016). How immersive is enough? a meta-analysis of the effect of immersive technology on user presence. Media Psychol. 19, 272–309. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2015.1015740
Cummings, J. J., Bailenson, J. N., and Fidler, M. J. (2012). “How immersive is enough? A foundation for a meta-analysis of the effect of immersive technology on measured presence,” in Proceedings of the International Society for Presence Research Annual Conference (Philadelphia, PA).
Cummings, J. J., and Wertz, B. (2018). “Technological predictors of social presence: A foundation for a meta-analytic review and empirical concept explication,” in Proceedings of the 10th Annual International Workshop on Presence (Prague).
Daft, R. L., and Lengel, R. H. (1986). Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Manage. Sci. 32, 554–571. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.32.5.554
Daher, S., Kim, K., Lee, M., Raij, A., Schubert, R., Bailenson, J., et al. (2016). “Exploring social presence transfer in real-virtual human interaction,” in Proceedings of IEEE Virtual Reality (Greenville, SC: IEEE).
Dalzel-Job, S. (2014). Social Interaction in Virtual Environments: the Relationship Between Mutual Gaze, Task Performance And Social Presence. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University of Edinburgh, Scotland.
de Greef, H. P. (2014). “Video communication best for female friends?” in Proceedings of the International Society for Presence Research Annual Conference (Vienna).
de Greef, H. P., and Ijsselsteijn, W. A. (2001). Social presence in a home tele-application. CyberPsychol. Behav. 4, 307–315. doi: 10.1089/109493101300117974
DeSchryver, M., Mishra, P., Koehleer, M., and Francis, A. (2009). “Moodle vs. Facebook: Does using Facebook for discussions in an online course enhance perceived social presence and student interaction?” in Proceedings of the Society for Information Technology & Teacher Education International Conference (Chesapeake, VA: Association for the Advancement of Computing in Education), 329–336.
Dicke, C., Aaltonen, V., Räm,ö, A., and Vilermo, M. (2010). Talk to me: “The influence of audio quality on the perception of social presence,” in Proceedings of the 24th BCS Interaction Specialist Group Conference (Swindon: BCS Learning & Development).
Dinakar, K., Chen, J., Lieberman, H., Picard, R., and Filbin, R. (2015). “Mixed-initiative real-time topic modeling & visualization for crisis counseling, March 29-April 1,” in Proceedings of the 20th International Conference on Intelligent User Interfaces (New York, NY: ACM).
Felnhofer, A., Kafka, J. X., Hlavacs, H., Beutl, L., Kryspin-Exner, I., and Kothgassner, O. D. (2018). Meeting others virtually in a day-to-day setting: investigating social avoidance and prosocial behavior towards avatars and agents. Comp. Hum. Behav. 80, 399–406. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.11.031
Felnhofer, A., Kothgassner, O. D., Hauk, N., Beutl, L., Hlavacs, H., and Kryspin-Exner, I. (2014). Physical and social presence in collaborative virtual environments: exploring age and gender differences with respect to empathy. Comput. Hum. Behav. 31, 272–279. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.045
Feng, B., Li, S., and Li, N. (2016). Is a profile worth a thousand words? How online support-seeker's profile features may influence the quality of received support messages. Commun. Res. 43, 253–276. doi: 10.1177/0093650213510942
Festinger, L. (1962). Cognitive dissonance. Sci. Am. 207, 93–106. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican1062-93
Fogg, B. J., and Tseng, H. (1999). “The elements of computer credibility,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (New York, NY: ACM).
Fortin, D. R., and Dholakia, R. R. (2005). Interactivity and vividness effects on social presence and involvement with a web-based advertisement. J. Bus. Res. 58, 387–396. doi: 10.1016/S0148-2963(03)00106-1
Fox, J., Ahn, S. J., Janssen, J. H., Yeykelis, L., Segovia, K. Y., and Bailenson, J. N. (2015). Avatars versus agents: a meta-analysis quantifying the effect of agency on social influence. Hum. Comp. Inter. 30, 401–432. doi: 10.1080/07370024.2014.921494
Francescato, D., Porcelli, R., Mebane, M., Cuddetta, M., Klobas, J., and Renzi, P. (2006). Evaluation of the efficacy of collaborative learning in face-to-face and computer-supported university contexts. Comput. Hum. Behav. 22, 163–176. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2005.03.001
Friedman, D., Brogni, A., Guger, C., Antley, A., Steed, A., and Slater, M. (2006). Sharing and analyzing data from presence experiments. Pre. Teleoper. Virtual Environ. 15, 599–610. doi: 10.1162/pres.15.5.599
Gajadhar, B. J., De Kort, Y. A., and Ijsselsteijn, W. A. (2008). “Shared fun is doubled fun: player enjoyment as a function of social setting,” in Fun and Games (Berlin: Springer), 106–117.
Garau, M., Slater, M., Bee, S., and Sasse, M. A. (2001). “The impact of eye gaze on communication using humanoid avatars,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (Seattle, WA).
Garau, M., Slater, M., Pertaub, D. P., and Razzaque, S. (2005). The responses of people to virtual humans in an immersive virtual environment. Pre. Teleoperat. Virtual Environ. 14, 104–116. doi: 10.1162/1054746053890242
Garau, M., Slater, M., Vinayagamoorthy, V., Brogni, A., Steed, A., and Sasse, M. A. (2003). “The impact of avatar realism and eye gaze control on perceived quality of communication in a shared immersive virtual environment,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (New York, NY: ACM).
Gefen, D., and Straub, D. (2003). Managing user trust in B2C e-services. e-Service 2, 7–24. doi: 10.2979/esj.2003.2.2.7
Gerhard, M., Moore, D., and Hobbs, D. (2001). “Continuous presence in collaborative virtual environments: Towards the evaluation of a hybrid avatar-agent model for user representation,” in Proceedings of the International Conference on Intelligent Virtual Agents (Madrid).
Giannopoulos, E., Eslava, V., Oyarzabal, M., Hierro, T., González, L., Ferre, M., et al. (2008). “The effect of haptic feedback on basic social interaction within shared virtual environments,” in Haptics: Perception, Devices and Scenarios. (Berlin: Springer).
Gimpel, H., Huber, J., and Sarikaya, S. (2016). “Customer satisfaction in digital service encounters: The role of media richness, social presence, and cultural distance,” in Proceedings of the 24th European Conference on Information Systems (Istanbul).
Gong, L. (2008). How social is social responses to computers? the function of the degree of anthropomorphism in computer representations. Comp. Hum. Behav. 24, 1494–1509. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2007.05.007
Gong, L., Appiah, O., and Elias, T. (2007). “See minorities through the lens of ethnic identity: Reflected unto racial representations of real humans and virtual humans,” in 93rd Annual Convention of the National Communication Association (Chicago, IL).
Gonzalez-Franco, M. G. F., and Peck, T. C. (2018). Avatar embodiment. Towards a standardized questionnaire. Front. Robotics AI 5:74. doi: 10.3389/frobt.2018.00074
Guadagno, R. E., Blascovich, J., Bailenson, J. N., and Mccall, C. (2007). Virtual humans and persuasion: the effects of agency and behavioral realism. Media Psychol. 10, 1–22. doi: 10.108/15213260701300865
Guadagno, R. E., Swinth, K. R., and Blascovich, J. (2011). Social evaluations of embodied agents and avatars. Comp. Hum. Beh. 27, 2380–2385. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2011.07.017
Gunawardena, C. N. (1995). Social presence theory and implications for interaction and collaborative learning in computer conferences. Int. J. Educ. Telecommun. 1, 147–166. Available online at: https://www.learntechlib.org/p/15156/
Gunawardena, C. N., and Zittle, F. J. (1997). Social presence as a predictor of satisfaction within a computer-mediated conferencing environment. Am. J. Dist. Edu. 11, 8–26. doi: 10.1080/08923649709526970
Hammick, J. K., and Lee, M. J. (2014). Do shy people feel less communication apprehension online? the effects of virtual reality on the relationship between personality characteristics and communication outcomes. Comput. Human Behav. 33, 302–310. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.01.046
Han, S., Min, J., and Lee, H. (2016). Building relationships within corporate SNS accounts through social presence formation. Int. J. Inform. Manag. 36, 945–962. doi: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2016.06.004
Harms, C., and Biocca, F. (2004). “Internal consistency and reliability of the networked minds social presence measure,” in Proceedings of the 7th Annual International Workshop on Presence (Valencia).
Harris, H., Bailenson, J. N., Nielsen, A., and Yee, N. (2009). The evolution of social behavior over time in second life. Pres. Teleoperat.Virtu. Environ. 18, 434–448. doi: 10.1162/pres.18.6.434
Hassanein, K., and Head, M. (2007). Manipulating perceived social presence through the web interface and its impact on attitude towards online shopping. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 65, 689–708. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2006.11.018
Hatta, T., and Ken-ichi, O. (2008). Effects of visual cue and spatial distance on exitability in electronic negotiation. Comp. Hum. Behav. 24, 1542–1551. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2007.05.008
Hauber, J., Regenbrecht, H., Billinghurst, M., and Cockburn, A. (2006). “Spatiality in videoconferencing: trade-offs between efficiency and social presence,” in Proceedings of the Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work (New York, NY: ACM).
Hauber, J., Regenbrecht, H., Cockburn, A., and Billinghurst, M. (2012). The impact of collaborative style on the perception of 2D and 3D videoconferencing interfaces. Open Software Eng. J. 6, 1–20. doi: 10.2174/1874107X01206010001
Hauber, J., Regenbrecht, H., Hills, A., Cockburn, A., and Billinghurst, M. (2005). “Social presence in two-and three-dimensional videoconferencing,” in Proceedings of 8th Annual International Workshop on Presence. (London).
Hayes, A. (2015). The Experience of Physical and Social Presence in a Virtual Learning Environment as Impacted by the Affordance of Movement Enabled by Motion Tracking. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Central Florida, USA.
Heidicker, P., Langbehn, E., and Steinicke, F. (2017). “Influence of avatar appearance on presence in social VR,” in Proceedings of the IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces. (Los Angeles, CA: IEEE).
Heldal, I., Schroeder, R., Steed, A., Axelsson, A. S., Spant, M., and Wideström, J. (2005). “Immersiveness and symmetry in copresent scenarios,” in Proceedings of IEEE VR (New York, NY: IEEE).
Herrewijn, L., and Poels, K. (2015). The impact of social setting on the recall and recognition of in-game advertising. Comput. Human Behav. 53, 544–555. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.06.012
Hertel, G., Schroer, J., Batinic, B., and Naumann, S. (2008). Do shy people prefer to send e-mail? Personality effects on communication media preferences in threatening and nonthreatening situations. Soc. Psychol. 39, 231–243. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335.39.4.231
Hills, A. (2005). Social Presence and Communication Quality in Videoconferencing. Unpublished bachelor dissertation, University of Otago, New Zealand.
Hills, A., Hauber, J., and Regenbrecht, H. (2005). “Videos in space: a study on presence in video mediating communication systems,” in Proceedings of the 2005 International Conference on Augmented Tele-Existence (New York, NY: ACM).
Homer, B. D., Plass, J. L., and Blake, L. (2008). The effects of video on cognitive load and social presence in multimedia-learning. Comput. Human Behav. 24, 786–797. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2007.02.009
Horvath, K., and Lombard, M. (2010). Social and spatial presence: an application to optimize human-computer interaction. PsychNol. J. 8, 85–114. Available online at: http://www.psychnology.org/File/PNJ8(1)/PSYCHNOLOGY_JOURNAL_8_1_HORVATH.pdf
Hostetter, C., and Busch, M. (2006). Measuring up online: the relationship between social presence and student learning satisfaction. J. Scholarsh. Teach. Learn. 6, 1–12. Available online at: https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ854921.pdf
Hoyt, C. L., Blascovich, J., and Swinth, K. R. (2003). Social inhibition in immersive virtual environments. Pres. Teleoperat.Virtu. Environ. 12, 183–195. doi: 10.1162/105474603321640932
IJsselsteijn, W., Van Den Hoogen, W., Klimmt, C., De Kort, Y., Lindley, C., Mathiak, K., et al. (2008). “Measuring the experience of digital game enjoyment,” in Proceedings of Measuring Behavior (Wageningen).
James, C. A., Haustein, K., Bednarz, T. P., Alem, L., Caris, C., and Castleden, A. (2011). Remote operation of mining equipment using panoramic display systems: Exploring the sense of presence. Ergonom. Open J. 4, 93–102. doi: 10.2174/1875934301104010093
Järvelä, S., Kätsyri, J., Ravaja, N., Chanel, G., and Henttonen, P. (2016). Intragroup emotions: physiological linkage and social presence. Front. Psychol. 7:105. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00105
Jin, S. A. A. (2009). Modality effects in second life: the mediating role of social presence and the moderating role of product involvement. CyberPsychol. Behav. 12, 717–721. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2008.0273
Jin, S. A. A. (2010). Parasocial interaction with an avatar in second life: a typology of the self and an empirical test of the mediating role of social presence. Pres. Teleoperat.Virtu. Environ. 19, 331–340. doi: 10.1162/PRES_a_00001
Jin, S. A. A. (2011). “I feel present. Therefore, I experience flow”: a structural equation modeling approach to flow and presence in video games. J. Broadcast. Electronic Media. 55, 114–136. doi: 10.1080/08838151.2011.546248
Jin, S. A. A. (2012). The virtual malleable self and the virtual identity discrepancy model: investigative frameworks for virtual possible selves and others in avatar-based identity construction and social interaction. Comput. Human Behav. 28, 2160–2168. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.06.022
Johnsen, K., and Lok, B. (2008). “An evaluation of immersive displays for virtual human experiences,” in Proceedings of IEEE VR(New York, NY: IEEE).
Johnson, D. M., and Stewart, J. E. (1999). Use of virtual environments for the acquisition of spatial knowledge: Comparison among different visual displays. Military Psychol. 11:129. doi: 10.1207/s15327876mp1102_1
Johnson, R. D. (2011). Gender differences in e-learning: communication, social presence, and learning outcomes. J. Organ. End User Comput. 23, 79–94. doi: 10.4018/joeuc.2011010105
Joinson, A. N. (2004). Self-esteem, interpersonal risk, and preference for e-mail to face-to-face communication. CyberPsychol. Behav. 7, 472–478. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2004.7.472
Jordan, J., Mortensen, J., Oliveira, M., Slater, M., Tay, B. K., Kim, J., et al. (2002). “Collaboration in a mediated haptic environment,” in Proceedings of the 5th Annual International Workshop on Presence. (Porto).
Jung, S., Roh, S., Yang, H., and Biocca, F. (2017). Location and modality effects in online dating: rich modality profile and location-based information cues increase social presence, while moderating the impact of uncertainty reduction strategy. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 20, 553–560. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2017.0027
Kang, S. H., and Gratch, J. (2014). Exploring users' social responses to computer counseling interviewers' behavior. Comput. Human Behav. 34, 120–130. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.006
Kang, S. H., and Watt, J. H. (2013). The impact of avatar realism and anonymity on effective communication via mobile devices. Comp. Hum. Behav. 29, 1169–1181. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.10.010
Kang, S. H., Watt, J. H., and Ala, S. K. (2008). “Communicators' perceptions of social presence as a function of avatar realism in small display mobile communication devices,” in Proceedings of the 41st Annual Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences (New York, NY: IEEE).
Kim, D., Frank, M. G., and Kim, S. T. (2014). Emotional display behavior in different forms of computer mediated communication. Comput. Human Behav. 30, 222–229. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.09.001
Kim, H., Suh, K. S., and Lee, U. K. (2013b). Effects of collaborative online shopping on shopping experience through social and relational perspectives. Inform. Manag. 50, 169–180. doi: 10.1016/j.im.2013.02.003
Kim, H. S., and Sundar, S. S. (2014). Can online buddies and bandwagon cues enhance user participation in online health communities? Comput. Hum. Behav. 37, 319–333. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.039
Kim, J., Kim, H., Tay, B. K., Muniyandi, M., Srinivasan, M. A., Jordan, J., et al. (2004). Transatlantic touch: a study of haptic collaboration over long distance. Pres. Teleoperat.Virtu. Environ. 13, 328–337. doi: 10.1162/1054746041422370
Kim, J., and Timmerman, C. E. (2018). Effects of supportive feedback messages on exergame experiences: a mediating role of social presence. J. Media Psychol. 30, 29–40. doi: 10.1027/1864-1105/a000175
Kim, K., Bolton, J., Girouard, A., Cooperstock, J., and Vertegaal, R. (2012). “TeleHuman: effects of 3d perspective on gaze and pose estimation with a life-size cylindrical telepresence pod,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. Austin, TX: ACM.
Kim, K., Bruder, G., Maloney, D., and Welch, G. (2016). “The influence of real human personality on social presence with a virtual human in augmented reality,” in Proceedings of the Combined International Conference on Artificial Reality & Telexistence and Eurographics Symposium on Virtual Environments (ICAT-EGVE), (Aire-la-Ville: Eurographics Association).
Kim, K., Maloney, D., Bruder, G., Bailenson, J. N., and Welch, G. F. (2017). The effects of virtual human's spatial and behavioral coherence with physical objects on social presence in AR. Comput. Anim. Virtual Worlds 28:e1771. doi: 10.1002/cav.1771
Kim, K. J. (2016). Interacting socially with the Internet of Things (IoT): effects of source attribution and specialization in human–IoT interaction. J. Comp. Mediat. Commun. 21, 420–435. doi: 10.1111/jcc4.12177
Kim, K. J., Park, E., and Sundar, S. S. (2013a). Caregiving role in human–robot interaction: a study of the mediating effects of perceived benefit and social presence. Comput. Human Behav. 29, 1799–1806. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.009
Kim, T., and Biocca, F. (1997). Telepresence via television: two dimensions of telepresence may have different connections to memory and persuasion. J. Comput. Mediat. Commun. 3:JCMC325. doi: 10.1111/j.1083-6101.1997.tb00073.x
Kim, Y., and Sundar, S. S. (2012). Anthropomorphism of computers: Is it mindful or mindless? Comput. Human Behav. 28, 241–250. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2011.09.006
Kothgassner, O. D., Griesinger, M., Kettner, K., Wayan, K., Völkl-Kernstock, S., Hlavacs, H. et al. (2017). Real-life prosocial behavior decreases after being socially excluded by avatars, not agents. Comput. Human Behav. 70, 261–269. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.12.059
Kothgassner, O. D., Kafka, J. X., Rudyk, J., Beutl, L., Hlavacs, H., and Felnhofer, A. (2014). “Does social exclusion hurt virtually like it hurts in real-life? the role of agency and social presence in the perception and experience of social exclusion,” in Proceedings of the International Society for Presence Research Annual Conference (Vienna).
Koudenburg, N., Gordijn, E. H., and Postmes, T. (2014). “More than words”: social validation in close relationships. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 40, 1517–1528. doi: 10.1177/0146167214549945
Kumar, N., and Benbasat, I. (2002). “Para-social presence: a re-conceptualization of “social presence” to capture the relationship between a web site and her visitors,” in Proceedings of the 35th Annual Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences (Big Island, HI).
Lanier, J. (2014). “Perspective of Jaron Lanier,” in Handbook of Virtual Environments, eds K. Hale and K. M. Stanney (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press), xiii–xvii
Lanier, J. (2017). Dawn of the New Everything: Encounters With Reality And Virtual Reality. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Company.
Lee, E. J. (2013). Effectiveness of politicians' soft campaign on Twitter versus TV: cognitive and experiential routes. J. Commun. 63, 953–974. doi: 10.1111/jcom.12049
Lee, E. J., and Jang, J. W. (2013). Not so imaginary interpersonal contact with public figures on social network sites: how affiliative tendency moderates its effects. Commun. Res. 40, 27–51. doi: 10.1177/0093650211431579
Lee, E. J., and Oh, S. Y. (2012). To personalize or depersonalize? when and how politicians' personalized tweets affect the public's reactions. J. Commun. 62, 932–949. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01681.x
Lee, E. J., and Shin, S. Y. (2012). Are they talking to me? Cognitive and affective effects of interactivity in politicians' Twitter communication. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network. 15, 515–520. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0228
Lee, E. J., and Shin, S. Y. (2014). When the medium is the message how transportability moderates the effects of politicians' Twitter communication. Commu. Res. 41, 1088–1110. doi: 10.1177/0093650212466407
Lee, K. M. (2004). Presence, explicated. Commun. Theory 14, 27–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2885.2004.tb00302.x
Lee, K. M., Jeong, E. J., Park, N., and Ryu, S. (2007). “Effects of networked interactivity in educational games: Mediating effects of social presence,” in Proceedings of the 10th Annual International Workshop on Presence (Barcelona).
Lee, K. M., Jeong, E. J., Park, N., and Ryu, S. (2011). Effects of interactivity in educational games: A mediating role of social presence on learning outcomes. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact. 27, 620–633. doi: 10.1080/10447318.2011.555302
Lee, K. M., Jung, Y., Kim, J., and Kim, S. R. (2006a). Are physically embodied social agents better than disembodied social agents? The effects of physical embodiment, tactile interaction, and people's loneliness in human–robot interaction. Int. J. Hum. Comp. Studies 64, 962–973. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2006.05.002
Lee, K. M., and Nass, C. (2004). The multiple source effect and synthesized speech. Hum. Commun. Res. 30, 182–207. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2004.tb00730.x
Lee, K. M., and Nass, C. (2005). Social-psychological origins of feelings of presence: Creating social presence with machine-generated voices. Media Psychol. 7, 31–45. doi: 10.1207/S1532785XMEP0701_2
Lee, K. M., Park, N., and Song, H. (2005). Can a robot be perceived as a developing creature? effects of a robot's long-term cognitive developments on its social presence and people's social responses toward it. Hum. Commun. Res. 31, 538–563. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2005.tb00882.x
Lee, K. M., Peng, W., Jin, S. A., and Yan, C. (2006b). Can robots manifest personality? an empirical test of personality recognition, social responses, and social presence in human–robot interaction. J. Commun. 56, 754–772. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00318.x
Lee, M., Bruder, G., and Welch, G. F. (2017). “Exploring the effect of vibrotactile feedback through the floor on social presence in an immersive virtual environment,” in Proceedings of IEEE VR (New York, NY: IEEE).
Lee, M., Kim, K., Daher, S., Raij, A., Schubert, R., Bailenson, J., et al. (2016). “The wobbly table: increased social presence via subtle incidental movement of a real-virtual table,” in Proceedings of IEEE VR (New York, NY: IEEE).
Lee, Y. H., Xiao, M., and Wells, R. H. (2018). The effects of avatars' age on older adults' self-disclosure and trust. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 21, 173–178. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2017.0451
Li, J., Kizilcec, R., Bailenson, J., and Ju, W. (2016). Social robots and virtual agents as lecturers for video instruction. Comput. Human Behav. 55, 1222–1230. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.04.005
Li, S., Feng, B., Li, N., and Tan, X. (2015). How social context cues in online support-seeking influence self-disclosure in support provision. Commun. Q. 63, 586–602. doi: 10.1080/01463373.2015.1078389
Lim, J., and Richardson, J. C. (2016). Exploring the effects of students' social networking experience on social presence and perceptions of using SNSs for educational purposes. Intern. High. Educ. 29, 31–39. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2015.12.001
Lim, S., and Reeves, B. (2010). Computer agents versus avatars: Responses to interactive game characters controlled by a computer or other player. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 68, 57–68. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2009.09.008
Lim, Y. S., and Lee-Won, R. J. (2017). When retweets persuade: the persuasive effects of dialogic retweeting and the role of social presence in organizations' Twitter-based communication. Telemat. Informat. 34, 422–433. doi: 10.1016/j.tele.2016.09.003
Lombard, M. (1995). Direct responses to people on the screen: television and personal space. Commun. Res. 22, 288–324. doi: 10.1177/009365095022003002
Lombard, M., and Ditton, T. (1997). At the heart of it all: The concept of presence. J. Comp. Mediat. Commun. 3.
Lombard, M., Ditton, T. B., Crane, D., Davis, B., Gil-Egui, G., Horvath, K., et al. (2000). “Measuring presence: a literature-based approach to the development of a standardized paper-and-pencil instrument,” in Proceedings of the 3rd International Workshop on Presence (Delft).
Lombard, M., Ditton, T. B., and Weinstein, L. (2009). “Measuring (tele)presence: the Temple Presence Inventory,” in Proceedings of the 12th International Workshop on Presence (Los Angeles, CA).
Lowden, R. J., and Hostetter, C. (2012). Access, utility, imperfection: The impact of videoconferencing on perceptions of social presence. Comput. Human Behav. 28, 377–383. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2011.10.007
McGregor, S. C. (2018). Personalization, social media, and voting: effects of candidate self-personalization on vote intention. New Media Soc. 20, 1139–1160. doi: 10.1177/1461444816686103
Mennecke, B. E., and Wheeler, B. C. (1993). “Tasks matter: modeling group task processes in experimental CSCW research,” in Proceedings of the 26th Annual Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences (New York, NY: IEEE).
Meyer, H. K., and Lohner, J. (2012). “Media equation revisited: does a reporter's presence matter in online video?” in 68th Annual Conference of the International Communication Association (Phoenix, AZ).
Moreno, R., and Mayer, R. E. (2004). Personalized messages that promote science learning in virtual environments. J. Educ. Psychol. 96, 165–173. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.165
Mühlbach, L., Bocker, M., and Prussog, A. (1995). Telepresence in videocommunications: a study on stereoscopy and individual eye contact. J. Hum. Factors Ergonom. Soc. 37, 290–305. doi: 10.1518/001872095779064582
Nakanishi, H., Murakami, Y., Nogami, D., and Ishiguro, H. (2008). “Minimum movement matters: Impact of robot-mounted cameras on social telepresence,” in Proceedings of the ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work & Social Computing (San Diego, CA).
Nam, C. S., Shu, J., and Chung, D. (2008). The roles of sensory modalities in collaborative virtual environments (CVEs). Comput. Human Behav. 24, 1404–1417. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2007.07.014
Nass, C., Steuer, J., and Tauber, E. R. (1994). “Computers are social actors,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human factors in Computing Systems (New York, NY: ACM).
Nowak, K. L. (2001). “Defining and differentiating copresence, social presence and presence as transportation,” in Proceedings of the Fourth International Workshop on Presence (Philadelphia, PA).
Nowak, K. L. (2003). Sex categorization in computer mediated communication (CMC): exploring the utopian promise. Media Psychol. 5, 83–103. doi: 10.1207/S1532785XMEP0501_4
Nowak, K. L., and Biocca, F. (2003). The effect of the agency and anthropomorphism on users' sense of telepresence, copresence, and social presence in virtual environments. Pres. Teleoperat. Virtu. Environ. 12, 481–494. doi: 10.1162/105474603322761289
Nowak, K. L., Krcmar, M., and Farrar, K. M. (2008). The causes and consequences of presence: considering the influence of violent video games on presence and aggression. Pres. Teleoperat. Virtu. Environ. 17, 256–268. doi: 10.1162/pres.17.3.256
Nowak, K. L., Watt, J., and Walther, J. B. (2009). Computer mediated teamwork and the efficiency framework: exploring the influence of synchrony and cues on media satisfaction and outcome success. Comput. Human Behav. 25, 1108–1119. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2009.05.006
Oh, S. Y., Bailenson, J., Krämer, N., and Li, B. (2016). Let the avatar brighten your smile: effects of enhancing facial expressions in virtual environments. PLoS ONE 11:161794. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161794
Pan, X., Gillies, M., and Slater, M. (2008). “The impact of avatar blushing on the duration of interaction between a real and virtual person,” in Proceedings of the 11th Annual International Workshop on Presence October 26–28 (Edinburgh).
Pan, X., Gillies, M., and Slater, M. (2015). Virtual character personality influences participant attitudes and behavior–an interview with a virtual human character about her social anxiety. Front. Robot. AI 2:1. doi: 10.3389/frobt.2015.00001
Park, E. K., and Sundar, S. S. (2015). Can synchronicity and visual modality enhance social presence in mobile messaging? Comput. Human Behav. 45, 121–128. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.12.001
Peña, J., Ghaznavi, J., Brody, N., Prada, R., Martinho, C., Santos, P. A., et al. (2017). Effects of human vs. computer-controlled characters and social identity cues on enjoyment: mediation effects of presence, similarity, and group identification. J. Media Psychol. doi: 10.1027/1864-1105/a000218
Phillips, B., and Lee, W. N. (2005). Interactive animation: exploring spokes-characters on the Internet. J. Curr. Issues Res. Advert. 27, 1–17. doi: 10.1080/10641734.2005.10505170
Poeschl, S. (2017). Virtual reality training for public speaking—a QUEST-VR framework validation. Front. ICT 4:13. doi: 10.3389/fict.2017.00013
Qin, J., Choi, K. S., Xu, R., Pang, W. M., and Heng, P. A. (2013). Effect of packet loss on collaborative haptic interactions in networked virtual environments: an experimental study. Pre. Teleoperat. Virtu. Environ. 22, 36–53. doi: 10.1162/PRES_a_00132
Qiu, L., and Benbasat, I. (2005). Online consumer trust and live help interfaces: the effects of text-to-speech voice and three-dimensional avatars. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact. 19, 75–94. doi: 10.1207/s15327590ijhc1901_6
Qiu, L., and Benbasat, I. (2010). A study of demographic embodiments of product recommendation agents in electronic commerce. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 68, 669–688. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2010.05.005
Ramirez, A., Walther, J. B., Burgoon, J. K., and Sunnafrank, M. (2002). Information-seeking strategies, uncertainty, and computer-mediated communication. Hum. Commun. Res. 28, 213–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2002.tb00804.x
Ratan, R. A., and Hasler, B. (2009). “Self-presence standardized: Introducing the self-presence questionnaire (SPQ),” in Proceedings of the 12th Annual International Workshop on Presence (Los Angeles, CA).
Rauh, C., and Renfro, S. (2004). “Feedback delay effects in video monitor communication,” Paper presented at: 54th Annual Conference of the International Communication Association (New Orleans, LA).
Rauwers, F., Voorveld, H. A., and Neijens, P. C. (2016). The effects of the integration of external and internal communication features in digital magazines on consumers' magazine attitude. Comput. Hum. Behav. 61, 454–462. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.042
Richardson, J. C., and Swan, K. (2003). Examining social presence in online courses in relation to students' perceived learning and satisfaction. J. Asynchron. Learn. Netw. 7, 68–88. Available online at: http://hdl.handle.net/2142/18713
Riggio, R. E., and Friedman, H. S. (1986). Impression formation: The role of expressive behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 50, 421–427. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.421
Robb, A., Kleinsmith, A., Cordar, A., White, C., Wendling, A., Lampotang, S., et al. (2016). Training together: how another human trainee's presence affects behavior during virtual human-based team training. Front. ICT 3:17. doi: 10.3389/fict.2016.00017
Rüggenberg, S. (2007). So Nah und Doch So Fern. Soziale Präsenz und Vertrauen in der Computervermittelten Kommunikation (So Close and Yet So Far Away: Social Presences and Trust in Computer-Mediated Communication). Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cologne, Cologne.
Sallnäs, E. L. (2005). Effects of communication mode on social presence, virtual presence, and performance in collaborative virtual environments. Pres. Teleoper. Virtu. Environ. 14, 434–449. doi: 10.1162/105474605774785253
Sallnäs, E. L. (2010). “Haptic feedback increases perceived social presence,” in Haptics: Generating and Perceiving Tangible Sensations, eds A. M. L. Kappers, J. B. F. van Erp, W. M. Bergmann Tiest, and F. C. T. van der Helm (Berlin: Springer). 178–185.
Sallnäs, E. L., Rassmus-Gröhn, K., and Sjöström, C. (2000). Supporting presence in collaborative environments by haptic force feedback. ACM Trans. Comput. Hum. Interact. 7, 461–476. doi: 10.1145/365058.365086
Schroeder, R., Steed, A., Axelsson, A. S., Heldal, I., Abelin, Å., Wideström, J., et al. (2001). Collaborating in networked immersive spaces: as good as being there together? Comput. Graph. 25, 781–788. doi: 10.1016/S0097-8493(01)00120-0
Schumann, S., Klein, O., Douglas, K., and Hewstone, M. (2017). When is computer-mediated intergroup contact most promising? Examining the effect of out-group members' anonymity on prejudice. Comput. Human Behav. 77, 198–210. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.006
Shahid, S., Krahmer, E., and Swerts, M. (2012). Video-mediated and co-present gameplay: effects of mutual gaze on game experience, expressiveness and perceived social presence. Interact. Comput. 24, 292–305. doi: 10.1016/j.intcom.2012.04.006
Shimoda, T. A. (2007). “Quitting smoking is easy, I've done it hundreds of times: intelligent agents, feedback loops, and relevant advice,” in 57th Annual Conference of the International Communication Association (San Francisco, CA).
Short, J., Williams, E., and Christie, B. (1976). The Social Psychology of Telecommunications. New York, NY: John Wiley.
Siriaraya, P., and Ang, C. S. (2012). Age differences in the perception of social presence in the use of 3D virtual world for social interaction. Interact. Comput. 24, 280–291. doi: 10.1016/j.intcom.2012.03.003
Skalski, P., and Tamborini, R. (2007). The role of social presence in interactive agent-based persuasion. Media Psychol. 10, 385–413. doi: 10.1080/15213260701533102
Skalski, P., and Whitbred, R. (2010). Image versus sound: a comparison of formal feature effects on presence and video game enjoyment. PsychNology J. 8, 67–84. Available online at: http://www.psychnology.org/File/PNJ8(1)/PSYCHNOLOGY_JOURNAL_8_1_SKALSKI.pdf
Skarbez, R., Neyret, S., Brooks, F. P., Slater, M., and Whitton, M. C. (2017). A psychophysical experiment regarding components of the plausibility illusion. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 23, 1369–1378. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2017.2657158
Slater, M. (2004). How colorful was your day? Why questionnaires cannot assess presence in virtual environments. Pres. Teleoperat. Virtu. Environ. 13, 484–493. doi: 10.1162/1054746041944849
Slater, M. (2009). Place illusion and plausibility can lead to realistic behaviour in immersive virtual environments. Philos. Transact. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 3549–3557. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0138
Slater, M., McCarthy, J., and Maringelli, F. (1998). The influence of body movement on subjective presence in virtual environments. Hum. Factors 40, 469–477. doi: 10.1518/001872098779591368
Slater, M., Pertaub, D. P., and Steed, A. (1999). Public speaking in virtual reality: Facing an audience of avatars. IEEE Comput. Graph. Appl. 19, 6–9. doi: 10.1109/38.749116
Slater, M., Rovira, A., Southern, R., Swapp, D., Zhang, J. J., Campbell, C., et al. (2013). Bystander responses to a violent incident in an immersive virtual environment. PLoS ONE 8:e52766. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052766
Slater, M., Sadagic, A., Usoh, M., and Schroeder, R. (2000). Small-group behavior in a virtual and real environment: a comparative study. Pres. Teleoperat. Virtu. Environ. 9, 37–51. doi: 10.1162/105474600566600
Slater, M., and Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2016). Enhancing our lives with immersive virtual reality. Front. Robot. AI 3:74. doi: 10.3389/frobt.2016.00074
Slater, M., Spanlang, B., and Corominas, D. (2010). Simulating virtual environments within virtual environments as the basis for a psychophysics of presence. ACM Trans. Graph. 29:92. doi: 10.1145/1778765.1778829
Slater, M., and Steed, A. (2000). A virtual presence counter. Pres. Teleoperat. Virtu. Environ. 9, 413–434. doi: 10.1162/105474600566925
Slater, M., and Wilbur, S. (1997). A framework for immersive virtual environments (FIVE): Speculations on the role of presence in virtual environments. Pre. Teleoperat. Virtu. Environ. 6, 603–616. doi: 10.1162/pres.1997.6.6.603
Steed, A., Slater, M., Sadagic, A., Bullock, A., and Tromp, J. (1999). “Leadership and collaboration in shared virtual environments,” in Proceedings of IEEE VR (New York, NY: IEEE).
Steuer, J. (1992). Defining virtual reality: dimensions determining telepresence. J. Commun. 42, 73–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1992.tb00812.x
Swan, K. (2002). Building learning communities in online courses: the importance of interaction. Educ. Commun. Inform. 2, 23–49. doi: 10.1080/1463631022000005016
Swinth, K. R., and Blascovich, J. (2001). “Conformity to group norms in an immersive virtual environment,” in Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Society (Toronto, ON).
Tajfel, H. (1979). Individuals and groups in social psychology. Br. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 18, 183–190. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1979.tb00324.x
Takatalo, J. (2002). Presence and Flow in Virtual Environments: An Explorative Study. Master's thesis, University of Helsinki, Helsinki.
Takatalo, J., Kawai, T., Kaistinen, J., Nyman, G., and Häkkinen, J. (2011). User experience in 3D stereoscopic games. Media Psychol. 14, 387–414. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2011.620538
Tanaka, K., Nakanishi, H., and Ishiguro, H. (2015). Physical embodiment can produce robot operator's pseudo presence. Front. ICT 2:8. doi: 10.3389/fict.2015.00008
Thayalan, X., Shanthi, A., and Paridi, T. (2012). Gender difference in social presence experienced in e-learning activities. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 67, 580–589. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.11.363
Tu, C.-H. (2002). The impacts of text-based CMC on online social presence. J. Interact. Online Learn. 1, 1–24. Available online at: http://openknowledge.nau.edu/2913/7/Chih-Hsiung_T_2002_The_Impacts_of_Text-based_CMC_on_Online_Social_Presence%281%29.pdf
Verhagen, T., van Nes, J., Feldberg, F., and van Dolen, W. (2014). Virtual customer service agents: using social presence and personalization to shape online service encounters. J. Comp. Med. Commun. 19, 529–545. doi: 10.1111/jcc4.12066
Vishwanath, A. (2016). Mobile device affordance: Explicating how smartphones influence the outcome of phishing attacks. Comput. Human Behav. 63, 198–207. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.035
von der Pütten, M., Krämer, N. C., Gratch, J., and Kang, S. H. (2010). “It doesn't matter what you are!” explaining social effects of agents and avatars. Comput. Human Behav. 26, 1641–1650. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.06.012
Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction a relational perspective. Communic. Res. 19, 52–90. doi: 10.1177/009365092019001003
Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interpersonal, and hyperpersonal interaction. Commun. Res. 23, 3–43. doi: 10.1177/009365096023001001
Walther, J. B., and Parks, M. R. (2002). “Cues filtered out, cues filtered in: computer-mediated communication and relationships,” in Handbook of Interpersonal Communication 3rd edn, eds M. L. Knapp and J. A. Daly (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage). 529–563.
Wei, C.-W., and Chen, N.-S. (2012). A model for social presence in online classrooms. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 60, 529–545. doi: 10.1007/s11423-012-9234-9
Welch, R. B., Blackmon, T. T., Liu, A., Mellers, B. A., and Stark, L. W. (1996). The effects of pictorial realism, delay of visual feedback, and observer interactivity on the subjective sense of presence. Pre Teleoperat. Virtu. Environ. 5, 263–273. doi: 10.1162/pres.1996.5.3.263
Wideström, J., Axelsson, A. S., Schroeder, R., Nilsson, A., Heldal, I., and Abelin, Å. (2000). “The collaborative cube puzzle: a comparison of virtual and real environments,” in Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Collaborative Virtual Environments (New York, NY: ACM).
Witmer, B. G., and Singer, M. J. (1998). Measuring presence in virtual environments: a presence questionnaire. Pres. Teleoperat. Virtu. Environ. 7, 225–240. doi: 10.1162/105474698565686
Wu, Y., Babu, S. V., Armstrong, R., Bertrand, J. W., Luo, J., Roy, T., et al. (2014). Effects of virtual human animation on emotion contagion in simulated inter-personal experiences. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 20, 626–635. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2014.19
Wu, Z., Li, J., and Theng, Y. L. (2015). Examining the influencing factors of exercise intention among older adults: a controlled study between exergame and traditional exercise. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 18, 521–527. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0065
Xu, K., and Lombard, M. (2017). Persuasive computing: Feeling peer pressure from multiple computer agents. Comput. Human Behav. 74, 152–162. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.04.043
Xu, Q. (2014). Should I trust him? The effects of reviewer profile characteristics on eWOM credibility. Comput. Hum. Behav. 33, 136–144. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.027
Yoo, Y., and Alavi, M. (2001). Media and group cohesion: Relative influences on social presence, task participation, and group consensus. Manage. Inform. Syst. Quart. 25, 371–390. doi: 10.2307/3250922
Zelenkauskaite, A., and Bucy, E. (2009). “Presence, participation, and political text-on-television: testing a converged technology,” in Proceedings of the 12th Annual International Workshop on Presence (Los Angeles, CA).
Zhan, Z., and Mei, H. (2013). Academic self-concept and social presence in face-to-face and online learning: Perceptions and effects on students' learning achievement and satisfaction across environments. Comput. Educ. 69, 131–138. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.07.002
Keywords: social presence, presence, virtual reality, virtual environments, immersion, computer-mediated communication
Citation: Oh CS, Bailenson JN and Welch GF (2018) A Systematic Review of Social Presence: Definition, Antecedents, and Implications. Front. Robot. AI 5:114. doi: 10.3389/frobt.2018.00114
Received: 20 June 2018; Accepted: 11 September 2018;
Published: 15 October 2018.
Edited by:
Doron Friedman, Interdisciplinary Center Herzliya, IsraelReviewed by:
Xueni Pan, Goldsmiths, University of London, United KingdomMarco Fyfe Pietro Gillies, Goldsmiths, University of London, United Kingdom
Regis Kopper, Duke University, United States
Copyright © 2018 Oh, Bailenson and Welch. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Catherine S. Oh, syoh@stanford.edu