
95% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
ORIGINAL RESEARCH article
Front. Public Health , 05 March 2025
Sec. Public Mental Health
Volume 13 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2025.1556230
Background: There has been increasing interest in creativity, heritage and nature to improve health-related outcomes. However, limited research has examined the intersection of heritage crafting in the context of natural spaces. This study aims to explore the feasibility and acceptability of an archaeologically informed outdoor heritage crafting intervention.
Methods: A mixed-methods single group before and after feasibility study was conducted. Participants completed questionnaires, including validated items measuring outcomes related to mental health, wellbeing, social connectedness, mindfulness, perceived state of flow and the connection with nature and the environment. Qualitative interviews were conducted with participants to explore their experiences, and data were analyzed using thematic analysis.
Results: Forty-eight participants from a University in the United Kingdom attended the workshops, achieving the recruitment target within the required timeframe. The response rate to all pre-workshop measures was 100% and remained at 100% post-workshop, with the exception of missing data for two participants (4.2%) for measures assessing wellbeing and anxiety, and missing data for five participants (10.4%) for the measure assessing depression. Therefore, response and retention rates indicate high levels of feasibility to conduct a robust evaluation of this intervention. Five themes were identified, including: participant motivation to sign up; engaging with creative activities in a natural setting; skilled facilitation and a flexible approach; group delivery, and duration and frequency of workshops. Overall, the workshops were positively received by participants, primarily attributing their satisfaction to engagement with group-based creative activities in a natural setting with an educational component.
Conclusion: This study has shown it is feasible and acceptable to deliver and evaluate an archaeologically informed heritage crafting intervention to support wellbeing. These results suggest the need for formal testing of the potential health benefits of the intervention to address policy imperatives for developing and implementing community- and place-based approaches to support mental health.
Mental health disorders are the third leading cause of years lived with disability, with a global prevalence of greater than 10% (1). However, mental health is not solely the absence of illbeing, it is also the presence of wellbeing (2), both hedonic (e.g., happiness) and eudaimonic (e.g., having meaning in life) (3, 4). Globally, wellbeing is an important topic for individuals, societies and public policy (5). It is increasingly being viewed as an important component for policy makers due to its impact on economic, health, social and cultural aspects of life (6). The variety of perceptions of what is meant by ‘positive wellbeing’ indicates that it is imperative to explore alternative ways of addressing quality of life and wellbeing for the population, at all stages of life and levels of health (5). Focus is increasingly turning toward the important role that both creativity (7) and the natural environment (8, 9) play in maintaining and enhancing mental health and wellbeing. Creative projects can serve as valuable community-based assets to support people to meet their health and wellbeing needs, but are difficult to sustain despite their impact (10). There is a need for substantial and sustained investment in new approaches that can enhance capacity and access to community, place-based mental health solutions.
It is well-established that creative activities contribute to positive development and wellbeing of a range of populations (11–14). The opportunity to participate in preferred creative activities may increase wellbeing and life satisfaction, while potentially decreasing psychological discomfort, depression, and anxiety (15). For example, existing evidence has reported engaging in craft activities can promote a sense of sharing and belonging (16, 17), offer a distraction from emotional stress by creating feelings of relaxation (17, 18), and provide a healing and protective effect on mental wellbeing (19). In evaluating the impact of crafting on wellbeing, literature frequently refers to the concept of flow (20), and mindfulness (21, 22). Flow refers to an intense involvement in an activity within which the person experiences meaningfulness, which may subsequently lead to positive outcomes (15, 23). Mindfulness is defined as a state of ‘nonjudgemental moment-to-moment awareness’ (24), and has the potential to regulate stress and improve cognitive and emotional functioning (25). Mindfulness may work synergistically with creativity (26). Research has indicated that experiential and social aspects of crafting fosters components of both creativity and mindfulness, through which craft can contribute to emotional satisfaction, social connection, personal agency, and overall wellbeing (22). Additionally, there is evidence that people who engage in heritage crafting activities expressed a connection to cultural tradition (27, 28). This perceived link between crafting and tradition provides the opportunity for heritage, crafting and wellbeing to work in tandem.
Intangible cultural heritage refers to the various practices, performances and expressions, knowledge systems, skills, crafts, and traditional culture that are inherited by various groups (29). Traditional handmade intangible cultural heritage projects involve skills handed down from previous generations of continuous development, and include a wide range of content, such as weaving, embroidery and ceramic making (30). There has been increasing evidence for links between craft heritage activities and wellbeing (31), and having access to these activities has been shown to enhance wellbeing (32–35). A growing body of literature indicates that craft heritage, including written heritage (36), material culture (37, 38), rock art (39), and ancient architecture (40), is advantageous for contemporary wellbeing (41). These forms of heritage crating offer diverse groups of individuals opportunities to engage in enriching activities that foster learning, tactile interaction, creativity, and storytelling.
In addition to creative activities, connection with and activities in natural environments can also play a critical role in maintaining and enhancing mental health and wellbeing (8, 9, 42, 43). Existing theories, such as the attention restoration theory (ART) (44) and the stress restoration theory (SRT) (45) suggest that contact with nature can influence both productivity and wellbeing. To recover from stressors and demands of daily lives, individuals need to replenish lost resources by engaging in activities that restore old resources or generate new resources (46). Both ART and SRT propose that exposure to nature is able to restore these emotional and cognitive resources, subsequently reducing levels of stress. Engagement with nature provides the opportunity to capture involuntary attention and provide relief from everyday stressors and demands (47). People who have higher levels of nature connectedness tend to have higher levels of both hedonic (48) and eudaimonic (9) wellbeing. Additionally, evidence suggests that community- and place-based participatory art practices may enhance subjective wellbeing, promote social cohesion, and strengthen community networks (49). These benefits may be further amplified when activities are culturally rooted (50, 51).
Given the salutogenic characteristics of creative tasks and engagement with outdoor natural spaces, we developed the means to pilot test the likely mental health and wellbeing benefits of an outdoor heritage crafting intervention known as Craftwell. Craftwell involved two workshops: (1) crafting replica Anglo-Saxon pots, or (2) crafting Mesolithic beads. This mixed-methods study aimed to assess the feasibility and acceptability of outdoor craft heritage workshops. Specifically, the research questions were:
1. Are outdoor craft heritage workshops feasible to deliver and evaluate (including recruitment rate, attrition rate, data collection procedures)?
2. What are participant perceptions of the outdoor craft heritage workshops?
A mixed-methods single group before and after feasibility study to explore outdoor heritage craft workshops delivered to a student population.
This study was conducted with adult students (18 years+) at the University of York, United Kingdom (UK), who were registered on undergraduate or postgraduate programmes at the time of the workshops (spring and summer term 2022–2023). The recruitment of student volunteers is a common example of convenience sampling for feasibility studies (52).
Students in the Department of Archaeology who had already completed a course of study that involved taking part in crafting activity at the York Experimental Archaeological Research (YEAR) Centre were not eligible.
The YEAR Centre is an outdoor space on campus used for teaching and researching experimental archaeology. It is located in a quiet area of woodland by a lake. In this space, there are reconstructions of a prehistoric house and a Viking workshop, as well as open structures with central hearths that provided shelter and warmth during the workshops (see Figure 1). The workshops were delivered by an archaeologist with extensive experience and expertise in engaging archaeology with the wider community.
The workshops took place between 8th March 2023 and 7th June 2023, offered once weekly on a Wednesday afternoon (14:00–16:00) during spring and summer term times (10 workshop dates available in this duration). A minimum of 4 and a maximum of 12 participants were invited to each workshop.
In the bead making workshop, participants recreated stone beads using the materials and techniques that archaeologists think people used 11,000 years ago. The beads emulated findings from the Mesolithic site of Starr Carr, North Yorkshire, where evidence for a bead making workshop has been found (53–55). The beads were made from a soft shale collected from the Yorkshire coast, which is the most likely source used by people at Star Carr (55). The holes were hand-drilled using specially made flint drill tips, known as awls, which have also been found at Starr Carr, with microscopic traces of wear suggesting they were used in this way (53). The workshops gave participants the opportunity to engage with prehistoric craft making using locally sourced materials, and explore different methods of drilling, decorating, and stringing the beads. To string them, participants recreated simple twisted string made from plant fibre, which has also been recovered from archaeological sites dating to the same period.
In the pottery workshop, participants made pots based on Anglo-Saxon types used during the 5-6th Century CE (56, 57). Pots of distinctly early Anglo-Saxon type are found on sites throughout eastern England. These vessels were used for a range of activities including cooking, storage, brewing, dairying and as containers to hold the cremated remains of the dead (58). Anglo-Saxon potters made their wares by hand, using coils of clay to build up the vessel wall, and these were fired in simple bonfires. Although most pots were plain, those used for brewing, drinking and as cremation urns were highly decorated with stamp impressions and incised lines (59). Participants used the same techniques to achieve their pots. Examples of decorative patterns that have been discovered on Anglo-Saxon pots were available for participants to recreate on their own pots.
A questionnaire was developed by a multi-disciplinary team of academics. The questionnaire included validated items, as detailed below.
Demographic data were collected about participants’ age (in bands), gender (male/female/non-binary), ethnicity, presence of a mental or physical health condition, and the extent to which these limit day-to-day activities (limited substantially/not limited substantially/not limited), stage of degree (undergraduate year 1, 2, or 3, master’s degree, or PhD), and name of degree.
The Short Warwick Edinburgh Mental Wellbeing Scale (SWEMWBS) (60), the PHQ-8 (61), and the GAD-7 (62) were included. The PHQ-8 is an eight-item version of the PHQ-9 (63) that omits the item assessing the risk of suicide. As such, this measure is commonly used in non-clinical populations. Higher scores on the SWEMWS represent better wellbeing, whereas higher scores on the PHQ-8 and GAD-7 represent higher depression and anxiety, respectively. These measures were used to collect data pre- and post-workshop.
The 3-item short version of the UCLA loneliness scale (64) was included to collect data pre- and post-workshop, and higher scores on this scale represent greater loneliness.
The Social Connectedness Scale-Revised (65) was included to assess the degree to which participants felt connected to others in their social environment pre- and post-workshop. Participants were asked to indicate their agreement with 20 statements on a 6-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree; 6 = strongly agree). Negatively worded items (e.g., ‘I feel like an outsider) were reverse coded, and items were calculated to generate a total score (1–120), with higher scores representing more connectedness to others.
The Social Inclusion Scale (66) incorporates three subscales measuring social isolation, relations, and acceptance. Participants were asked to indicate their agreement with 21 statements on a 4-point Likert scale (1 = not at all; 4 = definitely). Negatively worded items (e.g., I have felt terribly alone and isolated’) were reverse coded, and items were calculated to generate a total score overall (1–84), with higher scores representing a higher perception of social inclusion. This measure was used to collect data pre- and post-workshop.
The Nature Connection Index (NCI) (67) was included. Participants were asked to indicate agreement to six statements on a 6-point Likert scale (1 = completely disagree; 6 = strongly agree). Items included statements such as: ‘I always find beauty in nature’ or ‘I always treat nature with respect’. To score this scale, a conversion spreadsheet was available to calculate a total Nature Connection Index score from the individual items, with higher scores representing higher connection to nature. This measure was used to collect data pre- and post-workshop.
A question from the Natural England People and Nature Survey (GOV.UK., 2020) was included to ask participants about occasions in the last 12 months when they spent time outside (e.g., open spaces in and around towns and cities including nature areas). Participants were asked to indicate how often, on average, they had spent time outdoors, ranging from every day to less often than 2–3 months. This question was asked pre- and post-workshop.
Participants were asked whether they engaged regularly in recycling, buying eco-friendly products, buying seasonal or locally grown food, opting to walk or cycle rather than using a car, encouraging people to protect the environment, engaging in environmental or conservation organizations, volunteering to help care for the environment, or donating money to support an environmental or conservation organization.
The Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) (68) was included. Participants were asked to indicate agreement to 15 statements on a 6-point Likert scale (1 = almost always; 6 = almost never). Items included statements such as: ‘I do jobs or tasks automatically, without being aware of what I’m doing’, ‘I find myself preoccupied with the future or the past’, and ‘I find myself doing things without paying attention’. To score this scale, an average of the 15 items is calculated. This measure was used to collect data pre- and post-workshop.
The Flow Short Scale (69) was included in the post-workshop questionnaire only to measure the components of flow experience. Participants were asked to indicate agreement to 13 statements on a 3-point Likert scale (1 = not at all; 3 = very much). Items included statements such as: ‘I feel just the right amount of challenge’, and ‘I am totally absorbed in what I am doing’. The last item ‘I am worried about failing’ was reverse coded, and items were calculated to generate a total score (1–39), with a higher score representing a higher perceived sense of flow.
The Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) (70) was included in the post-workshop questionnaire only to evaluate the participant’s experience in the setting. Participants were asked to indicate agreement to 26 items on a 7-point Likert Scale (0 = not at all; 6 = completely). Items included statements such as: ‘being here is an escape experience’, and ‘the setting is fascinating’. Negatively worded statements (e.g., ‘this place is boring’) were reverse coded, and items were calculated to generate a total score (0–156), with a higher score representing better experiences.
Lastly, interviews were conducted with a sub-sample of participants. A semi-structured topic guide was developed by the research team to explore their experiences of taking part in the workshops (including the signing up process and completion of questionnaires). The full topic guide is presented in Supplementary Material 1.
Flyers were distributed to promote the workshops via social media (e.g., X), departmental bulletins, and announcements via the University of York Student’s Union webpages. The flyers included a QR code to take potential participants to an Expression of Interest form using a Google form. This form invited potential participants to confirm they were a student at the University of York and to select their preferred crafting activity (pottery making or bead making) and preferred dates for taking part.
Eligible participants were then contacted by a member of the research team to invite them to a workshop. This invitation included a personalized link, generated through Qualtrics (Qualtrics, Provo, UT) questionnaire software, for participants to complete a pre-workshop questionnaire before taking part in the study. All participants could complete the pre-workshop questionnaire within a two-week period before the workshop started. Participants were also directed to an embedded participant information sheet, a risk assessment summary, and a consent form. Consent to participate was indicated by ticking an online check box. Participants could also tick an optional consent box to participate in follow-up interviews. At the beginning of the questionnaire, each participant was assigned an ID number to anonymise all data. All pre-workshop data were stored on the Qualtrics server at the University of York until all workshops were completed.
Immediately after completion of the workshop, participants were required to complete a post-workshop questionnaire in-person. The data were inputted into Qualtrics by a member of the research team, using the same ID numbers generated during the pre-workshop questionnaire to match participant responses across timepoints. The post-workshop data were stored on the Qualtrics server at the University of York until all workshops were completed.
Following completion of all workshops, a purposive sampling approach was used to select a sub-sample of participants for interviews based on their age, gender, stage of degree, and the crafting activity attended. Participants were invited to interview via email, and interviews were conducted via Zoom. Interviews were audio-recorded and transcribed verbatim. Transcripts were assigned with the participant ID numbers to anonymise the transcripts.
Ethics approval was granted by the Department of Archaeology at the University of York on 20th February 2023.
Descriptive summary statistics are presented for demographic variables. The raw dataset was used to calculate mean scores, ranges, and standard deviations (SD) for outcome variables at the two timepoints, as is convention with feasibility studies (71). These descriptive statistics were calculated using SPSS version 28.0 (IBM®).
For qualitative data, thematic analysis was conducted and adopted a deductive a priori template of codes approach, which allows for codes to be applied as a means of organising text for subsequent information. Data were interrogated to extract meaningful data segments following the code template. Transcripts were systematically coded, and codes with similar content were collated into preliminary themes. Two authors (ES and PD) independently reviewed the themes to ensure consensus with the assignment of themes and illustrative quotes.
Seventy-six participants completed the Expression of Interest form. Of these, five were ineligible as they had previous experience at the YEAR Centre. Therefore, 71 eligible participants were invited to attend a workshop, of which 69 (97.2%) initially accepted. Forty-eight participants (69.6%) attended across four pottery and three bead making workshops; 21 (30.4%) consented participants did not attend. Reasons for non-attendance included illness (n = 4) and unavailability due to lectures or exams (n = 10); seven participants did not provide a reason (Figure 2).
Group size varied, with an average of seven participants per workshop (range = 4 to 9; Figure 2). Twenty-nine participants (60.4%) attended pottery workshops, and 19 participants (39.6%) attended bead making workshops. A summary of participant characteristics is presented in Table 1.
There were no missing data for measures assessing depression, anxiety, and wellbeing pre-workshop. For the post-workshop questionnaire, there were missing data for two participants on the GAD-7 and SWEMWBS measures, and missing data for five participants on the PHQ-8 measure (Table 2).
Table 2. Mean scores, ranges, and standard deviations for depression, anxiety, and wellbeing scores pre- and post-workshop.
There were no missing data for loneliness, social connectedness, or social inclusion measures at either timepoint (Table 3).
Table 3. Mean scores, ranges, and standard deviations for loneliness, social connectedness, and social inclusion scores pre- and post-workshop.
There was no missing data for the MAAS at either timepoint. The mean score for mindfulness pre-workshop was 3.6 (range = 2–5; SD = 0.82), and the mean score for mindfulness post-workshop was 3.4 (range = 1–5; SD = 0.86) (−0.2).
There was no missing data for nature connectedness at either timepoint. The mean Nature Connection Index score pre-workshop was 24.9 (range = 5–34; SD = 7.9), and the mean Nature Connection Index score post-workshop was 25.3 (range = 1–34; SD = 8.5).
For time spent outdoors, six (12.5%) participants indicated they spent time outside every day both pre- and post-workshop. Thirty-three participants (68.8%) reported during the pre-workshop questionnaire that they spent time outdoors once a week or more, but not every day, compared with 35 (72.9%) participants post-workshop. Only nine (18.7%) participants reported that they spent time outdoors less than this (e.g., once or twice a month, once every 2–3 months or less) pre-workshop, compared to seven (14.6%) participants post-workshop.
The majority of participants engaged in at least one environmental activity pre-workshop (Table 4), with the most common being recycling (93.8%, n = 45), and the least frequent being donating money to an environmental or conservation organization (6.25%, n = 3).
There was no missing data for the Flow Short Scale or PRS. The mean score for perceived sense of flow was 25.9 (range = 19–34; SD = 3.8), and the mean score for the PRS was 124.6 (range = 71–156; SD = 18.5). These scores indicate participants experienced a high sense of flow and positive experience of the workshop setting.
Interviews were conducted with 15 participants. Of these, 10 attended the pottery making workshop and five attended the bead making workshop. Participant characteristics for those completing interviews are presented in Table 5. The interview durations ranged from 24 min and 32 s to 89 min and 20 s (average = 44 min and 36 s).
Thematic analysis of interview data resulted in the identification of five themes, including: (1) participant motivation to sign up; (2) engaging with creative activities in a natural setting; (3) skilled facilitation and a flexible approach; (4) group delivery, and (5) duration and frequency of workshops. To illustrate themes, quotations are presented verbatim, with the gender, age range and degree level of participants provided in brackets.
When asked about their primary reason for signing up for the workshops, most participants cited the appeal of engaging in creative activities. They found these activities therapeutic and a good distraction from daily stress. Additionally, one participant mentioned that the high cost of such activities often creates a barrier, despite their potential benefits.
“Clay making, I was like, that’s for me, especially going to university with all the stress and being busy, I didn’t find time to do art, and I do find it quite therapeutic. So, I thought, that’s a great time, I will actually do it instead of just thinking about it.” (Female, 18-24yrs, Undergraduate Year 3)
“I really like pottery and ceramics, and it’s actually very expensive to do it outside of a study, and yeah, I just thought the chance to be able to do that would be great.” (Female, 25-34yrs, PhD)
In addition to engaging with creative activities, some participants acknowledged that the workshop offered a unique, creative task that was historically framed, providing the opportunity to try something new in addition to learning more about heritage crafting.
“It was something which I’ve not really done before, so yeah, it just seemed an interesting activity to go and give it a try, especially because it was framed in a historical context of Anglo-Saxon pottery, it was a bit more unique.” (Male, 18-24yrs, PhD)
“I very much appreciated the opportunity to have hands-on experience of archaeology and of practices, really, into archaeology. So, the idea of this being able to, sort of, learn how to make stone beads, well, that was all I really needed to become interested. I just like the idea of being able to do things hands-on and experience archaeology in new ways.” (Female, 18-24yrs, Postgraduate Masters)
One of the key reasons participants signed up for the workshop was the chance to engage in creative activities outdoors rather than indoors.
“I think actually, no, I wouldn’t have had the same motivation to sign up if it was an indoor activity, I think it was definitely a pretty big incentive that it was outdoors and that was another thing my flat mate said, it’s so peaceful, you sit around a little fire, it’s great.” (Female, 18-24yrs, Undergraduate Year 2)
Lastly, some participants expressed that no single factor motivated them to sign up for the workshop. Instead, a combination of interacting factors appealed to them, which they believed would offer them a more holistic experience.
“It works on more than one level, that sort of thing, being out in the environment in the fresh air with other people and then focusing on a thing and creating something. Yes, there’s all these different things at play and I just thought, yes, I’m fascinated, I want to know more, and I want to have a go myself.” (Female, 35-44yrs, PhD)
The majority of participants described the setting for the workshops as enjoyable and relaxing, and highlighted the value of being in an outdoor space that felt like a novel part of an otherwise familiar campus.
“It was pretty enjoyable. I went to the part of the university that I haven't been before actually, so it was nice being in nature and I loved the environment around all the trees and quiet there. So, that was nice sitting by the fire and, yeah, it's pretty enjoyable and relaxing.” (Female, 18-24yrs, Undergraduate Year 3)
“I really liked the setting; it was really nice. It was very relaxing, and you kind of felt like you were sort of tucked away and isolated from the rest of uni, even though we were literally right next to it, but that was nice. And then the fire was very nice. And yeah, I mean, it was nice just to slow down and take a pause and do something a bit creative for a few hours.” (Female, 25-34yrs, PhD)
Participants frequently mentioned elements of the natural environment that appeared to enhance their experiences of the workshop and promote wellbeing. These included the greenery of the location and indirect interaction with wildlife around them.
“I think it was the fact you could hear the birds, there was the odd squirrel running around, you could see down to the lake. Yes, it’s just nice to feel like… I think it’s particularly having the birdsong around just makes it that bit more relaxing” (Female, 25-34yrs, PhD)
“It feels very enclosed by trees, I think, yeah, the tree surroundings, especially as they’re right next to the stray [area of historic common land] as well, it takes you right out of the city or the town, and it felt quite sheltered and relaxed.” (Male, 35-44yrs, Undergraduate Year 3)
Despite these benefits, participants noted the limited surface space available outdoors made it challenging to engage with creative activities.
“The only thing would be the lack of, like, workspace. You know, like I say, people sometimes just had to hold the thing they were making, or I knelt down and did it, I found a piece of wood and I did it on a piece of wood, kind of thing. That was a little bit challenging, and it probably limited by ability to do as good as I could have done. Maybe that's one thing I would have changed, yeah, just something better to use to actually work on.” (Female, 55-64yrs, Postgraduate Masters)
For participants who had a physical disability or health condition, sitting on a log or having an unstable work surface could be uncomfortable or challenging. Specific techniques which required dexterity and fine motor skills were also experienced as challenging.
“Yes, I didn’t use too many of them [tools], I think, but because I think I knew that it would be a little bit fiddly for me, it was sort of the issue of not necessarily having a flat surface to work on and also trying to hold the thing and do something else with another hand, and I thought I’m not going to be very good at that, and I’ll probably end up dropping it.” (Female, 35-44yrs)
“So, it was a difficult process of doing it. It was a bit fiddlier, and the constant twisting, this is just a me issue, I’ve got bad joints, so the twisting of the flint, that was just something, that was to do with me. But it wasn’t unenjoyable because of it, and I just swapped hands.” (Non-binary, 18-24yrs)
However, one participant highlighted that altering the environment to facilitate the creative activity would have detracted from the importance of connecting with the past.
“The only thing is not having a stable space to work on and being sat on a log, but again I understand why it was like that, that was the point of it. And if you had then suddenly brought in pillows and cushions, and clipboards, it probably would have detracted from that immersion in the past and what you’re supposed to be achieving. So, it wasn’t something that I didn’t like, you know? It just was slightly awkward and uncomfortable at times, but we all made do, you know? It was fine.” (Female, 35-44yrs, PhD)
Lastly, some participants mentioned that their experience of engaging with crafting activities outdoors was particularly beneficial due to the time of year, and this may have been different if the weather was inclement. Whilst most participants felt that the fire enhanced the atmosphere, some mentioned that the smoke from the fire interfered with their crafting.
“The weather was beautiful, there were no mosquitoes or anything, so, yeah, it was great. But I know it would probably be significantly different if it was raining, or it was cold because of course then it's a bit uncomfortable.” (Female, 18-24yrs, Undergraduate Year 3)
“The only thing is the wind, but that’s impossible to manage, because all the fire, you know, the smoke, it started to come to our eyes, but that’s the only thing that I could find a challenge.” (Female, 35-44yrs, Postgraduate Masters)
Participants appeared to suggest that skilled and knowledgeable facilitation was a key contextual feature that enhanced their workshop experience, citing that the educational element was as valuable as the creative hands-on activity.
“Going round the actual site and the things that were there, which was fab, like having a nosey and the [facilitator] was very open and let us look around and ask loads of questions and stuff and were very knowledgeable. It was just a really interesting experience.” (Female, 35-44yrs, PhD)
Additionally, many participants appreciated the facilitator’s flexible approach. In both the bead making and pottery workshops, this flexible approach fostered a sense of autonomy, allowing participants to craft unique products aligned with their personal preferences and apply newly acquired skills right from the start of the session.
“I liked that I didn’t feel any pressure to create anything in particular. I knew that we’d been given parameters in the sense that we’d been given information about what clay and pots and tools had been used for in the past, but we didn’t have to recreate that, and I don’t think anyone had the sense of any pressure or anything, which I liked. And there was definitely a sense of, like, being inspired by past and present in terms of what we’d been told about the past but also the environment, so that was nice.” (Female, 18-24yrs, Undergraduate Year 2)
Furthermore, some participants expressed curiosity about their peers’ creations, adding to the overall intrigue of the experience.
“I enjoyed the bit where we were given a sharper or a thinner bit of the flint when we got to carve the lines and the actual physical personality and design into the little shale pieces. That was a fun bit because we all did different things, and I liked to see what everybody did. Especially because they were examples.” (Non-binary, 18-24yrs, Undergraduate Year 1)
Participants commonly expressed a preference for engaging in a group rather than a one-to-one workshop, suggesting that the group format was a key enabler to their participation. They frequently cited the sense of peer support from the new social group and the chance to share experiences with others as reasons for this preference.
“It was all just really nice to talk to people who were from different departments and realise that other people were interested in completing that activity, so yes, it just got me out of my usual social circle.” (Female, 18-24yrs, Undergraduate Year 2)
“I think [group activities] are fairly important. I think that common activity is one of the best ways to bond with people generally, so it’s fairly important to have that available.” (Female, 18-24yrs, Postgraduate Masters)
The majority of participants attended the workshop independently and mentioned this was beneficial as they could meet new people and concentrate on their own activity.
“I think it did impact my experience of the workshop because when you take opportunities like that by yourself, in my experience, it’s easier to, sort of, approach that community feeling.” (Female, 18-24yrs, Postgraduate Masters)
Additionally, some participants mentioned that attending with a peer they already knew may act as a distraction to the task at hand.
“If I had gone with a friend, it would be mostly an experience that I would be experiencing between me and my friend, and less between me and the group.” (Female, 18-24yrs, Postgraduate Masters)
“I was quite happy to go alone and meet new people really. I think, because of the way that the workshop was framed to me, it was more something to go along and, as sort of a solo activity and meet other people there. And I felt if I had taken friends along, it might have distracted a bit more from the overall experience.” (Male, 18-24yrs, PhD)
The majority of participants indicated that the duration of the workshop was appropriate. There was consensus that a shorter duration would not have allowed sufficient time to complete the activity, yet a longer duration may have restricted uptake due to individual time constraints.
“Yes, I think it was. If anything, we were all going, can we do another one, and then going, actually, no, we really ought to get back to our desks. And I think if it had been less…if it had been more than two hours, you'd have probably ended up with less signups, we’d be going, I can’t really afford that time. But actually, once you're there, you're going, this is really fun, we want to just keep grinding stones. So, yes, I think two hours was a good amount of time, actually, yes.” (Female, 25-34yrs, PhD)
Additionally, participants noted that the two-hour workshop struck an ideal balance. It provided educational material at the beginning to offer archaeological context for the crafting, while also allowing ample time for the practical creative activity.
“I’d say I enjoyed the combination. I think it was nice to have that introduction first and overview, before we got going, rather than just launching straight into the pottery making without any context.” (Male, 18-24yrs, PhD)
As well as there being a consensus that the duration was appropriate, there was further evidence that offering a series of workshops rather than a one-off event would be desirable. Some participants discussed the potential of the workshop in the context of social prescribing.
“Multiple exposure, I think, is better, I think I would have preferred to do it more times and be exposed more to the activities and the environment and things, over a period, rather than once.” (Female, 25-34yrs, PhD)
“I think it could end up being a really good social prescribing intervention that people could be referred to, even if it is that one-off session, I think it will be beneficial, but it would also be great as a session, like a series of sessions.” (Female, 35-44yrs, PhD)
While there was a consensus that offering a series of workshops could be beneficial, participants who lived off-campus suggested that the demands of travel to the centre could be a barrier to regular attendance.
“I just don’t know how personally whether I would have been able to because I think it would have been quite tiring for me. However, was it something nearby? Yes, I definitely think I would attend six different sessions on various different crafty Anglo-Saxony Stone Age things, I would definitely give it a go because I think it would be a lot of fun. But yes, travelling to York six weeks in a row? Probably not.” (Female, 35-44yrs, PhD)
To address the challenge of commuting for off-campus residents, one participant suggested that the facilitator could direct students to comparable activities nearby.
“I think something which would be useful actually, was if the workshop was able to then, at the end of the workshop there was a signpost way to find out more about those kinds of things.” (Male, 18-24yrs, PhD)
This study explored the feasibility and acceptability of offering a novel intervention that combined heritage crafting in an outdoor setting with natural features for mental health and wellbeing. The findings offer preliminary evidence that these workshops were feasible to deliver, as the study achieved its recruitment target within the required timeframe and had high uptake across two terms despite variable weather. We also demonstrated high levels of feasibility to undertake a robust evaluation of the health benefits of this intervention, as we collected a range of demographic and outcome-based data, with high retention of data across two timepoints. These findings highlight these tools and metrics have high face validity and acceptability. Additionally, the workshops were positively received with participants citing satisfaction with the opportunity to engage in an outdoor creative task that had educational value, provided for social interaction in a group format, and lasted for an optimal amount of time. Therefore, it appears feasible and acceptable to deliver this creative and archeologically informed workshop with few modifications required from its original content, and it could play a role in the context of alternative place-based wellbeing interventions.
While interacting with nature and creativity can enhance wellbeing for various populations (11–14), there is limited research that has examined the intersection of both creative tasks in the context of outdoor natural spaces. Younger generations are leading increasingly urban and technologically-centred lives, and a decline in the level of outdoor experiences and everyday connection with nature has been reported (72). However, existing studies exploring the impact of nature on students have found higher availability and use of outdoor space is associated with improved quality of life and perceived restoration (73, 74). ART and SRT state that natural environments offer a setting where participants are able to restore their directed attention, defined as conscious attention required for cognitive tasks. As this cognitive focus can become fatigued following prolonged mental activity, nature can offer an entirely different setting to gently distract people from the stressors of everyday life (75). Our findings align with previous research, indicating that participants recognized the benefits of outdoor environments. Spending time in natural settings facilitated detachment from the daily attentional demands and stressors. While practical challenges with the outdoor space were reported (e.g., lack of surface space, potential for inclement weather), these challenges did not affect the overall positive perception of the workshop.
In terms of creative activity, participants were offered the opportunity to create a product based on personal preferences and interests, which is crucial for enhanced wellbeing (15). Thematic analysis indicated that participants highly valued the ability to engage in activities they found personally meaningful, with many describing the crafting process as therapeutic and a welcome distraction from daily stress. Evidence supports that a key factor for improved wellbeing is the relevance and meaning incorporated into the crafting process (16, 18, 76). Participants echoed this, noting that the sensory aspects and historical context of the activities added depth and significance to their experience. The entire crafting process, including hands-on engagement and the educational component, was cited as enhancing their sense of satisfaction and enjoyment. This aligns with existing evidence that meaningful crafting activities, encompassing personal relevance and sensory engagement, are imperative for achieving wellbeing (15, 77).
In addition to the creative and environmental aspects of the intervention, participants frequently cited satisfaction with the educational and archaeological content and context of the workshop. Research has indicated that heritage interventions can increase participant knowledge (78, 79), and appreciation of the natural, social, and cultural environment (79). Subsequently, this can facilitate a sense of belonging to the community (78) and an increase in responsible attitudes of care for heritage assets (80). Heritage education develops identity links, whether individually or communally (81), and is linked to various factors including the importance of teachers being innovative and capable to deliver heritage education (79). This aligns with our current findings as skilled facilitation was commonly cited as a key contextual factor of the workshops, whereby the facilitator had extensive archaeological knowledge and expertise. In these senses, this innovative intervention, which combines archaeological-based creative activities with connection to nature, provides valuable insights into new approaches for enhancing capacity and access to alternative place-based wellbeing interventions.
Group-based delivery also appeared to be a key enabler to implementation. This has been shown to be relevant to other creative-based activities that aim to promote wellbeing for a range of populations. For example, previous studies have reported group-based delivery enhanced a sense of belonging (82), improved social relationships (83), social inclusion (84), and the opportunity for learning (85). Moreover, participants reported how shared, yet individual goals, made the workshops socially inclusive. This sense of inclusivity and shared understanding was felt as one of the key contributing factors to wellbeing as it facilitated the establishment of new connections (31). Evidence reports that a crucial component of good mental health is connectedness (86), which is conceptualised as a sense of belonging, perceived sense of support, a sense of perceived closeness to a group, and comfort in discussing problems with others (87). Therefore, providing future workshops in a group format is likely to enhance wellbeing by maximising peer-to-peer support.
Lastly, the majority of participants reported that the length of the workshops was beneficial and offered an ideal balance for educational material at the beginning to provide archaeological context to the crafting, and sufficient time to meaningfully engage with the creative process. Crafting activities are often structured to include an antecedent component prior to the crafting itself, such as art viewing or education (31, 88–90). While this structure has been successfully implemented and positive outcomes are often reported, longer-term heritage crafting activities have the potential to be more impactful, yield sustained mental and cognitive benefits, and offer participants the opportunity to engage in a wider range of crafting activities (89, 91). Therefore, future research should consider moving beyond ‘temporary relief’ provided by a one-off session and incorporate access to comparable workshops over the longer term. Here there are opportunities to exploit growing interest in the role of social prescribing and to evaluate the mental health and wellbeing benefits of outdoor heritage crafting in populations with high vulnerabilities for mental health problems.
Generalisability of our findings is limited by several factors. Firstly, our study recruited a convenience sample of student volunteers. While this is a commonly used approach in feasibility studies, students vary as much as the general population both between and within countries, therefore, generalising from students to the general public becomes problematic (92). Secondly, because participants self-selected to join the study, selection bias may influence the findings. For example, it may be possible that only individuals who were interested in craft heritage participated in the study, resulting in the overall positive perception of the workshops. However, there is no ‘one-size fits all’ approach to wellbeing interventions, and personal preferences for activities will inevitably vary depending on personal interests. Lastly, the sample was predominantly comprised of females (68.8%). However, this is a commonly cited finding in research exploring the impact of cultural and crafting activities (76, 93, 94). Despite these limitations, our findings contribute further insight into new approaches that may have the potential to enhance access to alternative wellbeing interventions.
This study has shown that it is feasible to deliver and evaluate an archaeologically informed outdoor heritage crafting intervention to support wellbeing. The workshops were perceived to be acceptable and highly beneficial among a student volunteer population. Critical to the success of the workshops was the role of a skilled facilitator with an archaeological background, pointing to the relevance of offering participants activities that have heritage value. Furthermore, offering the workshops in an outdoor space with natural features bolstered participants’ positive ratings of the intervention, highlighting the potential to accrue wellbeing benefits from contact with and connection with nature. The added value of undertaking these activities in groups was also key to high levels of engagement, suggesting that the intervention offers opportunities to build social connections and draw on peer-to-peer learning. There is scope to undertake further and definitive evaluation of the mental health and wellbeing benefits of outdoor heritage crafting and respond to policy imperatives to develop preventive approaches to mental health using community and place-based approaches.
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
The studies involving humans were approved by Department of Archaeology at the University of York. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
ES: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. PD: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Writing – review & editing. SP: Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing. PW: Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing. AN: Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing. AL: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. GP: Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing. PC: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the University of York via the York Environmental Sustainability Institute as part of the Environmental Sustainability at York initiative. ES, PD. and PC were supported by the NIHR Yorkshire and Humber Applied Research Collaboration: https://arc-yh.nihr.ac.uk.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
The views expressed are those of the authors, and not necessarily those of the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2025.1556230/full#supplementary-material
1. James, SL, Abate, D, Abate, KH, et al. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2017: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2017. Lancet. (2018) 392:1789–858. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32279-7
2. World Health Organisation (2018) Mental health: Strengthening our response. Available online at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/mental-health-strengthening-our-response (Accessed April 22, 2024).
3. Lambert, L, Passmore, H-A, and Holder, MD. Foundational frameworks of positive psychology: mapping well-being orientations. Can Psychol. (2015) 56:311–21. doi: 10.1037/cap0000033
4. Ryan, R, Huta, V, and Deci, E. Living well: a self-determination theory perspective on eudaimonia. J Happiness Stud. (2008) 9:139–70. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9023-4
5. Sheppard, A, and Broughton, MC. Promoting wellbeing and health through active participation in music and dance: a systematic review. Int J Qual Stud Health Well Being. (2020) 15:1732526. doi: 10.1080/17482631.2020.1732526
6. Adler, A, and Seligman, ME. Using wellbeing for public policy: theory, measurement, and recommendations. Int J Wellbeing. (2016) 6:1–35. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v6i1.429
7. Confederation, WN (2018) Arts, health and wellbeing. Available online at: https://www.nhsconfed.org/publications/arts-health-and-wellbeing (Accessed March 12, 2024).
8. Capaldi, C, Passmore, H-A, Nisbet, E, et al. Flourishing in nature: A review of the benefits of connecting with nature and its application as a wellbeing intervention. Int. J Wellbeing. (2015)
9. Pritchard, A, Richardson, M, Sheffield, D, et al. The relationship between nature connectedness and Eudaimonic well-being: a Meta-analysis. J Happiness Stud. (2020) 21:1145–67. doi: 10.1007/s10902-019-00118-6
10. Kenning, G. ‘Fiddling with threads’: craft--based textile activities and positive well--being. Textiles. (2015) 13:50–65.
11. Camic, PM, Dickens, L, Zeilig, H, and Strohmaier, S. Subjective wellbeing in people living with dementia: exploring processes of multiple object handling sessions in a museum setting. Wellcome Open Research. (2021) 6:96. doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.16819.2
12. Fancourt, D, and Finn, S. What is the evidence on the role of the arts in improving health and well-being? A scoping review. Copenhagen: World Health Organization. Regional Office for Europe (2019). Available at: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32091683/
13. Jensen, A, and Bonde, LO. The use of arts interventions for mental health and wellbeing in health settings. Perspect Public Health. (2018) 138:209–14. doi: 10.1177/1757913918772602
14. Poulos, RG, Marwood, S, Harkin, D, et al. Arts on prescription for community-dwelling older people with a range of health and wellness needs. Health Soc Care Community. (2019) 27:483–92. doi: 10.1111/hsc.12669
15. Pöllänen, S. Elements of crafts that enhance well-being. J Leis Res. (2015) 47:58–78. doi: 10.1080/00222216.2015.11950351
16. Burt, EL, and Atkinson, J. The relationship between quilting and wellbeing. J Public Health. (2012) 34:54–9. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdr041
17. Maidment, J, and Macfarlane, S. Craft groups: sites of friendship, empowerment, belonging and learning for older women. Groupwork Interdiscip J Working Groups. (2009) 19:10–25. doi: 10.1921/095182409X471802
18. Collier, AF. The well-being of women who create with textiles: implications for art therapy. Art Ther. (2011) 28:104–12. doi: 10.1080/07421656.2011.597025
19. Leckey, J. The therapeutic effectiveness of creative activities on mental well-being: a systematic review of the literature. J Psychiatr Ment Health Nurs. (2011) 18:501–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2850.2011.01693.x
20. Csikszentmihalyi, M. Creativity. Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York: Harper Collins (1996).
21. Kudesia, R In: J Reb and PWP Atkins, editors. Mindfulness and creativity in the workplace. Mindfulness in organizations: Foundations, research, and applications. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press (2015)
22. Niedderer, K, and Townsend, K. Embodied craft practices: mindful flow, creativity, and collaboration as drivers for wellbeing. Craft Design Practice Embodied Perspective. (2024):66–80. doi: 10.4324/9781003328018-8
23. Huotilainen, M, Rankanen, M, Groth, C, et al. Why our brains love arts and crafts: implications of creative practices on psychophysical well-being. FormAkademisk. (2018) 11. doi: 10.7577/formakademisk.1908
24. Kabat-Zinn, J. Full catastrophe living: A practical guide to mindfulness, meditation, and healing. Nova York: Delacorte (1990).
25. Sedlmeier, P, Eberth, J, Schwarz, M, Zimmermann, D, Haarig, F, Jaeger, S, et al. The psychological effects of meditation: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. (2012) 138:1139–71. doi: 10.1037/a0028168
26. Henriksen, D, Richardson, C, and Shack, K. Mindfulness and creativity: implications for thinking and learning. Think Skills Creat. (2020) 37:100689. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2020.100689
27. Boerema, C, Russell, M, and Aguilar, A. Sewing in the lives of immigrant women. J Occup Sci. (2010) 17. doi: 10.1080/14427591.2010.9686678
28. Riley, J, Corkhill, B, and Morris, C. The benefits of knitting for personal and social wellbeing in adulthood: findings from an international survey. Br J Occup Ther. (2013) 76:50–7. doi: 10.4276/030802213X13603244419077
29. Ding, H. Digital protection and development of intangible cultural heritage relying on high-performance computing. Comput Intell Neurosci. (2022) 2022:4955380. doi: 10.1155/2022/4955380
30. Li, J. Research on digital display of intangible cultural heritage. J Educ Human Soci Sci. (2023) 8:54–61. doi: 10.54097/ehss.v8i.4224
31. Darvill, T, Heaslip, V, Drysdale, L, et al. Historic landscapes and mental well-being. Oxford: Archaeopress (2019).
32. APPGAHW (2017) Creative health: The arts for health and wellbeing. Available online at: https://www.culturehealthandwellbeing.org.uk/appg-inquiry/ (Accessed March 24, 2024).
33. Grossi, E, Tavano Blessi, G, and Sacco, PL. Magic moments: determinants of stress relief and subjective wellbeing from visiting a cultural heritage site. Cult Med Psychiatry. (2019) 43:4–24. doi: 10.1007/s11013-018-9593-8
34. Power, A, and Smyth, K. Heritage, health and place: the legacies of local community-based heritage conservation on social wellbeing. Health Place. (2016) 39:160–7. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2016.04.005
35. Sayer, F. Can digging make you happy? Archaeological excavations, happiness and heritage. Arts Health. (2015) 7:247–60. doi: 10.1080/17533015.2015.1060615
36. Fujiwara, D, Lawton, R, and Mourato, S (2015) The health and wellbeing benefits of public libraries full report. Arts Council England and SIMETRICA.
37. Ander, E, Thomson, L, Noble, G, Lanceley, A, Menon, U, and Chatterjee, H. Heritage, health and well-being: assessing the impact of a heritage focused intervention on health and well-being. Int J Herit Stud. (2013) 19:229–42. doi: 10.1080/13527258.2011.651740
38. Solway, R, Thompson, L, Camic, PM, et al. Museum object handling groups in older adult mental health inpatient care. Int J Ment Health Promot. (2015) 17:201–14. doi: 10.1080/14623730.2015.1035520
39. Tacon, PS. Connecting to the ancestors: why rock art is important for indigenous Australians and their well-being. Rock Art Research. (2019) 36:5–14.
40. Gražulevičiūtė, I. Cultural heritage in the context of sustainable development. Environ Res Eng Manag. (2006) 37
41. Pennington, A, Jones, R, Bagnall, A-M, et al. (2019) Heritage and wellbeing: the impact of historic places and assets on community wellbeing - a scoping review. Available online at: https://whatworkswellbeing.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/01/Heritage-scoping-review-March-2019-1.pdf (Accessed April 13, 2024).
42. Bratman, GN, Anderson, CB, Berman, MG, et al. Nature and mental health: an ecosystem service perspective. Sci Adv. (2019) 5:eaax0903. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aax0903
43. Coventry, PA, Brown, JE, Pervin, J, et al. Nature-based outdoor activities for mental and physical health: systematic review and meta-analysis. SSM Popul Health. (2021) 16:100934. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2021.100934
44. Kaplan, S. The restorative benefits of nature: toward an integrative framework. J Environ Psychol. (1995) 15:169–82. doi: 10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2
45. Ulrich, RS, Simons, RF, Losito, BD, Fiorito, E, Miles, MA, and Zelson, M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J Environ Psychol. (1991) 11:201–30. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7
46. Sianoja, M, Syrek, CJ, de Bloom, J, et al. Enhancing daily well-being at work through lunchtime park walks and relaxation exercises: recovery experiences as mediators. J Occup Health Psychol. (2018) 23:428–42. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000083
47. Menardo, E, Di Marco, D, Ramos, S, et al. Nature and mindfulness to cope with work-related stress: a narrative review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:5948. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19105948
48. Capaldi, CA, Dopko, RL, and Zelenski, JM. The relationship between nature connectedness and happiness: a meta-analysis. Front Psychol. (2014) 5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00976
49. Ganga, RN, Davies, L, Wilson, K, et al. The social value of place-based creative wellbeing: a rapid review and evidence synthesis. Sociol Health Illn. (2025) 47:e13827. doi: 10.1111/1467-9566.13827
50. Tjarve, B, and Zemīte, I. The role of cultural activities in community development. Acta Universitatis Agriculturae et Silviculturae Mendelianae Brunensis. (2016) 64:2151–60. doi: 10.11118/actaun201664062151
51. Žilič Fišer, S, and Kožuh, I. The impact of cultural events on community reputation and pride in Maribor, the European Capital of Culture 2012. Soc Indic Res. (2019) 142:1055–73. doi: 10.1007/s11205-018-1958-4
52. Jager, J, Putnick, DL, and Bornstein, MH. Ii. More THAN JUST convenient: the scientific merits of homogeneous convenience SAMPLES. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. (2017) 82:13–30. doi: 10.1111/mono.12296
53. Bates, J, Needham, A, Conneller, C, et al. Flint awls at the Mesolithic site of star Carr: understanding tool use through integrated methods. J Archaeol Sci Rep. (2022) 43:103478
54. Milner, A, Bamforth, M, Beale, G, Carty, J, Chatzipanagis, K, and Croft, SCK. A unique, engraved pendant from the early Mesolithic site of star Carr. Internet Archaeology: UK (2016). 41 p.
55. Needham, A, Little, A, Conneller, C, Pomsta, D, Croft, S, and Milner, N. Beads and pendant In: N Milner, C Conneller, and B Taylor, editors. Star Carr: Volume 2: Studies in technology, subsistence and environment. York, UK: White Rose Press (2018)
56. Brisbane, M. Anglo-Saxon pottery, production and social status In: P Rahtz, T Dickinson, and L Watts, editors. Anglo-Saxon cemeteries. Oxford, UK: BAR British Series (1980). 209–16.
57. Hirst, S, and Clark, D. Excavations at mucking: Volume 3, the Anglo-Saxon cemeteries (2 vols). London, UK: Museum of London Archaeology (2009).
58. Perry, GJ. Beer, butter and burial: the pre-burial origins of cremation urns form the early Anglo-Saxon cemetery of Cleatham, North Lincolnshire. Medieval Ceramics. (2011) 32
59. Myres, JNL. A Corpus of Anglo-Saxon Pottery of the Pagan Period, 2 vols. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press (1977).
60. Shah, N, Cader, M, Andrews, B, McCabe, R, and Stewart-Brown, SL. Short Warwick-Edinburgh mental well-being scale (SWEMWBS): performance in a clinical sample in relation to PHQ-9 and GAD-7. Health Qual Life Outcomes. (2021) 19:1–9. doi: 10.1186/s12955-021-01882-x
61. Kroenke, K, Strine, TW, Spitzer, RL, Williams, JBW, Berry, JT, and Mokdad, AH. (2009) The PHQ-8 as a measure of current depression in the general population. J Affect Disord. 114:163–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.06.026
62. Plummer, F, Manea, L, Trepel, D, et al. Screening for anxiety disorders with the GAD-7 and GAD-2: a systematic review and diagnostic metaanalysis. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. (2016) 39:24–31. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2015.11.005
63. Kroenke, K, Spitzer, RL, and Williams, JB. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med. (2001) 16:606–13. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x
64. Russell, DW. UCLA loneliness scale (version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. J Pers Assess. (1996) 66:20–40. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6601_2
65. Lee, RS, Draper, M, and Lee, S. The social connectedness scale—revised. YouthRex Res Eval Exch. (2005):62–3.
66. Wilson, C, and Secker, J. Validation of the social inclusion scale with students. Soci Inclusion. (2015) 3:52–62. doi: 10.17645/si.v3i4.121
67. Richardson, M, Hunt, A, Hinds, J, et al. A measure of nature connectedness for children and adults: validation, performance, and insights. Sustain For. (2019) 11. doi: 10.3390/su11123250
68. MacKillop, J, and Anderson, EJ. Further psychometric validation of the mindful attention awareness scale (MAAS). J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (2007) 29:289–93. doi: 10.1007/s10862-007-9045-1
70. Negrín, F, Hernández-Fernaud, E, Hess, S, and Hernandez, B. Discrimination of urban spaces with different level of restorativeness based on the original and on a shorter version of hartig et al.’s perceived restorativeness scale. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:1735.
71. Tickle-Degnen, L. Nuts and bolts of conducting feasibility studies. Am J Occup Ther. (2013) 67:171–6. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2013.006270
72. Soga, M, and Gaston, KJ. Extinction of experience: the loss of human–nature interactions. Front Ecol Environ. (2016) 14:94–101. doi: 10.1002/fee.1225
73. Holt, EW, Lombard, QK, Best, N, et al. Active and passive use of green space, health, and well-being amongst university students. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:424. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16030424
74. van den Bogerd, N, Dijkstra, SC, Koole, SL, et al. Nature in the indoor and outdoor study environment and secondary and tertiary education students’ well-being, academic outcomes, and possible mediating pathways: a systematic review with recommendations for science and practice. Health Place. (2020) 66:102403. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2020.102403
75. Dilliard, K (2022) Potential benefits of the campus outdoor built environment on student stress, leading to improved retention.
76. Willekens, M, and Lievens, J. Who participates and how much? Explaining non-attendance and the frequency of attending arts and heritage activities. Poetics. (2016) 56:50–63. doi: 10.1016/j.poetic.2016.01.004
77. Genoe, MR, and Liechty, T. Meanings of participation in a leisure arts pottery programme. World Leis J. (2017) 59:91–104. doi: 10.1080/16078055.2016.1212733
78. Castro-Calviño, L, Rodríguez-Medina, J, and López-Facal, R. Heritage education under evaluation: the usefulness, efficiency and effectiveness of heritage education programmes. Human Soci Sci Commun. (2020) 7:146. doi: 10.1057/s41599-020-00639-z
79. Valencia Arnica, YK, Ccasani Rodriguez, JL, Rucano Paucar, FH, and Talavera-Mendoza, F. The status of didactic models for heritage education: a systematic review. Heritage. (2023) 6:7611–23. doi: 10.3390/heritage6120400
80. Hernández Carretero, AM, and Guillén Peñafiel, R (2017) La educación patrimonial en los manuales escolares de Educación Primaria: un recorrido desde LOGSE hasta LOMCE.
81. Trabajo-Rite, M, and Cuenca-López, JM. Student concepts after a didactic experiment in heritage education. Sustain For. (2020) 12:3046. doi: 10.3390/su12073046
82. Zarobe, L, and Bungay, H. The role of arts activities in developing resilience and mental wellbeing in children and young people a rapid review of the literature. Perspect Public Health. (2017) 137:337–47. doi: 10.1177/1757913917712283
83. Margrove, KL. Promoting the wellbeing and social inclusion of students through visual art at university: an open arts pilot project. J Furth High Educ. (2015) 39:147–62. doi: 10.1080/0309877X.2013.778967
84. Secker, J, Loughran, M, Heydinrych, K, et al. Promoting mental well-being and social inclusion through art: evaluation of an arts and mental health project. Arts Health. (2011) 3:51–60. doi: 10.1080/17533015.2010.541267
85. Paolantonio, P, Cavalli, S, Biasutti, M, et al. Art for ages: the effects of group music making on the wellbeing of nursing home residents. Front Psychol. (2020) 11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.575161
86. Weisz, JR, McCarty, CA, and Valeri, SM. Effects of psychotherapy for depression in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. (2006) 132:132–49. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.132
87. Barber, BK, and Schluterman, JM. Connectedness in the lives of children and adolescents: a call for greater conceptual clarity. J Adolesc Health. (2008) 43:209–16. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.01.012
88. Camic, PM, Baker, EL, and Tischler, V. Theorizing how art gallery interventions impact people with dementia and their caregivers. The Gerontologist. (2016) 56:1033–41. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnv063
89. Horghagen, S, Fostvedt, B, and Alsaker, S. Craft activities in groups at meeting places: supporting mental health users' everyday occupations. Scand J Occup Ther. (2014) 21:145–52. doi: 10.3109/11038128.2013.866691
90. Ullán, AM, Belver, MH, Badía, M, Moreno, C, Garrido, E, Gomez-Isla, J, et al. Contributions of an artistic educational program for older people with early dementia: an exploratory qualitative study. Dementia. (2013) 12:425–46. doi: 10.1177/1471301211430650
91. Garner, L. Creative expression: effectiveness of a weekly craft group with women who have experienced trauma. Open J Nur. (2015) 5:96–103. doi: 10.4236/ojn.2015.52011
92. Hanel, PH, and Vione, KC. Do student Samples provide an accurate estimate of the general public? PLoS One. (2016) 11:e0168354. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0168354
93. DiMaggio, P. Gender, networks, and cultural capital. Poetics. (2004) 32:99–103. doi: 10.1016/j.poetic.2004.02.004
94. Ter Bogt, TF, Delsing, MJ, Van Zalk, M, et al. Intergenerational continuity of taste: parental and adolescent music preferences. Soc Forces. (2011) 90:297–319. doi: 10.1093/sf/90.1.297
95. GOV.UK. (2020) The People and Nature Surveys for England. Available online at: https://www.gov.uk/government/collections/people-and-nature-survey-for-england (Accessed February 12, 2024).
Keywords: mental health, wellbeing, community, environment, crafting
Citation: Shoesmith E, Darcy PM, Piper S, White PCL, Needham A, Little A, Perry G and Coventry P (2025) Craftwell: a feasibility and acceptability study of outdoor heritage crafting for wellbeing and mental health. Front. Public Health. 13:1556230. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2025.1556230
Received: 06 January 2025; Accepted: 19 February 2025;
Published: 05 March 2025.
Edited by:
Yesen Zhong, City University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, ChinaReviewed by:
Carenza Lewis, University of Lincoln, United KingdomCopyright © 2025 Shoesmith, Darcy, Piper, White, Needham, Little, Perry and Coventry. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Emily Shoesmith, RW1pbHkuc2hvZXNtaXRoQHlvcmsuYWMudWs=
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.