Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 17 September 2024
Sec. Aging and Public Health
This article is part of the Research Topic International Perspectives on Older Adult Social Isolation and Loneliness View all 41 articles

Loneliness and pain among community-dwelling middle-aged and older Black, Latino, and White adults in the United States

  • 1Department of Disability and Human Development, University of Illinois Chicago, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 2School of Social Work, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, United States
  • 3USC Suzanne Dworak-Peck School of Social Work, Edward R. Roybal Institute on Aging, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 4Center on Aging and Behavioral Research, Division of Geriatrics and Palliative Medicine, Weill Cornell Medicine, New York, NY, United States
  • 5School of Social Work, Columbia University, New York, NY, United States
  • 6Division of Geriatrics and Palliative Medicine, Weill Cornell Medicine, New York, NY, United States
  • 7Department of Psychology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, United States
  • 8Department of Sociology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, United States

Background: Prior research has demonstrated a strong and independent association between loneliness and pain, but few studies to date have explored this relationship in racially and ethnically diverse groups of midlife and older adults. We drew on the diathesis stress model of chronic pain and cumulative inequality theory to examine the relationship of loneliness and the presence and intensity of pain in a nationally representative sample of Black, Latino, and White adults aged 50 or older in the United States.

Methods: Data were from Wave 3 of the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (n = 2,706). We used weighted logistic and ordinary least squares regression analyses to explore main and interactive effects of loneliness and race and ethnicity while adjusting for well-documented risk and protective factors (e.g., educational attainment, perceived relative income, inadequate health insurance, perceived discrimination) and salient social and health factors.

Results: Almost half (46%) of the participants reported feeling lonely and 70% reported the presence of pain. Among those who reported pain (n = 1,910), the mean intensity score was 2.89 (range = 1–6) and 22% reported severe or stronger pain. Greater loneliness was associated with increased odds of pain presence (AOR = 1.154, 95% CI [1.072, 1.242]) and higher pain intensity (β = 0.039, p < 0.01). We found no significant interaction effects involving Black participants. However, Latino participants who reported greater loneliness had significantly higher levels of pain (β = 0.187, p < 0.001) than their White counterparts with similar levels of loneliness.

Discussion: Loneliness is an important correlate of pain presence and intensity and may have a stronger effect on pain intensity among Latino adults aged 50 or older. We discuss clinical and research implications of these findings, including the need for more fine-grained analyses of different types of loneliness (e.g., social, emotional, existential) and their impact on these and other pain-related outcomes (e.g., interference). Our findings suggest a need for interventions to prevent and manage pain by targeting loneliness among middle-aged and older adults, particularly Latino persons.

Introduction

Pain refers to “an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with, or resembling that associated with, actual or potential tissue damage” [(1), p. 1976]. Pain is associated with significant impairments in mobility (2) and mood (3), earlier mortality (4), and increased medical expenditures (5). Pain disproportionately affects the growing population of midlife and older adults in the United States and constitutes a national research priority (5, 6). By 2060, the United States will be home to 95 million older adults (7). Of these, 1 in 3 will be Black or Latino (8, 9). To support the development of clinical programs, it is critical to unravel the complex individual variability in these populations and elucidate determinants and modifiable factors (e.g., loneliness) that affect pain (10, 11). However, these examinations should consider common contextual challenges experienced by Black and Latino people in the United States.

Loneliness is public health challenge [(12), p. iv] that occurs when people perceive that the number and quality of their social relationships do not meet their needs for social integration, contact, and interaction (13, 14). Loneliness is distinct from but may co-occur with perceived social isolation, symptoms of depression, or both. Social isolation refers to the absence or limited number of social relationships (15, 16), whereas the main symptoms of clinical depression are lack of pleasure or interest in activities (17) and low mood (e.g., sadness).

A person may feel lonely at any age. However, restrictions in later life can limit engagement in socialization activities, although they may also protect against loneliness (18, 19). For example, older adulthood enhances the likelihood of health challenges [e.g., chronic diseases; (20)] and frailty (21) that may reduce physical functioning and limit socialization (22). Older adulthood also enhances the likelihood of loss of significant others, diminishing the pool of people on whom older adults may count. However, older adults’ anticipation of chronic disease, restrictions in physical functioning, and social loss may foster compensatory mechanisms, including concentrating on fewer but higher-quality relationships (18, 19).

The diathesis-stress biopsychosocial model of chronic pain (23) posits that biological, psychological, and social factors interact with stress (such as relationship loss and interpersonal stress) to increase the probability of acute, chronic, and more intense pain. Related biological factors include female sex (24), chronic musculoskeletal diseases [e.g., arthritis; (25)] and diabetes (26). Psychological factors include symptoms of depression and other types of mental distress (27). Social factors include socioeconomic status [fewer years of education; (28)] and low income (29). These factors all may be associated with the experience of pain, especially when multiple factors are present.

Pain can impair physical functioning, raise the probability of injury, and increase the experience of psychological distress, which in turn may be associated with worsening of pain (23), contributing to a cascade of increasing pain and distress that may affect other areas of functioning. One such impact is hypercortisolism, which results from overactivation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis. Overactivation of this axis is associated with chronic psychological distress (30, 31), effects on the musculoskeletal system, and tissue deterioration that lead to cycling patterns of pain and stress that cause chronic pain (32).

Hawkley and Cacioppo’s seminal work suggested that stress plays an integral role in understanding the link between loneliness and health outcomes. They found that people who reported loneliness were more likely than those who did not report loneliness to suffer more chronic stressors (33), perceive daily events as stressful (34), and experience increased activation of the sympathetic nervous system (13, 35). The latter involves prolonged stimulation of the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis (30, 31, 36), which contributes to systemic inflammation (37), physical decline (38) and in turn, the likelihood of pain onset and worsening of pain intensity.

Loneliness is associated with mental and physical health outcomes (39), including pain presence and intensity (40, 41). Studies have identified a relationship between loneliness and pain, mostly in adults with specific clinical profiles, including cancer survivors (4244), people with fibromyalgia (41, 45), and individuals who report chronic pain (46). In a study with terminally ill patients in Hong Kong, Chan et al. (47) found that loneliness moderated the relationship between depressive symptoms and pain intensity. However, their findings indicated that compared to individuals without loneliness, those with loneliness reported lower levels of pain intensity. In a U.S. study of adults with serious mental illness (i.e., major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia spectrum), Fortuna et al. (48) found that loneliness was associated with greater pain interference. Finally, Wilson et al. (49) conducted a longitudinal online study with 93 predominantly U.S. White participants with chronic pain during the COVID-19 pandemic. They reported that loneliness was associated with greater pain catastrophizing 1 year later and that depressive symptoms fully mediated this relationship.

Despite growing interest, multiple gaps persist in knowledge of the relationship between loneliness and pain. First, experiences and management of both loneliness and pain may be shaped by sampling source. Clinical and community samples have important differences. Reporting pain presence and intensity, connecting to health care services, and obtaining treatment are influenced by subjective interpretations of the pain experience (50), barriers to treatment [e.g., having medical insurance; (51)], and cultural beliefs (52). For example, individuals who attend specialty pain clinics are likely to disclose pain to receive specialized pain care. However, community-dwelling individuals may or may not attend or receive pain treatment (e.g., due to cultural beliefs or a lack of adequate health insurance). Research with community-dwelling samples has the advantage of providing more generalizable results.

Second, the prevalence and experience of loneliness and pain vary across countries [(e.g., 53, 54)]. International studies have linked loneliness and pain in both clinical and community samples. Jacobs et al. (55) found that loneliness was positively associated with chronic back pain in a sample of older adults in West Jerusalem. Loneliness was also associated with the presence of acute and chronic pain in a study of adults in the United Kingdom (56). During the COVID 19 pandemic, Yamada et al. (57) found that loneliness was associated with the presence of acute and chronic pain and pain intensity in Japan. Nieto et al. (58) reported that loneliness triggered pain episodes in a sample of Spanish adults with chronic pain. Two recent analyses of longitudinal data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing found a bidirectional relationship between pain and loneliness (59, 60). Few studies have examined the relationship of loneliness and pain among community-dwelling midlife and older adults in the United States. Using data from the U.S. Health and Retirement Study, Powell et al. (61, 62) reported that loneliness was associated with greater presence of moderate to severe pain that interfered with everyday functioning.

Finally, there has been little research on loneliness among midlife and older Black or Latino people. Ojembe et al. (63) and Tibiriçá et al. (64) reported a link between loneliness and health outcomes, including frailty, cardiovascular disorders, and self-rated health. But to our knowledge, no studies have explored the relationship of loneliness and pain by race and ethnicity. Powell et al. (61, 62) reported on a racially and ethnically diverse sample of community-dwelling adults aged 50 or older, but not on interactions between race and ethnicity and loneliness. One recent study using National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (NSHAP) data supported the need to examine racial and ethnic differences in the impact of loneliness on health. Camacho et al. (65) noted that the relationship between loneliness and cognitive functioning varied across Black, Latino, and White individuals.

Reviews on the health impact of stress exposure on physical and mental health (66, 67) provided a strong basis for considering cumulative and joint effects of stressful experiences such as loneliness on pain. For example, low levels of education and low income were positively associated with the presence of loneliness and pain (6870). U.S. Black and Latino individuals were more likely than same-aged White individuals to experience socioeconomic stressors such as low levels of education and income (66, 67). Because loneliness enhances stressful perceptions of daily events or challenges (13, 35), the reciprocal relationship of risk exposure and loneliness may contribute to prolonged and higher levels of stress among Black and Latino persons across the life course. Experiencing more stress and its biological impact could accelerate physical decline and contribute to a cyclical relationship between stress and physical dysregulation that results in worse pain outcomes in midlife and older Black and Latino individuals (23). We thus suggest that the impact of loneliness on pain presence and intensity may be worse for Black and Latino individuals than their White counterparts.

In sum, there is a need to examine the relationship of loneliness and pain in community dwelling, racially and ethnically diverse midlife and older adults in the United States. In the current study, we sought to examine the effects of loneliness on pain presence and intensity among Black, Latino, and White adults aged 50 or older in the NSHAP study who completed the three-item NSHAP Felt Loneliness Measure (NFLM) and assessments of pain presence and intensity (71). We also examined if race and ethnicity moderated the relationship between loneliness and pain. We hypothesized that (1) loneliness would be positively associated with pain presence and pain intensity and (2) Black and Latino individuals would report greater pain presence and intensity compared to White individuals with the same levels of loneliness.

Method

Data source

The NSHAP is a nationally representative survey of midlife and older adults living in the community. It was designed to assess the physical, mental, and social well-being of home-dwelling midlife and older Americans (72). We analyzed Wave 3 data (n = 4,777; collected in 2015 and 2016), which included in-person interviews with two cohorts: (a) respondents continuing from the first rounds of interviews (born 1920–1947) and (b) newly recruited participants (born 1948–1965). Live-in partners of both cohorts were also eligible for interviews. In addition to in-person interviews, participants were asked to complete leave-behind, self-administered questionnaires and up to 11 biological measures.

We analyzed data from Wave 3 for two reasons. First, this wave included measures of pain and loneliness. The leave-behind questionnaire also included concepts of theoretical interest to the analyses—e.g., community participation and perceived discrimination. Second, inclusion of a fresh sample of midlife and older adults and live-in partners in Wave 3 increased the sample size for hypothesis testing and included the baby boomer cohort. Final return rates for the leave-behind questionnaire were 85% for the full sample, 91% for continuing participants, and 80% for newly recruited participants (73). The University of Chicago’s National Opinion Research Center collected the data in English and Spanish.

Population

Our target population was home-dwelling adults aged 50 or older who completed the NFLM and self-reported pain items (presence and intensity) and were Black, Latino, or White. Our final weighted sample was 2,706 individuals who were White (n = 2,252), Black (n = 276), or Latino (n = 178).

Dependent variables

Pain presence was determined by response to the question: “In the past 4 weeks, have you had any pain?” (71). Intensity was measured by an original survey item that asked participants to check the box beside the phrase that best described their level of pain in the past 4 weeks [0 = no pain to 6 = the most intense pain imaginable, (71, 74)]. We only included individuals who reported pain.

Independent variables

The NFLM assessed loneliness. Similar to the three-item UCLA Loneliness Scale, it assesses perceived frequency of lack of companionship, feeling left out, and feeling isolated (0 = never, 1 = hardly ever, 2 = some of the time, 3 = often). Following Payne et al. (75), we used a cutoff of 1 to determine the presence of loneliness and combined the categories of “never” and “hardly ever.” Total scores ranged from 0 to 6, with higher scores indicating greater levels of loneliness.

Covariates

We include available NSHAP measures indicative of individual cumulative inequality factors (education, perceived economic status, missing health care due to inadequate health insurance, perceived discrimination) to examine their effects on pain (66, 67). Available measures did not fully capture the complexity of these experiences, including their magnitude, onset, or duration of exposure and their associated advantages and disadvantages across the life course and social systems. Thus, we used the NSHAP-recommended categories of race and ethnicity to gauge group differences resulting from cumulative inequality.

Race and ethnicity was assessed by two questions: “Do you consider yourself primarily White or Caucasian, Black or African American, American Indian, Asian, or something else?” and “Do you consider yourself Hispanic or Latino?” We used an NSHAP-coded race and ethnicity variable that classified participants into four mutually exclusive groups: (a) non-Hispanic White, (b) Black (including Hispanics who self-reported Black race), (c) Hispanic (all races except Black), and (d) other (e.g., Asian, Native American, Pacific Islander).

Lower levels of education and income have been associated with loneliness and pain in U.S. samples of midlife and older adults (6870). Further, access to health care (e.g., adequate insurance) may contribute to better preventive care that can improve health outcomes (76). Educational attainment was measured as 1 (less than high school), 2 (high school or equivalent), 3 (vocational certificate, some college, or associate degree), and 4 (bachelor’s degree or more).

Perceived economic position was determined by the question: “Compared with American families in general, would you say that your household income is 1 = far below average; 2 = below average; 3 = average; 4 = above average, or 5 = far above average?” We treated this variable as continuous, with higher scores indicating higher perceived economic position.

Participants reported difficulty receiving health care services because of a lack of adequate insurance: (a) “In the past year, has a lack of adequate health insurance kept you from getting medical care?” and (b) “In the past year, has a lack of adequate health insurance kept you from getting prescription medications?” If participants answered “yes” to either original item, then they were coded as having inadequate health care insurance.

Perceived discrimination was measured by a two-item adapted version of the Perceived Discrimination Scale (77, 78): “In your day-to-day life, how often have you been treated with less courtesy than other people?” and “In your day-to-day life, how often have people acted as if they are better than you are?” Responses options were 0 (never), 1 (less than once a year), 2 (about once or twice a year), 3 (several times a year), 4 (about once a month), 5 (every week), and 6 (several times a week). We summed both items to create a total score (range = 0–12).

Age (79), sex (24), and marital status (80) are important predictors of pain. Sociodemographic variables included age (in years), sex (0 = male, 1 = female), and marital status (1 = married or living as married, 2 = divorced, separated, or never married, 3 = widowed).

We assessed employment status with the question: “Are you currently working?” (1 = yes, 0 = no). We determined the number of chronic diseases by responses to the stem: “Has a medical doctor ever told you that you have…,” followed by options of heart disease, arthritis, breathing problems, stroke, hypertension, diabetes, and cancer (range = 0–7). The NSHAP Depressive Symptoms Measure is similar to the CES-D depression instrument (75, 81) and assesses the frequency of 11 self-reported depressive symptoms in the past week (0 = rarely or none of the time, 1 = some of the time, 2 = much or most of the time). We removed the item “I feel lonely” because it conceptually overlapped with our measures of loneliness (82). Total scores for the remaining 10 items (range = 0–20, Cronbach’s α = 0.692). Finally, a trained research assistant measured height rounded to the nearest half-inch using a stiff tape measure and weight using a scale on a flat, uncarpeted surface [(83), Electronic Supplement 1]. We calculated body mass index (BMI) per the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (84).

Social relations may contribute to pain outcomes (85). Absent a direct measure of social isolation, we included two related concepts of social relationships (86). Community participation during the past 12 months was assessed by three items that examined frequency of volunteer work, attendance of social meetings, and gatherings with friends or relatives (87). Possible responses were 0 (never), 1 (less than once a year), 2 (about once or twice a year), 3 (several times a year), 4 (about once a month), 5 (every week), and 6 (several times per week). We summed the three scores (range = 0–18). Finally, lacking a household roster, we assessed living arrangement (alone vs. with others) based on social network questions that asked whether each person listed in the respondent’s networks lived in their home. Individuals were designated as living alone if they reported that nobody in their social network lived in their residence.

Analytic strategy

We calculated descriptive statistics for the sample, then conducted bivariate tests (chi-square or Pearson correlations) for primary predictors and pain outcomes. We examined the association of (a) loneliness with pain presence and (b) loneliness and intensity among respondents who reported pain (range = 1–6). Adjusted models included fixed classification factors: race and ethnicity (Black, Latino, White); sex (male, female); marital status (married or living with partner; divorced, separated, or never married; widowed); educational attainment (bachelor’s degree or more; vocation certificate, some college, or associate degree; high school or equivalent; less than high school); employment status (no, yes); inadequate health care (no, yes); and living alone (no, yes). Covariates included NFLM score, perceived discrimination score (range = 0–14), perceived economic positioning (range = 1–5), age (in years), chronic medical conditions (range = 0–7), depressive symptoms (range = 0–20), community participation (range = 0–18), and BMI.

We conceptualized racial and ethnic groups as a proxy for exposure to differential risks and opportunities and controlled for factors that contribute to cumulative inequality in health outcomes, including educational attainment, perceived economic position, inadequate health care insurance, and perceived discrimination. To test differences in the impact of loneliness on pain outcomes across races and ethnicities, we examined the interaction of loneliness score (NFLM) and race and ethnicity, controlling for exposure to risk, opportunity, sociodemographic, and health factors.

We used logistic regression and ordinary least squares regression to model pain presence and intensity, respectively. We used NSHAP-generated person-level weights that accounted for nonresponse for all statistical analyses [(see 72) for details on weighting procedures]. We used the recommended Wave 3 NSHAP variable (weight_adj), which assigns different weights (by simulated replication) each case to provide unbiased estimates of population parameters (72). Assigned weights indicated the number of observations represented by each case. We excluded cases with missing values. We also used linear mixed models to control for nesting among subjects (i.e., spouses or partners from the same household). We used SPSS version 27 survey procedures for analyses.

Results

We present results for our original unnested models. Our nested analyses supported the robustness of our primary findings. Table 1 presents descriptive data on the weighted sample of 2,706 NSHAP participants with and without pain and bivariate statistics of study variables. Approximately 70% of the sample reported pain during the previous 4 weeks. Among those with pain, mean pain intensity was moderate at 2.89 (SD = 0.99). The average score on the NFLM was 1.11 (SD = 1.50); 46% of participants scored 1 or higher, indicating loneliness (75). In bivariate analyses, higher NFLM scores were positively associated with pain presence and intensity.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Weighted descriptive statistics overall and by pain group and weighted bivariate analyses of pain presence and intensity, National Social Life, Health, and Aging, Wave 3 (N = 2,706).

Compared to White respondents, Black and Latino participants reported lower rates of pain but Latino individuals experienced greater pain intensity. Female sex; being divorced, separated, or never married; lower educational attainment; inadequate health care insurance; perceived discrimination; more chronic diseases; more depressive symptoms; and higher BMI were associated with greater pain presence and intensity scores. Perceived economic position and community participation scores were associated with lower pain presence and intensity scores. Widowhood was associated with greater pain intensity, whereas living alone was associated with lower pain intensity.

Relationship between loneliness and pain presence and intensity

Table 2 presents the results of adjusted logistic and ordinary least squares regression models. Loneliness was positively associated with higher rates of pain (adjusted odds ratio [AOR] = 1.154, 95% CI [1.072, 1.242]). Compared to their White counterparts, Black (AOR = 0.588, 95% CI [0.438, 0.789]) and Latino (AOR = 0.652, 95% CI [0.459, 0.925]) participants were less likely to report the presence of pain. Individuals missing health care services due to inadequate insurance (AOR = 1.893, 95% CI [1.366, 2.623]) and female participants (AOR = 1.540, 95% CI [1.283, 1.857]) were associated with more frequent pain presence. Higher levels of perceived discrimination (AOR = 1.092, 95% CI [1.050, 1.137]), more chronic diseases (AOR = 1.331, 95% CI [1.220, 1.451]), more depressive symptoms (AOR = 1.112, 95% CI [1.073, 1.153]), and higher BMI (AOR = 1.016, 95% CI [1.001, 1.031]) were associated with a higher likelihood of reporting the presence of pain.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Associations of loneliness, sociodemographic, and health variables on pain presence and intensity (N = 2,706).

With respect to pain intensity, NFLM scores (β = 0.039, SE = 0.015, p < 0.01) were associated with greater intensity. Latino participants (β = 0.269, SE = 0.108, p < 0.005) reported significantly higher pain intensity than White respondents. Compared to individuals with a bachelor’s degree or higher level of education, individuals who completed a vocational certificate, some college, or associate degree (β = 0.125, SE = 0.053, p = 0.018); high school or its equivalent (β = 0.150, SE = 0.065, p = 0.020); or less than high school (β = 0.184, SE = 0.092, p = 0.045) reported stronger pain intensity. Similarly, being female (β = 0.960, SE = 0.044, p = 0.028), reporting more chronic disease (β = 0.138, SE = 0.018, p < 0.001), total depressive symptoms (β = 0.045, SE = 0.008, p < 0.001), and BMI (β = 0.012, SE = 0.003, p < 0.001) were positively associated with greater pain intensity. Individuals who lived alone (β = −0.192, SE = 0.050, p < 0.001) and those who were employed (β = −0.199, SE = 0.049, p < 0.001) reported lower levels of pain intensity. Finally, as seen in Table 3, for Latino individuals, increased loneliness predicted greater increases in the intensity of pain relative to White individuals (β = 0.187, SE = 0.051, p < 0.001). No other interactions were significant.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Interactions between loneliness and race and ethnicity on pain presence and intensity.

Discussion

The current study examined the relationship between loneliness and pain presence and intensity in a nationally representative sample of midlife and older community-dwelling Black, Latino, and White adults in the United States. Individuals who reported loneliness were significantly more likely to report both the presence of pain and higher levels of pain intensity. Contrary to our hypothesis, Black race did not moderate the association of loneliness with pain presence or pain intensity. However, loneliness contributed to more intense pain for Latino individuals compared to their White counterparts. These findings contribute to the limited literature regarding the effects of loneliness on pain outcomes in community-dwelling racially and ethnically diverse midlife and older adults in the United States.

Our findings support our hypothesis that loneliness would be positively associated with pain presence and pain intensity. These findings add to limited knowledge on loneliness as an important correlate of pain presence and intensity in racially and ethnically diverse community- dwelling midlife and older adults in the United States (61, 62). Our findings also suggest the need for further research. Few studies have explored mechanisms that underlie the relationship between loneliness and pain. In line with the diathesis-stress model of chronic pain (23), we suggest that loneliness may heighten stress levels that subsequently contribute to declines in physical functioning, presence of pain, and worse pain intensity (23, 3032, 36, 38). Future research should seek to clarify how objective and subjective stress measures mediate or moderate the relationship of loneliness with dimensions of pain.

We hypothesized that loneliness would be associated with pain presence, although previous studies have documented pain as a predictor of loneliness (8891) and the relationship is likely to be bidirectional (59, 60). The ongoing presence of unresolved pain may contribute to stress and negative cognitions that cyclically reinforce and increase feelings of loneliness. Given the high rates of each condition and their negative impact on multiple health outcomes (5, 40), future studies should explore correlates and predictors of bidirectionality. Powell et al. (62) noted a longitudinal relationship between loneliness and the presence of pain among midlife and older adults in the United States. More nuanced measures and longitudinal approaches will deepen our understanding of variations in the relationship of loneliness and pain depending on the chronicity of each experience.

Contrary to our hypotheses, loneliness and pain outcomes did not differ for middle-aged and older Black and White participants. Black individuals may underreport pain if they interpret it as a personal inadequacy or weakness (92, 93), normal part of aging (52), or obligation to be stalwart and limit disclosure or reporting of distress (94). Subjective perceptions and personal resources may also affect the level of stress caused by loneliness [(66, 95), p. 142]. Black individuals may appraise loneliness relative to other persistent contextual threats, such as economic adversity and social discrimination (66, 96). Further investigation of subjective assessments of how loneliness contributes to stress levels and ultimately, the presence and intensity of pain in subpopulations of midlife and older adults are thus warranted.

Latino participants who reported loneliness were not more likely to report the presence of pain than their White same-aged peers, but loneliness had a stronger effect on pain intensity, even after accounting for common sources of stress, depressive symptoms, and social relationship measures. Loneliness is an important and underexamined psychological experience for Latino midlife and older adults (64). Our findings suggest that it may enhance stress across shared lived experiences of cumulative inequality because social relationships are essential for coping with cumulative inequalities. For example, low income can challenge basic needs, whereas social networks can facilitate access to resources to meet these needs (97). But individuals experiencing loneliness may perceive their challenges as more severe (34) and identify fewer resources (33). Because Latino communities tend to experience more of these challenges (66, 67) than White communities, objective and subjective perceptions of the quantity and quality of relationships may contribute to spikes in stress and consequently, worsening physical health, including pain intensity.

Our findings also underscore the need to understand better loneliness and its health effects across diverse groups of Latino midlife and older adults. Due to data limitations, we could not determine important sources of diversity, including country of origin, years living in the United States, and citizenship status. Nativity, migration trajectories, and acculturation levels may expose people to different risks and opportunities that may contribute to loneliness, distress, and pain in later life. Loneliness is a common feature of migration, because individuals leave behind their homeland and loved ones and become part of a socially excluded and minoritized group in the United States [(e.g., 98)]. Loneliness may become a chronic problem for some, because more undocumented Latino midlife and older adults in the United States have limited access to social services, health care, and employment opportunities and protections; live with the constant threat of deportation; and experience social exclusion (99). They may also have fewer resources to mitigate loneliness, having left behind loved ones and lacking the resources and ability to leave and re-enter the United States (100). On the other hand, some Latino participants in our sample may have found effective ways of coping or overcoming stressors associated with loneliness. Further work that examines how diverse Latino subgroups make sense of and cope with loneliness is critical to understanding its relationships with stress and pain.

Finally, cultural factors may exacerbate the incidence and impact of loneliness on pain (11, 101). Cultural core values (e.g., communal vs. individualist), perceptions (e.g., nature and extent of closeness in relationships and social connectedness) and behaviors (e.g., help-seeking) may influence the meaning attached to loneliness and their sequelae (14, 102104). As Latino individuals age in the United States, they may prefer and expect to maintain close relationships, including multigenerational households (105). But acculturation may create discordance in their relationships with U.S.-born children and extended family (100, 106).

Limitations

This study had several limitations. First, we examined data from only one wave of the NSHAP and therefore, we could not establish directionality in the association of loneliness and pain presence or intensity. Second, our sample included different proportions of Black, Latino, and White groups. Future examinations with comparable subsample sizes may enhance understanding and possibly confirm racial and ethnic differences in the relationship between loneliness and pain outcomes. Third, we could not assess other relevant dimensions of older adults’ experiences with loneliness and pain, such as grief and loss, that may affect stress and pain (107), nor could we examine the association of these variables in other important subpopulations, such as sexual and gender minorities or people with disabilities. Finally, our data did not include assessments of social isolation. However, our models controlled for two related concepts (community participation, living alone). Future large national surveys should include validated measures of social isolation.

Conclusion

Our results reinforce previous findings that loneliness is independently associated both with the presence and intensity of pain among midlife and older adults. Further attention is needed to determine how various clinically relevant dimensions of loneliness, such as social, emotional, and existential (108), affect pain outcomes. Future research should also examine whether and how objective and subjective measures of stress mediate or moderate the relationship of loneliness and pain outcomes. In a heterogeneous society that struggles with health disparities across the life course, it will be important to further examine inter- and intragroup differences and identify unique and shared elements of loneliness–pain pathways for Black and Latino midlife and older adults.

Interventions exist for loneliness and pain, but few focus on these groups of midlife and older adults (109, 110). Future clinical and research efforts should examine how evidence-based interventions may be adapted and implemented to manage the onset, progression, and effects of loneliness and pain [e.g., PATH-Pain; (111)]. Finally, there is a growing need for culturally and linguistically proficient professionals and peer advocates, who have successfully led other cost-effective health interventions (112, 113), to address issues of loneliness and pain across populations and subgroups of older adults.

Data availability statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: National Social Life, Health & Aging Project data are restricted. Users interested in obtaining the Restricted-Use data from NACDA must request and complete the NSHAP Restricted Data Use Agreement form. Users can download this form from the download page associated with this data set. Completed forms with original signature(s) should be emailed to aWNwc3ItbmFjZGFAdW1pY2guZWR1. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to aWNwc3ItbmFjZGFAdW1pY2guZWR1.

Ethics statement

This study is a secondary analysis of de-identified data. The data were acquired for analysis under a data use agreement from the National Archive of Computerized Data on Aging that precluded the investigators from performing analyses that might re-identify participants in the study. Use of the de-identified data for this study was reviewed by the Weill Cornell Medicine Institutional Review Board. The original data were collected by the National Opinion Research Center which obtained informed consent from the participants.

Author contributions

DC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. DB: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. MA: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. JM: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. EL: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. MCR: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. EW: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by a fellowship from National Institutes of Health (T32 AG049666).Funding for this work to Dr. Aranda was provided by the National Institute on Aging (NIA) of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) under award numbers P30AG066530, and P30AG043073. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Raja, SN, Carr, DB, Cohen, M, Finnerup, NB, Flor, H, Gibson, S, et al. The revised IASP definition of pain: concepts, challenges, and compromises. Pain. (2020) 161:1976–82. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001939

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Peat, G, Thomas, E, Wilkie, R, and Croft, P. Multiple joint pain and lower extremity disability in middle and old age. Disabil Rehabil. (2006) 28:1543–9. doi: 10.1080/09638280600646250

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Bierman, A, and Lee, Y. Chronic pain and psychological distress among older adults: a national longitudinal study. Res Aging. (2018) 40:432–55. doi: 10.1177/0164027517704970

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Smith, D, Wilkie, R, Croft, P, Parmar, S, and McBeth, J. Pain and mortality: mechanisms for a relationship. Pain. (2018) 159:1112–8. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001193

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Simon, LS . Relieving pain in America: a blueprint for transforming prevention, care, education, and research. J Pain Palliat Care Pharmacother. (2012) 26:197–8. doi: 10.3109/15360288.2012.678473

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Nahin, RL . Estimates of pain prevalence and severity in adults: United States, 2012. J Pain. (2015) 16:769–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2015.05.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Administration of Community Living . Profile of older Americans. (2021). Available at: https://acl.gov/aging-and-disability-in-america/data-and-research/profile-older-americans

Google Scholar

8. Administration on Community Living . Profile of African Americans age 65 and over. (2019). Available at: https://acl.gov/sites/default/files/Aging%20and%20Disability%20in%20America/2018AA_OAProfile.pdf

Google Scholar

9. Administration on Community Living . Profile of Hispanic Americans age 65 and over. (2019). Available at: https://acl.gov/sites/default/files/Aging%20and%20Disability%20in%20America/2018HA_OAProfile.pdf

Google Scholar

10. Janevic, MR, Mathur, VA, Booker, SQ, Morais, C, Meints, SM, Yeager, KA, et al. Making pain research more inclusive: why and how. J Pain. (2022) 23:707–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2021.10.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Palermo, TM, Davis, KD, Bouhassira, D, Hurley, RW, Katz, JD, Keefe, FJ, et al. Promoting inclusion, diversity, and equity in pain science. Can J Pain. (2023) 7:2161272. doi: 10.1080/24740527.2022.2161272

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. National Academies of Sciences, Division of Behavioral, Social Sciences, Medicine Division, Board on Behavioral, Sensory Sciences, & Loneliness in Older Adults . Social isolation and loneliness in older adults: opportunities for the health care system. Washington, DC: National Academies Press (2020).

Google Scholar

13. Hawkley, LC, and Cacioppo, JT. Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Ann Behav Med. (2010) 40:218–27. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Perlman, D, and Peplau, LA. Toward a social psychology of loneliness. Pers Relat. (1981) 3:31–56.

Google Scholar

15. de Jong-Gierveld, J, van Tilburg, TG, and Dykstra, PA. Loneliness and social isolation In: AL Vangelisti and D Perlman, editors. The Cambridge handbook of personal relationships. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press (2006). 485–500.

Google Scholar

16. Perissinotto, CM, and Covinsky, KE. Living alone, socially isolated or lonely—what are we measuring? J Gen Intern Med. (2014) 29:1429–31. doi: 10.1007/s11606-014-2977-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Uher, R, Payne, JL, Pavlova, B, and Perlis, RH. Major depressive disorder in DSM-5: implications for clinical practice and research of changes from DSM-IV. Depress Anxiety. (2014) 31:459–71. doi: 10.1002/da.22217

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Baltes, MM, and Carstensen, LL. The process of successful aging: selection, optimization, and compensation In: UM Staudinger and U Lindenberger, editors. Understanding human development: Dialogues with lifespan psychology. Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic (2003). 81–104.

Google Scholar

19. Carstensen, LL . Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: support for socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychol Aging. (1992) 7:331–8.

Google Scholar

20. Vetrano, DL, Calderón-Larrañaga, A, Marengoni, A, Onder, G, Bauer, JM, Cesari, M, et al. An international perspective on chronic multimorbidity: approaching the elephant in the room. J Gerontol Ser A. (2018) 73:1350–6. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glx178

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Bandeen-Roche, K, Seplaki, CL, Huang, J, Buta, B, Kalyani, RR, Varadhan, R, et al. Frailty in older adults: a nationally representative profile in the United States. J Gerontol Ser A Biomed Sci Med Sci. (2015) 70:1427–34. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glv133

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hoogendijk, EO, Suanet, B, Dent, E, Deeg, DJ, and Aartsen, MJ. Adverse effects of frailty on social functioning in older adults: results from the longitudinal aging study Amsterdam. Maturitas. (2016) 83:45–50. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2015.09.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Bevers, K, Watts, L, Kishino, ND, and Gatchel, RJ. The biopsychosocial model of the assessment, prevention, and treatment of chronic pain. US Neurol. (2016) 12:98–104. doi: 10.17925/USN.2016.12.02.98

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Fillingim, RB, King, CD, Ribeiro-Dasilva, MC, Rahim-Williams, B, and Riley, JL 3rd. Sex, gender, and pain: a review of recent clinical and experimental findings. J Pain. (2009) 10:447–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.12.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Sarzi-Puttini, P, Salaffi, F, Di Franco, M, Bazzichi, L, Cassisi, G, Casale, R, et al. Pain in rheumatoid arthritis: a critical review. Reumatismo. (2014) 66:18–27. doi: 10.4081/reumatismo.2014.760

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Merashli, M, Chowdhury, TA, and Jawad, ASM. Musculoskeletal manifestations of diabetes mellitus. QJM. (2015) 108:853–7. doi: 10.1093/qjmed/hcv106

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Surah, A, Baranidharan, G, and Morley, S. Chronic pain and depression. Continuing Educ Anaesthesia Crit Care Pain. (2014) 14:85–9. doi: 10.1093/bjaceaccp/mkt046

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Großschädl, F, Stolz, E, Mayerl, H, Rásky, É, Freidl, W, and Stronegger, W. Educational inequality as a predictor of rising back pain prevalence in Austria—sex differences. Eur J Public Health. (2015) 26:248–53. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckv163

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Fliesser, M, Huberts, JDW, and Wippert, PM. The choice that matters: the relative influence of socioeconomic status indicators on chronic back pain—a longitudinal study. BMC Health Serv Res. (2017) 17:800. doi: 10.1186/s12913-017-2735-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Adam, EK, Hawkley, LC, Kudielka, BM, and Cacioppo, JT. Day-to-day dynamics of experience–cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2006) 103:17058–63. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605053103

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Dallman, MF, la Fleur, SE, Pecoraro, NC, Gomez, F, Houshyar, H, and Akana, SF. Minireview: glucocorticoids—food intake, abdominal obesity, and wealthy nations in 2004. Endocrinology. (2004) 145:2633–8. doi: 10.1210/en.2004-0037

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Gatchel, RJ . Comorbidity of chronic pain and mental health disorders: the biopsychosocial perspective. Am Psychol. (2004) 59:795–805. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.8.795

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hawkley, LC, and Cacioppo, JT. Aging and loneliness: downhill quickly? Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2007) 16:187–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00501.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Cacioppo, JT . Social neuroscience: Autonomic, neuroendocrine, and immune responses to stress. Psychophysiology. (1994) 31:113–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb01032.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Cacioppo, JT, Hawkley, LC, Ernst, JM, Burleson, M, Berntson, GG, Nouriani, B, et al. Loneliness within a nomological net: an evolutionary perspective. J Res Pers. (2006) 40:1054–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.11.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Steptoe, A, Owen, N, Kunz-Ebrecht, SR, and Brydon, L. Loneliness and neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, and inflammatory stress responses in middle-aged men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2004) 29:593–611. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00086-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Nersesian, PV, Han, HR, Yenokyan, G, Blumenthal, RS, Nolan, MT, Hladek, MD, et al. Loneliness in middle age and biomarkers of systemic inflammation: findings from midlife in the United States. Soc Sci Med. (2018) 209:174–81. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.04.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Gardner, MP, Lightman, S, Sayer, AA, Cooper, C, Cooper, R, Deeg, D, et al. Dysregulation of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis and physical performance at older ages: an individual participant meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2013) 38:40–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.04.016

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Ong, AD, Uchino, BN, and Wethington, E. Loneliness and health in older adults: a mini-review and synthesis. Gerontology. (2016) 62:443–9. doi: 10.1159/000441651

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Wolf, LD, and Davis, MC. Loneliness, daily pain, and perceptions of interpersonal events in adults with fibromyalgia. Health Psychol. (2014) 33:929–37. doi: 10.1037/hea0000059

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Jaremka, LM, Andridge, RR, Fagundes, CP, Alfano, CM, Povoski, SP, Lipari, AM, et al. Pain, depression, and fatigue: loneliness as a longitudinal risk factor. Health Psychol. (2014) 33:948–57. doi: 10.1037/a0034012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Jaremka, LM, Fagundes, CP, Glaser, R, Bennett, JM, Malarkey, WB, and Kiecolt-Glaser, JK. Loneliness predicts pain, depression, and fatigue: understanding the role of immune dysregulation. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2013) 38:1310–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.11.016

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Miaskowski, C, Paul, SM, Snowberg, K, Abbott, M, Borno, H, Chang, S, et al. Stress and symptom burden in oncology patients during the COVID-19 pandemic. J Pain Symptom Manag. (2020) 60:e25–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2020.08.037

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Wolf, LD, Davis, MC, Yeung, EW, and Tennen, HA. The within-day relation between lonely episodes and subsequent clinical pain in individuals with fibromyalgia: mediating role of pain cognitions. J Psychosom Res. (2015) 79:202–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.12.018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Khazen, O, Rosoklija, G, Custozzo, A, Gillogly, M, Bridger, C, Hobson, E, et al. Correlation between aspects of perceived patient loneliness and spinal cord stimulation outcomes. Neuromodulation Technol Neural Interface. (2021) 24:150–5. doi: 10.1111/ner.13299

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Chan, WCH, Kwan, CW, Chi, I, and Chong, AML. The impact of loneliness on the relationship between depression and pain of Hong Kong Chinese terminally ill patients. J Palliat Med. (2014) 17:527–32. doi: 10.1089/jpm.2013.0555

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Fortuna, KL, Brusilovskiy, E, Snethen, G, Brooks, JM, Townley, G, and Salzer, MS. Loneliness and its association with physical health conditions and psychiatric hospitalizations in people with serious mental illness. Soc Work Ment Health. (2020) 18:571–85. doi: 10.1080/15332985.2020.1810197

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Wilson, JM, Colebaugh, CA, Meints, SM, Flowers, KM, Edwards, RR, and Schreiber, KL. Loneliness and pain catastrophizing among individuals with chronic pain: the mediating role of depression. J Pain Res. (2022) 15:2939–48. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S377789

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Riley, JL III, Wade, JB, Robinson, ME, and Price, DD. The stages of pain processing across the adult lifespan. J Pain. (2000) 1:162–70. doi: 10.1016/S1526-5900(00)90101-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Park, J, Hirz, CE, Manotas, K, and Hooyman, N. Nonpharmacological pain management by ethnically diverse older adults with chronic pain: barriers and facilitators. J Gerontol Soc Work. (2013) 56:487–508. doi: 10.1080/01634372.2013.808725

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Meghani, SH, and Houldin, AD. The meanings of and attitudes about cancer pain among African Americans. Oncol Nurs Forum. (2007) 34:1179–86.

Google Scholar

53. Rokach, A . The effect of gender and culture on loneliness: a min review. Emerging Sci J. (2018) 2:59–64. doi: 10.28991/esj-2018-01128

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Tsang, A, Von Korff, M, Lee, S, Alonso, J, Karam, E, Angermeyer, MC, et al. Common chronic pain conditions in developed and developing countries: gender and age differences and comorbidity with depression-anxiety disorders. J Pain. (2008) 9:883–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.05.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Jacobs, JM, Hammerman-Rozenberg, R, Cohen, A, and Stessman, J. Chronic back pain among the elderly: prevalence, associations, and predictors. Spine. (2006) 31:203–7. doi: 10.1097/01.brs.0000206367.57918.3c

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Allen, SF, Gilbody, S, Atkin, K, and van der Feltz-Cornelis, C. The associations between loneliness, social exclusion and pain in the general population: a N= 502,528 cross-sectional UK biobank study. J Psychiatr Res. (2020) 130:68–74. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.06.028

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Yamada, K, Wakaizumi, K, Kubota, Y, Murayama, H, and Tabuchi, T. Loneliness, social isolation, and pain following the COVID-19 outbreak: data from a nationwide internet survey in Japan. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:1–16. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-97136-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Nieto, R, Pardo, R, Sora, B, Feliu-Soler, A, and Luciano, JV. Impact of COVID-19 lockdown measures on Spanish people with chronic pain: an online study survey. J Clin Med. (2020) 9:3558. doi: 10.3390/jcm9113558

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Loeffler, A, and Steptoe, A. Bidirectional longitudinal associations between loneliness and pain, and the role of inflammation. Pain. (2021) 162:930–7. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002082

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Suzuki, Y, Suzuki, T, Takagi, M, Murakami, M, and Ikeda, T. Bidirectional longitudinal association between back pain and loneliness in later life: evidence from English longitudinal study of ageing. Ann Geriatric Med Res. (2024) 28:27–35. doi: 10.4235%2Fagmr.23.0136”10.4235/agmr.23.0136

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Powell, VD, Abedini, NC, Galecki, AT, Kabeto, M, Kumar, N, and Silveira, MJ. Unwelcome companions: loneliness associates with the cluster of pain, fatigue, and depression in older adults. Gerontol Geriatric Med. (2021) 7:233372142199762. doi: 10.1177/2333721421997620

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Powell, VD, Kumar, N, Galecki, AT, Kabeto, M, Clauw, DJ, Williams, DA, et al. Bad company: loneliness longitudinally predicts the symptom cluster of pain, fatigue, and depression in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2022) 70:2225–34. doi: 10.1111/jgs.17796

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Ojembe, BU, Kalu, ME, Donatus Ezulike, C, Iwuagwu, AO, Ekoh, PC, Oyinlola, O, et al. Understanding social and emotional loneliness among black older adults: a scoping review. J Appl Gerontol. (2022) 41:2594–608. doi: 10.1177/07334648221118357

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Tibiriçá, L, Jester, DJ, and Jeste, DV. A systematic review of loneliness and social isolation among Hispanic/Latinx older adults in the United States. Psychiatry Res. (2022) 313:114568. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2022.114568

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Camacho, D, Pacheco, K, Moxley, J, Aranda, MP, Reid, MC, and Wethington, E. Loneliness and global cognitive functioning in racially/ethnically diverse US older adults. Front Psychol. (2024) 15:1344044. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1344044

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Ferraro, KF, Shippee, TP, and Schafer, MH. Cumulative inequality theory for research on aging and the life course In: VL Bengston, D Gans, NM Pulney, and M Silverstein, editors. Handbook of theories of aging. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Springer (2009). 413–33.

Google Scholar

67. Forrester, SN, Gallo, JJ, Whitfield, KE, and Thorpe, RJ Jr. A framework of minority stress: from physiological manifestations to cognitive outcomes. Gerontologist. (2019) 59:1017–23. doi: 10.1093/geront/gny104

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Assari, S, Darvishi, M, Rahmani, A, Khatami, SM, Najand, I, Najand, B, et al. Racial and ethnic differences in the protective effect of educational attainment on chronic pain. Hosp Pract Res. (2022) 7:138–44. doi: 10.34172/hpr.2022.27

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Chen, Y, Hicks, A, and While, AE. Loneliness and social support of older people in China: a systematic literature review. Health Soc Care Community. (2014) 22:113–23. doi: 10.1111/hsc.12051

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Theeke, LA . Sociodemographic and health-related risks for loneliness and outcome differences by loneliness status in a sample of US older adults. Res Gerontol Nurs. (2010) 3:113–25. doi: 10.3928/19404921-20091103-99

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Shega, JW, Tiedt, AD, Grant, K, and Dale, W. Pain measurement in the National Social Life, health, and aging project: presence, intensity, and location. J Gerontol Ser B. (2014) 69:S191–7. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu101

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. O’Muircheartaigh, C, English, N, Pedlow, S, and Schumm, LP. Sample design and estimation in the National Social Life, health, and aging project: round 3 (2015–2016). J Gerontol Ser B. (2021) 76:S207–14. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbab182

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. O’Doherty, K, Lawrence, D, Wiencrot, A, Walsh, S, Satorius, J, Burgess, E, et al. Ongoing refinement and innovation in the data collection protocols of the third round of the National Social Life, health, and aging project. J Gerontol Ser B. (2021) 76:S215–25. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbab179

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Scudds, RJ, and Østbye, T. Pain and pain-related interference with function in older Canadians: the Canadian study of health and aging. Disabil Rehabil. (2001) 23:654–64. doi: 10.1080/09638280110043942

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Payne, C, Hedberg, EC, Kozloski, M, Dale, W, and McClintock, MK. Using and interpreting mental health measures in the national social life, health, and aging project. J. Gerontol. Ser. B. (2014) 69:S99–S116. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu100

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. McMaughan, DJ, Oloruntoba, O, and Smith, ML. Socioeconomic status and access to healthcare: interrelated drivers for healthy aging. Front Public Health. (2020) 8:231. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.00231

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Monk, EP Jr, Kaufman, J, and Montoya, Y. Skin tone and perceived discrimination: health and aging beyond the binary in NSHAP 2015. J Gerontol Ser B. (2021) 76:S313–21. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbab098

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Williams, DR, Yu, Y, Jackson, JS, and Anderson, NB. Racial differences in physical and mental health: socio-economic status, stress and discrimination. J Health Psychol. (1997) 2:335–51.

Google Scholar

79. Lautenbacher, S, Peters, JH, Heesen, M, Scheel, J, and Kunz, M. Age changes in pain perception: a systematic-review and meta-analysis of age effects on pain and tolerance thresholds. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 75:104–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.039

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Reese, JB, Somers, TJ, Keefe, FJ, Mosley-Williams, A, and Lumley, MA. Pain and functioning of rheumatoid arthritis patients based on marital status: is a distressed marriage preferable to no marriage? J Pain. (2010) 11:958–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.01.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Radloff, LS . The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. (1977) 1:385–401. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Hawkley, LC, Masi, CM, Berry, JD, and Cacioppo, JT. Loneliness is a unique predictor of age-related differences in systolic blood pressure. Psychol Aging. (2006) 21:152–64. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.152

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

83. O’Doherty, K, Jaszczak, A, Hoffmann, JN, You, HM, Kern, DW, Pagel, K, et al. Survey field methods for expanded biospecimen and biomeasure collection in NSHAP wave 2. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2014) 69:S27–37. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu045

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention . How is BMI calculated. (2022). Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/assessing/bmi/adult_bmi/index.html#Interpreted

Google Scholar

85. Bannon, S, Greenberg, J, Mace, RA, Locascio, JJ, and Vranceanu, AM. The role of social isolation in physical and emotional outcomes among patients with chronic pain. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. (2021) 69:50–4. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2021.01.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Wang, J, Lloyd-Evans, B, Giacco, D, Forsyth, R, Nebo, C, Mann, F, et al. Social isolation in mental health: a conceptual and methodological review. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2017) 52:1451–61. doi: 10.1007/s00127-017-1446-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Cornwell, EY, and Waite, LJ. Social disconnectedness, perceived isolation, and health among older adults. J Health Soc Behav. (2009) 50:31–48. doi: 10.1177/002214650905000103

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Boggero, IA, Sturgeon, JA, Arewasikporn, A, Castro, SA, King, CD, and Segerstrom, SC. Associations of pain intensity and frequency with loneliness, hostility, and social functioning: cross-sectional, longitudinal, and within-person relationships. Int J Behav Med. (2019) 26:217–29. doi: 10.1007/s12529-019-09776-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Emerson, K, Boggero, I, Ostir, G, and Jayawardhana, J. Pain as a risk factor for loneliness among older adults. J Aging Health. (2018) 30:1450–61. doi: 10.1177/0898264317721348

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Nicolson, PJ, Williamson, E, Morris, A, Sanchez-Santos, MT, Bruce, J, Silman, A, et al. Musculoskeletal pain and loneliness, social support and social engagement among older adults: analysis of the Oxford pain, Activity and Lifestyle cohort. Musculoskeletal Care. (2021) 19:269–77. doi: 10.1002/msc.1526

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Smith, TO, Dainty, JR, Williamson, E, and Martin, KR. Association between musculoskeletal pain with social isolation and loneliness: analysis of the English longitudinal study of ageing. Br J Pain. (2019) 13:82–90. doi: 10.1177/2049463718802868

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Booker, SQ . African Americans’ perceptions of pain and pain management: a systematic review. J Transcult Nurs. (2016) 27:73–80. doi: 10.1177/1043659614526250

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Lipton, JA, and Marbach, JJ. Ethnicity and the pain experience. Soc Sci Med. (1984) 19:1279–98. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(84)90015-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Woods-Giscombé, CL . Superwoman schema: African American women’s views on stress, strength, and health. Qual Health Res. (2010) 20:668–83. doi: 10.1177/1049732310361892

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Lazarus, RS, and Folkman, S. Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York, NY: Springer (1984).

Google Scholar

96. Ferraro, KF, Kemp, BR, and Williams, MM. Diverse aging and health inequality by race and ethnicity. Innov Aging. (2017) 1:igx0002. doi: 10.1093/geroni/igx0002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Holt-Lunstad, J . Social connection as a public health issue: the evidence and a systemic framework for prioritizing the “social” in social determinants of health. Annu Rev Public Health. (2022) 43:193–213. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-052020-110732

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Negi, NJ, Siegel, JL, Sharma, PB, and Fiallos, G. “The solitude absorbs and it oppresses”: ‘illegality’ and its implications on Latino immigrant day laborers’ social isolation, loneliness and health. Soc Sci Med. (2021) 273:113737. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.113737

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Bekteshi, V, and Kang, SW. Contextualizing acculturative stress among Latino immigrants in the United States: a systematic review. Ethn Health. (2020) 25:897–914. doi: 10.1080/13557858.2018.1469733

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Camacho, D, Pacheco, K, Becker, T, Reid, C, and Wethington, E. Loneliness in community-dwelling US older Latinx adults. Innov Aging. (2022) 6:432. doi: 10.1093/geroni/igac059.1695

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Blakely, KK, and Baker, NR. Loneliness in community-dwelling, older adults: an integrative review. J Nurse Pract. (2023) 19:104390 doi: 10.1016/j.nurpra.2022.06.015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Barreto, M, Victor, C, Hammond, C, Eccles, A, Richins, MT, and Qualter, P. Loneliness around the world: age, gender, and cultural differences in loneliness. Personal Individ Differ. (2021) 169:110066 doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110066

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Rokach, A, Orzeck, T, and Neto, F. Coping with loneliness in old age: a cross-cultural comparison. Curr Psychol. (2004) 23:124–37. doi: 10.1007/BF02903073

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

104. van Staden, WC, and Coetzee, K. Conceptual relations between loneliness and culture. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2010) 23:524–9. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32833f2ff9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Viruell-Fuentes, EA, and Schulz, AJ. Toward a dynamic conceptualization of social ties and context: implications for understanding immigrant and Latino health. Am J Public Health. (2009) 99:2167–75. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.158956

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Garcia Diaz, L, Savundranayagam, MY, Kloseck, M, and Fitzsimmons, D. The role of cultural and family values on social connectedness and loneliness among ethnic minority elders. Clin Gerontol. (2019) 42:114–26. doi: 10.1080/07317115.2017.1395377

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Stroebe, M, Schut, H, and Stroebe, W. Health outcomes of bereavement. Lancet. (2007) 370:1960–73.

Google Scholar

108. Weiss, RS . Loneliness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1973).

Google Scholar

109. Hickin, N, Käll, A, Shafran, R, Sutcliffe, S, Manzotti, G, and Langan, D. The effectiveness of psychological interventions for loneliness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. (2021) 88:102066 doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102066

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Niknejad, B, Bolier, R, Henderson, CR, Delgado, D, Kozlov, E, Löckenhoff, CE, et al. Association between psychological interventions and chronic pain outcomes in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Intern Med. (2018) 178:830–9. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2018.0756

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Kiosses, DN, Ravdin, LD, Stern, A, Bolier, R, Kenien, C, and Reid, MC. Problem adaptation therapy for pain (PATH-pain): a psychosocial intervention for older adults with chronic pain and negative emotions in primary care. Geriatrics. (2017) 2:5. doi: 10.3390/geriatrics2010005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Cabassa, LJ, Camacho, D, Vélez-Grau, CM, and Stefancic, A. Peer-based health interventions for people with serious mental illness: a systematic literature review. J Psychiatr Res. (2017) 84:80–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.09.021

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Camacho, D, Estrada, E, Lagomasino, IT, Aranda, MP, and Green, J. Descriptions of depression and depression treatment in older Hispanic immigrants in a geriatric collaborative care program. Aging Ment Health. (2018) 22:1056–62. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2017.1332159

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: minority aging, perceived social isolation, Hispanic Latino, African American, cumulative (dis)advantage

Citation: Camacho D, Burnette D, Aranda MP, Moxley JH, Lukens EP, Reid MC and Wethington E (2024) Loneliness and pain among community-dwelling middle-aged and older Black, Latino, and White adults in the United States. Front. Public Health. 12:1429739. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1429739

Received: 08 May 2024; Accepted: 22 August 2024;
Published: 17 September 2024.

Edited by:

Lenard Kaye, University of Maine, United States

Reviewed by:

James Lubben, Boston College, United States
Patricia M. Alt, Towson University, United States

Copyright © 2024 Camacho, Burnette, Aranda, Moxley, Lukens, Reid and Wethington. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: David Camacho, ZGMxOTgxQHVpYy5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.