Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Public Health, 16 August 2024
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic World Health Day 2024: Frontiers in Public Health presents: "My Health, My Right" View all 7 articles

Creating arts and crafting positively predicts subjective wellbeing

Helen Keyes
Helen Keyes*Sarah GradidgeSarah GradidgeSuzanna Elizabeth ForwoodSuzanna Elizabeth ForwoodNic GibsonNic GibsonAnnelie HarveyAnnelie HarveyEvelin KisEvelin KisKaren MutsatsaKaren MutsatsaRachel OwnsworthRachel OwnsworthShyanne RoeloffsShyanne RoeloffsMagdalena ZawiszaMagdalena Zawisza
  • Applied Social Change Hub, School of Psychology and Sport Science, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge, United Kingdom

Introduction: This study explored whether engagement with Creating Arts and Crafting (CAC) predicted subjective wellbeing and loneliness, above and beyond known sociodemographic predictors.

Methods: Secondary data from 7,182 adults living in England from the Taking Part Survey (a 2019–2020 UK household survey of culture and sport participation) were analyzed. Hierarchical Linear regressions were used to explore the predictive effect of engagement with CAC on aspects of subjective wellbeing (anxiety, happiness, life satisfaction and a sense that life is worthwhile) and loneliness. Covariates included gender, Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD), age group, health and employment status.

Results: Engaging in CAC significantly predicted increased life satisfaction, a sense that life is worthwhile and happiness, above and beyond known sociodemographic predictors.

Conclusion: Our study provides support for the wellbeing benefits of engagement with creating arts and crafting, and we suggest that this will be a useful tool at a public health level, noting that relative accessibility and affordability creating arts and crafting.

1 Introduction

Since the COVID-19 pandemic, mental health provisions are failing to meet the growing demand on their services from people in need (1). Wellbeing describes how positive an individual’s psychological state is, which encompasses both feeling well [positive emotions and life satisfaction; (2)] and doing well [a sense of fulfillment; (3)]. Positive wellbeing is associated with a multitude of beneficial outcomes, including higher educational achievement (4), better physical health outcomes (5), more positive physical health behaviors (6) and reduced mortality (7). Loneliness is a negative psychological experience as a consequence of a deficit in high quality social connection (8, 9), and is associated with lower quality of life (10), as well as negative health behaviors and outcomes, such as substance misuse, smoking, eating disorders, depression, stress and premature death (8, 9). Any interventions to combat the profound public health issues of low wellbeing and high loneliness could therefore have a significant impact.

One suggested tool to improve wellbeing and combat loneliness is engagement with Creating Arts and Crafting (CAC). CAC can refer to numerous activities, such as pottery, drawing, painting, knitting, sewing, and crochet. There is wide-scale public interest in CAC, with British TV programs, such as The Great British Sewing Bee and All that Glitters populating prime time television slots. Approximately 20% of the British population are estimated to engage in arts and crafts (11). The arts and crafts industry contributes about £3.4 billion to the UK economy (12) and is forecast to be worth $50.91 billion worldwide by 2024 (13). Given its popularity, the unique purpose of this paper is to address gaps in the literature and directly assess the benefits of engagement in CAC on subjective wellbeing and loneliness, above and beyond known sociodemographic variable predictors within the general population.

Engagement with CAC is not a new intervention to improve mental health and wellbeing in clinical populations [(e.g., 14)]. Indeed, CAC has been found to help with mental health issues (14, 15), enhance subjective wellbeing (1620) and reportedly reduce suicidal tendencies (21). Therefore, not only is engagement with CAC of public interest, but it may also bring profound wellbeing benefits. As such, it has potential to be an optimal wellbeing intervention. However, while a substantial body of work has demonstrated the positive outcomes of engaging with CAC, most studies have investigated therapeutic interventions in clinical populations, only explored specific types of crafts and/or utilized small sample sizes, often with a qualitative approach [(e.g., 14, 2228)]. Thus, the extent to which the broad spectrum of CAC activities quantitatively contributes to wellbeing and loneliness outcomes in the everyday lives of the general population is comparatively understudied, and is the focus of this paper.

Less studied is whether engagement with CAC might be associated with reduced loneliness. Mindfulness is known to be effective in reducing loneliness [(e.g., 29)], and there are parallels between mindfulness and CAC; motivations to engage in creative activities, including crafting, often center around their use as a tool to disengage from unwanted thoughts and feelings and to direct and focus attention onto the task at hand (30). This has been applied in therapies that use arts to facilitate mindfulness (31, 32). Considering the parallels between mindfulness and people’s motivations to engage in creative tasks, we seek to investigate here whether engagement with CAC can be effective in reducing loneliness.

1.1 Research question, aims and hypothesis

The current study aims to address these gaps in the literature. To this end we utilize a dataset from the Taking Part survey, an annual survey conducted by the UK Department for Culture, Media and Sport, which assesses a sample of the general population’s engagement in cultural, digital and sporting activities. We aim to quantitatively investigate whether engagement with general (rather than specific) CAC acts as a protective factor to enhance subjective wellbeing and reduce loneliness in a general (rather than clinical) population in a large, representative sample.

The current study controls for sociodemographic variables that are already known to be linked to subjective wellbeing and loneliness outcomes (33): level of deprivation, gender, age group, health and employment status. For instance, lower wellbeing and greater loneliness have been linked to greater deprivation (34, 35), poorer health (36, 37), and unemployment (38, 39). Loneliness also increases with age (4042), with subjective wellbeing showing a U-shaped curve in high income countries, rising from the 45–54 age range onwards (36). Studies on gender differences in wellbeing and loneliness are more mixed [(e.g., 4245)] and sometimes yield only small effect sizes [(e.g., 44)]. We include these sociodemographic variables in our analyses to: (a) control for statistical relationships between these known sociodemographic predictors and wellbeing/loneliness, and (b) to assess the relationship between engagement with CAC and wellbeing/loneliness above and beyond these known sociodemographic predictors. The latter enables us to determine by how much engagement with CAC contributes to subjective wellbeing and loneliness, in comparison to known sociodemographic predictors and their effect sizes. This assessment of magnitude allows us to evaluate how beneficial engagement with CAC may be to wellbeing and loneliness in a real world setting.

Wellbeing is measured here through Subjective Wellbeing (SWB), which comprises the variables of happiness, anxiety, life satisfaction and a sense that life is worthwhile, in line with the UK Office for National Statistics [ONS; (e.g., 46)], as these variables provide a good measure of both feeling well [positive emotions and life satisfaction; (2)] and doing well [a sense of fulfillment; (3)]. The ONS has previously outlined the rationale for using these questions to measure subjective wellbeing (47). Our key research question asks: Does engagement with CAC predict SWB and loneliness above and beyond sociodemographic predictors of gender, deprivation, age group, health and employment? We predict that engagement with creating arts and crafting (vs. no engagement) will be associated with greater SWB and lower loneliness, over and above known sociodemographic predictors.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

A random representative sample of 7,182 individuals were derived from the Taking Part Survey, a face-to-face household survey undertaken prior to COVID, in Year 13 (April 2019–March 2020), by the Department of Digital, Culture, Media and Sport (48). This dataset is available from the UK Data Service (48). The participants were aged 16 and over and living in England. Although the original dataset has 7,502 participants, the final sample size used in the main analyses for the current study was 7,182 due to missing data on outcome variables and/or covariates from 320 participants. Table 1 reports participants’ characteristics.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics.

2.2 Measures

2.2.1 Sociodemographics

As per Table 1, participants reported sociodemographic data regarding their age (categorical), gender (binary: male vs. female) and home postcode. The latter informed an index of multiple deprivation (IMD (49, 50)). IMD ranges from 1 to 10 where 1 represents the relatively most and 10 least deprived deciles, respectively. Participants were also asked to self-report their health (‘How is your health in general?’, 1–5, higher scores indicate poor health) and employment status (‘Are you working?’, options: ‘working’ vs. ‘not working’).

2.2.2 Engagement with creating arts and crafting

To capture this concept, we compared participants who indicated they engaged in at least one of the following activities within the last 12 months (vs. none): ‘Painting, drawing, printmaking or sculpture’ (question artp13), ‘Photography as an artistic activity (no family or holiday ‘snaps’)’ (artp14), ‘Made films or videos as an artistic activity (not family or holidays)’ (artp15), ‘Used a computer to create original artworks or animation’ (artp16), ‘Textile crafts such as embroidery, crocheting or knitting’ (artp17), ‘Wood crafts such as wood turning, carving or furniture making’ (artp18) and ‘Other crafts such as calligraphy, pottery or jewellery for yourself’ (artp19). Specifically, we used the variable artp263Y13 (whereby participants were asked to indicate if they participated in ‘none of these’ activities) and reversed coded this variable for the current study, so that one indicated a participant had taken part in at least one craft activity, and zero indicated a participant had not taken part in any of these craft activities. Overall, 37.4% of participants in the survey had engaged in at least one craft activity within the past 12 months, while 62.6% had not.

2.2.3 Subjective wellbeing (SWB)

Subjective wellbeing was measured through four single-item questions, whereby each was assessed on 0–10 Likert scales and analysed individually following ONS guidance (44). These items were: life satisfaction: “Overall, how satisfied are you with your life nowadays” (0 = “not at all satisfied?” to 10 = “completely satisfied”), life being worthwhile: “To what extent do you feel that the things in your life are worthwhile?” (0 = “not at all worthwhile” to 10 = “completely”), happiness: “Taking all things together, how happy would you say you are?” (0 = “extremely unhappy” to 10 = “extremely happy”), and anxiety: “On a scale where 0 is “not at all anxious” and 10 is “completely anxious,” overall, how anxious did you feel yesterday?” (0 = “not at all anxious” to 10 = “completely anxious”).

2.2.4 Loneliness

A single item measured loneliness: “How often do you feel lonely?” where 1 = ‘often or always’, 2 = ‘some of the time’, 3 = ‘occasionally’, 4 = ‘hardly ever’ and 5 = ‘never’. This variable was reverse-coded for the current study, so that higher scores reflect greater loneliness.

3 Results

3.1 Analyses

To test our hypothesis, we ran hierarchical linear regressions, with engagement with CAC and sociodemographics (gender, level of deprivation, age, general health and employment status) as the predictor variables, and SWB (life satisfaction, life being worthwhile, happiness and anxiety) and loneliness as the outcome variables. Therefore, a total of five hierarchical regressions were conducted, one on each outcome variable. Sociodemographic variables were entered in Block 1, with engagement with CAC entered in Block 2. All analyses were conducted via Jamovi. The categorical predictor variables were dummy coded: engagement with CAC (reference: none of these), gender (reference: female), and employment status (reference: not working). There was no multicollinearity between the predictor variables, as assessed through VIFs ≤1.37.

3.1.1 Life satisfaction

Engagement with creating arts and crafting (CAC) significantly predicted participants’ reported life satisfaction, above and beyond the effects of age, gender, deprivation, poor health, and being in work. Inclusion of engagement with CAC in the model explained an additional 0.1% of variance in life satisfaction scores compared to the effects of a model including only age, gender, deprivation, poor health and being in work, F (1, 7,175) = 4.59, p = 0.032. Engagement with CAC predicted a greater increase in life satisfaction (β = 0.088) than the increase associated with living in a less deprived area (β = 0.018). In the final stage of the model, deprivation, age, poor health and engaging with CAC were all significant predictors of life satisfaction, with those in less deprived areas, older age groups, those in better health and those who had engaged in CAC in the last year all reporting higher life satisfaction scores. The final model accounted for 16.4% of the variance in life satisfaction scores, R2 = 0.164, F (6, 7,175) = 235, p < 0.001. These findings align with our hypothesis.

3.1.2 Sense of life being worthwhile

Engagement with CAC significantly predicted participants’ sense that life is worthwhile, above and beyond the effects of age, gender, deprivation, poor health and being in work. Indeed, inclusion of engagement in CAC in the model explains an additional 0.4% variance in participants’ sense that life is worthwhile compared to a model that only includes age, gender, deprivation, poor health and being in work, F (1, 7,175) = 27.5, p < 0.001. Participants’ engagement with CAC in the last 12 months had a larger effect on their sense that life is worthwhile (β = 0.218) than being in employment (β = 0.136), than aging by one decile (~ 20 years; β = 0.082) or than living in a less deprived area (β = 0.019). In the final model, all six variables significantly predicted participants’ sense that life is worthwhile, with women, those in older age groups, those living in less derived areas, those reporting better health, those in employment and those who had engaged with CAC in the last 12 months all reporting a greater sense that life is worthwhile. The final model accounted for 11.2% of variance in scores measuring a sense that life is worthwhile, R2 = 0.112, F (6, 7,175) = 150, p < 0.001. This is in line with our hypothesis.

3.1.3 Happiness

Participants’ reported happiness was significantly predicted by their engagement with CAC, above and beyond the effects of age, gender, deprivation, poor health and being in work. An additional 0.1% of variance in participants’ happiness scores could be explained by the inclusion of engagement with CAC in the model, compared to the effects of age, gender, deprivation, poor health or being in work, F (1, 7,175) = 6.71, p = 0.010. Engagement with CAC predicted a similar increase in happiness (β = 0.128) as aging by one decile (~ 20 years; β = 0.127). In the final stage of the model, gender, age, poor health and engagement with CAC all significantly predicted happiness scores, with women, those from an older age group, those reporting better health and those who had engaged with CAC in the last 12 months all reporting being happier. The final model accounted for 10.6% of variance in participants’ happiness scores, R2 = 0.106, F (6, 7,175) = 142, p < 0.001. These findings align with our hypothesis.

3.1.4 Anxiety

Contrary to our hypothesis, engagement with CAC did not provide additional predictive power for anxiety scores to the model (p = 0.108). The final model accounts for 7.1% variance in anxiety scores, with women, those in younger age groups, and those in poorer health reporting higher anxiety scores, R2 = 0.071, F (6, 7,175) = 91.5, p < 0.001.

3.1.5 Loneliness

Contrary to our hypothesis, a model including engagement with CAC did not predict significantly more variance in loneliness scores than a model including age, gender, deprivation, poor health and being in work alone (p = 0.805). The final model accounted for 7.7% of variance in loneliness scores, with men, older age groups, those living in less deprived areas, those in better health and those in employment all reporting being less lonely, R2 = 0.077, F (6, 7,175) = 99, p < 0.001.

3.2 Summary

Our findings support aspects of our hypothesis; specifically that engagement with CAC is associated with significantly higher life satisfaction, happiness, and a sense that life is worthwhile. Engagement with CAC is not associated with lower levels of anxiety or loneliness (Table 2).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of two-step hierarchical multiple regressions predicting satisfaction, happiness, life being worthwhile, anxiety and loneliness with regression coefficients (B) specified for all predictor variables at each step of the regression.

4 Discussion

Given the concern for improving the population’s wellbeing (1) and decreasing loneliness (51), this paper set out to uniquely and directly assess the benefits of engagement with CAC to combat poor wellbeing and loneliness. For the first time, our paper demonstrates that engaging in general (rather than specific) crafting and arts activities may be beneficial in improving subjective wellbeing, over and above one’s gender, health, age, employment status and affluence. Importantly, we show that this pattern holds for the general (rather than clinical) population using quantitative (rather than qualitative) methods, addressing several gaps in the literature.

Specifically, in line with our hypothesis, engagement with CAC was linked to significantly increased life satisfaction, a greater sense that life is worthwhile and increased happiness, above and beyond known sociodemographic predictors. While these findings are of a small-sized magnitude, they are comparable to the effects of known sociodemographic variables, and are of practical importance at a population level. For example, the predictive effect of engagement with CAC on life satisfaction that we observed is an order of magnitude greater than the effect of living in a less deprived area. Similarly, the predictive effect of engagement with CAC on people’s sense that life is worthwhile is greater than the individual effect of being in employment, greater than the effect of living in a less deprived area, and greater than the effect of aging by one decile (~ 20 years). The effect of engagement with CAC on happiness was of a similar magnitude as the effect of aging by one decile or of being female. That is, although variances explained by engaging with CAC were objectively low, they are relatively larger (for life satisfaction and life being worthwhile) or comparable (for happiness) than those explained by other sociodemographic variables known to impact wellbeing. The relationships between engagement with CAC and subjective wellbeing measures in relation to sociodemographic predictors is noteworthy, because unlike static sociodemographic variables, engagement with CAC is a variable that can be manipulated and is therefore open to intervention. Thus, not only is engagement with CAC possibly more effective than many sociodemographic variables on improving aspects of wellbeing, but it is also an easier variable to manipulate and influence. We note also that engagement with crafting or creating arts has a relatively low entry point; crafting and creating arts is an accessible and relatively affordable pastime, and a popular pursuit among the general population. This is in contrast to attending live sporting events, which has also been demonstrated to positively predict SWM in a large UK sample (33), though attending live sporting events predicted lower loneliness, whereas engagement with CAC did not.

The implications of these findings are particularly significant given the importance of wellbeing. Higher SWB predicts health (52) and longevity (53), with some evidence pointing to positive effects on those with certain illnesses (54). Diener and Chan (55) outline the causal evidence demonstrating the beneficial influence SWB has on health and longevity. They also suggest policy makers include interventions to improve SWB across the population, given the potential impact of small SWB increases. Engagement with CAC is related to three of the four SWB measures assessed in this study. So, while the effects may be small, the contribution to SWB as measured by life satisfaction, happiness and life being worthwhile may provide a meaningful influence across society as a whole.

Our hypothesis was not supported with regards to one component of SWB (anxiety) and loneliness, as engagement with CAC had no significant predictive effect on either, above and beyond other sociodemographic predictors. Arts and crafts activities included here (e.g., drawing, painting, knitting, sewing, and crochet) can be considered as relatively solitary activities. This likely explains why we did not observe an effect of engaging with CAC on reducing loneliness. Further research should unpick the importance of the social elements of engagement with CAC, especially given the rich literature on social support and wellbeing (56).

Regarding the role of sociodemographic factors on SWB and loneliness, we largely replicate findings from previous research, as expected. We found that loneliness and perceiving life as worthwhile were both predicted by all sociodemographic variables here (deprivation, age group, gender, general health and employment status). Life satisfaction was predicted by deprivation, age and general health, whereas happiness and anxiety were both predicted by gender, age and health. In general, better health (36, 37) and older age (36, 4042) are predictive of decreased loneliness and higher SWB. Further, higher levels of life satisfaction and the sense that life is worthwhile were associated with living in less deprived areas, as shown in previous research (34, 35). Similarly, loneliness is reported as lower in less deprived areas (57, 58). Previous literature also supports employment status being linked to factors associated with enhanced SWB (38, 59) and decreased loneliness (39). We found gender was not associated with life satisfaction, and previous literature has also been mixed in this regard (4244, 57).

While our study’s strengths include the use of a nationally representative sample, investigation of overall engagement in craft activities and the use of quantitative methods, it is not without limitations. Firstly, our list of arts and crafts is not exhaustive and other arts categories, such as metalworking and literary arts were not considered. We also cannot be sure that participants shared the same understanding of what the various crafts listed in the survey entail. Of interest, many of the crafts included here may be seen as stereotypically feminine and thus the choice of CAC activities may be confounded with gender as a sociodemographic. It is unclear whether different arts and crafts affect men and women differently. Literature on masculinity threat and precarious manhood suggests that crafts such as knitting may be seen as threatening for many men (60). This question awaits further investigation, in particular whether stereotypically ‘masculine’ crafts (e.g., carpentry, metalworking) may have a larger impact on men’s wellbeing (29, 30). Secondly, it is important to note that the present study was correlational and therefore while we can speculate on the impact of engaging with CAC on life satisfaction, causation cannot be determined. The next step for future research is to experimentally manipulate whether or not participants engage in CAC and for how long to measure the causal impact on life satisfaction.

Further studies should also focus on the mechanisms through which engagement with CAC relates to wellbeing, which were not measured here. For example, a key component of creativity is the experience of flow (61), an intrinsically rewarding state characterized by total immersion in an activity. Frequent flow experiences have been linked to better quality of life, in particular wellbeing, satisfaction and a sense of mastery (62, 63). Creative arts are also associated with improved self-esteem and emotion regulation, and provide a means of authentic self-expression (6470). A range of studies show that engagement in art therapy results in a reduction in anxiety (71), depression (72) and stress (73, 74), and improvements in social connection, wellbeing and life satisfaction (27, 68, 75, 76). Determining the exact mechanisms through which engagement with CAC may be beneficial for well-being awaits further studies.

Overall, our study provides support for exploring the wellbeing benefits of engagement with creative arts and crafting as a useful strategy to improve wellbeing at a population level, with the positive effects observed here being comparable to or greater than known sociodemographic predictors of wellbeing. Being already popular (37.4% of the current national sample), relatively cheap and accessible, engagement with CAC activities lends itself to government support and public uptake. Increased funding for creative arts and crafting activities for the general population may benefit society as a whole by improving wellbeing in modern living.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://beta.ukdataservice.ac.uk/datacatalogue/doi/?id=8745#!#0.

Ethics statement

Ethical approval was not required for the study involving humans in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent to participate in this study was not required from the participants or the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author contributions

HK: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SG: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SF: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. NG: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. AH: Writing – review & editing. EK: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. KM: Writing – review & editing. RO: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SR: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – original draft. MZ: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. British Medical Association. Mental health pressures data analysis. The British Medical Association is the trade union and professional body for doctors in the UK. (2023) Available at: https://www.bma.org.uk/advice-and-support/nhs-delivery-and-workforce/pressures/mental-health-pressures-data-analysis. Accessed October 10, 2023

Google Scholar

2. Martela, F, and Sheldon, KM. Clarifying the concept of well-being: psychological need satisfaction as the common core connecting eudaimonic and subjective well-being. Rev Gen Psychol. (2019) 23:458–74. doi: 10.1177/1089268019880886

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Deci, EL, and Ryan, RM. Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: an introduction. J Happiness Stud. (2006) 9:1–11. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9018-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. du Toit, A, Thomson, R, and Page, A. A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies of the antecedents and consequences of wellbeing among university students. Int J Wellbeing. (2022) 12:163–206. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v12i2.1897

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Ngamaba, KH, Panagioti, M, and Armitage, CJ. How strongly related are health status and subjective well-being? Systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur J Pub Health. (2017) 27:879–85. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckx081

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Kushlev, K, Drummond, DM, and Diener, E. Subjective well-being and health behaviors in 2.5 million Americans. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. (2019) 12:166–87. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12178

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Trudel-Fitzgerald, C, Millstein, RA, von Hippel, C, Howe, CJ, Tomasso, LP, Wagner, GR, et al. Psychological well-being as part of the public health debate? Insight into dimensions, interventions, and policy. BMC Public Health. (2019) 19:1712. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-8029-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Shrum, LJ, Fumagalli, E, and Lowrey, TM. Coping with loneliness through consumption. J Consum Psychol. (2022) 33:441–65. doi: 10.1002/jcpy.1329

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Prohaska, T, Burholt, V, Burns, A, Golden, J, Hawkley, L, Lawlor, B, et al. Consensus statement: loneliness in older adults, the 21st century social determinant of health? BMJ Open. (2020) 10:e034967. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-034967

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Rumas, R, Shamblaw, AL, Jagtap, S, and Best, MW. Predictors and consequences of loneliness during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatry Res. (2021) 300:113934. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2021.113934

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Statista. Arts and crafts as free time activity by age in England 2016. Statista. (2023) Available at: https://www.statista.com/statistics/557600/free-time-arts-and-crafts-by-age-uk-england/. Accessed October 10, 2023

Google Scholar

13. Tighe, D. Arts and crafts market value forecast worldwide 2017-2024. Statista. (2022) Available at: https://www.statista.com/statistics/1017884/arts-and-crafts-market-value-forecast-worldwide/. Accessed October 10, 2023

Google Scholar

14. Macpherson, H, Hart, A, and Heaver, B. Building resilience through group visual arts activities: findings from a scoping study with young people who experience mental health complexities and/or learning difficulties. J Soc Work. (2016) 16:541–60. doi: 10.1177/1468017315581772

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Van Lith, T, Schofield, MJ, and Fenner, P. Identifying the evidence-base for art-based practices and their potential benefit for mental health recovery: a critical review. Disabil Rehabil. (2012) 35:1309–23. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2012.732188

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Wheatley, D, and Bickerton, C. Measuring changes in subjective well-being from engagement in the arts, culture and sport. J Cult Econ. (2019) 43:421–42. doi: 10.1007/s10824-019-09342-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Wheatley, D, and Bickerton, C. Subjective well-being and engagement in arts, culture and sport. J Cult Econ. (2017) 41:23–45. doi: 10.1007/s10824-016-9270-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Pesata, V, Colverson, A, Sonke, J, Morgan-Daniel, J, Schaefer, N, Sams, K, et al. Engaging the arts for wellbeing in the United States of America: a scoping review. Front Psychol. (2022) 12:791773. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.791773

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Michalos, AC, and Kahlke, PM. Impact of arts-related activities on the perceived quality of life. Soc Indic Res. (2008) 89:193–258. doi: 10.1007/s11205-007-9236-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Bone, JK, Fancourt, D, Fluharty, ME, Paul, E, Sonke, JK, and Bu, F. Associations between participation in community arts groups and aspects of wellbeing in older adults in the United States: a propensity score matching analysis. Aging Ment Health. (2022) 27:1163–72. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2022.2068129

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Sonke, J, Sams, K, Morgan-Daniel, J, Pumariega, A, Mallick, F, Pesata, V, et al. Systematic review of arts-based interventions to address suicide prevention and survivorship in Australia, Canada, the United Kingdom, and the United States of America. Health Promot Pract. (2021) 22:53S–63S. doi: 10.1177/1524839921996350

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Williams, E, Dingle, GA, Jetten, J, and Rowan, C. Identification with arts-based groups improves mental wellbeing in adults with chronic mental health conditions. J Appl Soc Psychol. (2018) 49:15–26. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12561

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Perruzza, N, and Kinsella, EA. Creative arts occupations in therapeutic practice: a review of the literature. Br J Occup Ther. (2010) 73:261–8. doi: 10.4276/030802210x12759925468943

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Keisari, S, Piol, S, Elkarif, T, Mola, G, and Testoni, I. Crafting life stories in Photocollage: an online creative art-based intervention for older adults. Behav Sci. (2021) 12:1. doi: 10.3390/bs12010001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Curtis, A, Gibson, L, O’Brien, M, and Roe, B. Systematic review of the impact of arts for health activities on health, wellbeing and quality of life of older people living in care homes. Dementia. (2018) 17:645–69. doi: 10.1177/1471301217740960

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Burt, EL, and Atkinson, J. The relationship between quilting and wellbeing. J Public Health. (2011) 34:54–9. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdr041

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Riley, J, Corkhill, B, and Morris, C. The benefits of knitting for personal and social wellbeing in adulthood: findings from an international survey. Br J Occup Ther. (2013) 76:50–7. doi: 10.4276/030802213x13603244419077

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Burns, P, and Van Der Meer, R. Happy hookers: findings from an international study exploring the effects of crochet on wellbeing. Perspect Public Health. (2020) 141:149–57. doi: 10.1177/1757913920911961

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Kelly, D, and Steiner, A. The impact of community Men’s sheds on the physical health of their users. Health Place. (2021) 71:102649. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2021.102649

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Milligan, C, Neary, D, Payne, S, Hanratty, B, Irwin, P, and Dowrick, C. Older men and social activity: a scoping review of men’s sheds and other gendered interventions. Ageing Soc. (2015) 36:895–923. doi: 10.1017/s0144686x14001524

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Coholic, DA. Exploring the feasibility and benefits of arts-based mindfulness-based practices with young people in need: aiming to improve aspects of self-awareness and resilience. Child Youth Care Forum. (2011) 40:303–17. doi: 10.1007/s10566-010-9139-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Coholic, DA, Hardy, A, Patricia, G, and Stephanie, M. Exploring the benefits of an arts-based mindfulness group intervention for teachers, university students, and women survivors of intimate partner violence. Diver Res Health J. (2021) 4:16. doi: 10.28984/drhj.v4i2.323

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Keyes, H, Gradidge, S, Gibson, N, Harvey, A, Roeloffs, S, Zawisza, M, et al. Attending live sporting events predicts subjective wellbeing and reduces loneliness. Front Public Health. (2023) 10:989706. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.989706

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. De Koning, JL, Stathi, A, and Richards, S. Predictors of loneliness and different types of social isolation of rural-living older adults in the United Kingdom. Ageing Soc. (2016) 37:2012–43. doi: 10.1017/s0144686x16000696

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Bellis, MA, Lowey, H, Hughes, K, Deacon, L, Stansfield, J, and Perkins, C. Variations in risk and protective factors for life satisfaction and mental wellbeing with deprivation: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. (2012) 12:492. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-492

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Steptoe, A, Deaton, A, and Stone, AA. Subjective wellbeing, health, and ageing. Lancet. (2015) 385:640–8. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(13)61489-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Jessen, MA, Pallesen, AV, Kriegbaum, M, and Kristiansen, M. The association between loneliness and health – a survey-based study among middle-aged and older adults in Denmark. Aging Mental Health. (2017) 22:1338–43. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2017.1348480

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Bonanomi, A, and Rosina, A. Employment status and well-being: a longitudinal study on young Italian people. Soc Indic Res. (2022) 161:581–98. doi: 10.1007/s11205-020-02376-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Morrish, N, and Medina-Lara, A. Does unemployment lead to greater levels of loneliness? A systematic review. Soc Sci Med. (2021) 287:114339. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114339

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Victor, CR, and Yang, K. The prevalence of loneliness among adults: a case study of the United Kingdom. J Psychol. (2012) 146:85–104. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2011.613875

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Hawkley, LC, Buecker, S, Kaiser, T, and Luhmann, M. Loneliness from young adulthood to old age: explaining age differences in loneliness. Int J Behav Dev. (2020) 46:39–49. doi: 10.1177/0165025420971048

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Barreto, M, Victor, C, Hammond, C, Eccles, A, Richins, MT, and Qualter, P. Loneliness around the world: age, gender, and cultural differences in loneliness. Personal Individ Differ. (2021) 169:110066. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110066

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Maes, M, Qualter, P, Vanhalst, J, Van den Noortgate, W, and Goossens, L. Gender differences in loneliness across the lifespan: a meta–analysis. Eur J Personal. (2019) 33:642–54. doi: 10.1002/per.2220

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Arrondo, R, Cárcaba, A, and González, E. Drivers of subjective well-being in Spain: are there gender differences? Appl Res Qual Life. (2020) 16:2131–54. doi: 10.1007/s11482-020-09862-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Batz, C, and Tay, L. Gender differences in subjective well-being In: E Diener, S Oishi, and L Tay, editors. Handbook of well-being. Salt Lake City, UT, USA: DEF Publishers (2018)

Google Scholar

46. The Office for National Statistics. Personal well-being user guidance. Personal well-being user guidance - Office for National Statistics. (2018) Available at: https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/methodologies/personalwellbeingsurveyuserguide. Accessed November 19, 2023

Google Scholar

47. The Office for National Statistics. Measuring subjective wellbeing – Office for National Statistics. (2011) Available at: http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/guide-method/user-guidance/well-being/wellbeing-knowledge-bank/understanding-wellbeing/measuring-subjective-well-being.pdf. Accessed June 18, 2024

Google Scholar

48. Department for Digital, Culture, Media and Sport. Taking part: the National Survey of culture, leisure and sport, 2019-2020; adult and child data. UK data service. SN:8745 (2021). doi: 10.5255/UKDA-SN-8745-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Noble, S, McLennan, D, Noble, M, Plunkett, E, Gutacker, N, Silk, M, et al., The English indices of deprivation 2019. Ministry of Housing, communities and local government. (2019) Available at: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/833947/IoD2019_Research_Report.pdf.

Google Scholar

50. Smith, T, Noble, M, Noble, S, Wright, G, McLennan, D, and Plunkett, E. The english indices of deprivation 2015: Research report – warrington. (2015) Available at: https://www.warrington.gov.uk/sites/default/files/2019-09/english-indices-of-deprivation-2015-research-report.pdf. Accessed November 19, 2023

Google Scholar

51. Jeste, DV, Lee, EE, and Cacioppo, S. Battling the modern behavioral epidemic of loneliness. JAMA Psychiatry. (2020) 77:553–4. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.0027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Lyubomirsky, S, King, L, and Diener, E. The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success? Psychol Bull. (2005) 131:803–55. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Howell, RT, Kern, ML, and Lyubomirsky, S. Health benefits: Meta-analytically determining the impact of well-being on objective health outcomes. Health Psychol Rev. (2007) 1:83–136. doi: 10.1080/17437190701492486

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Chida, Y, and Steptoe, A. Positive psychological well-being and mortality: a quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Psychosom Med. (2008) 70:741–56. doi: 10.1097/psy.0b013e31818105ba

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Diener, E, and Chan, MY. Happy people live longer: subjective well-being contributes to health and longevity. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. (2011) 3:1–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2010.01045.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Zee, KS, Bolger, N, and Higgins, ET. Regulatory effectiveness of social support. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2020) 119:1316–58. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000235

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Batz-Barbarich, C, Tay, L, Kuykendall, L, and Cheung, HK. A meta-analysis of gender differences in subjective well-being: estimating effect sizes and associations with gender inequality. Psychol Sci. (2018) 29:1491–503. doi: 10.1177/0956797618774796

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. VanderWeele, TJ, Hawkley, LC, and Cacioppo, JT. On the reciprocal association between loneliness and subjective well-being. Am J Epidemiol. (2012) 176:777–84. doi: 10.1093/aje/kws173

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Dolan, P, Peasgood, T, and White, M. Do we really know what makes us happy? A review of the economic literature on the factors associated with subjective well-being. J Econ Psychol. (2008) 29:94–122. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2007.09.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Bosson, JK, Wilkerson, M, Kosakowska-Berezecka, N, Jurek, P, and Olech, M. Harder won and easier lost? Testing the double standard in gender rules in 62 countries. Sex Roles. (2022) 87:1–19. doi: 10.1007/s11199-022-01297-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Huotilainen, M, Rankanen, M, Groth, C, Seitamaa-Hakkarainen, P, and Mäkelä, M. Why our brains love arts and crafts. FormAkademisk - forskningstidsskrift for design og designdidaktikk. (2018) 11, 1–18. doi: 10.7577/formakademisk.1908

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Nakamura, J, and Csikszentmihalyi, M. Flow theory and research In: CR Snyder and SJ Lope, editors. The Oxford handbook of positive psychology. Oxford, MS: Oxford University Press (2009). 194–206.

Google Scholar

63. Isham, A, Gatersleben, B, and Jackson, T. Flow activities as a route to living well with less. Environ Behav. (2018) 51:431–61. doi: 10.1177/0013916518799826

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Nazeri, A, Ghamarani, A, Darouei, P, and Ghasemi, TG. The effect of expressive arts therapy on emotion regulation of primary school students. Quart J Child Mental Health. (2020) 7:132–43. doi: 10.29252/jcmh.7.2.12

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Morris, JA, Urbanski, J, and Fuller, J. Using poetry and the visual arts to develop emotional intelligence. J Manag Educ. (2005) 29:888–904. doi: 10.1177/1052562905277313

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Nan, JKM, Hinz, LD, and Lusebrink, VB. Clay art therapy on emotion regulation: research, theoretical underpinnings, and treatment mechanisms. Neurosci Depress. (2021):431–42. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-817933-8.00009-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Fancourt, D, Garnett, C, Spiro, N, West, R, and Müllensiefen, D. How do artistic creative activities regulate our emotions? Validation of the emotion regulation strategies for artistic creative activities scale (ERS-ACA). PLoS One. (2019) 14:e0211362. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0211362

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Reynolds, F. ‘Colour and communion’: exploring the influences of visual art-making as a leisure activity on older women’s subjective well-being. J Aging Stud. (2010) 24:135–43. doi: 10.1016/j.jaging.2008.10.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Mak, HW, and Fancourt, D. Arts engagement and self-esteem in children: results from a propensity score matching analysis. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2019) 1449:36–45. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14056

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Fraser, C, and Keating, M. The effect of a creative art program on self-esteem, hope, perceived social support, and self-efficacy in individuals with multiple sclerosis. J Neurosci Nurs. (2014) 46:330–6. doi: 10.1097/jnn.0000000000000094

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Jenabi, E, Bashirian, S, Ayubi, E, Rafiee, M, and Bashirian, M. The effect of the art therapy interventions on depression symptoms among older adults: a meta-analysis of controlled clinical trials. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. (2022) 36:185–92. doi: 10.1177/08919887221130264

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Abbing, A, Ponstein, A, van Hooren, S, de Sonneville, L, Swaab, H, and Baars, E. The effectiveness of art therapy for anxiety in adults: a systematic review of randomised and non-randomised controlled trials. PLoS One. (2018) 13:e0208716. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0208716

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Kaimal, G, Ray, K, and Muniz, J. Reduction of cortisol levels and participants’ responses following art making. Art Ther. (2016) 33:74–80. doi: 10.1080/07421656.2016.1166832

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Abbott, KA, Shanahan, MJ, and Neufeld, RW. Artistic tasks outperform nonartistic tasks for stress reduction. Art Ther. (2013) 30:71–8. doi: 10.1080/07421656.2013.787214

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Liddle, JLM, Parkinson, L, and Sibbritt, DW. Purpose and pleasure in late life: Conceptualising older women’s participation in art and craft activities. J Aging Stud. (2013) 27:330–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jaging.2013.08.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Gorny-Wegrzyn, E, and Perry, B. Creative art: connection to health and well-being. Open J Soc Sci. (2022) 10:290–303. doi: 10.4236/jss.2022.1012020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: wellbeing, arts and crafts, life satisfaction, happiness, worthwhile life

Citation: Keyes H, Gradidge S, Forwood SE, Gibson N, Harvey A, Kis E, Mutsatsa K, Ownsworth R, Roeloffs S and Zawisza M (2024) Creating arts and crafting positively predicts subjective wellbeing. Front. Public Health. 12:1417997. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1417997

Received: 15 April 2024; Accepted: 21 June 2024;
Published: 16 August 2024.

Edited by:

Gustavo Cunha de Araujo, Federal University of North Tocantins (UFNT), Brazil

Reviewed by:

Baiba Martinsone, University of Latvia, Latvia
Barbara Sawicka, University of Life Sciences of Lublin, Poland

Copyright © 2024 Keyes, Gradidge, Forwood, Gibson, Harvey, Kis, Mutsatsa, Ownsworth, Roeloffs and Zawisza. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Helen Keyes, helen.keyes@aru.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.