Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 21 May 2024
Sec. Children and Health

Motor development-focused exercise training enhances gross motor skills more effectively than ordinary physical activity in healthy preschool children: an updated meta-analysis

Xinchen WangXinchen WangBo Zhou
Bo Zhou*
  • College of Physical Education, Hunan Normal University, Changsha, China

Purpose: The growth of certain human brain structures peaks at early ages, and complex motor interventions could positively facilitate this process. This study aims to offer an updated meta-analysis regarding the effectiveness of motor development-focused exercise training on gross motor skills in preschool children.

Methods: We searched English- and Chinese-language electronic databases as of March 2024. The main eligibility criteria were as follows: participants were healthy children aged 3 to 6 years old, and the experimental design was a randomized controlled trial, with the control arm participating in either free play or ordinary physical education curriculum. We conducted a Hartung-Knapp random-effects meta-analysis of the standardized mean difference for locomotor, object control, or gross motor quotient.

Results: The search identified 23 eligible studies, of which approximately 75% were considered to have a low risk of bias. Compared with active control, exercise training showed a large to very large effect size. Cohen’s d values were 1.13, 1.55, and 1.53 for locomotor, object control, and gross motor quotient, respectively. From a probabilistic viewpoint, these effect sizes correspond to events that are “very likely to occur” and “almost sure to occur.” Due to variations in intervention programs, all outcome measures showed high heterogeneity.

Conclusion: This updated meta-analysis offers a realistic synthesis of the current evidence, leading to the conclusion that targeted motor skill exercise training can almost certainly enhance preschool children’s gross motor skills. Practical implications are discussed regarding the refinement of the instructional framework and the dissemination of these findings in preschool settings.

1 Introduction

Not only are motor skills important for children’s physical development, but a wealth of literature also demonstrates that they serve as crucial developmental catalysts for executive functions, prosocial behaviors, and intellectual abilities (1, 2). Failure to develop motor proficiency in early childhood can have lifelong consequences. Evidence suggests that motor development promotes neurocognition, occurring in brain regions such as the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex (3). Meanwhile, human brain development peaks in early life; for instance, Lobule IX and Crus II of the cerebellum mature at 5 and 7 years old (4), respectively, and are correlated with motor and cognitive functions (5). Therefore, early childhood complex motor intervention programs could lead to improved brain morphology in childhood (6) and enhanced prospects later in life (7). In this context, preschool children, typically aged 3 to 6 years old, form a unique intervention cohort as they are often in daycare settings that lack formalized physical activity guidelines, leading to difficulties in meeting recommended physical activity levels (8).

It is useful to first distinguish between two types of motor skills. Fine motor skills involve the coordination of the hands, fingers, and wrists for tasks like grabbing, holding, and manipulating objects, as well as visual motor coordination. On the other hand, gross motor skills, often referred to as fundamental movement skills, encompass basic movements such as locomotor (e.g., hopping) and object control (e.g., ball handling). While both fine and gross motor skills are crucial for children’s all-round development, current evidence indicates that gross motor skills demonstrate greater predictive power regarding children’s high-functioning brain structures (9) and neurocognitive development (10). Accordingly, physical education (PE) programs that focus on developing children’s gross motor skills are of great value in preschool settings.

While randomized controlled studies on gross motor interventions in children have been accumulating slowly over the past decades, current studies suggest that motor development-focused exercise training is effective in improving children’s gross motor skills. Nevertheless, there are two critical shortcomings in the existing literature. Firstly, several studies and literature reviews compare intervention programs to passive control (i.e., no physical activity) (1113), which does not accurately reflect realistic settings. For example, in China, the Ministry of Education suggests that all preschools ensure children have 2 hours of outdoor activities per day, with a minimum of 1 hour dedicated to physical activities. While it may be true that daily physical activity cannot be guaranteed in some kindergartens or daycare centers due to environmental or socioeconomic factors, most modern schools allocate physical activities for children’s optimal physical development. Consequently, studies designed with a passive control group offer limited insight into the efficacy of motor intervention programs. Secondly, although a recent systematic review (12) conducted a meta-analysis of intervention programs on children’s gross motor skills and extended findings from two earlier meta-analyses (13, 14), it did not incorporate several new evidence, compromising an already small pool of literature in this field. Zhang et al. included only seven studies that measured gross motor skills and concluded that motor intervention programs have a large effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.889) in preschool children. However, it missed several high-quality studies that reported very large effect sizes of motor intervention programs (15) (see also the complete citation list in Results). As a result, conclusions from the existing meta-analysis (1214) did not fully demonstrate the value of motor intervention programs.

A pressing question arises: Is targeted exercise training more advantageous than free play or an ordinary PE curriculum in promoting gross motor skills in preschool children? This question is substantiated by two key justifications. Firstly, preschool children are undergoing a period of rapid growth in both the structure and functionality of their brains, as aforementioned. Therefore, it is essential to implement targeted intervention programs for preschool children. Secondly, it is imperative to establish an efficient instructional framework. While routine daycare in kindergartens does provide time for children to engage in physical activities, these activities are often unstructured or follow an ordinary PE curriculum, such as radio gymnastics in China. Motor proficiency is an outward manifestation of one’s executive functions. Current research indicates that young children show higher levels of motivation to enhance their cognitive abilities when guided by a teacher or involved in a well-structured program (16). Thus, the active education model proposes that teacher-directed activities may be more effective in developing executive functions compared to liberal programs (17). Hence, it is crucial to understand the efficacy of these two types of instructional frameworks in a typical preschool setting. While most data supports the effectiveness of targeted exercise training, there is also negative evidence suggesting that ordinary physical activity is equally effective in enhancing preschool children’s gross motor skills (18). Analyzing the collective effect of targeted exercise training through meta-analysis can contribute to creating a scientific PE curriculum aimed at optimizing young children’s brain development.

Therefore, the purpose of this study is to provide an updated meta-analysis of the efficacy of motor development-focused exercise training on gross motor skills compared to active control in typical preschool settings. To expand the available evidence, this meta-analysis also includes high-quality research published in the Chinese language. Ultimately, the goal is to provide up-to-date evidence to guide an effective PE curriculum for preschool children.

2 Methods

2.1 Search strategy

We conducted an electronic database search across multiple databases, including MEDLINE, Web of Science’s Core Collection, the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, and Wanfang Data, from their inception to March 2024. Our search utilized English and Chinese keyword combinations as follows: (“fundamental motor skill*” OR “fundamental movement skill*” OR “gross motor”) AND (“physical activity”, “exercise”, “training”); (“动作技能” [“fundamental motor skill”] OR “粗大动作” [“gross motor”]) AND (“运动” [“exercise”], OR “训练” [“training”]). Additionally, we reviewed citations from four systematic reviews and included relevant studies.

2.2 Eligibility criteria

The inclusion criteria were as follows: participants were healthy children aged 3 to 6 years old; the intervention was an exercise training program aimed at enhancing preschool children’s gross motor skills; the outcomes included at least one gross motor skill metric; and the experimental design was a randomized controlled trial. The exclusion criteria were as follows: studies that enrolled children presenting physical disability, intellectual or developmental disorders; reviews, conference abstracts, protocols, or cross-sectional studies; and the control arm involved with no confirmed physical activity. Two authors independently screened the records, resolving disagreements by consensus.

2.3 Outcomes

The outcomes of interest in this meta-analysis were gross motor skills. Commonly used assessments for these skills include the Test of Gross Motor Development 2nd/3rd Edition (TGMD-2/3) or the Peabody Developmental Motor Scales 2nd Edition (PDMS-2). The TGMD-2/3 comprises two tests: locomotor, which evaluates children’s gross motor skills involving smooth and coordinated movements in one direction, and object control (referred to as ball skills in the 3rd Edition), which assesses children’s ability to throw, strike, and catch objects efficiently. The gross motor quotient is derived from summing the scores of these two tests. In this meta-analysis, all three metrics of the TGMD-2/3 and the gross motor score of the PDMS-2 were considered as outcome measures.

2.4 Data extraction

The first author extracted data using a standardized spreadsheet. The extracted dataset can be assessed on Figshare (DOI: 10.6084/m9.figshare.25545703.v1). The certainty of evidence was evaluated using the RoB 2 tool (19), which is organized into five bias domains: bias arising from the randomization process, bias due to deviations from intended interventions, bias due to missing outcome data, bias in the measurement of the outcome, and bias in the selection of the reported result. Two authors independently assessed potential biases for each study and resolved any discrepancies through discussion.

2.5 Data synthesis and analysis

This meta-analysis was conducted using the R package meta version 7.0-0. Among the included studies, one study reported findings separately for males and females (20), while another study provided data for different age groups (4 and 5 years old) (21). To reduce the potential impact of bias from a single study on the overall effect, data from two separate groups within each study were combined to generate a composite score. This composite score was calculated using Eq. 1:

Ncombined = N 1 + N 2 Meancombined = N 1 M 1 + N 2 M 2 N 1 + N 2 SDcombined = N 1 1 S D 1 2 + N 2 1 S D 2 2 + N 1 N 2 N 1 + N 2 M 1 2 + M 2 2 2 M 1 M 2 N 1 + N 2 1     (1)

We pooled data as standardized mean difference (Cohen’s d) and 95% confidence interval. Due to the diverse nature of exercise training in terms of intensities, duration, and instructional methods, the between-study variance cannot be solely assumed from sample variation. Hence, we employed the Hartung-Knapp random-effects model. The I2 statistic was utilized to gauge the proportion of total variability attributed to heterogeneity between the studies. Furthermore, publication bias was evaluated using Egger’s regression test along with a visual examination of the funnel plot. To facilitate the dissemination of our research to the general public, we further converted the effect size to a common-language effect size using Eq. 2 (22):

Common language effect size = Φ d 2     (2)

where, Ф is the normal cumulative distribution function.

3 Results

3.1 Search results

Figure 1 illustrates the search process. As of March 2024, our search strategy yielded a total of 899 records after removing duplicates. Two researchers independently assessed 37 full-text articles, resulting in the removal of 11 records due to the absence of control groups or the cross-sectional nature of the study. Another six records were excluded due to methodological issues. While Foulkes et al.’s study (23) met all eligibility criteria, their statistical reporting cannot be used for the calculation of effect size. Calero-Morales et al. examined the effects of a conductive educational model compared to a constructive model on fundamental motor development (24) but lacked crucial details, such as information on PE curricula, affecting the research’s quality and result validity. Melvin Chung et al.’s study (25) lacked clarity regarding the control group’s inclusion of a physical education class and had incomplete statistical reporting (i.e., the lack of reporting standard deviations) for post-analysis. In Rocha et al.’s study (26), it was unclear if the control group received ordinary physical activity in kindergarten. Lee et al. (27) reported a subset of data from their publication (28), which was included in this meta-analysis. Mostafavi et al.’s study (29) lacked explicit information on sample sizes for both intervention and control groups, making it unsuitable for data synthesis.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart of the literature search.

3.2 Included studies

This meta-analysis included a total of sixteen studies published in English literature (15, 18, 20, 21, 28, 3039) and seven studies published in Chinese literature (4046). Table 1 summarizes the key characteristics of the included studies. In total, this meta-analysis included 1,074 children enrolled in the exercise training group and 996 children enrolled in the control group. Except for one study that only enrolled female participants (18), the remaining studies included both male and female participants. Among the sixteen studies that reported the gender distribution, the proportions of males and females were found to be 49.1 and 50.9%, respectively. Nineteen studies evaluated gross motor skills using the TGMD-2, while four studies used the TGMD-3, and one study used the PDMS-2 (34).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of exercise training from included studies.

Figure 2 illustrates the assessment of bias risk. Overall, approximately 75% of the included studies were classified as having a low risk of bias. However, despite Hamilton and Liu (34), as well as Engel et al. (39), utilizing a randomized controlled research design, these studies did not include baseline data, raising concerns about outcome measurement (i.e., RoB 2 “3.2 Is there evidence that the result was not biased by missing outcome data?”). Robinson and Goodway only assessed object control, introducing bias in the selection of the reported result (i.e., RoB 2 “5.2 Is the numerical result being assessed likely to have been selected, on the basis of the results, from multiple eligible outcome measurements (eg, scales, definitions, time points) within the outcome domain?”). Similarly, Jones et al. (31, 33) and Marinšek and Denac (38) reported only a subset of locomotor and object control, leading to a high risk of bias in the selection of the reported result (i.e., RoB 2 “5.3 Is the numerical result being assessed likely to have been selected, on the basis of the results, from multiple eligible analyses of the data?”).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Risk of bias in included studies.

3.3 Effect of exercise training

Figure 3 illustrates the pooled effect size. Locomotor scores were reported in eighteen studies, revealing a large effect size for exercise training compared to free play or PE curriculum. Object control scores were reported in nineteen studies, indicating a very large effect size for exercise training compared to both free play and PE curriculum. Overall, exercise training demonstrates superiority (Cohen’s d = 1.53, 95% CI 1.08 to 1.98) over free play or PE curriculum in developing gross motor skills in preschool children. Based on the common-language effect size, exercise training has a 78.8, 86.3, and 86.0% probability of being superior to free play or PE curriculum in enhancing locomotor, object control, and gross motor quotient, respectively. It is noteworthy that all three outcomes show a high I2 statistic, indicating significant variation among the included studies.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Cohen’s d values of studies comparing gross motor skill development between exercise training and ordinary physical activity among 3–6 years old children: (A) locomotor; (B) object control; (C) gross motor quotient. Each square represents the observed effect size of a study, with its size corresponding to the study’s weight. The diamond on the plot represents the pooled effect size.

Based on Egger’s regression test, the p-values for locomotor, object control, and gross motor quotient were 0.09, 0.02, and 0.01, respectively. This indicates statistically significant publication bias in object control and gross motor quotient. However, it raises the question of whether this bias reflects non-significant studies not being published or a genuinely superior effect of exercise training. A visual examination of the funnel plot (Figure 4) suggests that most studies exhibit highly statistically significant effects (p < 0.01) of exercise training compared to free play or PE curriculum. Given this substantial evidence favoring motor development-focused exercise training, we conclude that there is no publication bias among the included studies.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Contour-enhanced funnel plot: (A) locomotor; (B) object control; (C) gross motor quotient. Each circle on the plot represents an included study. The vertical line indicates the overall Cohen’s d.

4 Discussion

Our meta-analysis revealed that compared to free play or PE curriculum, exercise training demonstrates a large to very large effect size in developing gross motor skills in preschool children. It is important to begin by discussing how our meta-analysis updates previous findings (1214) and the main practical implication of these results. Van Capelle et al. (14), based on nine studies, reported an effect size (Cohen’s d) of intervention programs on gross motor proficiency over passive or active control of 0.13, corresponding to a common-language effect size of 53.7%. Wick et al. (13), based on thirteen studies, found an effect size (Cohen’s d) of 0.46, corresponding to a common-language effect size of 62.8%. Zhang et al. (12), based on seven studies, reported an effect size (Cohen’s d) of 0.889, corresponding to a common-language effect size of 73.5%. In comparison, our updated meta-analysis, based on twenty studies comparing intervention programs to active control, shows a common-language effect size of 86.0%. From a probabilistic point of view (48), 53.7, 62.8, 73.5, and 86.0% represent events of “an even chance to occur”, “somewhat greater than an even chance”, “likely to occur”, and “almost sure to occur”, respectively. Not only does this updated meta-analysis provide the most realistic synthesis of the evidence to date, but in practical terms, we can now conclude that motor development-focused exercise training offers 3- to 6-year-old children an almost certain effect to enhance gross motor skills at this critical growth stage.

Three possible mechanisms contribute to the better outcome from motor development-focused exercise training. First, the interventions were well-targeted, incorporating activities that balanced motor skill development with children’s interests. Unlike self-directed activities or general PE curriculum, these interventions were designed to seamlessly integrate gross motor movements into a diverse range of exercises and/or fun games. This approach effectively stimulates the development of motor and sensory coordination in children’s cerebral cortex during complex activities (49), resulting in significant improvements in gross motor skills. For example, Fu et al. introduced the concept of functional rehabilitation training and integrated fun games, leading to a huge effect size of 2.9 (15). It is noteworthy that Fu et al. provided an explicit description of their training program, making it highly suitable for adoption by preschools in developing countries.

Second, the active role of teachers, who serve both promotion and prevention purposes, may also be an important factor. Previous research has shown that the development of executive functions in preschool children is more effective in a mentoring situation (50). In the included studies, even though some experiments provided the same equipment, children in the active control group did not show significant motor development in the non-instructed condition (34, 47), highlighting the critical role of teacher instruction in motor skill development for preschool children. Notably, the study by Hamilton and Liu (34), used one teacher instructing four children to improve gross motor skills, showing an effect size of 2.68, which can be categorized as an “extremely sure to occur” event from a probabilistic point of view. Their instructional framework may also suggest that a small-group intervention model could be especially effective for young children experiencing delayed motor skill development.

Third, the overall amount of physical activity in the exercise training group might be greater than in free play or PE curriculum, which may also be a positive factor. For example, Lee et al. showed that daily moderate to vigorous physical activity increased moderately (Cohen’s d = 0.65; common-language effect size = 67.7%; a “likely to occur” event) under exercise training compared to free play (28). Therefore, a program aimed at enhancing gross motor skills could additionally boost the overall physical activity levels of preschool children during non-intervention periods, establishing a beneficial cycle of enhanced motor proficiency, heightened activity levels, and further improvements in motor skills.

It is worth noting that in this updated meta-analysis, as well as in previous meta-analyses, all results indicate that motor development-focused exercise training can yield larger effects for object control than locomotor skills. Children’s participation in free play or PE curriculum involves a variety of running and jumping activities, thus locomotor development can occur to some extent. However, in general, structured ball-skill activities are less often involved in free time or PE curriculum. As a result, children’s object control skills may not be effectively improved, which, in turn, affects the balanced development of gross motor skills.

Considering that the intervention programs varied in frequencies, duration, exercise intensities, and program difficulty, we employed a random-effects model to quantify the effect size. Due to these differences, this updated meta-analysis also cannot isolate the contributing factors that led to the high heterogeneity among the studies. Nevertheless, it is recommended that researchers and practitioners refer to training programs from studies that yielded an effect size greater than 2 (see also Figure 3), which indicates an “extremely sure to occur” event. While these specific programs varied in frequency and modes, the authors provided detailed instructional modalities that can be replicated in practical and other research settings. In addition, we recommend that future studies focus on two nuances to optimize interventions. Firstly, while most included studies used a frequency of two to three sessions per week, we believe that preschool children would benefit more from daily participation in targeted physical activity. Increasing the frequency of exercise training is expected to be more effective in enhancing gross motor skills. Secondly, building on the second mechanism we proposed earlier, there is a need to explore the co-teaching model (51). Early childhood education often requires frequent instruction in proper motor skills during complex physical activities. The traditional one-teacher model may struggle to cater to the individual development needs of 20–30 preschool children simultaneously. Therefore, a co-teaching model could be more conducive to the overall coordination of physical activities and the development of gross motor skills. In China, where the physical education market is rapidly expanding (52), optimizing early childhood education scientifically can not only enhance the quality of education but also create new job opportunities.

Although the benefits of targeted motor skill exercise training for preschool children have been well-documented, we must also acknowledge the practical challenges involved. It is important to note that the teacher-directed programs in the included studies were either supervised by researchers or achieved after early childhood education teachers underwent standardized training courses. At the practical level, teachers working in early childhood education are generalists: They are trained in various aspects of early childhood education but are not specialized in PE. Therefore, the lack of specialized PE training presents a major barrier to the widespread adoption of intervention programs. To address this challenge, there are two potential approaches. The first approach, through policy intervention, involves requiring early childhood education teachers to obtain specialized PE certification. This model necessitates comprehensive policy reform, which can be challenging to implement initially but can lead to the widespread adoption of advanced pedagogical approaches later on. The second approach involves the commercialization of scientifically validated lessons, with preschools conducting their teacher training. This model is better suited for private schools with higher educational standards and financial resources, and it can yield immediate outcomes.

The studies included in the analysis underscored the significant role of teacher-led targeted exercise training. Additionally, it is crucial to acknowledge that the majority of motor skill training requires specialized equipment (15, 20, 31, 35, 37, 39, 40, 43, 45), necessitating policy-level support. In China, physical activities in kindergartens mainly involve free outdoor play that requires minimal or no specialized sports equipment. Consequently, these activities do not effectively facilitate adequate motor skill development. This barrier related to the physical environment may be even noticeable in underdeveloped regions (53, 54). Therefore, policymakers need to coordinate financial allocations, such as one-time grants, to assist public kindergartens in acquiring appropriate sports equipment to enhance preschool children’s gross motor skills.

To ensure accurate and timely responses to developmental delays, it is advisable to establish a scientific standard for preschool-specific physical development. We recommend that Chinese policymakers conduct a nationwide assessment of gross motor skills, leading to the creation of a gross motor development curve for 3- to 6-year-old children in China, similar to Children’s general growth standards (55). Kindergarten teachers are advised to incorporate gross motor skills as a regular assessment item for children. Implementing a continuous monitoring system, such as a Chinese child gross motor development curve, will enable the identification of preschool children lagging behind in motor skills development and allow for tailored skill-enhancing training. This approach could also assist teachers in adjusting training courses (e.g., difficulty levels, exercise intensity) to match children’s capacities throughout an academic year.

In conclusion, existing studies offer compelling evidence regarding the effectiveness of motor development-focused exercise training compared to both free play and PE curriculum. Therefore, preschools are advised to organize systematic and teacher-guided complex physical activities to improve the gross motor skills of children aged 3 to 6 years.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.25545703.v1.

Author contributions

XW: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Writing – original draft. BZ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by Social Science Foundation of Hunan Province, grant number 19YBA235.

Acknowledgments

We sincerely appreciate Yang Zhang (ORCID: 0000-0001-9290-5891) for his critical comments and review in helping us prepare the draft of this manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Gandotra, A, Kotyuk, E, Bizonics, R, Khan, I, Petánszki, M, Kiss, L, et al. An exploratory study of the relationship between motor skills and indicators of cognitive and socio-emotional development in preschoolers. Eur J Dev Psychol. (2023) 20:50–65. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2022.2028617

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Diamond, A, and Lee, K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. (2011) 333:959–64. doi: 10.1126/science.1204529

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Diamond, A . Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. (2000) 71:44–56. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00117

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Romero, JE, Coupe, P, Lanuza, E, Catheline, G, and Manjón, JV. Alzheimer's disease neuroimaging initiative. Toward a unified analysis of cerebellum maturation and aging across the entire lifespan: a MRI analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. (2021) 42:1287–303. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25293

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Koppelmans, V, Hoogendam, YY, Hirsiger, S, Mérillat, S, Jäncke, L, and Seidler, RD. Regional cerebellar volumetric correlates of manual motor and cognitive function. Brain Struct Funct. (2017) 222:1929–44. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1317-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Ortega, FB, Campos, D, Cadenas-Sanchez, C, Altmäe, S, Martínez-Zaldívar, C, Martín-Matillas, M, et al. Physical fitness and shapes of subcortical brain structures in children. Br J Nutr. (2019) 122:S49–58. doi: 10.1017/S0007114516001239

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Hj, S, Kankaanpää, A, Joensuu, L, Kallio, J, Hakonen, H, Ch, H, et al. The longitudinal associations of fitness and motor skills with academic achievement. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2019) 51:2050–7. doi: 10.1249/mss.0000000000002031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Tucker, P . The physical activity levels of preschool-aged children: a systematic review. Early Child Res Q. (2008) 23:547–58. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.08.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Meijer, A, Pouwels, PJW, Smith, J, Visscher, C, Bosker, RJ, Hartman, E, et al. The relationship between white matter microstructure, cardiovascular fitness, gross motor skills, and neurocognitive functioning in children. J Neurosci Res. (2021) 99:2201–15. doi: 10.1002/jnr.24851

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Piek, JP, Dawson, L, Smith, LM, and Gasson, N. The role of early fine and gross motor development on later motor and cognitive ability. Hum Mov Sci. (2008) 27:668–81. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.11.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Veldman, SLC, Jones, RA, and Okely, AD. Efficacy of gross motor skill interventions in young children: an updated systematic review. BMJ Open Sport Exerc Med. (2016) 2:e000067. doi: 10.1136/bmjsem-2015-000067

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Zhang, D, Soh, KG, Chan, YM, and Zaremohzzabieh, Z. Effect of intervention programs to promote fundamental motor skills among typically developing children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Youth Serv Rev. (2024) 156:107320. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2023.107320

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Wick, K, Leeger-Aschmann, CS, Monn, ND, Radtke, T, Ott, LV, Rebholz, CE, et al. Interventions to promote fundamental movement skills in childcare and kindergarten: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Sports Med. (2017) 47:2045–68. doi: 10.1007/s40279-017-0723-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Van Capelle, A, Broderick, CR, van Doorn, N, Ward, RE, and Parmenter, BJ. Interventions to improve fundamental motor skills in pre-school aged children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Sci Med Sport. (2017) 20:658–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2016.11.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Fu, T, Zhang, D, Wang, W, Geng, H, Lv, Y, Shen, R, et al. Functional training focused on motor development enhances gross motor, Physical fitness, and sensory integration in 5–6-year-old healthy Chinese children [original research]. Front Pediatr. (2022) 10:10. doi: 10.3389/fped.2022.936799

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Vandenbroucke, L, Spilt, J, Verschueren, K, Piccinin, C, and Baeyens, D. The classroom as a developmental context for cognitive development: a Meta-analysis on the importance of teacher–student interactions for Children’s executive functions. Rev Educ Res. (2018) 88:125–64. doi: 10.3102/0034654317743200

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Sankalaite, S, Huizinga, M, Dewandeleer, J, Xu, C, de Vries, N, Hens, E, et al. Strengthening executive function and self-regulation through teacher-student interaction in preschool and primary school children: a systematic review [systematic review]. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:718262. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.718262

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Abusleme-Allimant, R, Hurtado-Almonacid, J, Reyes-Amigo, T, Yáñez-Sepúlveda, R, Cortés-Roco, G, Arroyo-Jofré, P, et al. Effects of structured and unstructured physical activity on gross motor skills in preschool students to promote sustainability in the physical education classroom. Sustainability. (2023) 15:10167. doi: 10.3390/su151310167

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Sterne, JAC, Savović, J, Page, MJ, Elbers, RG, Blencowe, NS, Boutron, I, et al. RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. (2019) 366:l4898. doi: 10.1136/bmj.l4898

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Gu, Y, Chen, Y, Ma, J, Ren, Z, Li, H, and Kim, H. The influence of a table tennis physical activity program on the gross motor development of Chinese preschoolers of different sexes. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:2627. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18052627

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hu, X, Jiang, G-p, Ji, Z-q, Pang, B, and Liu, J. Effect of novel rhythmic physical activities on fundamental movement skills in 3- to 5-year-old children. Biomed Res Int. (2020) 2020:8861379–10. doi: 10.1155/2020/8861379

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Ruscio, J . A probability-based measure of effect size: robustness to base rates and other factors. Psychol Methods. (2008) 13:19–30. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.13.1.19

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Foulkes, JD, Knowles, Z, Fairclough, SJ, Stratton, G, O’Dwyer, M, Ridgers, ND, et al. Effect of a 6-week active play intervention on fundamental movement skill competence of preschool children: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Percept Mot Skills. (2017) 124:393–412. doi: 10.1177/0031512516685200

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Calero-Morales, S, Vinueza-Burgos, GC, Yance-Carvajal, CL, and Paguay-Balladares, WJ. Gross motor development in preschoolers through Conductivist and constructivist physical recreational activities: comparative research. Sports. (2023) 11:61. doi: 10.3390/sports11030061

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Melvin Chung, HL, Cheah, WL, and Hazmi, H. Physical activity and fundamental motor skill outcome: a quasi-experimental study among rural pre-schoolers in Kuching, Sarawak. Int J Early Child. (2023) 55:155–67. doi: 10.1007/s13158-022-00322-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Rocha, HA, Marinho, DA, Jidovtseff, B, Silva, AJ, and Costa, AM. Influence of regular soccer or swimming practice on gross motor development in childhood. Motricidade. (2016) 12:33–43. doi: 10.6063/motricidade.7477

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Lee, J, Zhang, T, Chu, TL, and Gu, X. Effects of a need-supportive motor skill intervention on Children’s motor skill competence and physical activity. Children. (2020) 7:21. doi: 10.3390/children7030021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Lee, J, Zhang, T, Chu, TL, Gu, X, and Zhu, P. Effects of a fundamental motor skill-based afterschool program on Children’s physical and cognitive health outcomes. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:733. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17030733

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Mostafavi, R, Ziaee, V, Akbari, H, and Haji-Hosseini, S. The effects of SPARK physical education program on fundamental motor skills in 4–6 year-old children. Iran J Pediatr. (2013) 23:216–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

30. Robinson, LE, and Goodway, JD. Instructional climates in preschool children who are at-risk. Part I: object-control skill development. Res Q Exerc Sport. (2009) 80:533–42. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2009.10599591

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Jones, RA, Riethmuller, A, Hesketh, K, Trezise, J, Batterham, M, and Okely, AD. Promoting fundamental movement skill development and physical activity in early childhood settings: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Pediatr Exerc Sci. (2011) 23:600–15. doi: 10.1123/pes.23.4.600

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Bardid, F, Deconinck, FJA, Descamps, S, Verhoeven, L, De Pooter, G, Lenoir, M, et al. The effectiveness of a fundamental motor skill intervention in pre-schoolers with motor problems depends on gender but not environmental context. Res Dev Disabil. (2013) 34:4571–81. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.09.035

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Jones, RA, Okely, AD, Hinkley, T, Batterham, M, and Burke, C. Promoting gross motor skills and physical activity in childcare: a translational randomized controlled trial. J Sci Med Sport. (2016) 19:744–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2015.10.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Hamilton, M, and Liu, T. The effects of an intervention on the gross and fine motor skills of Hispanic pre-K children from low SES backgrounds. Early Child Educ J. (2018) 46:223–30. doi: 10.1007/s10643-017-0845-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Roach, L, and Keats, M. Skill-based and planned active play versus free-play effects on fundamental movement skills in preschoolers. Percept Mot Skills. (2018) 125:651–68. doi: 10.1177/0031512518773281

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Bolger, LE, Bolger, LA, O’Neill, C, Coughlan, E, O’Brien, W, Lacey, S, et al. The effectiveness of two interventions on fundamental movement skill proficiency among a cohort of Irish primary school children. J Mot Learn Dev. (2019) 7:153–79. doi: 10.1123/jmld.2018-0011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Palmer, KK, Chinn, KM, and Robinson, LE. The effect of the CHAMP intervention on fundamental motor skills and outdoor physical activity in preschoolers. J Sport Health Sci. (2019) 8:98–105. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2018.12.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Marinšek, M, and Denac, O. The effects of an integrated Programme on developing fundamental movement skills and rhythmic abilities in early childhood. Early Child Educ J. (2020) 48:751–8. doi: 10.1007/s10643-020-01042-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Engel, A, Broderick, C, van Doorn, N, Hardy, L, Ward, R, Kwai, N, et al. Effect of a fundamental motor skills intervention on fundamental motor skill and physical activity in a preschool setting: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Pediatr Exerc Sci. (2022) 34:57–66. English. doi: 10.1123/pes.2021-0021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Zhou, Z, Meng, H, Zhao, H, Bai, R, and Lei, Y. An empirical study on promoting gross motor skill of preschoolers aged 5 to 6 through functional training. J Chengdu Sport Univ. (2016) 42:16–22. doi: 10.15942/j.jcsu.2016.05.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Lei, Y, Zhou, L, and Wang, G. Research on the Design of Functional Movement Training Program Based on gross motor development for pre-school children. J Chengdu Sport Univ. (2018) 44:122–6. doi: 10.15942/j.jcsu.2018.01.020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Zha, P, Shen, Q, and Ren, Y. Effects of child gymnastics on gross moor development. Chin J Sch Health. (2018) 39:197–9. doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2018.02.012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Wen, R, Jiang, G, Zhao, P, Bin, X, and Dong, J. Impact of situational sport games on gross movements and social development among children aged 4-5 years old. Chin J Sch Health. (2021) 42:1220–4. doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2021.08.023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Gu, Y, and Li, Y. An empirical study on the effects of special functional training promoting the development of fundamental movement skills of preschoolers (3~6). J Liaoning Norm Univ Nat Sci Ed. (2022) 45:123–34. doi: 10.11679/lsxblk2022010123

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Liu, H, and Fan, Y. 跳绳运动对5~6岁幼儿基本动作技能发展影响的实验研究[an experimental study of the effect of jumping rope exercise on the development of basic motor skills in 5- to 6-year-old children]. Youth Sport. (2023) 3:128–31. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.2095-4581.2023.03.038

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Liu, T, Yang, T, and Lv, W. An experimental study on the effect of happy gymnastics on the development of large muscle groups movements in children aged 5-6 years. J Anhui Sports Sci. (2023) 44:73–9.

Google Scholar

47. Johnson, JL, Rudisill, ME, Hastie, P, Wadsworth, D, Strunk, K, Venezia, A, et al. Changes in fundamental motor-skill performance following a nine-month mastery motivational climate intervention. Res Q Exerc Sport. (2019) 90:517–26. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2019.1628909

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Garvey, PR . Track 2: implementing a risk management process for a large scale information system upgrade — a case study. Insight. (2001) 4:15–22. doi: 10.1002/inst.20014115

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Best, JR . Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function: contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Dev Rev. (2010) 30:331–51. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2010.08.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Gmitrová, V, and Gmitrov, J. The impact of teacher-directed and child-directed pretend play on cognitive competence in kindergarten children. Early Child Educ J. (2003) 30:241–6. doi: 10.1023/A:1023339724780

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Bardaglio, G, Marasso, D, Magno, F, Rabaglietti, E, and Ciairano, S. Team-teaching in physical education for promoting coordinative motor skills in children: the more you invest the more you get. Phys Educ Sport Pedagogy. (2015) 20:268–82. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2013.837434

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Liu, Z, Duan, X, Cheng, H, Liu, Z, Li, P, and Zhang, Y. Empowering high-quality development of the Chinese sports education market in light of the “double reduction” policy: a hybrid SWOT-AHP analysis. Sustainability. (2023) 15:2017. doi: 10.3390/su15032107

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Luo, R, Zhang, L, Liu, C, Zhao, Q, Shi, Y, Rozelle, S, et al. Behind before they begin: the challenge of early childhood education in rural China. Australas J Early Child. (2012) 37:55–64. doi: 10.1177/183693911203700107

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Hu, B, Roberts, SK, Leng Ieong, SS, and Guo, H. Challenges to early childhood education in rural China: lessons from the Hebei province. Early Child Dev Care. (2016) 186:815–31. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2015.1062984

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Ma, X, Mao, Y, Wang, J, and Zewangzhandui, WX. Anthropometric indices, body function, and physical fitness reference values for Tibetan ethnic children aged 6–17 residing at 3,650 meters above sea level. Front Nutr. (2022) 9:9. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.1036470

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: early childhood, fundamental movement skills, growth, pediatrics, physical education

Citation: Wang X and Zhou B (2024) Motor development-focused exercise training enhances gross motor skills more effectively than ordinary physical activity in healthy preschool children: an updated meta-analysis. Front. Public Health. 12:1414152. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1414152

Received: 08 April 2024; Accepted: 07 May 2024;
Published: 21 May 2024.

Edited by:

Stevo Popovic, University of Montenegro, Montenegro

Reviewed by:

Selcuk Akpinar, Nevsehir University, Türkiye
Bilal Biçer, Mustafa Kemal University, Türkiye

Copyright © 2024 Wang and Zhou. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Bo Zhou, zhoubo@hunnu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.