
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article
Front. Public Health , 23 January 2025
Sec. Aging and Public Health
Volume 12 - 2024 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2024.1405791
This article is part of the Research Topic Exploring Motor Imagery Across the Lifespan – Gaps Between Practical Applications and Theoretical Frameworks View all 5 articles
Objective: Explore motor imagery (MI) abilities in healthy older adults compared with healthy younger adults.
Methods: A systematic review with meta-analysis.
Results: Twenty-seven cross-sectional studies were included. Meta-analyses explored MI abilities between healthy older and younger adults for the ability to generate kinesthetic (60–70 years: g = −0.24, 95%CI = −1.61, 1.13; 70–80 years: g = −1.29, 95%CI = −2.75, 0.17), and visual modality (g = −0.08, 95%CI = −0.71, 0.86); vividness in kinesthetic (g = 0.14, 95%CI = −0.13, 0.41), IV (g = 0.11, 95%CI = −0.16, 0.38), and EV modalities (g = 0.05, 95%CI = −0.15, 0.24); mental chronometry in timed-up and go (seconds = 0.63, 95%CI = −0.02, 1.27), and linear walk (seconds = 0.75, 95%CI = −0.55, 2.06); and MI-execution time congruence (performance overestimation) in linear walk (g = −0.02, 95%CI = −0.73, 0.69). Mental chronometry in upper limb movements was analyzed visually in forest plot indicating tendencies of greater time in older adults. Hand recognition in hand laterality judgment task visual analysis revealed a poorer accuracy, greater response time and lower efficiency in older adults.
Conclusion: Vividness of MI in kinesthetic and visual modalities appears to be preserved in older adults. Tendencies for greater time in mental chronometry were observed in older adults in TUG, linear walk and upper limb tasks. Implicit MI assessed with hand laterality showed older adults have lower accuracy, longer response times and lower efficiency. The ability to generate MI in kinesthetic and visual modalities presented imprecise results, and no clear conclusions could be drawn on MI-execution temporal congruence due to imprecision. Further research is needed to potentially clarify these findings.
Systematic review registration: PROSPERO: CRD42023384916.
• The ability to generate motor imagery shows inconclusive results regarding how it varies with aging.
• Vividness during motor imagery remains preserved with healthy aging.
• Mental chronometry tends to be greater in older adults.
• Results from MI-execution temporal congruence are inconclusive.
• Implicit motor imagery, through hand recognition tasks, declines with aging, loosing accuracy, with greater response times, deriving into a lower efficiency.
Aging is an intrinsic process of the human life cycle, in which physiological function declines, impacting cognitive (1), emotional (2–4), physical (5–7), and social spheres (8), affecting quality of life (9–11). Many sectors are influenced by aging, generating a high economic burden (12–14).
Physical functioning is a key component for healthy aging. It relies on the confluence of multiple integrated systems, including cognitive, emotional, sensory, musculoskeletal and cardiovascular systems. Among these, the proper functioning of motor-control-related components in the central and peripheral nervous system, alongside the integrity of musculoskeletal structures, is particularly critical. Several changes have been reported in literature to occur with aging. Notable changes include a reduction in the volume of several encephalic regions, such as the hippocampus, caudate nucleus, lateral prefrontal cortex, and the cerebellum, while other areas like the primary visual and entorhinal remain relatively unchanged (15). Additionally, white matter hyperintensities in the brain increase exponentially with age, doubling approximately every 10 years (16). In addition to these volumetric changes, aging is associated with reduced cerebral blood flow perfusion, particularly in cerebral white matter and certain cortical regions (17). On a peripheral level, aging affects secondary motor neurons through decreased depolarization frequencies, reduced persistent inward currents, and structural axonal changes (18). Despite age-related changes in the nervous system, older adults can adapt and learn new movements, showing neuroplasticity and improved cerebral efficiency. A recent review, pointed that skill training often reduces brain activation, reflecting greater neural efficiency in older adults. However, cortical hyperactivation remains common compared to younger adults (19).
MI is a mental process where the subject mentally simulates actions without its overt execution (20). MI can be subclassified into explicit and implicit MI (21). Explicit MI involves the mental performance of actions (22), whereas implicit MI entails the projection and manipulation of the body schema (23).
Explicit MI can be practiced using different strategies, which include 3 modalities: external visual (EV), internal visual (IV) and kinesthetic (KI). In the EV modality, subjects imagine their body movement from a third-person perspective, as though observing themselves from the outside. In contrast, the IV modality involves imagining body movements from a first person perspective, as if looking through its eyes. Lastly, in the KI modality, the focus is on the sensory experiences of the imagined movement, including tactile, proprioceptive and kinetic sensations.
A subject’s performance during explicit MI can be evaluated across its 3 modalities through various domains, which include: the ability to generate MI, vividness, mental chronometry, and MI-execution temporal congruence (24). The ability to generate MI refers to how challenging is for an individual to construct the MI process (25). This capacity is closely related to vividness, which refers to the realism of the MI experience (26). Depending on the modality employed (visual or kinesthetic) specific aspects can be assessed. For instance, in visual modalities, this includes the visual clarity of the imagined movement, while in KI modality, it refers to the intensity of the KI experience (26, 27). There is, however, some terminological ambiguity in the literature regarding the terms to describe the time taken to imagine an action and how closely this duration couples with the time required to physically execute it. In this review, we will use “mental chronometry” to refer to the time needed to imagine a movement and “MI-execution temporal congruence” to describe the degree of coupling between MI and execution durations.
It is important to note that explicit MI always involves an implicit evocation of the body schema (implicit MI). Therefore, the actual performance on explicit MI relies on the evocation of the body schema and other parameters related to the generation of its movement. To date, the closest method for assessing the quality of implicit MI relies on voluntarily evoking, manipulating and recognizing the body schema. This approach is typically evaluated through tasks that measure accuracy and response time in body recognition exercises, such as determining laterality (left or right side) of specific body parts (24) or whole-body images (28).
MI has been extensively studied as an intervention for motor learning, demonstrating its effectiveness in improving physical performance both as an isolated intervention (29), and in combination with physical practice. Several theories have proposed mechanisms to explain how MI facilitates motor learning without physical practice. These mechanisms include long-term potentiation (on the overlapping neural correlates with physical execution), the reorganization and refinement of motor plans, facilitation of movement encoding, and the anticipation to sensory stimuli (30).
The benefits of MI have been explored across various populations. In children and adolescents, it has been shown to enhance movement learning (31), and improving motor skills in healthy adults (32). Furthermore, its benefits extend to older adults (33), with evidence pointing that MI results in more pronounced strength gains in older than younger adults (34). Beyond healthy individuals, MI improves physical functioning in patients with neurological and musculoskeletal disorders (35, 36), and reduces pain perception (37).
Evidence from prior research has shown significant similarities in the central nervous system substrates involved in overt movement execution and MI in healthy subjects (38). In younger subjects, it has been observed that MI neural substrates vary across the employed modality. EV modality is primarily associated with spatial and temporal aspects of movement, activating brain areas related to visual perception, planning, and memory, particularly involving areas in the ventral stream (39). IV modality activates regions involved in movement planning and execution, such as parietal, frontal and occipital brain regions (39). This modality plays a relevant role in motor learning by integrating perception, action and memory. Among the visual modalities, the internal perspective appears to rely more heavily on motor system substrates (40). Lastly, KI modality activates subcortical brain regions, such as the bilateral caudate, along with cerebellum, primary and secondary somatosensory cortices, and temporal lobe areas (41). These regions are associated with sensory perception and motor control, and their activity closely mirrors the networks involved in overt movement, more so than either of the visual modalities (39, 42).
The shared neural regions suggest that age-related changes in motor control may not only lead to declines in physical function, as previously mentioned, but also may impair MI performance. The effects of aging could vary across different MI modalities, as even in younger individuals, these domains present slightly different neural correlates. This is a relevant question to address, as current literature points that older adults can be benefitted from MI interventions for improving motor performance. However, their basal abilities across the different MI domains may play a critical role for the effectiveness of MI interventions.
Various original studies have explored changes in MI abilities through aging, with previous literature reviews analyzing the tendency of these results. These reviews already detected a lower performance with aging in the ability to generate MI (43), mental chronometry, MI-execution temporal congruence, especially in complex tasks, and implicit MI (44). Conversely, outcomes such as MI vividness may be preserved (43, 44).
The objective with this systematic review relies on gathering the existing literature exploring differences in MI abilities between older (≥60 years) and younger (<60 years) healthy adults. Meta-analyses would be conducted to summarize the result of those studies.
We followed Preferred Reporting Guidelines for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (45) during this systematic review. The protocol is listed as CRD42023384916 in the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews.
The case participants selected for study were healthy older adults (≥60 years), compared with healthy younger adults (18–59 years).
The outcome measures of interest included the following:
• Ability to generate MI: This variable explores the difficulty to construct MI. Procedures can vary, but they usually request the subject to overtly generate the movement, and imagine it afterwards, asking about the difficulty to generate that mental representation. Instruments such as the MIQ and others evaluate this phenomenon. Eligible outcome measures would include the ability to generate MI from KI, and visual (internal and external modalities grouped).
• Vividness of MI: The concept of vividness refers to the realism of the MI experience. It can also be explored in terms of visual clarity or KI intensity across in the respective modalities. Instruments such as the VMIQ assess this variable. Eligible outcome measures would include vividness of MI from EV, IV an KI modalities.
• Temporal features of MI: MI can be assessed with temporal features, such as the time or speed required to imagine an action. Eligible outcome measures would include the timed-up-and go (TUG), linear walk, and upper limb (UL) tasks.
• MI-execution temporal congruence: Temporal features of MI can be contrasted with the actual temporal features of the overtly executed task. This is usually conducted calculating the difference and/or ratio between MI and execution temporal features. This variable can be computed as “performance overestimation” when the subject imagines with a better performance than its actual execution , or as “performance underestimation” when the subject imagines with a poorer performance than its actual execution . These two forms of estimating MI-execution temporal congruence would be eligible, exclusively for the TUG, linear walk, and UL tasks.
• Hand recognition: This outcome measure is an indicator of implicit MI through the recognition and manipulation of the body schema. The eligible tasks for this outcome would include the hand laterality judgment (HLJ) task. This task explores the ability of a subject to recognize left and right hands in different rotations and views (palm and/or back). Eligible measures would include accuracy, response time, and efficiency (the capacity to provide a correct response within its response time).
Observational studies were eligible for inclusion.
Two independent reviewers employed the same search equations for MEDLINE (PubMed), EBSCO, Web of Science, SciELO, ScienceDirect, Scopus, and Google Scholar, in January 2023. Manual searches were performed until October 2023. Free terms, descriptors, and Boolean operators were used in the English searches, as well as Spanish terms (in Google Scholar). No language, population, study design, or time filter was used.
The reviewers independently carried out screening and eligibility. This process was performed using the Rayyan AI tool (46), analyzing Title-Abstract and Full-Text. If there was insufficient information for inclusion, researchers would contact the corresponding authors for additional information.
Non-scientific articles, study protocols, and articles without full text were excluded. No language restrictions were applied. Additionally, in case of disagreement on selection, during the final eligibility phase a third reviewer was employed to reach a consensus.
Study information on authors, publication date, design, inclusion and exclusion criteria, population characteristics, groups, and sample size were extracted. Only groups meeting the age criteria (≥60 and < 60 years) were extracted. The following outcome measures were included: ability to generate KI or visual MI; vividness of MI in KI, IV, or EV modalities; mental chronometry or mental speed in TUG, linear walk, and UL movements; performance over/underestimation coefficients in TUG, linear walk, and UL movements; and accuracy, response time, and efficiency in HLJ task.
Instructions for testing temporal MI features, including modality, eye status, posture, and other varying difficulty constraints were recorded. Measurement tools were extracted, and the results were synthesized.
One reviewer assessed the methodological quality of studies with the Johanna Briggs Institute Critical Appraisal Checklist (JBI) for analytical cross-sectional studies [(47), p. 7]. The scale includes 8 items, with 4 response options (Yes; No; Unclear; and Not applicable). Authors provided a punctuation of 1 point for “Yes” responses, and 0 point for “No,” “Unclear” and “Not applicable” responses, accounting for a total score between 0 and 8 points. Greater punctuations would indicate a greater methodological quality.
The sample size, mean, and SD of outcomes were extracted for meta-analyses. Data were extracted from tables/text, and from graphics using the PDF X-Change Editor ruler.
Median and quartile data were converted to mean and SD using equations n°14 and 15 proposed by Wan et al. (48). Standard errors of the mean and confidence intervals were also transformed to SD following the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions section 6.5.2.2 (49). Performance underestimation coefficients were transformed into overestimation coefficients.
Meta-analyses were conducted if (1) 3 or more studies explored the same outcome measure; (2) studies presented similar age groups; (3) the sample size, mean, and SD were available; and (4) they presented moderate or good methodological quality. In cases where only 1 or 2 studies explored a specific outcome, forest plots were included to provide a visual representation of effect sizes and trends. This exploratory approach was aimed to (1) offer preliminary insights into the direction and magnitude of effects between older and younger adults, even when data were sparse; (2) facilitate identification of potential patterns that could inform future studies or highlight gaps in the literature; and (3) ensure transparency in presenting all available evidence, minimizing the risk of selective reporting.
Random effect meta-analyses were conducted employing the Hedges’ model with a 95%CI. Pooled results were displayed as the raw mean difference (MD) if studies presented the same measurement instrument and unit. If not, data were treated with the Hedges’ g as standardized mean differences (50). The Hedges’ g is standardized mean difference (Cohen’s d) adjusted by the sample size, to prevent its overestimation. It will be interpreted following the criteria by Cohen (51): “very small” < 0.20; “small,” if 0.20–0.49; “medium” if 0.5–0.79, and “large” ≥ 0.8.
Heterogeneity was examined with Cochran’s Q test, the Inconsistency index (I2) and Tau squared (τ2). Cochran’s Q test presents limitations of underpower for meta-analyses with a low amount of studies or sample sizes; therefore, a p-value threshold of <0.1 would be used for considering heterogeneity across studies (52). Heterogeneity would be considered significant if either Cochran’s Q test p-value was <0.1, or I2 > 75%.
Funnel plots were employed for spotting outliers exceeding the 95%CI. Publication and selection bias was assessed with Egger’s Regression test (53), and Doi plot’s LFK index, with its threshold for detecting publication and selection bias if <−1 or > 1 (54). Publication and selection bias would be confirmed if any of the employed tests resulted positive.
Sensitivity analyses were explored with the Leave-One-Out Test (55) for meta-analyses with 4 or more studies. A significant influence would be confirmed if the extraction of any study would significantly modify the pooled result (generating a change over p < 0.05 or p > 0.05).
These procedures were conducted in R Studio software version 2023.06.0 + 421, employing R version 4.3.1 (56). MD, Hedges’ g calculations, random effect meta-analyses, and heterogeneity and sensitivity analyses were performed with the package “metafor” version 3.8.2 (57). Doi plots and LFK index were generated with the package “metasens” version 1.5–2 (58).
A total of 27 cross-sectional studies were included in the review (25, 59–84). The studies of Watanabe and Tani (82) and Kotegawa et al. (64) were included after the age range data were provided by the corresponding authors. Watanabe and Tani (82) additionally provided their data of mental chronometry.
Table 1. Quality assessment of analytical cross-sectional studies with Johanna-Briggs Institute Critical Appraisal Checklist.
See Supplementary material for details of the search engines, databases, number of searches, and equations with their retrievals. Figure 1 represents the selection process via a flow-chart.
Studies presented an average methodological quality of 4.44 ± 1.69 (1–6 points). Nine studies presented 6 points (59, 69, 71, 72, 74, 76–78, 84), eight studies presented 5 points (63, 65–67, 70, 75, 79, 82), four studies reported 4 points (25, 60, 64, 80), one study 3 points (62), and two studies 2 points (61, 73) (see Table 1).
A total of 1,160 older adults (60–93 years) were compared to 1,345 younger adults (18–60 years). Among these participants, there were a total of 556 female older and 560 younger adults, with no sex information reported in 3 studies (25, 70, 84). See Table 2 for further details of demographic information.
All studies included participants that were self-reported or considered healthy by the researcher, additionally excluding participants with physical or musculoskeletal impairments in 23 studies (25, 59, 60, 62–67, 69–71, 73–78, 80–84), neurological or mental conditions in 18 studies (59–61, 63, 67, 69–77, 79–82), and diminished cognitive functions in 17 studies (59, 62–65, 69, 71, 72, 74–80, 82, 84). See Table 2 for further details of studies’ eligibility criteria.
Nineteen studies explored explicit MI domains (25, 59, 60, 63–68, 71, 73–75, 77–80, 82, 83).
Four studies explored the ability to generate KI MI (25, 63, 77, 78). The instruments included MIQ-3sf (25), MIQ-R (63, 77), and MIQ-RS (78).
Three studies explored this outcome measure, not specifying the perspective (first or third) (63, 77, 78), 1 study from IV (25), and 1 study from EV modalities (25). The instruments included MIQ-3sf (25), MIQ-R (63, 77), and MIQ-RS (78).
Four studies explored vividness during MI from a KI modality (65, 66, 74, 75). Assessment tools included the Vividness of Movement Imagery Questionnaire revised version (VMIQ-2) (65), and the Kinesthetic and Visual Imagery Questionnaire (KVIQ-10) (66, 71, 74, 75).
Four studies explored vividness during MI from a IV modality (65, 66, 74, 75). Assessment tools included the VMIQ-2 (65), and the KVIQ-10 (66, 71, 74, 75).
Four studies explored vividness during MI from EV modality (65, 68, 73, 79). Assessment tools included the VMIQ original version (68, 73, 79), and the VMIQ-2 (65).
Fourteen studies explored temporal features (59, 60, 63, 64, 67, 71, 74, 77–80, 82–84), in which they explored mental chronometry (59, 60, 63, 64, 67, 71, 74, 77–80, 82–84), and mental speed (84).
Studies explored these features for imagined TUG (59, 77, 78), imagined linear walk (64, 71, 74, 77, 79, 80, 82–84), arm elevation movements (67, 80), and finger tapping task (60).
MI-execution temporal congruence, through difference or ratios between MI and execution temporal features were assessed in 9 studies, with 5 computing “performance overestimation” measures (59, 62, 64, 78, 82), and 4 “performance underestimation” measures (71, 74, 79, 83). Two studies explored these variables for imagined TUG (59, 78), and 7 explored linear walk (62, 64, 71, 74, 79, 82, 83). No studies explored this variable for UL movements.
Eight studies explored implicit MI through the HLJ task (61, 69, 70, 72, 76–78, 81).
Five studies explored hand recognition ability through the hand laterality judgment (HLJ) in terms of accuracy (61, 70, 72, 76, 81). These studies explored this outcome grouping the results across different hand rotations (70, 72), and analyzing specifically the outcome at 0° (61, 76, 81), 30° (61), 45° (81), 60° (61), 90° (61, 76, 81), 120° (61), 135° (81), 150° (61), and 180° rotation (61, 76, 81).
Five studies explored response time in the HLJ task (61, 69, 70, 76, 81). One study explored response time grouping the results across different hand rotations (70), at 0° (61, 76, 81), 30° (61), 45° (69, 81), 60° (61), 90° (61, 69, 76, 81), 120° (61), 135° (69, 81), 150° (61), and 180° (61, 76, 81).
One study explored the HLJ task in terms of efficiency grouping different angular rotations for palm, and back views for medial and lateral rotations (70). This was explored with the inverse efficiency score, a ratio between response time and the rate of correct responses.
Based on the criteria for conducting meta-analyses, the authors were only able to conduct meta-analyses for (1) ability to generate MI from KI modality conducting 2 meta-analyses based on age groups; (2) ability to generate MI from visual modality; (3) vividness of MI in KI modality; (4) vividness of MI in IV modality; (5) vividness of MI in EV modality; (6) Temporal features of MI (in terms of mental chronometry) for TUG, and linear walk tasks; and (7) MI-execution temporal congruence (in terms of performance overestimation) for linear walk tasks. The following outcome measures did not fulfill the criteria for conducting meta-analyses (number of available studies): temporal features of MI (in terms of mental chronometry) for UL tasks, hand recognition accuracy, hand recognition response time and hand recognition efficiency. However, forest plots were presented for observing difference tendencies between groups. See Table 3 for the detailed process to select studies in meta-analyses.
Table 3. Data availability, extraction and processing for meta-analyses between healthy older and younger adults.
Four studies explored this outcome measure and were included in the meta-analysis. Healthy older adults aged 60–70 years were compared with healthy younger adults aged 18–30 years (25, 63, 77, 78). Studies presented a methodological quality of 4–6 points.
The meta-analysis revealed a non-significant small difference (g = −0.240; 95%CI = −1.611, 1.130), with 95%CI showing a large imprecision, considering that the capacity could range between large in favor of older adults, and large in favor of younger adults, preventing stablishing clear conclusions of groups’ difference. Current findings prevent drawing clear conclusions. The heterogeneity was significant (Q = 69.017, p < 0.001; I2 = 96.71%; τ2 = 1.883; see Figure 2). All studies were outliers in the funnel plot. Publication and selection bias were confirmed through asymmetry in the Doi plot (LFK = −1.09), but not with Egger’s regression test (p = 0.918). The sensitivity analysis did not reveal a significant influence of any study on the pooled result.
Figure 2. Meta-analysis: Ability to generate MI in kinesthetic modality in healthy older adults aged 60–70 years compared to healthy younger adults aged 18–30 years.
Three studies explored this outcome measure in healthy older adults of 70–82 years, being compared with healthy younger adults aged 18–30 years (25, 77, 78). These studies were included in the meta-analysis, presenting a methodological quality of 4–6 points.
A non-significant difference was observed (g = −1.290; 95%CI = −2.748, 0.168), with 95%CI indicating that the capacity could range between large in favor of younger adults, or very small in favor of older adults. An imprecise, but observable tendency can be drawn from these findings in favor of younger adults. The heterogeneity was significant (Q = 45.479, p < 0.001; I2 = 96.30%; τ2 = 1.594; see Figure 3). Two studies were outliers in the funnel plot (25, 78). Publication and selection bias were confirmed through asymmetry in the Doi plot (LFK = −4.12), and with Egger’s regression test (p = 0.004).
Figure 3. Meta-analysis: Ability to generate MI in kinesthetic modality in healthy older adults aged 70–82 years compared to healthy younger adults aged 18–30 years.
Three studies explored this outcome, and were included in the meta-analysis, comparing healthy older adults aged 60–69 years with healthy younger adults aged 20–30 years (63, 77, 78). Studies presented a methodological quality of 5–6 points.
A non-significant trivial difference was obtained (g = −0.076; 95%CI = −0.708, 0.859), with 95%CI showing imprecise findings, with the possibility of difference ranging between a moderate difference in favor of younger adults to a large difference in favor of older adults, preventing stablishing clear conclusions of the findings. The heterogeneity was significant (Q = 12.294, p = 0.002; I2 = 87.35%; τ2 = 0.414; see Figure 4). Two studies were outliers in the funnel plot (63, 77). Publication and selection bias confirmed with asymmetry in the Doi plot (LFK = 1.07), and not reaching significance in Egger’s regression test (p = 0.628).
Figure 4. Meta-analysis: Ability to generate MI in visual modalities in healthy older adults aged 60–69 years compared to healthy younger adults aged 20–30 years.
Four studies explored this outcome measure and were included in the meta-analysis. They compared healthy older adults aged 60–89 years with healthy younger adults aged 18–37 years (65, 66, 74, 75). Studies presented a methodological quality of 5–6 points.
A non-significant trivial difference was obtained (g = 0.140; 95%CI = −0.130, 0.411), with 95%CI indicating that the difference range between very small in favor of older adults, to small in favor of younger adults. Therefore, this capacity could be similar between groups. Heterogeneity was not significant (Q = 2.114, p = 0.549; I2 = 0%; τ2 = 0; see Figure 5). No outliers were identified in the funnel plot. Publication and selection bias was absent, as observed in the Doi plot (LFK = 0.09) and Egger’s regression test (p = 0.562). The sensitivity analysis did not reveal a significant influence of any study on the pooled result.
Figure 5. Meta-analysis: Vividness of MI in kinesthetic modality in healthy older adults aged 60–89 years compared to healthy younger adults aged 18–37 years.
Four studies explored this variable and were included in the meta-analysis. Studies compared healthy older adults aged 60–89 years with healthy younger adults aged 18–37 years (65, 66, 74, 75). Studies presented a methodological quality of 5–6 points.
A non-significant trivial difference was obtained (g = 0.107; 95%CI = −0.164, 0.377), with 95%CI indicating that the difference could range between very small in favor of older adults, to small in favor of younger adults. Therefore, this capacity could be similar between groups. Heterogeneity was not significant (Q = 1.541, p = 0.673; I2 = 0%; τ2 = 0; see Figure 6). No outliers were identified in the funnel plot. Publication and selection bias were confirmed with asymmetry in the Doi plot (LFK = 1.75), but not with Egger’s regression test (p = 0.111). The sensitivity analysis did not reveal a significant influence of any study on the pooled result.
Figure 6. Meta-analysis: Vividness of MI in internal visual modality in healthy older adults aged 60–89 years compared to healthy younger adults aged 18–37 years.
Four studies explored this variable and were included in the meta-analysis. They analyzed healthy older adults aged 62–93 years compared with healthy younger adults aged 18–35 years (65, 68, 73, 79). Studies presented a methodological quality of 1–5 points.
A trivial non-significant difference was obtained (g = 0.047; 95%CI = −0.148, 0.242), with 95%CI indicating that the difference could range between very small in favor of older adults, to small in favor of younger adults. Therefore, this capacity could be similar between groups. Heterogeneity was not significant (Q = 1.936, p = 0.586; I2 = 0%; τ2 = 0; see Figure 7). No outliers were identified in the funnel plot. Publication and selection bias were confirmed through asymmetry in the Doi plot (LFK = 5.83), but not through Egger’s regression test (p = 0.178). The sensitivity analysis did not reveal a significant influence of any study on the pooled result.
Figure 7. Meta-analysis: Vividness of MI in external visual modality in healthy older adults aged 62–93 years compared to healthy younger adults aged 18–35 years.
Three studies exploring this outcome measure, in terms of mental chronometry (time), were included in the meta-analysis. Healthy older adults aged 70–87 years were compared with healthy younger adults aged 20–58 years (59, 77, 78). Studies presented a methodological quality of 6 points.
A non-significant moderate difference was observed (MD, seconds = 0.625; 95%CI = −0.017, 1.268), with 95%CI indicating that the capacity could range between a similar between groups, to small difference in favor of older adults. An imprecise, but observable tendency can be drawn from these findings with older adults tending to require greater time. Heterogeneity was not significant (Q = 1.147, p = 0.563; I2 = 0%; τ2 = 0; see Figure 8). No outliers were identified in the funnel plot. Publication and selection bias were absent, with observed symmetry in the Doi plot (LFK = 0.42) and absent in Egger’s regression test (p = 0.321).
Figure 8. Meta-analysis: Mental chronometry in Timed-Up and Go test in healthy older adults aged 70–87 years compared to healthy younger adults aged 20–58 years.
Five studies were meta-analyzed (64, 74, 80, 82, 83) from the eight studies exploring this variable (64, 71, 74, 77, 79, 80, 82, 83). Included studies compared healthy older adults aged 60–82 years with healthy younger adults aged 18–30 years, presenting a methodological quality of 1–6 points.
A non-significantly moderate difference was observed (MD, seconds = 0.754; 95%CI = −0.552, 2.059), with 95%CI indicating that differences could range between trivial to relevant differences in favor of older adults. Although these findings were imprecise, an observable tendency could be stablished from these findings, with older adults tending to require greater times. Heterogeneity was significant (Q = 21.574, p < 0.001; I2 = 89.39%; τ2 = 1.935; see Figure 9). Two outliers were identified in the funnel plot (82, 83). Publication and selection bias were confirmed with asymmetry in the Doi plot (LFK = 3.53), but not through Egger’s regression test (p = 0.054). The sensitivity analysis did not reveal a significant influence of any study on the pooled result.
Figure 9. Meta-analysis: Mental chronometry in Linear Walk (5-10 m) in healthy older adults aged 60–82 years compared to healthy younger adults aged 18–30 years.
Two studies explored this variable in forward arm elevation task, and were analyzed visually through a forest plot, as they did not fulfill meta-analysis criteria (67, 80). They compared a sample of healthy older adults (62–80 years) with healthy younger adults (18–30 years). Studies presented a methodological quality of 4–5 points. An observable but not significant tendency was detected with older adults presenting greater mental chronometry time than younger adults (see Figure 10).
Figure 10. Visual forest plot: Mental chronometry in UL tasks (forward arm elevation task) in healthy older adults aged 62–80 years compared to healthy younger adults aged 18–30 years.
Fron the seven studies exploring MI-execution temporal congruence, only five were meta-analyzed (62, 64, 74, 82, 83). Performance underestimation measures were transformed to overestimation measures for inclusion in the meta-analysis. Studies compared healthy older adults (60–82 years) with healthy younger adults (18–35 years) presenting a methodological quality of 1–6 points.
A non-significant trivial difference was obtained (g = −0.022; 95%CI = −0.731, 0.687), with 95%CI indicating imprecise findings, with overestimations in linear walk ranging from moderate in favor of younger adults to a moderate difference in favor of older adults. The heterogeneity was significant (Q = 39.788; p < 0.001; I2 = 87.88%; τ2 = 0.568; see Figure 11). Two studies were outliers in the funnel plot (62, 82). Publication and selection bias were confirmed with asymmetry in the Doi plot (LFK = 2.57), but not through Egger’s regression test (p = 0.107). The sensitivity analysis did not reveal a significant influence of any study on the pooled result.
Figure 11. Meta-analysis: MI-execution temporal congruence (performance overestimation) in Linear Walk (5–10 m) in healthy older adults aged 60–82 years compared to healthy younger adults aged 18–35 years.
Two studies analyzed hand recognition accuracy (70, 72) and were included in the forest plot visual analysis, as they did not fulfill meta-analysis criteria. These studies presented a methodological quality score of 5–6 points. Specific rotations were explored visually in the forest plots with only 2 studies (61, 81) from the originally 3 available studies (61, 76, 81). They explored accuracy in 0°, 30°, 45°, 60°, 90°, 120°, 135°, 150°, and 180°, presenting a methodological quality of 1–2 points.
An observable and significant tendency was detected for accuracy in HLJ tasks grouping rotations, with younger adults presenting greater accuracy. This difference was not relevant in specific HLJ rotations. Accuracy at 0°, 45° and 150° no differences were observable, tendencies for younger adults presenting greater accuracy was observable at 30°, 60°, 120°, 135°, and 180°. A tendency for greater accuracy in older adults was observed at 90° (see Figure 12).
Figure 12. Visual forest plot: Implicit MI – Hand recognition accuracy in hand laterality judgement task in healthy older adults compared to healthy younger adults.
Only one study exploring response time in HLJ task grouping rotations was finally included for visual analysis in the forest plot (70). The study presented a methodological quality of 5 points. Specific rotations was originally explored in 4 studies (61, 69, 76, 81), of which only 1 was included for visual analysis in the forest plot (61), analyzing specific rotations at 0°, 30°, 60°, 90°, 120°, 150°, and 180°. This study presented 2 points of methodological quality.
An observable and significant tendency was detected for response time in HLJ tasks with older adults presenting greater response time, analyzing grouped rotations and individual specific rotations (see Figure 13).
Figure 13. Visual forest plot: Implicit MI – Hand recognition response time in hand laterality judgement task in healthy older adults compared to healthy younger adults.
One study explored this variable and was analyzed visually in the forest plot, as the outcome measure did not fulfill meta-analysis criteria (70). The study explored efficiency in hand recognition for back-view and palm-view medial and lateral rotations, presenting a methodological quality of 5 points.
An observable and significant tendency was detected for efficiency in HLJ tasks with younger adults presenting greater efficiency across views (back and palm) and medial and lateral rotations (see Figure 14).
Figure 14. Visual forest plot: Implicit MI – Hand recognition efficiency in hand laterality judgement task in healthy older adults compared to healthy younger adults.
Meta-analysis summary results are shown in Table 4. Funnel and Doi plots are presented in Supplementary material.
Table 4. Summary information of all meta-analyses performed for MI capacities between healthy older and younger adults.
The purpose of this meta-analysis was to evaluate the differences in MI abilities between older and younger adults. MI assessments included the ability to generate KI and visual MI, the vividness of MI across KI, IV, and EV modalities, mental chronometry, the synchrony of MI and execution time, and hand recognition accuracy, response time and efficiency.
Discrepant results were observed across variables. Results for the capacity of generating MI were mainly imprecise. However, the most consistent results were pooled from the MI vividness measure, with meta-analyses for KI, IV and EV showing with precision that older and younger adults presented similar vividness. Mental chronometry for TUG and linear walk, showed a tendency for older adults to require greater times. Performance overestimation in linear walk presented imprecise results. Hand recognition accuracy was diminished in older adults (grouping rotations), their response time was greater, added to a lower efficiency than younger adults.
Kinesthetic MI entails the mental simulation of movement without its actual execution, focusing on the haptic sensations experienced during real movement, such as tactile, proprioceptive, and KI feedback (85, 86). A theory has been proposed suggesting KI MI is rooted in the internal activation of anticipatory representations of the action’s effects. This mechanism could potentially facilitate enhanced motor performance through an internal emulation of the action, obviating the need for physical execution (87).
KI MI is considered a complex process, intimately linked to prior movement experience (88) and has been proposed to be more complex than visual MI. This difference might be attributed to the requirement of reactivating multiple perceptions that are typically present during physical movement, perceptions that are ordinarily not consciously attended. Additionally, these perceptions demands a high level of cognitive availability (89).
Two meta-analyses were conducted for the capacity of geniting kinesthetic MI, analyzing two age groups. Their results were mainly imprecise, not being able to stablish clear conclusions about the difference between older and younger adults. However a tendency for presenting a lower capacity was observed in the meta-analysis which included elder older adults (70–82 years).
Additionally, one meta-analysis was performed with older adults of 60–70 years for the ability to generate visual MI, also presenting a relevant imprecision.
Selection criteria for meta-analyses were well-stablished including homogeneous populations, and outcome measures. The population tested comprised healthy older adults (with some studies establishing cognitively healthy cut-offs), which would explain the absence of differences between groups. For example, pathological populations such as patients with spinal cord injuries present greater difficulties for the construction of both KI sensations and visual images during MI compared with healthy individuals (90).
We acknowledge the imprecision of the results on the between-study variability. There could be probably cultural differences that stress the differences between studies, as observed from heterogeneity results and the presence of outliers in the funnel plot. In fact, some studies detected that older adults presented a lower capacity, others revealed similar capacities, and another a higher capacity. Further studies should explore this concern, potentially changing the actual findings.
In this meta-analysis, no relevant differences were found in the evaluation of MI vividness in KI, IV, and EV modalities, between younger (18–37 years) and older (60–89 years) adults. We consider that these meta-analyses were the most consistent and precise, with 4 studies included in each meta-analysis, with a methodological quality of 5–6 points in JBI, and with a marked absence of heterogeneity. These results were not changed with leave-one-out analysis. Therefore, clear conclusions can be extracted from these results. However, the reader should be concerned that there were a low amount of studies meta-analyzed, so the present findings could be changed in the future.
High vividness in MI ensures that the imagery retains a strong resemblance to actual movement. It has been studied that vivid motor images enhances neural networks responsible for movement control, facilitating motor learning and brain plasticity (91). Vividness can be preserved under certain conditions even in the context of disease, as healthy and early-stage patients with Parkinson’s disease present similar vividness in (92).
Higher vividness has been associated to lower brain activation compared to those of poor imagers, which could be due to the compensatory activation of executive regions with potential to drive the imagery process (93).
A recent study conducted in a population of 18–60 years reported that the vividness of visual images significantly decreases with aging (94), and this aspect has also been observed in the older adult population in a study which, due to inclusion criteria, was not part of the meta-analysis (95).
However, vividness represents an additional cognitive dimension in MI and does not directly imply an influence on motor performance (96). Previous studies have suggested that the level of imagery vividness is not precisely associated with various aspects related to motor performance (95, 97) or with the ability to generate specific MI (95). Some factors may influence vividness preservation in aging, such as familiarity with the task, task-specific focus and the engagement of compensatory neural mechanisms (98–100).
Looking ahead, it is advisable for future research to focus on determining whether the vividness of MI among young and older adults varies depending on the complexity of the movement or in relation to other factors such as the difference between movements of the upper and lower limbs.
The meta-analyses were imprecise; however, observable tendencies were identified across mental chronometry in TUG, linear walk (5–10 m), and forward arm elevation tasks, with older adults requiring greater time.
No significant heterogeneity was observed in the TUG meta-analysis, whereas it was present in the linear walk. This was probably derived from the variability of linear walk distances assessed between studies, ranging from 5 to 10 m. In fact, the study of Watanabe and Tani (82) assessing mental chronometry in 10 m walk task was a relevant outlier, potentially generating the observed heterogeneity. Authors should point out that the differences in mental chronometry could be compatible with an increase in execution time to perform that action, as a result of the process of aging.
Another point contributing into the observed heterogeneity is the difficulty of the imagined task. The evaluated tasks appeared to produce similar imagination times between older and younger adults, with a tendency to present greater differences with larger imagined linear walk distances. In fact, other studies have also reported trends that mental chronometry begins to differ between older and younger adults with greater age (79), and when the difficulty of the task increases, for example, with higher linear walk distances (19–20 m) (79, 83). Furthermore, prior evidence has suggested that brain activity varies in MI processes depending on the task’s difficulty or complexity (101), and MI capacity is known to be less affected in simple tasks than in complex tasks (102).
Therefore, the meta-analysis results should be interpreted with caution, as these influential variables may change the direction of results. Although an observable tendency is being observed in the present meta-analyses, it is probable that these values vary with greater age and more difficult tasks.
Performance over/underestimation referred to the process in which an individual imagined a task with an improved or reduced performance, respectively, to the actual performance of its execution. It is expected that individuals present similar temporal features during MI and execution; nevertheless, the process of aging and the difficulty of the task can imply a tendency to over/underestimate their performance. The meta-analysis results were mainly imprecise, avoiding the establishment of a clear direction of the results. The meta-analysis explored older adults (60–82 years) with younger adults (18–35 years) in linear walk tasks of 5–10 m, with 95%CI indicating that either older or younger adults could present large differences in over/underestimation of their performance. A great heterogeneity was observed between studies, accompanied with the presence of outliers in the funnel plot. As mentioned previously, it could be derived from the between-study variability of meta-analyzed tasks, with one outlier being the 10 m linear walk task (82).
Other studies have observed that MI-execution temporal congruence is affected by age (79, 103) and the complexity of the task (79, 82, 83).
The assessment of over/underestimation exposes the quality of movement planification, affecting the efficiency and safety of movement (104). Several studies have been conducted in older and younger adults, exploring their mental chronometry for crossing a street. This is a relevant situation in which both a greater over/underestimation of their performance could lead to an accident. Not only is performance over/underestimation influencing this process, but additionally, the reaction time to make the decision to cross the street. Although younger and older adults can both over/underestimate their performance, previous research has suggested that older adults greatly overestimate their performance compared with younger adults, leading to unsafe crossing decisions (62, 105).
Additionally, greater performance overestimations are present in older adults with fear of falling, additionally explaining the increased risk of falls or recurrent falls in this population (106, 107). Further research should explore the observed between-study heterogeneity as possible confounding factors, such as the cultural context, difficulty of the task (linear walk distance), and older ages could moderate these results.
Implicit MI involves an internal process of mentally project and manipulate body schema (23). This process is implicit to any mental representation of movement, and participates simultaneously with explicit MI. In fact, both explicit and implicit MI activate similar neural networks associated with the somatosensory and motor regions (108).
Implicit MI was analyzed in terms of accuracy, response time, and efficiency on the ability to solve HLJ task. These variables did not fulfill criteria for meta-analysis, however, group differences were explored visually analyzing forest plots. Accuracy in HLJ revealed relevant results, with older adults presenting an observable reduction to accurately solve the task when grouping rotations. However, we hypothesize that this was not observed with specific rotations due to a lack of studies. Nevertheless, significant tendencies were observed in HLJ response time, with grouped, and individual rotations showing that older adults presented greater response time than younger adults. This was also observed with a reduced efficiency in the task.
Caution should be taken when extrapolating these results due to the limited number of studies analyzed. Nonetheless, these findings appear to be consistent with previous literature, which indicates that response time tends to increase with healthy aging (109). This increase is likely due to greater difficulty in processing stimuli and preparing movement rather than from an avoidance to respond (110).
Several investigations have examined whether HLJ performance is based on the ability to manipulate the body schema, or essentially on the ability to mentally rotate images. For such purpose, studies have compared the ability to identify rotated body images and unrelated body images, such as letters. Muto et al. (69), provided significant results addressing this question through response time performance. Older adults showed longer response times as a function of medial/lateral rotations of hand and foot images compared to younger adults, with response times increasing further with age. In contrast, older and younger adults exhibited similar response times for clockwise/counterclockwise rotations of letters. The same pattern occurred for the combination of medial/lateral and angular rotations of hand and foot images, while no differences were found between groups in response times for the interaction of clockwise/counterclockwise and angular rotation of letters.
The results of the present meta-analysis project significant clinical implications, mainly derived from the findings on vividness and mental chronometry. These meta-analyses showed, respectively, a preservation of the vividness during MI, and a tendency to present greater times in mental chronometry in TUG, linear walk and upper limb tasks. Additionally, observable tendencies were identified for the ability to mentally manipulate body schema, with older adults presenting lower accuracy, longer response time and reduced efficiency. These observed changes may be attributed to healthy aging. No clear conclusions can be drawn from the ability to generate MI or MI-execution temporal congruence. We acknowledge that the observed heterogeneity, limiting a precise conclusion, on the ability to generate MI and MI-Execution temporal congruence, may be derive from sampling differences across studies, as robust selection criteria were employed for inclusion in meta-analyses.
While vividness during MI appears to remain preserved, the overall quality of MI may decline due to age-related reductions in visuospatial and kinesthetic working memory (66). Furthermore, the representation of the body schema seems impaired, alongside increased task imagination times. These changes align with the typical decrease in physical performance associated with aging. Rehabilitation efforts aimed at improving vividness could potentially enhance both working memory and functional performance (66, 111).
Exploring MI capacities in healthy aging is essential for determining whether older adults are suitable candidates for interventions based on MI. In this context, the ability to generate MI plays a critical role in the successful implementation of MI-based interventions. Although no clear conclusions could be drawn this outcome, this ability can be improved with MI training (112). Similarly, MI-execution temporal congruence can also be enhanced, as recent research indicates that the inclusion of external feedback improves the temporal synchrony between MI and execution (113). Although the variation of this outcome with aging remains unclear, these findings indicate that it is possible to develop improvements on it. In the case of implicit MI ability, the inclusion of its training, can produce improvements in postural balance and postural control (114).
MI have been proposed as useful therapeutic tools for physical and functional rehabilitation in older adults, showing specific benefits for strength development (34). Current evidence, though of low to very-low quality, supports the use of MI to improve balance, gait speed, and lower limb function in this population (115). Furthermore, MI training has demonstrated potential benefits for cognitive functions. Improvements have been reported in patients with stroke (116, 117), multiple sclerosis (118), Parkinson’s disease (119, 120), healthy middle-aged adults (121), healthy older adults (122) and older adults individuals with cognitive impairment (123–125).
MI training represents a safe and relatively simple strategy for reducing the risk of falls and other mobility-related incidents. For instance, MI interventions could be particularly beneficial in enhancing the estimation of body movements. This improvement could help older adults in daily activities, such as street-crossing, where misjudging crossing time may pose significant risks (126).
Researchers and clinicians should consider that MI abilities may vary based on an individual’s age and the complexity of the imagined task. Tailoring interventions to these factors may optimize outcomes and ensure the effective application of MI in rehabilitation programs.
This meta-analysis has notable limitations that warrant consideration. First, the small number of studies included in some of the meta-analyses significantly limits the certainty of the results. For example, while the meta-analysis of the vividness of MI were the most consistent, they included only four studies each, raising concerns about the stability of these results. Similarly, the meta-analysis of ability to generate MI and MI-execution temporal congruence would have benefited from additional studies to reduce imprecision.
Second, the heterogeneity observed across meta-analyses may arise from variations in participant sampling, including differences in participant origins, sampling methods, and case–control matching techniques to control for covariates. Additional factors, such as variations in measurement tools and the difficulty levels of the tasks assessed, may also contribute to inconsistencies in the results.
Another important consideration for further research is addressing the influence of covariates, such as physical functioning, spatial representation ability or cognitive functioning, which may have influenced the outcomes. This could be managed through specific recruitment methods, like pairing older and younger adults based on these covariates, and statistically adjusting for these differences in the final analysis. This would help clarifying the real relationship between aging and MI capacities.
The vividness of MI in KI, IV and EV modalities seem to be preserved in older adults. Additionally, older adults tend to require more time for mental chronometry tasks in TUG, linear walk, and upper limb movements. Implicit MI as assessed through HLJ task indicates that older adults exhibit lower accuracy, greater response times, resulting in decreased efficiency. The ability to generate MI in KI and visual modalities yielded imprecise results, and no clear conclusions could be either drawn regarding MI-execution temporal congruence due to imprecision. Further research is needed, which could potentially modify the current results.
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
JF-M: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MG-I: Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. AM-R: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JL-V: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – original draft. AP-A: Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. RT: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The project that gave rise to these results received the support of a fellowship from “la Caixa” Foundation (ID 100010434). The fellowship code is “LCF/BQ/DR22/11950015.”
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2024.1405791/full#supplementary-material
1. Rivan, NFM, Singh, DKA, Shahar, S, Wen, GJ, Rajab, NF, Din, NC, et al. Cognitive frailty is a robust predictor of falls, injuries, and disability among community-dwelling older adults. BMC Geriatr. (2021) 21:593. doi: 10.1186/S12877-021-02525-Y
2. Curran, E, Rosato, M, Ferry, F, and Leavey, G. Prevalence and factors associated with anxiety and depression in older adults: gender differences in psychosocial indicators. J Affect Disord. (2020) 267:114–22. doi: 10.1016/J.JAD.2020.02.018
3. Li, D, Zhang, D, Shao, JJ, Qi, XD, and Tian, LA. A meta-analysis of the prevalence of depressive symptoms in Chinese older adults. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. (2014) 58:1–9. doi: 10.1016/J.ARCHGER.2013.07.016
4. Vaughan, L, Corbin, AL, and Goveas, JS. Depression and frailty in later life: a systematic review. Clin Interv Aging. (2015) 10:1947–58. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S69632
5. Gao, Q, Hu, K, Yan, C, Zhao, B, Mei, F, Chen, F, et al. Associated factors of sarcopenia in community-dwelling older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutrients. (2021) 13:4291. doi: 10.3390/NU13124291
6. Knoop, V, Cloots, B, Costenoble, A, Debain, A, Vella Azzopardi, R, Vermeiren, S, et al. Fatigue and the prediction of negative health outcomes: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. (2021) 67:101261. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2021.101261
7. Vermeiren, S, Vella-Azzopardi, R, Beckwée, D, Habbig, AK, Scafoglieri, A, Jansen, B, et al. Frailty and the prediction of negative health outcomes: a meta-analysis. J Am Med Dir Assoc. (2016) 17:1163.e1–1163.e17. doi: 10.1016/J.JAMDA.2016.09.010
8. Cacioppo, JT, Hughes, ME, Waite, LJ, Hawkley, LC, and Thisted, RA. Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychol Aging. (2006) 21:140–51. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.140
9. Burnette, D, Ye, X, Cheng, Z, and Ruan, H. Living alone, social cohesion, and quality of life among older adults in rural and urban China: a conditional process analysis. Int Psychogeriatr. (2021) 33:469–79. doi: 10.1017/S1041610220001210
10. Hill, NL, McDermott, C, Mogle, J, Munoz, E, Depasquale, N, Wion, R, et al. Subjective cognitive impairment and quality of life: a systematic review. Int Psychogeriatr. (2017) 29:1965–77. doi: 10.1017/S1041610217001636
11. Roberts, HJ, Johnson, KM, Sullivan, JE, and Hoppes, CW. Balance confidence and balance performance, but not fall history are associated with quality of life in community-dwelling older adults: a cross sectional study. J Geriatr Phys Ther. (2023) 46:82–9. doi: 10.1519/JPT.0000000000000349
12. Deardorff, WJ, Liu, PL, Sloane, R, Houtven, CV, Pieper, CF, Hastings, SN, et al. Association of Sensory and Cognitive Impairment with Healthcare Utilization and cost in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2019) 67:1617–24. doi: 10.1111/JGS.15891
13. Kim, MJ, Jang, SY, Cheong, HK, and Oh, IH. Association of Frailty with healthcare costs using claims data in Korean older adults aged 66. J Nutr Health Aging. (2021) 25:653–9. doi: 10.1007/S12603-021-1612-8
14. Picco, L, Achilla, E, Abdin, E, Chong, SA, Vaingankar, JA, McCrone, P, et al. Economic burden of multimorbidity among older adults: impact on healthcare and societal costs. BMC Health Serv Res. (2016) 16:173. doi: 10.1186/S12913-016-1421-7
15. Raz, N, Lindenberger, U, Rodrigue, KM, Kennedy, KM, Head, D, Williamson, A, et al. Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral Cortex. (2005) 15:1676–89. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi044
16. de Kort, FAS, Vinke, EJ, van der Lelij, EJ, Anblagan, D, Bastin, ME, Beiser, A, et al. Cerebral white matter hyperintensity volumes: normative age- and sex-specific values from 15 population-based cohorts comprising 14,876 individuals. Neurobiol Aging. (2024) 146:38–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2024.11.006
17. Hu, J, Craig, MS, Knight, SP, De Looze, C, Meaney, JF, Kenny, RA, et al. Regional changes in cerebral perfusion with age when accounting for changes in gray-matter volume. Magn Reson Med. (2024). doi: 10.1002/mrm.30376
18. Orssatto, LBR, Blazevich, AJ, and Trajano, GS. Ageing reduces persistent inward current contribution to motor neurone firing: potential mechanisms and the role of exercise. J Physiol. (2023) 601:3705–16. doi: 10.1113/JP284603
19. Gooijers, J, Pauwels, L, Hehl, M, Seer, C, Cuypers, K, and Swinnen, SP. Aging, brain plasticity, and motor learning. Ageing Res Rev. (2024) 102:102569. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2024.102569
20. Decety, J. The neurophysiological basis of motor imagery. Behav Brain Res. (1996) 77:45–52. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00225-1
21. La Touche, R. Métodos de representación del movimiento en rehabilitación. Construyendo un marco conceptual para la aplicación en clínica. J MOVE Ther Sci. (2020) 2:152–159. doi: 10.37382/jomts.v2i2.42
22. Jeannerod, M, and Frak, V. Mental imaging of motor activity in humans. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (1999) 9:735–9. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00038-0
23. Jongsma, MLA, Baas, CM, Sangen, AFM, Aarts, PBM, Van Der Lubbe, RHJ, Meulenbroek, RGJ, et al. Children with unilateral cerebral palsy show diminished implicit motor imagery with the affected hand. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2016) 58:277–84. doi: 10.1111/DMCN.12819
24. Suica, Z, Behrendt, F, Gäumann, S, Gerth, U, Schmidt-Trucksäss, A, Ettlin, T, et al. Imagery ability assessments: a cross-disciplinary systematic review and quality evaluation of psychometric properties. BMC Med. (2022) 20:166. doi: 10.1186/s12916-022-02295-3
25. Robin, N, Coudevylle, GR, Dominique, L, Rulleau, T, Champagne, R, Guillot, A, et al. Translation and validation of the movement imagery questionnaire-3 second French version. J Bodyw Mov Ther. (2021) 28:540–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jbmt.2021.09.004
26. Roberts, R, Callow, N, Hardy, L, Markland, D, and Bringer, J. Movement imagery ability: development and assessment of a revised version of the vividness of movement imagery questionnaire. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2008) 30:200–21. doi: 10.1123/JSEP.30.2.200
27. Malouin, F, Richards, CL, Jackson, PL, Lafleur, MF, Durand, A, and Doyon, J. The Kinesthetic and visual imagery questionnaire (KVIQ) for assessing motor imagery in persons with physical disabilities: a reliability and construct validity study. J Neurol Phys Ther. (2007) 31:20–9. doi: 10.1097/01.NPT.0000260567.24122.64
28. Butti, N, Montirosso, R, Giusti, L, Piccinini, L, Borgatti, R, and Urgesi, C. Early brain damage affects body schema and person perception abilities in children and adolescents with spastic diplegia. Neural Plast. (2019) 2019:1–17. doi: 10.1155/2019/1678984
29. Gentili, R, Han, CE, Schweighofer, N, and Papaxanthis, C. Motor learning without doing: trial-by-trial improvement in motor performance during mental training. J Neurophysiol. (2010) 104:774–83. doi: 10.1152/jn.00257.2010
30. Hurst, AJ, and Boe, SG. Imagining the way forward: a review of contemporary motor imagery theory. Front Hum Neurosci. (2022) 16:1033493. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.1033493
31. Behrendt, F, Zumbrunnen, V, Brem, L, Suica, Z, Gäumann, S, Ziller, C, et al. Effect of motor imagery training on motor learning in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:9467. doi: 10.3390/IJERPH18189467
32. Paravlic, AH, Slimani, M, Tod, D, Marusic, U, Milanovic, Z, and Pisot, R. Effects and dose-response relationships of motor imagery practice on strength development in healthy adult populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. (2018) 48:1165–87. doi: 10.1007/S40279-018-0874-8
33. Marusic, U, and Grosprêtre, S. Non-physical approaches to counteract age-related functional deterioration: applications for rehabilitation and neural mechanisms. Eur J Sport Sci. (2018) 18:639–49. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2018.1447018
34. Liu, XJ, Ge, S, Cordova, A, Yaghi, Z, Jiang, BY, Yue, GH, et al. Elderly may benefit more from motor imagery training in gaining muscle strength than young adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Psychol. (2023) 13:1052826. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1052826
35. Herranz-Gómez, A, Gaudiosi, C, Angulo-Díaz-Parreño, S, Suso-Martí, L, Touche, RL, and Cuenca-Martínez, F. Effectiveness of motor imagery and action observation on functional variables: an umbrella and mapping review with meta-meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 118:828–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.09.009
36. Kwok, JYY, Smith, R, Chan, LML, Lam, LCC, Fong, DYT, Choi, EPH, et al. Managing freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. J Neurol. (2022) 269:3310–24. doi: 10.1007/S00415-022-11031-Z
37. Suso-Martí, L, Touche, RL, Angulo-Díaz-Parreño, S, and Cuenca-Martínez, F. Effectiveness of motor imagery and action observation training on musculoskeletal pain intensity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur J Pain. (2020) 24:886–901. doi: 10.1002/EJP.1540
38. Hardwick, RM, Caspers, S, Eickhoff, SB, and Swinnen, SP. Neural correlates of action: comparing meta-analyses of imagery, observation, and execution. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 94:31–44. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2018.08.003
39. Jiang, D, Edwards, MG, Mullins, P, and Callow, N. The neural substrates for the different modalities of movement imagery. Brain Cogn. (2015) 97:22–31. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.04.005
40. Callow, N, Edwards, MG, Jones, AL, Hardy, L, and Connell, S. Action dual tasks reveal differential effects of visual imagery perspectives on motor performance. Quart J Exp Psychol. (2019) 72:1401–11. doi: 10.1177/1747021818811464
41. Kwon, S, Kim, J, and Kim, T. Neuropsychological activations and networks while performing visual and Kinesthetic motor imagery. Brain Sci. (2023) 13:983. doi: 10.3390/brainsci13070983
42. Yang, YJ, Jeon, EJ, Kim, JS, and Chung, CK. Characterization of kinesthetic motor imagery compared with visual motor imageries. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:3751. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-82241-0
43. Passarello, N, Liparoti, M, Padulo, C, Sorrentino, P, Alivernini, F, Fairfield, B, et al. Motor imagery as a key factor for healthy ageing: a review of new insights and techniques. Brain Sci. (2022) 12:1492. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12111492
44. Saimpont, A, Malouin, F, Tousignant, B, and Jackson, PL. Motor imagery and aging. J Mot Behav. (2013) 45:21–8. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2012.740098
45. Page, MJ, McKenzie, JE, Bossuyt, PM, Boutron, I, Hoffmann, TC, Mulrow, CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. (2021) 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/BMJ.N71
46. Ouzzani, M, Hammady, H, Fedorowicz, Z, and Elmagarmid, A. Rayyan—a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Syst Rev. (2016) 5:210. doi: 10.1186/s13643-016-0384-4
47. Moola, S, Muun, Z, Tufanaru, C, Aromataris, E, Sears, K, Sfetcu, R, et al. Systematic reviews of etiology and risk. A JBI Manual for Evidence Synthesis. JBI. eds. E Aromataris, C Lockwood, K Porritt, B Pilla, and Z Jordan (2020) doi: 10.46658/JBIMES-24-06
48. Wan, X, Wang, W, Liu, J, and Tong, T. Estimating the sample mean and standard deviation from the sample size, median, range and/or interquartile range. BMC Med Res Methodol. (2014) 14:135. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-14-135
49. Higgins, J. P. T., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M. J., et al. (2022). “Chapter 6: Choosing effect measures and computing estimates of effect,” Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 6.3. eds. JPT Higgins, J Thomas, J Chandler, M Cumpston, T Li, and MJ Page, et al. Cochrane. Available at: https://www.training.cochrane.org/handbook
50. Hedges, LV. Distribution theory for Glass’s estimator of effect size and related estimators. J Educ Stat. (1981) 6:107–28. doi: 10.3102/10769986006002107
51. Cohen, J. The statistical power of abnormal-social psychological research: a review. J Abnorm Soc Psychol. (1962) 65:145–53. doi: 10.1037/h0045186
52. Deeks, JJ, Higgins, JPT, Altman, DG, McKenzie, JE, and Veroniki, AA. Chapter 10.10.2: identifying and measuring heterogeneity. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions: Vol. version 6.4 (updated august 2023). Cochrane. eds. JPT Higgins, J Thomas, J Chandler, M Cumpston, T Li, and MJ Page, et al. (2023).
53. Egger, M, Smith, GD, Schneider, M, and Minder, C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. (1997) 315:629–34. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629
54. Furuya-Kanamori, L, Barendregt, JJ, and Doi, SAR. A new improved graphical and quantitative method for detecting bias in meta-analysis. Int J Evid Based Healthc. (2018) 16:195–203. doi: 10.1097/XEB.0000000000000141
55. Viechtbauer, W, and Cheung, MW-L. Outlier and influence diagnostics for meta-analysis. Res Synth Methods. (2010) 1:112–25. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.11
56. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing [computer software]. R foundation for statistical computing. (2021). Available at: https://www.R-project.org/
57. Viechtbauer, W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw. (2010) 36:1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v036.i03
58. Schwarzer, G., Carpenter, J. R., and Rücker, G. Metasens: statistical methods for sensitivity analysis in meta-analysis (version R package version 1.5-2) [computer software]. (2023). Available at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=metasens
59. Beauchet, O, Launay, CP, Sekhon, H, Gautier, J, Chabot, J, Levinoff, EJ, et al. Body position and motor imagery strategy effects on imagining gait in healthy adults: results from a cross-sectional study. PLoS One. (2018) 13:e0191513. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0191513
60. Caçola, P, Roberson, J, and Gabbard, C. Aging in movement representations for sequential finger movements: a comparison between young-, middle-aged, and older adults. Brain Cogn. (2013) 82:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.02.003
61. Devlin, AL, and Wilson, PH. Adult age differences in the ability to mentally transform object and body stimuli. Aging Neuropsychol Cognit. (2010) 17:709–29. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2010.510554
62. Dommes, A, Cavallo, V, and Oxley, J. Functional declines as predictors of risky street-crossing decisions in older pedestrians. Accid Anal Prev. (2013) 59:135–43. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2013.05.017
63. Kanokwan, S, Pramkamol, W, Wipatcharee, K, Warissara, W, Siwarit, R, Sompiya, S, et al. Age-related differences in brain activity during physical and imagined sit-to-stand in healthy young and older adults. J Phys Ther Sci. (2019) 31:440–8. doi: 10.1589/jpts.31.440
64. Kotegawa, K, Yasumura, A, and Teramoto, W. Changes in prefrontal cortical activation during motor imagery of precision gait with age and task difficulty. Behav Brain Res. (2021) 399:113046. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.113046
65. Liu, KPY, Lai, M, Fong, SSM, and Bissett, M. Imagery ability and imagery perspective preference: a study of their relationship and age- and gender-related changes. Behav Neurol. (2019) 2019:1–8. doi: 10.1155/2019/7536957
66. Malouin, F, Richards, CL, and Durand, A. Normal aging and motor imagery vividness: implications for mental practice training in rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2010) 91:1122–7. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2010.03.007
67. Mitra, S, Doherty, N, Boulton, H, and Maylor, EA. Age-related reversal of postural adjustment characteristics during motor imagery. Psychol Aging. (2016) 31:958–69. doi: 10.1037/pag0000120
68. Mulder, T, Hochstenbach, JBH, van Heuvelen, MJG, and den Otter, AR. Motor imagery: the relation between age and imagery capacity. Hum Mov Sci. (2007) 26:203–11. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.01.001
69. Muto, H, Suzuki, M, and Sekiyama, K. Advanced aging effects on implicit motor imagery and its links to motor performance: an investigation via mental rotation of letters, hands, and feet. Front Aging Neurosci. (2022) 14:1025667. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.1025667
70. Nagashima, I, Takeda, K, Harada, Y, Mochizuki, H, and Shimoda, N. Age-related differences in strategy in the hand mental rotation task. Front Hum Neurosci. (2021) 15:615584. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.615584
71. Naveteur, J, Delzenne, J, Sockeel, P, Watelain, E, and Dupuy, MA. Crosswalk time estimation and time perception: an experimental study among older female pedestrians. Accid Anal Prev. (2013) 60:42–9. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2013.08.014
72. Raimo, S, Boccia, M, Di Vita, A, Cropano, M, Guariglia, C, Grossi, D, et al. The body across adulthood: on the relation between Interoception and body representations. Front Neurosci. (2021) 15:586684. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.586684
73. Rulleau, T, Robin, N, Abou-Dest, A, Chesnet, D, and Toussaint, L. Does the improvement of position sense following motor imagery practice vary as a function of age and time of day? Exp Aging Res. (2018) 44:443–54. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2018.1521496
74. Saimpont, A, Malouin, F, Tousignant, B, and Jackson, PL. The influence of body configuration on motor imagery of walking in younger and older adults. Neuroscience. (2012) 222:49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.06.066
75. Saimpont, A, Malouin, F, Tousignant, B, and Jackson, PL. Assessing motor imagery ability in younger and older adults by combining measures of vividness, controllability and timing of motor imagery. Brain Res. (2015) 1597:196–209. doi: 10.1016/J.BRAINRES.2014.11.050
76. Saimpont, A, Pozzo, T, and Papaxanthis, C. Aging affects the mental rotation of left and right hands. PLoS One. (2009) 4:e6714. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006714
77. Schott, N. Age-related differences in motor imagery: working memory as a mediator. Exp Aging Res. (2012) 38:559–83. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2012.726045
78. Schott, N. German test of the controllability of motor imagery in older adults. Zeitschrift Fur Gerontologie Und Geriatrie. (2013) 46:663–72. doi: 10.1007/S00391-013-0520-X
79. Schott, N, and Munzert, J. Temporal accuracy of motor imagery in older women. Int J Sport Psychol. (2007) 38:304–20.
80. Skoura, X, Papaxanthis, C, Vinter, A, and Pozzo, T. Mentally represented motor actions in normal aging. I. Age effects on the temporal features of overt and covert execution of actions. Behav Brain Res. (2005) 165:229–39. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.07.023
81. Wang, S, Williams, J, and Wilmut, K. Constraints on motor planning across the life span: physical, cognitive, and motor factors. Psychol Aging. (2020) 35:421–33. doi: 10.1037/pag0000408
82. Watanabe, M, and Tani, H. Using crutches during walking possibly reduces gait imagery accuracy among healthy young and older adults. J Phys Ther Sci. (2022) 34:673–7. doi: 10.1589/jpts.34.673
83. Zhuang, X, Zhang, T, Chen, W, Jiang, R, and Ma, G. Pedestrian estimation of their crossing time on multi-lane roads. Accid Anal Prev. (2020) 143:105581. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2020.105581
84. Zito, GA, Cazzoli, D, Scheffler, L, Jäger, M, Müri, RM, Mosimann, UP, et al. Street crossing behavior in younger and older pedestrians: an eye- and head-tracking study. BMC Geriatr. (2015) 15:176. doi: 10.1186/s12877-015-0175-0
85. Guillot, A, Collet, C, Nguyen, VA, Malouin, F, Richards, C, and Doyon, J. Brain activity during visual versus kinesthetic imagery: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. (2009) 30:2157–72. doi: 10.1002/HBM.20658
86. Neuper, C, Scherer, R, Reiner, M, and Pfurtscheller, G. Imagery of motor actions: differential effects of kinesthetic and visual-motor mode of imagery in single-trial EEG. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. (2005) 25:668–77. doi: 10.1016/J.COGBRAINRES.2005.08.014
87. Ridderinkhof, KR, and Brass, M. How Kinesthetic motor imagery works: a predictive-processing theory of visualization in sports and motor expertise. J Physiol Paris. (2015) 109:53–63. doi: 10.1016/J.JPHYSPARIS.2015.02.003
88. Orlandi, A, Arno, E, and Proverbio, AM. The effect of expertise on Kinesthetic motor imagery of complex actions. Brain Topogr. (2020) 33:238–54. doi: 10.1007/s10548-020-00760-x
89. Rimbert, S, and Fleck, S. Long-term kinesthetic motor imagery practice with a BCI: impacts on user experience, motor cortex oscillations and BCI performances. Comput Hum Behav. (2023) 146:107789. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2023.107789
90. Thomschewski, A, Ströhlein, A, Langthaler, PB, Schmid, E, Potthoff, J, Höller, P, et al. Imagine there is no Plegia. Mental motor imagery difficulties in patients with traumatic spinal cord injury. Front Neurosci. (2017) 11:689. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00689
91. Jackson, PL, Lafleur, MF, Malouin, F, Richards, CL, and Doyon, J. Functional cerebral reorganization following motor sequence learning through mental practice with motor imagery. NeuroImage. (2003) 20:1171–80. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00369-0
92. Readman, MR, Crawford, TJ, Linkenauger, SA, Bek, J, and Poliakoff, E. Motor imagery vividness and symptom severity in Parkinson’s disease. J Neuropsychol. (2023) 17:180–92. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12293
93. Fulford, J, Milton, F, Salas, D, Smith, A, Simler, A, Winlove, C, et al. The neural correlates of visual imagery vividness—an fMRI study and literature review. Cortex. (2018) 105:26–40. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.014
94. Gulyás, E, Gombos, F, Sütöri, S, Lovas, A, Ziman, G, and Kovács, I. Visual imagery vividness declines across the lifespan. Cortex. (2022) 154:365–74. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.06.011
95. Subirats, L, Allali, G, Briansoulet, M, Salle, JY, and Perrochon, A. Age and gender differences in motor imagery. J Neurol Sci. (2018) 391:114–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2018.06.015
96. Mizuguchi, N, Suezawa, M, and Kanosue, K. Vividness and accuracy: two independent aspects of motor imagery. Neurosci Res. (2019) 147:17–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2018.12.005
97. Lawrence, G, Callow, N, and Roberts, R. Watch me if you can: imagery ability moderates observational learning effectiveness. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:522. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00522
98. Baddeley, AD, and Andrade, J. Working memory and the vividness of imagery. J Exp Psychol Gen. (2000) 129:126–45. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.1.126
99. Johnson, MK, Kuhl, BA, Mitchell, KJ, Ankudowich, E, and Durbin, KA. Age-related differences in the neural basis of the subjective vividness of memories: evidence from multivoxel pattern classification. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. (2015) 15:644–61. doi: 10.3758/s13415-015-0352-9
100. Mulder, T. Motor imagery and action observation: cognitive tools for rehabilitation. J Neural Transm. (2007) 114:1265–78. doi: 10.1007/s00702-007-0763-z
101. Kotegawa, K, Yasumura, A, and Teramoto, W. Activity in the prefrontal cortex during motor imagery of precision gait: an fNIRS study. Exp Brain Res. (2020) 238:221–8. doi: 10.1007/s00221-019-05706-9
102. Kalicinski, M, Kempe, M, and Bock, O. Motor imagery: effects of age, task complexity, and task setting. Exp Aging Res. (2015) 41:25–38. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2015.978202
103. Personnier, P, Ballay, Y, and Papaxanthis, C. Mentally represented motor actions in normal aging: III. Electromyographic features of imagined arm movements. Behav Brain Res. (2010) 206:184–91. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.09.011
104. Gabbard, C. Mental representation for action in the elderly: implications for movement efficiency and injury risk. J Appl Gerontol. (2015) 34:NP202–NP212. doi: 10.1177/0733464813497255
105. Oxley, JA, Ihsen, E, Fildes, BN, Charlton, JL, and Day, RH. Crossing roads safely: an experimental study of age differences in gap selection by pedestrians. Accid Anal Prev. (2005) 37:962–71. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2005.04.017
106. Sakurai, R, Fujiwara, Y, Yasunaga, M, Suzuki, H, Sakuma, N, Imanaka, K, et al. Older adults with fear of falling show deficits in motor imagery of gait. J Nutr Health Aging. (2017) 21:721–6. doi: 10.1007/s12603-016-0811-1
107. Sakurai, R, Montero-Odasso, M, Suzuki, H, Ogawa, S, and Fujiwara, Y. Motor imagery deficits in high-functioning older adults and its impact on fear of falling and falls. J Gerontol Ser A. (2021) 76:e228–34. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glab073
108. Osuagwu, BA, and Vuckovic, A. Similarities between explicit and implicit motor imagery in mental rotation of hands: an EEG study. Neuropsychologia. (2014) 65:197–210. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.029
109. Fozard, JL, Vercruyssen, M, Reynolds, SL, Hancock, PA, and Quilter, RE. Age differences and changes in reaction time: the Baltimore longitudinal study of aging. J Gerontol. (1994) 49:P179–89. doi: 10.1093/geronj/49.4.P179
110. Hardwick, RM, Forrence, AD, Costello, MG, Zackowski, K, and Haith, AM. Age-related increases in reaction time result from slower preparation, not delayed initiation. J Neurophysiol. (2022) 128:582–92. doi: 10.1152/jn.00072.2022
111. Mateo, S, Reilly, KT, Collet, C, and Rode, G. Descriptive pilot study of vividness and temporal equivalence during motor imagery training after quadriplegia. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. (2018) 61:300–8. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2018.06.003
112. Zafar, M, Ghous, M, and Jahan, S. An eight-week single-blind randomised controlled trial on dual task balance training and motor imagery practice in ischaemic stroke survivors. J Pak Med Assoc. (2024) 74:773–7. doi: 10.47391/JPMA.7152
113. Stuerner, J, Sehle, A, and Liepert, J. Extrinsic feedback facilitates mental chronometry abilities in stroke patients. NeuroRehabilitation. (2023) 53:347–54. doi: 10.3233/NRE-230093
114. Díaz-López, N, Monge-Pereira, E, Jodra-Centeno, E, Molina-Rueda, F, and Miangolarra-Page, JC. Use of recognition of laterality through implicit motor imagery for the improvement of postural control and balance in subacute stroke patients: a randomized controlled study. Rev Neurol. (2022) 74:375–82. doi: 10.33588/rn.7412.2022039
115. Nicholson, V, Watts, N, Chani, Y, and Keogh, JW. Motor imagery training improves balance and mobility outcomes in older adults: a systematic review. J Physiother. (2019) 65:200–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jphys.2019.08.007
116. Haire, CM, Vuong, V, Tremblay, L, Patterson, KK, Chen, JL, and Thaut, MH. Effects of therapeutic instrumental music performance and motor imagery on chronic post-stroke cognition and affect: a randomized controlled trial. NeuroRehabilitation. (2021) 48:195–208. doi: 10.3233/NRE-208014
117. Mao, H, Li, Y, Tang, L, Chen, Y, Ni, J, Liu, L, et al. Effects of mirror neuron system-based training on rehabilitation of stroke patients. Brain Behav. (2020) 10:e01729. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1729
118. Cordani, C, Valsasina, P, Preziosa, P, Meani, A, Filippi, M, and Rocca, MA. Action observation training promotes motor improvement and modulates functional network dynamic connectivity in multiple sclerosis. Multiple Sclerosis. (2021) 27:139–46. doi: 10.1177/1352458519887332
119. Caligiore, D, Mustile, M, Fineschi, A, Romano, L, Piras, F, Assogna, F, et al. Action observation with dual task for improving cognitive abilities in Parkinson’s disease: a pilot study. Front Syst Neurosci. (2019) 13:7. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00007
120. Tamir, R, Dickstein, R, and Huberman, M. Integration of motor imagery and physical practice in group treatment applied to subjects with Parkinson’s disease. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. (2007) 21:68–75. doi: 10.1177/1545968306292608
121. Rocca, MA, Fumagalli, S, Pagani, E, Gatti, R, Riccitelli, GC, Preziosa, P, et al. Action observation training modifies brain gray matter structure in healthy adult individuals. Brain Imaging Behav. (2017) 11:1343–52. doi: 10.1007/S11682-016-9625-3
122. Gomez-Pilar, J, Corralejo, R, Nicolas-Alonso, LF, Alvarez, D, and Hornero, R. Assessment of neurofeedback training by means of motor imagery based-BCI for cognitive rehabilitation. Ann Int Conf. (2014) 2014:3630–3. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2014.6944409
123. Estrada-Barranco, C, Martinez-Javaloyes, MLÁ, Rodriguez-Costa, I, Sanz-Esteban, I, Bermejo-Franco, A, Aranda-Ruiperez, A, et al. Effectiveness of a program based on action-observation training (AOT) on motor, functional and cognitive aspects in patients with cognitive impairment: a non-randomized controlled trial. Healthcare. (2023) 11:1030. doi: 10.3390/healthcare11071030
124. Liu, W, Li, Z, Xie, Y, He, A, Hao, D, and Dong, A. Effects of a combined motor imagery and action observation intervention on vascular cognitive impairment: a randomized pilot study. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. (2022) 101:358–66. doi: 10.1097/PHM.0000000000001827
125. Rojasavastera, R, Bovonsunthonchai, S, Hiengkaew, V, and Senanarong, V. Action observation combined with gait training to improve gait and cognition in elderly with mild cognitive impairment a randomized controlled trial. Dementia Neuropsychol. (2020) 14:118–27. doi: 10.1590/1980-57642020DN14-020004
Keywords: motor imagery, movement imagery, mental representation, older adults, aging, geriatrics, systematic review, meta-analysis
Citation: Fierro-Marrero J, González-Iglesias M, Melis-Romeu A, López-Vidal JA, Paris-Alemany A and La Touche R (2025) Exploring the impact of aging on motor imagery abilities: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Front. Public Health. 12:1405791. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1405791
Received: 28 March 2024; Accepted: 30 December 2024;
Published: 23 January 2025.
Edited by:
Pedro Alexandre Duarte-Mendes, Polytechnic Institute of Castelo Branco, PortugalReviewed by:
Bernadette Ann Murphy, Ontario Tech University, CanadaCopyright © 2025 Fierro-Marrero, González-Iglesias, Melis-Romeu, López-Vidal, Paris-Alemany and La Touche. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Alba Paris-Alemany, YWxiYXBhcmlzQGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==
†These authors have contributed equally to this work and share secondary authorship
‡These authors have contributed equally to this work and share senior authorship
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.