Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Public Health, 18 July 2024
Sec. Public Health Education and Promotion

Investigating seasonal changes in factors associated with COVID-19 concerns: Results from a serial cross-sectional survey study in Germany between 2020 and 2023

  • Department Risk Communication, German Federal Institute for Risk Assessment (BfR), Berlin, Germany

Objective: COVID-19 risk perceptions are discussed to be volatile and have been shown to be connected to the adoption of preventive public health behaviors. This study aimed to investigate changes in COVID-19 concerns and influencing factors as a function of season among the German public.

Methods: Sixty-three waves of cross-sectional telephone surveys with German participants aged 14 years and older conducted at least monthly between June 2020 and April 2023 provided the data basis (N = 63,471). After pooling participants of different waves by season (spring, summer, fall, winter), data were analyzed with regard to changes in physical health, mental health, economic, and social COVID-19 concerns. Individual characteristics (e.g., age), COVID-19 behavior (e.g., hygiene practices), and related perceptions (e.g., controllability of risk) were considered as predictors of composite concerns in different seasons.

Results: Results showed a higher between-seasons than within-seasons variability in concerns, with rises in physical and mental health and social concerns during fall. Multivariate regressions revealed being female, lower education, adopting protective measures, and higher perceived probability of infection in both public and private settings to be consistent predictors of higher COVID-19 concerns. Coefficients of these predictors remained comparatively stable over seasons and years.

Conclusion: Results indicate re-occurring changes in concerns during a prolonged crisis, with distinct characteristics being consistently associated with higher reported concerns. To ensure the application of protective measures, communicators should consider that risk perceptions are subject to fluctuations, but that certain groups of individuals tend to develop them and therefore deserve particular focus.

1 Introduction

1.1 Prolonged public health crises and the development of concerns

The global COVID-19 pandemic has had a profound impact on people’s lives in many ways in recent years. Not only have people been physically affected by the disease itself caused by SARS-CoV-2, but there have also been problems with mental health as a result of measures taken by governments (1, 2). Other affected areas relate to financial conditions, for example due to the loss of jobs (3), or social issues due to fewer and altered opportunities for interpersonal interaction (4). Individuals form their own opinions on these topics and such perceptions can have an impact on behavior with regard to compliance with measures (5). In this context, several studies have shown that risk perceptions in relation to COVID-19 were associated with general behavior change (6) or adopting specific protective measures (7).

Accordingly, understanding risk perceptions has been an important task of research during the pandemic. To investigate risk perceptions, studies often measure individuals’ concerns about a topic (8). The literature provides several approaches to conceptualize concerns, with contemporary approaches focusing on cognitive processes and viewing concerns as a product of thought patterns (9). Relatedly, concerns can develop through an interplay of different cognitive, emotional, and social factors (10), stressing the importance of context and culture (11). A number of studies provided evidence of high levels of concerns about COVID-19 in many regions of the world (12).

Further research has examined which individual and contextual factors influence these perceptions in different countries. For instance, studies have explored the impact of sociodemographic characteristics such as gender, age, and education on concerns. While some studies found higher concerns in female participants (13, 14), others did not find an effect of gender (15). In terms of age, some works showed positive associations with levels of concerns (16) and others negative associations (17). A more conclusive picture emerged for effects of education, as several studies found less educated participants to be more concerned (3, 18). Another important set of variables to explain concerns have been related perceptions, as these are assumed to provide the basis for the development of concerns. For instance, Solymosi et al. (19) found lower perceived control in participants exhibiting higher levels of worry about COVID-19, while Khaira and Sari (4) showed perceived susceptibility to being infected to be positively associated with worry. Further studies provided evidence of negative associations of knowledge about COVID-19 (20) and perceived appropriateness of government measures (21) with concerns. Similarly, some studies investigated the relationship between COVID-19-related behavior and concerns. Participants adopting preventive measures (e.g., social isolation) were more likely to report higher levels of concerns (22).

However, the majority of these existing studies focused either on individual points in time during the pandemic or on relatively short periods. As the development of concerns about a specific object can be subject to changes over time due to adjustment to circumstances (23, 24), studies over longer periods could help to better understand the potential variability of concerns and identify relevant influencing factors. What the literature is lacking so far is the perspective on seasonality in this context. As with other respiratory diseases, seasonal fluctuations in the spread of COVID-19 have been observed in temperate regions (2527). While it is assumed that climatic conditions such as temperature and humidity affect the survival of the virus, mechanisms of transmission are less clear (28). This much-discussed effect of season could also play a role in shaping conditions for the development of concerns. Transmission patterns in the form of peaks in infection numbers in fall and winter were directly observable for the public, and governments as well as health organizations often intensified communication efforts in colder months (29, 30). Moreover, behavioral changes, such as spending more time indoors during colder months (31), and seasonal variations in media coverage, with potentially increased attention during infection outbreaks, could contribute to increased awareness, potentially influencing risk perceptions.

Against this background, further investigations into the research gaps on the development of concerns about COVID-19 over longer periods during the pandemic, on the effect of season, and on the consistent nature of influencing factors of concerns appear to be useful.

1.2 The current study

This study aimed to add a temporal perspective to the literature on risk perceptions of COVID-19, with a particular focus on the effect of season. The examination of concerns provides insights into a core variable in the process of health behavior in the context of infectious diseases (5, 12). The study draws on data from Germany that span several years and cover various aspects of how individuals deal with COVID-19, reflecting important determinants of health-related conditions as assessed in epidemiological studies. This allows a comprehensive view of the pandemic and thus goes beyond the consideration of individual key events (e.g., the introduction of vaccinations). The deeper understanding of the timely course and correlates of concerns helps to determine which individuals show concerns at which times and are therefore more likely to adapt their behavior. These findings offer starting points for risk communication in the light of future health crises and can also aid clinical decision-making by understanding prospective patients’ ways of thinking. Addressing the research gaps outlined above, the study was guided by these research questions:

RQ1: How do concerns about COVID-19 vary with different seasons?

RQ2: How do concerns about COVID-19 in different seasons relate to (a) individual characteristics, (b) related behavior, and (c) related perceptions?

2 Methods

2.1 Procedure and participants

The current study used data from the BfR-Corona-Monitor (32).1 This multi-wave, cross-sectional telephone survey of the general population in Germany aged 14 and over was conducted between 2020 and 2023 in cooperation with a professional service provider for market and social research. The participants answered the questions as part of an omnibus survey (i.e., together with other topics that changed per wave). This procedure reduces the effect of self-selection, as participants cannot make their participation dependent on their interest in a particular topic.

The data used here comprised a total of 63 of 73 waves of the survey, which were conducted at least monthly between June 2020 and April 2023, each on two consecutive days. Sample sizes ranged between 978 and 1,037 respondents per wave. Data included a total of N = 63,471 participants. Based on sociodemographic characteristics, the sample resembled the German population (see Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participants’ overall sociodemographic characteristics.

To compare seasons, participants of each wave were pooled based on the month data collection took place in: summer (June, July, August), fall (September, October, November), winter (December, January, February), spring (March, April, May); this was done for each year, respectively, resulting in 12 segments (see Supplementary material).

2.2 Measurements

2.2.1 COVID-19 concerns

The measurement of COVID-19 concerns included four items beginning with the question “To what extent are you personally concerned or not concerned about the impact of the novel coronavirus in the following areas of life?” Reflecting central, overarching domains, these were: economic situation, social relationships, physical as well as mental health. Items were measured on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = “not concerned at all” to 5 = “very concerned.” The mean index showed satisfying reliability across waves (αRange = 0.62–0.74).

2.2.2 Individual characteristics

The measurement of individual characteristics comprised five socio-demographics. Gender was measured dichotomously (1 = male, 2 = female) and age continuously in years. The measurement of education was based on five increasing levels from 1 = “pupil” to 5 = “academic degree (university, academy, polytechnic).” The dichotomous measurement of occupation compared participants in terms of their employment status (1 = non-full-time, 2 = full-time) and with household size, the number of people living in each participant’s home was assessed.

2.2.3 COVID-19 behavior

COVID-19 behavior was measured retrospectively using 11 items following the multiple-selection question “Which of the following measures have you taken within the past 2 weeks to protect yourself or others from the novel coronavirus?” Based on theoretical argument, items were pooled across waves into the three constructs hygiene (4 items; e.g., “washed hands more thoroughly”), isolation (3 items; e.g., “met friends or family less frequently”), and provision (4 items; e.g., “built up larger stocks”). Reliability scores were below common thresholds; however, this was not deemed problematic given the formative measurement model. The inclusion began in the segment for summer 2021.

2.2.4 COVID-19 perceptions

The measurement of COVID-19 perceptions included seven constructs. Controllability of risk measured participants’ level of certainty regarding protection from infection (1 item; 5-point scale from 1 = “not sure at all” to 5 = “very sure”). Participants evaluated the appropriateness of two COVID-19 measures, the mask mandate and the cancelation of events (each 1 item, 2-point scale from 1 = “not appropriate” to 2 = “appropriate”). Participants’ perceived level of information was measured by asking them how well they feel informed about central COVID-19 topics (5-point scale from 1 = “not well informed at all” to 5 = “very well informed”); after pooling corresponding items, the two indices virus (3 items; e.g., “infection routes of the coronavirus”) and societal implications (4 items; e.g., “measures currently in effect”) were built. Finally, participants estimated the probability of infection for different locations on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = “very low” to 5 = “very high”; corresponding items were pooled within the two indices private context (2 items; e.g., “at home”) and public context (5 items; e.g., “workplace”). The inclusion of measurements regarding level of information and probability of infection began in the segment for summer 2022.

Table 2 gives an overview of descriptive statistics for the measurements related to COVID-19.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and internal consistency of measured variables.

2.3 Data analysis

In line with Haddad et al. (35) and similar to Betsch et al. (36), this study used unweighted data. As weighting would be based on both variables included as predictors in regression models (e.g., gender) and variables not considered in them (e.g., federal state), it could not be guaranteed that the relationship between the measured weighting variables and the outcome variable of concerns would be correctly specified, thus potentially introducing additional bias in analyses (3739). However, respecifying models using weighted data led to similar results.

Analyses regarding RQ1 were based on inspecting means and standard deviations in the 12 different season segments as well as a series of one-way between-subjects ANOVAs to compare the effect of seasons on types of concerns. Considering inequality of variances, Games-Howell post-hoc tests were computed for individual comparisons.

In terms of RQ2 regarding predictors and changes over time, a series of linear multivariate regression analyses predicting the mean index of concerns was performed. All predictors at hand were simultaneously included in the analysis of each of the 12 season segments. Relevant statistical requirements (e.g., multicollinearity) for the analyses were checked in advance. A-priori power analyses using G*Power (version 3.1) (40) for small effects (f2 = 0.02) at α = 0.05 and 1–β = 0.99 indicated required sample sizes of at least 1,526 (8 predictors, summer 2020 to spring 2021), 1,675 (11 predictors, summer 2021 to winter 2021), and 1,842 (15 predictors, spring 2022 to spring 2023). Given pooled sample sizes ranging from 1,844 to 6,789 participants per season segment, analyses were sufficiently powered.

Data preparation was done using SPSS (version 26) (41). Analyses were performed in R (version 4.3.2) (42).

3 Results

3.1 Changes in COVID-19 concerns over time (RQ1)

RQ1 investigated the time-dependent course of concerns about the impact of COVID-19 on different areas of participants’ lives and whether there were effects of season. Figure 1 gives an impression of the distribution of the different types of concerns between 2020 and 2023. At first, results showed that respondents’ concerns were in the lower half of the 5-point scale over the entire investigated period. The highest value was found for social concerns in winter 2020 (M = 2.85, SD = 1.33), while the lowest was measured for mental concerns in spring 2023 (M = 1.99, SD = 1.23). Over the entire period, social concerns were the most pronounced (M = 2.51, SD = 1.30), followed by physical health concerns (M = 2.36, SD = 1.27), with mental (M = 2.17, SD = 1.24) and economic (M = 2.16, SD = 1.26) concerns showing lower but similar values. The overall variability of values was similar over time (SD = 1.18–1.35).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Means of concerns by season in connection to vaccination rate and confirmed cases. nSeasons = 1,946–7,095. Vaccination rate in Germany (number of people vaccinated at least once in millions) based on https://impfdashboard.de/. Confirmed cases in Germany (number of laboratory-confirmed COVID-19 cases per 100,000 inhabitants per week) based on https://infektionsradar.gesund.bund.de/de/covid/inzidenz/. Values of vaccination rate and confirmed cases per season based on the last day of each segment’s survey data collection, respectively.

Descriptive data also suggest differences depending on season. With regard to physical health concerns, average values in spring (M = 2.22–2.42) and summer (M = 2.26–2.37) were lower than those in fall (M = 2.25–2.45) and winter (M = 2.19–2.54). Mean values differed significantly by season, F(3, 34,951) = 24.25, p < 0.001, ω2 = 0.002. Post-hoc comparisons indicated that these concerns differed by each season (ps < 0.05). However, only the comparison of spring and winter showed no difference (p = 0.883).

A slightly different picture emerged for mental health concerns. There were different ranges of mean values for winter (M = 2.02–2.33) and spring (M = 1.99–2.31) compared to summer (M = 2.02–2.17) and fall (M = 2.01–2.19). An ANOVA again showed a significant effect of season, F(3, 34,895) = 34.79, p < 0.001, ω2 = 0.003, with post-hoc comparisons indicating differences between each season (ps < 0.05) with one exception; there was no difference between spring and fall (p = 0.112).

In the case of economic concerns, values were again more comparable in spring (M = 2.07–2.21) and summer (M = 2.05–2.29) as opposed to fall (M = 2.14–2.22) and winter (M = 2.13–2.17). Season also had a significant influence on being concerned about economic consequences, F(3, 34,846) = 5.29, p = 0.001, ω2 = 0.000. Post-hoc comparisons showed differences in each case between spring, summer, and fall paired with winter (ps < 0.05).

In terms of concerns about the development of social relationships, values for fall (M = 2.17–2.62), winter (M = 2.10–2.85), and spring (M = 2.05–2.80) were similar, while the range of means for summer (M = 2.37–2.52) was comparatively smaller. There was also an influence of the season here, F(3, 34,771) = 57.99, p < 0.001, ω2 = 0.005, with post-hoc comparisons indicating significant differences between each time segment (ps < 0.05).

Finally, results showed season to affect the mean index of concerns, F(3, 35,085) = 41.87, p < 0.001, ω2 = 0.003.

3.2 Predictors of COVID-19 concerns over time (RQ2)

RQ2 aimed to examine possible factors associated with COVID-19 concerns depending on season. First inspecting correlations in the whole sample revealed all investigated variables to be significantly correlated with concerns (see Supplementary material).

As can be seen in Table 3 on results of multivariate regressions, significant predictors remained comparatively stable over seasons. In response to RQ2a on individual characteristics, females were more likely to report concerns about the effects of COVID-19 in 10 of 12 time segments (β = 0.06–0.12, ps < 0.001). While age was a significant negative predictor in winter 2020 as well as spring, summer, and winter 2021 (β = −0.04–0.10, ps < 0.01), the effect diminished with the inclusion of further perceptions into analyses. Conversely, lower education was significantly associated with higher levels of concerns consistently over all time segments (β = −0.07–0.13, ps < 0.001). With exceptions of smaller effects in summer and winter of 2022 (β = 0.05, ps < 0.05), being full-time worker did not significantly predict concerns. Regarding the influence of household size, there was no obvious pattern. Having fewer people in the household was associated with stating higher concerns in summer and fall 2020, spring and summer 2021, as well as spring 2022 (β = −0.03–0.05, ps < 0.05), but there were no effects in the seven remaining time segments.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of regression analyses predicting COVID-19 concerns by season.

Investigating the role of behavior for concerns revealed a clearer picture (RQ2b). Participants taking measures regarding hygiene such as using covers for mouth and nose were more likely to be concerned about COVID-19 in seven of eight covered time segments (β = 0.03–0.08, ps < 0.05). Similarly, isolation behavior (e.g., leaving home less frequently) was consistently significantly related to higher levels of concerns (β = 0.08–0.21, ps < 0.01). Increasing provisional measures (e.g., having food delivered more frequently) positively predicted being concerned until spring 2022 (β = 0.04, ps < 0.01), but the effect was no longer statistically significant in the four time segments after that.

In terms of RQ2c, several COVID-19-related perceptions predicted concerns. Perceived controllability of risk of infection was significantly related to lower concerns in nine of 12 time segments (β = −0.04–0.14, ps < 0.05), becoming less important from fall 2022. Respondents evaluating the mandatory use of masks as appropriate were more likely to state both higher and lower levels of concerns, depending on the time frame. There were negative associations in the beginning of the pandemic in summer and fall 2020 (β = −0.03, ps < 0.05) and positive associations in eight of 10 remaining time segments after that (β = 0.03–0.09, ps < 0.05). Alternatively, reported appropriateness of the cancelation of events did not significantly predict concerns with the exception of spring 2021 (β = 0.05, p < 0.001). Those participants feeling more informed about the virus (e.g., symptoms) were less likely to wbe concerned about COVID-19 in four of five segments (β = −0.06–0.09, ps < 0.05), while feeling being informed about societal implications (e.g., current infection numbers) was only significantly related to lower concerns in spring and winter 2022 (β = −0.06–0.07, ps < 0.01). Finally, perceived probability of infection in both private (β = 0.09–0.14, ps < 0.001) and public settings (β = 0.05–0.15, ps < 0.05) positively predicted levels of concerns about COVID-19 in all of the five investigated time segments.

4 Discussion

This study extends the extant literature on infectious disease risk perceptions by examining the time-dependent trajectory of concerns about the impact of COVID-19, with a particular focus on the previously understudied effect of season. The results suggest that the development of concerns is subject to an interplay of individual factors, behavioral patterns, and beliefs based on the enduring influence of key variables.

The results on overall patterns and seasonal changes in concerns (RQ1) indicated that the concerns in different domains of people’s lives were similar over the period under consideration, but with some variations. Social concerns took on a special position in comparison, which underlines the influence of the pandemic on personal relationships, as already demonstrated in other studies (4, 43). Although the differences are within the range of less than one scale point, they are meaningful in view of the sample size and the observation over a period of 3 years.

Exploring the effect of seasons, there was a higher between-seasons than within-seasons variability in concerns. Physical health concerns showing significant differences by season including higher values in fall and winter may reflect seasonal specifics in health-related behaviors (e.g., physical activity) (44) or the confluence of the flu season (45). A seasonal pattern could also be identified for mental concerns, in which the values for winter and spring were more similar than those for summer and fall. In addition to the intensified reporting of COVID-19 in the colder seasons with a particular focus on increasing infection numbers and the uncertainty associated with this (46), the compounding effect of cold weather and the influence of reduced daylight could also be explanations. For economic concerns, the pattern was similar to that of physical health concerns, with a significant effect of season. Changing perceptions of the economic stability of the country may play a role here, as well as corresponding differences in lifestyle costs (e.g., vacation in summer). Moreover, there was an effect of season on social concerns, with the pooled mean values for summer exhibiting a different pattern than those for the other seasons. The results suggest that seasonally different arrangements of social activities and the perceived intensity of interpersonal relationships may have an impact on assessments of the pandemic. Further explanations may also lie in the effect of lockdowns (17, 37) or some preventive measures being modified quickly due to ineffectiveness (e.g., closure of certain shops), as well as in the fact that seasons may differ between different years.

With regard to RQ2 on the predictors of concerns over time, the results point less to an effect of season than to consistent influencing factors. With regard to the role of individual characteristics (RQ2a), there was first a gender disparity in reported concerns about COVID-19, with females consistently expressing higher levels. This finding aligns with research on gender-based differences in health-related anxieties (47), but raises the question for future works as to which underlying factors should be investigated for a more nuanced explanation. In addition, lower education was found to be a robust predictor of concerns over the entire period. Individuals with lower education tend to profit less from health-related knowledge, social inclusion, and financial stability (48), which may result in the development of concerns. With varied patterns regarding age, occupation, and household size, vulnerabilities, work-related stressors, and the perception of more household members having the chance of spreading the disease were less important when controlled for other factors.

Investigating the role of behavioral patterns for levels of concerns (RQ2b), taking hygiene measures positively affecting concerns may reflect an increased sense of vulnerability (49). Similarly, individuals who reported having taken isolation-related measures also showed higher levels of concerns. This underlines the possible psychological burden of reduced social interactions, which can translate into further thoughts about problems in other areas of life (1, 2). Taking provisional measures was positively related to concerns as well, but the effect diminished in summer 2022 as pandemic-related disturbances in supply chains may have been publicly discussed to a lesser extent. Importantly, potential circular relationships between perceptions and behavioral patterns should be taken into account. Concern and other dimensions of risk perception (e.g., persistent thinking about an issue) (50) may also affect individuals’ ability to adapt to circumstances, influencing actions. Suggesting resilience as a potential explanatory mechanism, Morales-Vives et al. (51) found more resilient participants to be more likely to better adapt to pandemic measures. Further, season may impact this resistance to circumstances (e.g., in summer with different resting times).

Echoing the literature on perceptions of control and risk (52), perceived controllability of the risk of infection emerged as a key predictor of higher levels of concerns within the block of perceptions (RQ2c). Moreover, feeling more informed about both the virus and its toll on society positively predicting concerns emphasizes the role of believing to be accurately informed in alleviating risk perceptions (22, 53). Positive relationships of perceived probability of infection in both private and public contexts and concerns may be explained by the significance of perceived threat in shaping individual apprehensions (54). Acknowledging the ongoing discussion about the interpretation of effect sizes (55), it should be noted that these were comparatively small, calling for caution in deriving practical relevance from them.

Nonetheless, these results have implications for future risk and crisis communication. Communication should be tailored to seasonal patterns to take into account variations in concerns. The specific impact on social concerns should be acknowledged and addressing these can help to raise awareness of social networks in a prolonged crisis. Understanding disparities in concerns depending on gender and education can guide the design of specific formats. Furthermore, the results suggest a greater focus on the link between the adoption of measures and the development of concerns; individuals who engage in desirable behaviors may be more psychologically burdened even though their actions are beneficial. Finally, communicators should recognize that people’s perceptions adapt to changing circumstances, which makes it necessary to monitor the potential influence of public discussions.

5 Limitations and conclusion

Limitations of this study should be acknowledged. First, it is important to emphasize that the data basis for this study were serial cross-sectional data, which do not allow causal conclusions to be drawn. Although the results indicate a time course, they do not reveal changes in perceptions and behaviors in individual subjects, making it difficult to compare different years and seasons within years (including different pandemic management strategies). These variations should be confirmed in future, timely longitudinal studies.

Second, the measurements used were based on self-reports. It should be noted that reports of concerns may be subject to social desirability and, in particular, reports of past behavior may be biased by recall issues. Experience sampling may provide a solution for future studies in this context. Moreover, future research could expand by considering the everyday relevance of an issue, facilitating a more nuanced understanding beyond concern as only one dimension of risk perception (50).

Finally, the results refer to Germany, a central European country with distinct differences in seasons. This western population in the northern hemisphere may differ from others culturally and participants in other countries may perceive seasonal variations differently, calling for studies on this topic in other latitudes and caution in using the results for international communication strategies.

In conclusion, the results provide evidence for the dynamic nature of concerns in different areas of life during a pandemic and the interplay of individual, behavioral, and cognitive factors in their development. While there is considerable research on the influence of seasonality on infection rates, it also appears to have an impact on perceptions. Although future public health crises might have other causes such as different pathogens, this understanding can be a starting point to find adequate responses to emerging concerns in order to mitigate effects on people’s lives.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

Ethical approval was not required for the studies involving humans because no personally identifiable information, including sensitive or confidential data, was collected. Studies did not deal with medical aspects or involve an experimental manipulation. The service provider conducting the field work fulfills the requirements of ISO 20252:2012 for market, opinion and social research, adheres to the ICC/ESOMAR ethics code for social research and data analytics, and operates by the standards established by the Working Group of German Market and Social Research Institutes (ADM). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required from the participants or the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin in accordance with the national legislation and institutional requirements because they stated their consent to participate in the surveys and could opt out or skip questions at any time. Data were collected and processed in anonymized form.

Author contributions

RJ: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. DB: Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. G-FB: Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Grace Fox for her help in searching relevant literature for this article. The authors also thank Kantar GmbH for their support with data collection and the participants of the BfR-Corona-Monitor for their time and participation.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2024.1397283/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1. ^These data have been the basis for analyses in previous publications, (see 33, 34).

References

1. O’Connor, DB, Wilding, S, Ferguson, E, Cleare, S, Wetherall, K, McClelland, H, et al. Effects of COVID-19-related worry and rumination on mental health and loneliness during the pandemic: longitudinal analyses of adults in the UK COVID-19 mental health & wellbeing study. J Ment Health. (2023) 32:1122–33. doi: 10.1080/09638237.2022.2069716

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Varga, TV, Bu, F, Dissing, AS, Elsenburg, LK, Bustamante, JJH, Matta, J, et al. Loneliness, worries, anxiety, and precautionary behaviours in response to the COVID-19 pandemic: a longitudinal analysis of 200,000 Western and northern Europeans. Lancet Reg Health Eur. (2021) 2:100020. doi: 10.1016/j.lanepe.2020.100020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Stojanovic, J, Raparelli, V, Boccia, S, Bacon, SL, and Lavoie, KL. For the iCARE study team. Public perceptions and behavioural responses to the first COVID-19 pandemic wave in Italy: results from the iCARE study. Eur J Pub Health. (2021) 31:1270–7. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckab147

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Khaira, F, and Sari, L. Perceived severity of COVID-19, social support, and pandemic-related stress associated with lifestyle changes among undergraduate students in Indonesia. J Public Health Res. (2022) 11:227990362211033. doi: 10.1177/22799036221103382

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Sprengholz, P, Bruckmann, R, Wiedermann, M, Brockmann, D, and Betsch, C. From delta to omicron: the role of individual factors and social context in self-reported compliance with pandemic regulations and recommendations. Soc Sci Med. (2023) 317:115633. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.115633

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Barber, SJ, and Kim, H. COVID-19 worries and behavior changes in older and younger men and women. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2021) 76:e17–23. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbaa068

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Rattay, P, Michalski, N, Domanska, OM, Kaltwasser, A, De Bock, F, Wieler, LH, et al. Differences in risk perception, knowledge and protective behaviour regarding COVID-19 by education level among women and men in Germany. Results from the COVID-19 snapshot monitoring (COSMO) study. PLoS One. (2021) 16:e0251694. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0251694

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Wilson, RS, Zwickle, A, and Walpole, H. Developing a broadly applicable measure of risk perception. Risk Anal. (2019) 39:777–91. doi: 10.1111/risa.13207

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Slovic, P, Finucane, ML, Peters, E, and MacGregor, DG. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Anal. (2004) 24:311–22. doi: 10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00433.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Siegrist, M, and Árvai, J. Risk perception: reflections on 40 years of research. Risk Anal. (2020) 40:2191–206. doi: 10.1111/risa.13599

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Cornia, A, Dressel, K, and Pfeil, P. Risk cultures and dominant approaches towards disasters in seven European countries. J Risk Res. (2016) 19:288–304. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2014.961520

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Dryhurst, S, Schneider, CR, Kerr, J, Freeman, ALJ, Recchia, G, van der Bles, AM, et al. Risk perceptions of COVID-19 around the world In: JK Wardman and R Löfstedt, editors. COVID-19: Confronting a new world risk. Oxfordshire, England, UK: Routledge (2022). 162–74.

Google Scholar

13. Benjamin, A, Kuperman, Y, Eren, N, Rotkopf, R, Amitai, M, Rossman, H, et al. Stress-related emotional and behavioural impact following the first COVID-19 outbreak peak. Mol Psychiatry. (2021) 26:6149–58. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01219-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Pedersen, MT, Andersen TOClotworthy, A, Jensen, AK, Strandberg-Larsen, K, Rod, NH, et al. Time trends in mental health indicators during the initial 16 months of the COVID-19 pandemic in Denmark. BMC Psychiatry. (2022) 22:25. doi: 10.1186/s12888-021-03655-8,

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Köverová, M, Ráczová, B, and Kováčová, HB. Predictors of anxiety, stress, and concern of COVID-19 infection in older adults during the first and the second waves of the COVID-19 pandemic in Slovakia. Gerontol Geriatr Med. (2021) 7:233372142110476. doi: 10.1177/23337214211047642

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. de Vries, M, Claassen, L, te Wierik, MJM, van den Hof, S, Brabers, AEM, de Jong, JD, et al. Dynamic public perceptions of the coronavirus disease crisis, the Netherlands, 2020. Emerg Infect Dis. (2021) 27:1098–109. doi: 10.3201/eid2704.203328

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Schou-Bredal, I, Skogstad, L, Grimholt, TK, Bonsaksen, T, Ekeberg, Ø, and Heir, T. Concerns in the Norwegian population during the initial lockdown due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:6119. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18116119

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Matthes, J, Corbu, N, Jin, S, Theocharis, Y, Schemer, C, van Aelst, P, et al. Perceived prevalence of misinformation fuels worries about COVID-19: a cross-country, multi-method investigation. Inf Commun Soc. (2023) 26:3133–56. doi: 10.1080/1369118x.2022.2146983

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Solymosi, R, Jackson, J, Pósch, K, Yesberg, JA, Bradford, B, and Kyprianides, A. Functional and dysfunctional fear of COVID-19: a classification scheme. Crime Sci. (2021) 10:4. doi: 10.1186/s40163-020-00137-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Meltzer, GY, Chang, VW, Lieff, SA, Grivel, MM, Yang, LH, and Des Jarlais, DC. Behavioral correlates of COVID-19 worry: stigma, knowledge, and news source. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:11436. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182111436

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Schulz, PJ, Andersson, EM, Bizzotto, N, and Norberg, M. Using ecological momentary assessment to study the development of COVID-19 worries in Sweden: longitudinal study. J Med Internet Res. (2021) 23:e26743. doi: 10.2196/26743

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Wilson, JM, Lee, J, and Shook, NJ. COVID-19 worries and mental health: the moderating effect of age. Aging Ment Health. (2021) 25:1289–96. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2020.1856778

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Liu, S, Huang, JC, and Brown, GL. Information and risk perception: a dynamic adjustment process. Risk Anal. (1998) 18:689–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.1998.tb01113.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Rogers, GO. The dynamics of risk perception: how does perceived risk respond to risk events? Risk Anal. (1997) 17:745–57. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.1997.tb01280.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Audi, A, AlIbrahim, M, Kaddoura, M, Hijazi, G, Yassine, HM, and Zaraket, H. Seasonality of respiratory viral infections: will COVID-19 follow suit? Front Public Health. (2020) 8. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.567184

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Carlson, CJ, Gomez, ACR, Bansal, S, and Ryan, SJ. Misconceptions about weather and seasonality must not misguide COVID-19 response. Nat Commun. (2020) 11:4312. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-18150-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Liu, X, Huang, J, Li, C, Zhao, Y, Wang, D, Huang, Z, et al. The role of seasonality in the spread of COVID-19 pandemic. Environ Res. (2021) 195:110874. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2021.110874

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Sajadi, MM, Habibzadeh, P, Vintzileos, A, Shokouhi, S, Miralles-Wilhelm, F, and Amoroso, A. Temperature, humidity, and latitude analysis to estimate potential spread and seasonality of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19). JAMA Netw Open. (2020) 3:e2011834. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.11834

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Böl, GF. The COVID-19 pandemic: agile versus blundering communication during a worldwide crisis: important lessons for efficient communication to maintain public trust and ensure public safety. EMBO Rep. (2021) 22:e53182. doi: 10.15252/embr.202153182

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Warren, GW, Lofstedt, R, and Wardman, JK. COVID-19: the winter lockdown strategy in five European nations. J Risk Res. (2021) 24:267–93. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2021.1891802

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Azuma, K, Yanagi, U, Kagi, N, Kim, H, Ogata, M, and Hayashi, M. Environmental factors involved in SARS-CoV-2 transmission: effect and role of indoor environmental quality in the strategy for COVID-19 infection control. Environ Health Prev Med. (2020) 25:66. doi: 10.1186/s12199-020-00904-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. German Federal Institute for Risk Assessment (BfR). BfR-Corona-Monitor 04|2023 (2023). Available at: https://www.bfr.bund.de/cm/349/230404-bfr-corona-monitor-en.pdf Accessed March 4, 2024]

Google Scholar

33. Kirsch, F, Lindemann, A-K, Lohmann, M, and Böl, G-F. Serial cross-sectional data on the public’s perception on the coronavirus during the first months of the pandemic in Germany. Data Brief. (2021) 38:107430. doi: 10.1016/j.dib.2021.107430

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Kirsch, F, Lindemann, A-K, Geppert, J, Borzekowski, D, Lohmann, M, and Böl, G-F. Personal protective measures during the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany. Int J Infect Dis. (2022) 121:177–83. doi: 10.1016/j.ijid.2022.05.036

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Haddad, C, Sacre, H, Zeenny, RM, Hajj, A, Akel, M, Iskandar, K, et al. Should samples be weighted to decrease selection bias in online surveys during the COVID-19 pandemic? Data from seven datasets. BMC Med Res Methodol. (2022) 22:63. doi: 10.1186/s12874-022-01547-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Betsch, C, Korn, L, Burgard, T, Gaissmaier, W, Felgendreff, L, Eitze, S, et al. The four weeks before lockdown during the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany: a weekly serial cross-sectional survey on risk perceptions, knowledge, public trust and behaviour, 3 to 25 march 2020. Euro Surveill. (2021) 26:2001900. doi: 10.2807/1560-7917.es.2021.26.42.2001900

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Gelman, A. Struggles with survey weighting and regression modeling. Stat Sci. (2007) 22:153–64. doi: 10.1214/088342306000000691

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Solon, G, Haider, SJ, and Wooldridge, JM. What are we weighting for? J Hum Resour. (2015) 50:301–16. doi: 10.3368/jhr.50.2.301

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Winship, C, and Radbill, L. Sampling weights and regression analysis. Sociol Methods Res. (1994) 23:230–57. doi: 10.1177/0049124194023002004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Faul, F, Erdfelder, E, Buchner, A, and Lang, AG. Statistical power analyses using G*power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. (2009) 41:1149–60. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS statistics for windows (version 26.0). Armonk, NY, USA: IBM Corp (2019).

Google Scholar

42. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing (version 4.3.2). Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2023).

Google Scholar

43. Bechard, LE, Bergelt, M, Neudorf, B, DeSouza, TC, and Middleton, LE. Using the health belief model to understand age differences in perceptions and responses to the COVID-19 pandemic. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.609893

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Reilly, T, and Peiser, B. Seasonal variations in health-related human physical activity. Sports Med. (2006) 36:473–85. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200636060-00002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Jones, N. How COVID-19 is changing the cold and flu season. Nature. (2020) 588:388–90. doi: 10.1038/d41586-020-03519-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Su, Z, McDonnell, D, Wen, J, Kozak, M, Abbas, J, Šegalo, S, et al. Mental health consequences of COVID-19 media coverage: the need for effective crisis communication practices. Glob Health. (2021) 17:4. doi: 10.1186/s12992-020-00654-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. MacSwain, KLH, Sherry, SB, Stewart, SH, Watt, MC, Hadjistavropoulos, HD, and Graham, AR. Gender differences in health anxiety: an investigation of the interpersonal model of health anxiety. Personal Individ Differ. (2009) 47:938–43. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.07.020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. van der Heide, I, Wang, J, Droomers, M, Spreeuwenberg, P, Rademakers, J, and Uiters, E. The relationship between health, education, and health literacy: results from the Dutch adult literacy and life skills survey. J Health Commun. (2013) 18:172–84. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2013.825668

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Blix, I, Birkeland, MS, and Thoresen, S. Worry and mental health in the Covid-19 pandemic: vulnerability factors in the general Norwegian population. BMC Public Health. (2021) 21:928. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-10927-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Wiedemann, P, Freudenstein, F, Böhmert, C, Wiart, J, and Croft, R. RF EMF risk perception revisited: is the focus on concern sufficient for risk perception studies? Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2017) 14:620. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14060620

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Morales-Vives, F, Dueñas, JM, Vigil-Colet, A, and Camarero-Figuerola, M. Psychological variables related to adaptation to the COVID-19 lockdown in Spain. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.565634

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Slovic, P, and Peters, E. Risk perception and affect. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2006) 15:322–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00461.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Martelletti, CP, Santirocchi, A, Spataro, P, Rossi-Arnaud, C, Löfstedt, RE, and Cestari, V. Predictors of COVID-19 risk perception, worry and anxiety in Italy at the end of the 2020 national lockdown. J Risk Res. (2022) 25:1306–20. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2022.2038245

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Ferrer, RA, and Klein, WMP. Risk perceptions and health behavior. Curr Opin Psychol. (2015) 5:85–9. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.03.012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Sauer, JD, and Drummond, A. Boundary conditions for the practical importance of small effects in long runs: a comment on funder and Ozer (2019). Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. (2020) 3:502–4. doi: 10.1177/2515245920957607

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19, concerns, behavior, perceptions, serial cross-sectional data, survey study

Citation: Janzik R, Borzekowski D and Böl G-F (2024) Investigating seasonal changes in factors associated with COVID-19 concerns: Results from a serial cross-sectional survey study in Germany between 2020 and 2023. Front. Public Health. 12:1397283. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1397283

Received: 11 March 2024; Accepted: 17 June 2024;
Published: 18 July 2024.

Edited by:

Nicolai Savaskan, District Office Neukölln of Berlin Neukölln, Germany

Reviewed by:

Rene Gottschalk, University Hospital Frankfurt, Germany
Voltaire Alvarado Peterson, University of Concepcion, Chile
Teddy Craciunescu, National Institute for Laser Plasma and Radiation Physics, Romania

Copyright © 2024 Janzik, Borzekowski and Böl. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Robin Janzik, robin.janzik@bfr.bund.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.