Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 24 July 2024
Sec. Aging and Public Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Impairment and Physical Function in Older Adults View all 25 articles

Relationship between domain-specific physical activity and cognitive function in older adults – findings from NHANES 2011–2014

Sijun WuSijun Wu1Lin Wang
Lin Wang1*Shijie LiuShijie Liu1Juancai QiJuancai Qi1Fengrui ShiFengrui Shi1Huiqi ZhuangHuiqi Zhuang1Youling QianYouling Qian2Linqi MeiLinqi Mei3Maolin ZhangMaolin Zhang4
  • 1School of Physical Education, Wuhan University of Technology, Wuhan, China
  • 2School of Physical Education, Hubei Minzu University, Enshi, China
  • 3School of Physical Education, Hubei University, Wuhan, China
  • 4School of Wushu, Shandong Sport University, Jinan, China

Objective: To determine the relationship between domain-specific physical activity (PA) (e.g., occupational PA [OPA], transport-related PA [TPA], and recreational PA [RPA]) and cognitive function in older adults.

Methods: The data was obtained from the 2011–2014 cycle of the NHANES. We utilized weighted multivariate linear regression models among the included 2,924 people aged 60 years or older for our purposes.

Results: RPA and total PA according to WHO guidelines were associated with verbal fluency (RPA β: 1.400, 95% CI: 0.776, 2.024, p = 0.002; total PA β: 1.115, 95% CI: 0.571, 1.659, p = 0.001), processing speed and executive function (RPA β: 2.912, 95% CI. 1.291, 4.534, p = 0.005; total PA β: 2.974, 95% CI: 1.683, 4.265, p < 0.001) were positively correlated, and total PA was correlated with delayed memory performance (β: 0.254, 95% CI: 0.058, 0.449, p = 0.019). No significant association was observed between OPA, TPA, and various aspects of cognitive function among individuals over 60 years.

Conclusion: There was no noteworthy correlation discovered between OPA and TPA in relation to cognitive function. However, RPA and total PA exhibited significant associations with verbal fluency, processing speed, and executive function. Additionally, maintaining PA levels ranging from 600 to 1,200 MET-min/week would yield the most favorable outcomes for cognitive function.

1 Introduction

Recent data indicates a rapid global increase in the older adult population, with an estimated 2.1 billion individuals aged 60 and above expected by the mid-21st century (1). Aging is often accompanied by a decrease in cognitive abilities, including memory, verbal expression, attention, and executive function (2). Without effective intervention, the age-related cognitive function decline can be a precursor to cognitive impairment related to dementia or Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (2). Currently, more than 55 million people are suffering from dementia, with AD being the most common form of cognitive decline in older individuals (3). In 2019, healthcare expenditures resulting from dementia reached $1.3 trillion, imposing a substantial financial burden on governments, families, and individuals (4). Due to the current lack of effective drugs for dementia worldwide, there is an urgent requirement for a cost-effective, non-pharmacological intervention that can help maintain and enhance cognitive function in older adults.

Physical activity (PA) has been shown to be beneficial in assisting in the treatment of illnesses like diabetes (5), depression (6), liver and gallbladder disease (7), cancer (8), chronic kidney disease (9). PA has also been shown to positively impact body composition by increasing muscle mass (10), reducing body fat percentage (11), and enhancing bone mineral density (12). Physical activity has become a kind of low cost benefits of effective non-pharmaceutical interventions (13, 14). It is important to note that PA covers a variety of intricate behaviors, classified into three primary domains by the Global Physical Activity Questionnaire (GPAQ): occupational PA (OPA), transport-related PA (TPA), and recreational PA (RPA) (15). OPA includes a range of tasks that necessitate completion, whether they involve remuneration or not, including academic or vocational pursuits, household chores, and tending to gardens; TPA pertains to the mode of transportation typically employed by individuals to commute to various destinations (workplace, educational institutions, shopping centers), encompassing activities like walking or cycling; and RPA is defined as a sport, fitness or leisure activity that is carried out in addition to the two physical activities listed above (16). Different domains of PA may have different effects: studies indicate that OPA and TPA are not effective in preventing or inhibiting the development of diabetes (5) and depression (6), while RPA has shown more favorable outcomes. A study has also indicated that OPA, but not TPA and RPA, is related to a lower risk of chronic kidney disease (9).

Cognitive functions encompass various aspects such as memory, attention, perception, and thinking, constituting higher brain functions (17). According to neurocognitive psychiatry textbooks, cognitive functions are categorized into 12 domains: visual–spatial abilities, attention, problem-solving, general intelligence, psychomotor speed, sensory processing, verbal memory, non-verbal memory, processing speed, motor control/performance, working memory, and verbal reasoning (18). Previous researches have mainly investigated the correlation between physical activity and cognitive function in older adults, particularly focusing on RPA and specific types of exercise (1924). For example, one study discovered that RPA exhibited a positive correlation with cognitive function in older adults with shorter sleep durations (19), and that aerobic exercise [walking (24), dancing (23)], resistance exercise (22), and mind–body exercise [tai chi (20), yoga (21)] also had beneficial effects on cognitive function in older individuals. Nonetheless, engaging in these forms of exercise is not indicative of physical activity in a specific domain and is mostly done during leisure time for health benefits. Based on this, there is currently a lack of research exploring the association between specific domains of PA (OPA, TPA, and RPA) and cognitive function in older adults, and whether these associations might be influenced by sociodemographic factors (such as gender, age, race, and education) and behavioral characteristics (like smoking and alcohol consumption).

Therefore, we propose the following hypotheses:

H1: Different domains of PA (OPA, TPA, and RPA) may exhibit varying associations with cognitive function in older adults.

H2: The associations between different domains of physical activity and cognitive function in older adults may be influenced by sociodemographic factors (gender, age, race, education) and behavioral characteristics (smoking, alcohol consumption).

2 Methods

2.1 Study population

The purpose of the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) is to evaluate the health and nutritional well-being of adults and children residing in the United States, while also ensuring that the sample represents the non-institutionalized civilian population of the country (25). Since all data collection procedures in NHANES were performed in compliance with the research ethics regulations of the National Center for Health Statistics Review Board, no additional ethical review was required for this study.

In the survey conducted from 2011 to 2014, a total of 19,331 participants were initially involved. Given that the cognitive tests of interest in this study were specifically administered to individuals aged 60 and older, participants under the age of 60 were excluded from the analysis (n = 16,299). Subsequently, from the remaining 3,632 participants, those who were missing cognitive test data (n = 698) or information on physical activity (n = 10) were further excluded. Finally, 2,924 participants were included in the current analyses, and the screening process is shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart of research sample selection.

2.2 Physical activity

The Global Physical Activity Questionnaire (GPAQ) was utilized to evaluate participants’ physical activity, which consisted of the frequency of physical activity (1–7 times per week), the duration of a single exercise session (minutes per session), and the intensity of the exercise (moderate- or vigorous-intensity) in a typical week for three different PA domains (OPA, TPA, and TPA) (26). PA in each domain is measured as the corresponding metabolic equivalent (MET) multiplied by the number of minutes of activity per week (MET-min/week). According to the MET score recommended by NHANES, moderate OPA, TPA, and RPA, which result in a slight increase in respiration or heart rate, were classified as 4 MET, whereas vigorous OPA and TPA, which lead to a significant increase in respiration or heart rate, were identified as 8 MET (16). In addition, the activity levels of OPA, TPA and RPA were pooled to obtain total PA.

As the WHO Physical Activity and Sedentary Behavior Guidelines (hereafter referred to as the guidelines) propose that individuals aged 60 and above should engage in a minimum of 150–300 min per week of moderate-intensity aerobic activity (equivalent to 600–1,200 MET-min/week) for considerable health benefits. Further advantages can be attained by exceeding 300 min per week (>1,200 MET-min/week) of moderate-intensity aerobic activity (27). Therefore, we first categorized the amount of physical activity in each domain into two groups, meeting guideline recommendations and not meeting guideline recommendations, to assess the effect on cognitive function of those meeting guideline recommendations compared to those not meeting guideline recommendations. The amount of physical activity in each domain was then further categorized into 4 groups (1) none (0 MET-min/week), (2) low (<600 MET-min/week), (3) moderate (600–1,200 MET-min/week), and (4) high (>1,200 MET-min/week) to assess the relationship between different doses of domain-specific PA and cognition.

2.3 Cognitive function

Trained interviewers assisted participants in a series of cognitive tests (28). The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Word Learning Subtest (CERAD W-L) consists of three immediate recall (CERAD-IR, total score of 30) and one delayed recall (CERAD-DR, total score of 10) tests to assess immediate and delayed learning of new verbal information (29). The test has been shown to be effective in distinguishing the presence of underlying cognitive impairment (30, 31). For CERAD-IR, participants were instructed to orally read aloud 10 unrelated words one by one and then immediately recall as many words as possible after their presentation, which was repeated three times with different word orders each time. Delayed word recall followed the completion of the subsequent two cognitive test items (32). The number of words recalled correctly is the score for the test. The Animal Fluency test (AF) assesses categorical verbal fluency by requiring participants to name as many different animals as possible within 1 min, with one name per animal. This score distinguishes individuals with normal cognitive function from those with mild cognitive impairment and more severe forms of cognitive decline, such as Alzheimer’s disease (33, 34). The Digit Symbol Substitution test (DSST), a component of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III), comprehensively evaluates processing speed, sustained attention, and executive function (35). Administered on paper, the test sheet includes a key with 9 numbers and symbols. Participants have 2 min to transcribe the corresponding symbols into 133 boxes next to the numbers; the score reflects the total number of correct matches (36). Higher scores across all tests indicate better cognitive function.

2.4 Covariates

Based on previous studies (19, 37), we adjusted for several confounders that may influence cognitive function and physical activity. These confounders include demographics such as sex (male, female), age (60–69 years, 70–79 years, 80+ years), race, educational attainment, and poverty income ratio (PIR). Race was further divided into Non-Hispanic White, Non-Hispanic Black, Other Hispanic, Mexican American and Other/multiracial. Educational attainment was categorized from lowest to highest as Less Than 9th Grade, 9th-11th Grade, High School Grad/GED, Some College or AA degree, and College Graduate or above. The PIR is calculated based on household income in relation to the poverty guidelines set by the government, with higher values representing better household economic conditions. Body Mass Index (BMI) is obtained by calculating the ratio of weight in kilograms to the square of height in meters. Lifestyle factors were primarily assessed by evaluating participants’ smoking and drinking habits. Smoking status was determined through questions such as ‘Have you smoked at least 100 cigarettes?,’ ‘Do you currently smoke?,’ and ‘How long ago did you quit smoking?’ Participants were divided into three groups: ‘current smokers,’ ‘former smokers,’ and ‘never smokers.’ Drinking status was classified into four groups based on the frequency of drinking: ‘non-drinkers,’ ‘1–5 drinks per month,’ ‘5–10 drinks per month,’ and ‘10+ drinks per month.’ This classification was determined using criteria such as ‘drinking at least 12 times a year,’ ‘having imbibed at least 12 alcoholic beverages throughout their lifetime,’ and the frequency of drinking in the past 12 months (weekly, monthly, yearly). Additionally, taking into account the potential impact of the illness on cognitive performance and physical activities, we identified depression, diabetes, hypertension, and cardiovascular-related diseases by combining participants’ self-reports with objective measures. Depression was assessed using the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9), which consists of 9 items worth a total of 27 points, with a total score of ≥10 considered to be depression (38). Diabetes was defined by fulfilling any of the subsequent requirements: (1) Being informed by a doctor about having diabetes, (2) Currently taking anti-diabetic drugs, or (3) Fasting blood glucose (mmol/l) ≥ 7.0. Hypertension was diagnosed as having an average systolic blood pressure ≥ 140 mmHg or diastolic blood pressure ≥ 90 mmHg, or if the subject reported taking prescription drugs for hypertension (39). Cardiovascular diseases were defined as having been reported to the subject by medical staff as congestive heart failure, coronary artery disease, angina pectoris, or heart disease. In addition, a ‘trouble remembering’ group was created based on the frequency of memory problems the subjects had experienced in the past 7 days: never, about once, two or three times, almost every day, and several times a day.

2.5 Statistical analysis

The NHANES study used a complex multi-stage probability sampling design, which requires specific weighting procedures during analysis. To obtain the weight variables needed for this study, we divided the weight of the two-cycle Mobile Examination Center interviews by 2, following the recommendations of Tutorials (32). Since deleting missing covariates directly would result in the loss of information on physical activity and cognitive function, and considering that there were only a few missing covariates (Supplementary Table 1), we used the MICE package in R software to impute the missing variables in order to avoid potential bias.

The baseline characteristics of the participants in the study were analyzed by presenting the means and standard deviations for continuous variables, as well as frequencies (%) for categorical variables. T-tests or chi-squared tests were used to determine any significant differences between the groups. Considering that the dependent variable of interest in this study is a continuous variable, the relationship between domain-specific PA and cognitive function was examined by calculating effect sizes (β) with 95% confidence intervals (CI) using weighted multivariate linear regression models. Three models were used: Model 1, which was not adjusted for any variables, and Model 2, which was adjusted for age, sex, PIR, race, BMI, educational attainment, smoking and alcohol consumption. Model 3 served as a fully adjusted model, further adjusting for depression, diabetes, hypertension, cardiovascular diseases, and difficulty with memory in the past 7 days based on model 2. In addition, the fully adjusted model was used to perform subgroup analyses, grouping people according to age, sex, race, educational attainment, smoking and drinking status, to ascertain the relationship between domain-specific PA and cognitive function in different subgroups. Finally, sensitivity analyses were performed by adding PA volumes from the other two domains to model 3, to avoid potential benefits of simultaneous PA in multiple domains. All statistical analyses were conducted using the R software (version 4.3.0) and p < 0.05 for two-tailed test indicates statistical significance.

3 Results

3.1 Descriptive characteristics

The 2,924 respondents included in this study (Table 1) represented a total of 53,109,681 US non-institutionalized civilian population, the majority of whom were non-Hispanic white (n = 1,396), with 1,504 (54.54%) females versus 1,420 (45. 46%) males, and an overall mean age of 69.20 (6.65) years, BMI 29.06 (6.29) kg/m2, PIR 3.12 (1.58), and cognitive test scores were CERAD-IR 19.72 (4.49), CERAD-DR 6.23 (2.30), AF 18.08 (5.70) and DSST 51.96 (16.81). Half of the respondents were non-smokers [1,443 (49.72%)] and consumed alcohol 1–5 times per month [1,379 (47.46%)]. Most were free of depression [2,598 (92.72%)], diabetes [2,106 (76.49%)] and cardiovascular disease [2,415 (82.40%)], except for hypertension [1,970 (63.61%)], which was more common. The percentages of people with OPA, TPA and RPA were 34, 19, and 45% respectively, and 33% were not physically active in any domain. In all domains, those who were physically active were younger than those who were not (p < 0.05), had higher AF test and DSST scores; with the exception of TPA, participants with OPA or RPA had better economic conditions and higher CERAD.IR and CERAD.DR scores compared to those without (p < 0.05).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of study population.

3.2 Relationship between domain-specific PA and cognitive function

Table 2 presents the results of the weighted multivariate linear regression model. In model 3 (fully adjusted), OPA, TPA, and RPA that met guideline recommendations were not significantly associated with immediate versus delayed recall compared to those who did not meet the guidelines. However, total PA showed a significant association with better delayed recall performance (β: 0.254, 95% CI: 0.058, 0.449, p = 0.019). For the AF test, adherence to guideline recommendations for OPA (β: 0.011, 95% CI: −0.461, 0.483, p = 0.955) and TPA (β: 0.179, 95% CI: −0.955, 1.312, p = 0. 713) did not show a significant association, whereas RPA (β: 1.400, 95% CI: 0.776, 2.024, p = 0.002) and total PA (β: 1.115, 95% CI: 0.571, 1.659, p = 0.001) showed a significant association. Similarly, in DSST, meeting guideline recommendations for OPA (β: 0.821, 95% CI: −0.341, 5.272, p = 0.365) and TPA (β: −1.575, 95% CI: −3.596, 0.445, p = 0.105) remained non-significantly associated with test performance, whereas RPA (β: 2.912, 95% CI: 1.291, 4.534, p = 0.005) and total PA (β: 2.974, 95% CI: 1.683, 4.265, p < 0.001) were significantly associated with higher test scores.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The relationship between Domain-specific PA and Cognitive function when meeting PA guidelines.

Continuing with the subgroup analyses based on the fully adjusted model (Supplementary Table 2), it can be seen that adherence to recommended PA guidelines interacts with age, gender, race, education level, smoking, and alcohol consumption in influencing the relationship between specific domain PA and cognitive function (p < 0.05). Specifically, among older adults, both RPA and total PA were significantly associated with immediate recall, delayed recall, language fluency, processing speed, and executive function, while OPA enhanced immediate and delayed recall. Total PA positively correlated with cognitive test scores among non-Hispanic White individuals. RPA was significantly associated with higher AF test and DSST scores. RPA and total PA were linked to better AF test and DSST performance in subgroups characterized by alcohol consumption and smoking, whereas immediate and delayed recall were associated with individuals who did not smoke or consume alcohol. Total PA was associated with better delayed recall among women, whereas RPA correlated positively with processing speed and executive function in men. Additionally, education level did not affect associations between PA in various domains and CERAD.DR test performance; however, certain stratifications influenced associations between RPA, TPA, and CERAD.IR, AF test, and DSST.

3.3 Dose-effect analysis

Table 3 shows the dose–response relationship between domain-specific PA and cognitive function based on the fully adjusted model, and Supplementary Table 2 contains details of model 1 and model 2. Compared to the physically inactive population, no dose of OPA, TPA or RPA was significantly associated with scores on the CERAD W-L test, and only total PA (β: 0.321, 95% CI: 0.037, 0.604, p = 0.033) was associated with better delayed recall in the high activity group. In the AF test, OPA and TPA were not significantly correlated with test scores in all dose groups, whereas a significant and positive correlation was observed between RPA and scores in the group undergoing moderate activity (β: 1.313, 95% CI: 0.280, 2.346, p = 0.024) and the high activity (β: 1.726, 95% CI: 0.792, 2.659, p = 0.007). Total PA also showed a significant positive association with this cognitive score at all doses (p < 0.05). In the DSST, neither OPA nor TPA had a significant association with test scores, regardless of the dosage. However, RPA and total PA demonstrated a positive correlation with higher scores. It is noteworthy that individuals engaging in a total PA of 600–1,200 MET-min/week show higher effect sizes (β) in the relationship between AF tests and DSST scores compared to other activity levels. This suggests a significant association between achieving 600–1,200 MET-min/week of PA and better cognitive performance. Similarly, a comparable relationship exists between RPA and DSST.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Dose–response relationships between domain-specific PA and cognitive function.

3.4 Sensitivity analysis

Sensitivity analysis revealed (Supplementary Table 4) that OPA and TPA are still not significantly related to the cognitive function of older adults when the activity levels of the other two PA domains are added to the model 3. Except that the relationship between RPA and DSST score weakened in the low PA group (β: 1.960, 95% CI: −0.299, 4.220, p = 0.070), the other associations were still significant.

4 Discussion

This study presents, for the first time, varying associations between different domains of PA (OPA, TPA, RPA) and cognitive function in older adults. Regarding language fluency (AF) and processing speed, executive function (DSST), adherence to guideline-recommended OPA and TPA did not exhibit significant associations, whereas RPA showed significant correlations with total PA, supporting hypothesis H1.

We posit that RPA may have a stronger effect than OPA and TPA in adults aged 60 and above due to a potentially passive selection of OPA and TPA by older adults (40). Previous research has suggested that this phenomenon could be influenced by the nature of physical activity and psychological factors (41). Work-related physical activities, such as household chores, for older adults may be obligatory, accompanied by greater stress and discomfort (42). Another study indicated a 2.28-fold increase in stress levels following moderate to intense occupational PA (43). Prolonged exposure to stress triggers dysregulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, leading to elevated glucocorticoid levels, which are neurotoxic to the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus, thereby affecting their normal functional expression including executive function and episodic memory (44). Furthermore, stress may impair cognitive function through behaviors such as smoking and alcohol consumption, recognized risk factors for cognitive impairment (45, 46). While these findings contrast with previous studies that found no associations between isolated RPA, OPA, TPA, and cognitive function (47), we attribute this discrepancy to differences in participant age ranges—prior studies included individuals aged 21–60, unlike our study’s focus on those aged 60 and above—and variations in cognitive assessment tools. Another reason we believe RPA yields superior effects over OPA and TPA is its social engagement aspect as a form of exercise, fitness, or recreational leisure activity (48). Research suggests that older adults with stronger social ties are less susceptible to cognitive decline (49). Studies such as NIE’s longitudinal investigation into the relationship between older adults’ social networks and cognitive function found a positive correlation between cognitive function and engagement in social activities and the size of one’s social circle; social gatherings and interactions provide emotional support (49). MOGIC’s systematic review, employing cross-sectional and cohort analyses, indicated that older adults receiving greater emotional support demonstrate better cognitive abilities. This viewpoint aptly explains why guideline-recommended OPA and TPA did not show significant associations (50). Furthermore, it’s notable that while RPA correlates with better language fluency, processing speed, and executive function, it shows no association with memory performance. This disparity may stem from different exercises selectively modulating specific brain regions to enhance diverse cognitive functions (51). For instance, resistance training has been shown to induce functional changes in the frontal lobe, thereby enhancing executive function (52). Conversely, studies have observed reduced gray matter in the hippocampus (linked to memory function) even after a year of stretching exercises (53).

Consistent with previous studies, neither TPA nor OPA showed significant associations with cognitive functions (54). Lund et al.’s prospective cohort study highlighted that physical exposure in occupational settings correlates with long-term sick leave, thereby compromising physical health to some extent (55). Additionally, it has been suggested that traffic-related air pollution may alter neurobehavioral functions (56). Considering environmental factors, studies have indicated that built environment and community characteristics influence TPA (57). Therefore, future research should explore how addressing neighborhood and architectural differences could promote PA adoption and alleviate disparities in cognitive function among older adults. Regarding TPA, while studies have suggested that activities like walking and biking effectively maintain muscle function and reduce disease risks (58, 59), our interpretation from this study’s findings is that cognitive functions involve higher-order brain activities, and independent TPA alone may not sufficiently impact cognitive function in older adults. Further research is needed to understand the varying associations between age-related TPA intensity and specific cognitive domains.

We also found that engaging in PA, regardless of whether it meets recommended guidelines, correlates with better AF test and DSST performance in older adults. This aligns with existing views that regular PA can improve overall cognitive function in older adults (6062). This phenomenon may stem from the additional benefits of combining different types of exercises. Early meta-analyses suggest that combining aerobic and resistance training maximizes cognitive improvements in older adults (63), and multifaceted physical exercise has positive effects on overall cognition in MCI or dementia patients, particularly aerobic exercises (61). Notably, besides significantly correlating with AF test and DSST scores across all dosage groups, total PA is also associated with better delayed recall (CERAD-DR) in the high-activity group. Potential physiological mechanisms include frontal lobe gray matter volume reduction leading to decreased executive function (64), and hippocampal gray matter volume reduction closely tied to memory decline (65). Adequate physical activity has been shown to counteract or delay brain tissue atrophy due to aging and improve memory (22, 53, 66).

Furthermore, another finding of this study supports hypothesis H2. In subgroup analyses, we observed that age, race, gender, education level, smoking, and alcohol consumption can influence the relationship between different types of PA and cognitive function in older adults. Among demographic factors, older age groups showed significant correlations between regular physical activity (RPA) and total PA with immediate and delayed recall, language fluency, processing speed, and executive function, which is consistent with the findings of most previous studies (67, 68). A meta-analysis in China demonstrated that with every 5-year increase in age among adults aged 60 and above, the prevalence of mild cognitive impairment rises by 1.27–1.45 times, associated with degeneration in temporal and frontal brain structures due to accumulated DNA damage during brain aging, significantly impairing brain functions (69). Adequate RPA may therefore help to mitigate cognitive declines in older adults (70). Moreover, we found significant gender differences favoring males in delayed recall, language fluency, processing speed, and executive function. Although females experience faster cognitive decline than males due to changing estrogen levels during menopause, gender differences in various dimensions of cognitive function may be associated with higher levels of physical activity in older men, while postmenopausal type II muscle fiber loss leads to weakened muscle contraction in women, explaining why a higher proportion of older women transition to low physical activity compared to older men (71). Similarly, we found that education also influenced the associations between RPA and total PA with immediate recall, verbal fluency, processing speed, and executive function. A previous study reasonably interpreted the reason why higher education levels are likely to reduce the possibility of better cognitive function associated with physical activity and older adults with higher education levels are more likely to understand health care and brain training (47). Actively stimulating older adult learning can make up for the brain damage caused by aging, improve brain utilization efficiency, and maintain better cognitive function. It is worth noting that non-Spanish-speaking white people have better language fluency, processing speed, and executive function with RPA and total PA. In behavioral characteristics, the RPA and total PA of older adults who smoke and drink alcohol have differences in language fluency, processing speed, and executive function. The reason for this is that older adults who smoke and drink alcohol have degraded cognitive function, and daily alcohol consumption and monthly alcohol consumption days will lead to damage to neurons and the loss of synaptic connections, thereby affecting cognitive function (72, 73). We further clarified the dose of alcohol and found that older adults consuming 1–5 drinks/month exhibited more significant differences compared to those consuming 5–10 drinks/month. Previous survey studies also supported this view, showing that older adults who drink have a lower likelihood of cognitive impairment than non-drinkers. Moderate alcohol consumption has a protective effect on cognitive function, whereas excessive or no alcohol consumption does not provide this protection (74).

Finally, we found that meeting the recommended activity levels (600–1,200 MET-min/week) was associated with better AF and DSST scores in older adults for both regular physical activity (RPA) and total physical activity (TPA), while occupational physical activity (OPA) and transport-related physical activity (TPA) showed no significant correlation with cognitive test scores among this population group. This finding aligns with previous research suggesting that excessive exercise can lead to physical and mental fatigue, characterized by reduced activity in the prefrontal cortex, a crucial area for cognitive control (75). Our study also supports WHO guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behavior, indicating that meeting the minimum weekly total physical activity (>600 MET*minutes per week) is associated with enhanced cognitive function in adults aged 60 and above. While increasing physical activity further may still provide benefits, the range of 600–1,200 MET-min/week appears to offer optimal cognitive benefits.

There are some limitations to our study: first, owing to the cross-sectional design employed, the establishment of a causative connection between PA and cognitive performance was unattainable, for example, older adults with impaired cognitive abilities may not be physically active; second, physical activity was estimated by the amount of physical activity recalled by respondents during a typical week, which may introduce some bias, and at the same time, different types of exercise, intensity, frequency, and duration combinations are difficult to determine, which may result in different health benefits; third, although covariates were controlled for in the model, physical and mental health or physical activity in old age may still be affected by various other confounding factors, such as lack of space to exercise and environmental pollution. Fourth, although the association between domain-specific PA and cognitive function varied across subgroups, the results need to be interpreted with caution given the influence of the potential factors mentioned above. Finally, as the participants’ daily nutritional intake is complex and whichever nutrient is included in the analysis is somewhat biased, nutrition was not considered in this study, which presents some limitations.

5 Conclusion

OPA and TPA were not associated with all aspects of cognitive function in older adults over 60, and RPA and total PA were only positively correlated with some cognitive functions (verbal fluency, processing speed and executive function), total PA also demonstrates an association with delayed recall. In addition, achieving 600–1,200 MET*minutes per week of RPA and total PA may be associated with better processing speed and executive function in older adults. Further, rigorously designed randomized controlled trials and longitudinal studies are still necessary to validate our findings.

Data availability statement

The publicly available data used in this study can be found at https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhanes/index.htm.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by NCHS Ethics Review Board (ERB) approval. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required from the participants or the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin in accordance with the national legislation and institutional requirements.

Author contributions

SW: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LW: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. SL: Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. JQ: Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. FS: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. HZ: Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. YQ: Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. LM: Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. MZ: Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by the National Social Science Fund of China (20BTY104 and 19BTY114) and Hubei Provincial Teaching Reform Research Project for Undergraduate Universities (2023116).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2024.1390511/full#supplementary-material

References

1. World Health Organization . Ageing and health. Available at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/ageing-and-health (Accessed 2024-01-06).

Google Scholar

2. Salthouse, T . Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Annu Rev Psychol. (2011) 63:201–26. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100328

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Ashraf, GM, Chibber, S, Mohammad,, Zaidi, SK, Tabrez, S, Ahmad, A, et al. Recent updates on the association between Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. Med Chem. (2016) 12:226–37. doi: 10.2174/1573406411666151030111820

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. World Health Organization . Global status report on the public health response to dementia. Available at: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789240033245 (Accessed 2024-01-06).

Google Scholar

5. Lee, E, Hong, S, Min, J, Park, D, Cho, W, Suh, S, et al. Association between domain-specific physical activity and diabetes in Korean adults. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-92560-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. He, F, Li, Y, Hu, Z, and Zhang, H. Association of domain-specific physical activity with depressive symptoms: a population-based study. Eur Psychiatry. (2022) 66:e5. doi: 10.1192/j.eurpsy.2022.2350

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Pang, Y, Lv, J, Kartsonaki, C, Yu, C, Guo, Y, Du, H, et al. Association of physical activity with risk of hepatobiliary diseases in China: a prospective cohort study of 0.5 million people. Br J Sports Med. (2021) 55:1024–33. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2020-102174

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Mahmood, S, English, DR, Macinnis, RJ, Karahalios, A, Owen, N, Milne, RL, et al. Domain-specific physical activity and the risk of colorectal cancer: results from the Melbourne collaborative cohort study. BMC Cancer. (2018) 18:18. doi: 10.1186/s12885-018-4961-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Yamamoto, S, Inoue, Y, Kuwahara, K, Miki, T, Nakagawa, T, Honda, T, et al. Leisure-time, occupational, and commuting physical activity and the risk of chronic kidney disease in a working population. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:12308. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-91525-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Zogg, S, Duerr, S, Maier, S, Tomatis, L, Uehli, K, Miedinger, D, et al. Relationship between domain-specific physical activity and different body composition measures in a working population. J Occup Environ Med. (2014) 56:1074–81. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0000000000000224

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Harmouche-Karaki, M, Mahfouz, M, Mahfouz, Y, Fakhoury-Sayegh, N, and Helou, K. Combined effect of physical activity and sedentary behavior on body composition in university students. Clin Nutr. (2020) 39:1517–24. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2019.06.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Greendale, GA, Huang, MH, Wang, Y, Finkelstein, JS, Danielson, ME, and Sternfeld, B. Sport and home physical activity are independently associated with bone density. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2003) 35:506–12. doi: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000056725.64347.C9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Chirles, TJ, Reiter, K, Weiss, LR, Alfini, AJ, Nielson, KA, and Smith, JC. Exercise training and functional connectivity changes in mild cognitive impairment and healthy elders. J Alzheimers Dis. (2017) 57:845–56. doi: 10.3233/JAD-161151

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Muscari, A, Giannoni, C, Pierpaoli, L, Berzigotti, A, Maietta, P, Foschi, E, et al. Chronic endurance exercise training prevents aging-related cognitive decline in healthy older adults: a randomized controlled trial. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2010) 25:1055–64. doi: 10.1002/gps.2462

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Armstrong, T, and Bull, F. Development of the world health organization global physical activity questionnaire (gpaq). J Public Health. (2006) 14:66–70. doi: 10.1007/s10389-006-0024-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention . National health and nutrition examination survey 2013–2014 data documentation, codebook, and frequencies: physical activity (paq_h). (2017). Available at: https://wwwn.cdc.gov/Nchs/Nhanes/2013-2014/PAQ_H.htm (Accessed 2024-01-06).

Google Scholar

17. Wu, Z, Su, G, Lu, W, Liu, L, Zhou, Z, and Xie, B. Clinical symptoms and their relationship with cognitive impairment in elderly patients with depressive disorder. Front Psych. (2022) 13:1009653. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1009653

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Ximing, C, Chen, W, and Zhonghua, S. Effects of benzodiazepines on cognitive function and related mechanisms. Chinese J Drug Abuse Preven Control. (2023) 29:2071–6. doi: 10.15900/j.cnki.zylf1995.2023.12.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. You, Y, Chen, Y, Chen, X, Wei, M, Yin, J, Zhang, Q, et al. Threshold effects of the relationship between physical exercise and cognitive function in the short-sleep elder population. Front Aging Neurosci. (2023) 15:1214748. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.1214748

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Liu, F, Chen, X, Nie, P, Lin, S, Guo, J, Chen, J, et al. Can tai chi improve cognitive function? A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Altern Complement Med. (2021) 27:1070–83. doi: 10.1089/acm.2021.0084

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hoy, S, Östh, J, Pascoe, M, Kandola, A, and Hallgren, M. Effects of yoga-based interventions on cognitive function in healthy older adults: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Complement Ther Med. (2021) 58:102690. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2021.102690

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Herold, F, Törpel, A, Schega, L, and Müller, NG. Functional and/or structural brain changes in response to resistance exercises and resistance training lead to cognitive improvements – a systematic review. Eur Rev Aging Phys Act. (2019) 16:10. doi: 10.1186/s11556-019-0217-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Barnes, DE, Santos-Modesitt, W, Poelke, G, Kramer, AF, Castro, C, Middleton, LE, et al. The mental activity and exercise (max) trial: a randomized controlled trial to enhance cognitive function in older adults. JAMA Intern Med. (2013) 173:797–804. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.189

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Lautenschlager, NT, Cox, KL, Flicker, L, Foster, JK, van Bockxmeer, FM, Xiao, J, et al. Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA. (2008) 300:1027–37. doi: 10.1001/jama.300.9.1027

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention . National health and nutrition examination survey tutorials: weighting module. Available at: https://wwwn.cdc.gov/nchs/nhanes/tutorials/Weighting.aspx (Accessed 2024-01-06).

Google Scholar

26. World Health Organization . Global physical activity questionnaire (gpaq) analysis guide. (2021). Available at: https://www.who.int/docs/default-source/ncds/ncd-surveillance/gpaq-analysis-guide.pdf (Accessed 2024-01-06).

Google Scholar

27. World Health Organization . Who guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour. (2020). Available at: https://iris.who.int/bitstream/handle/10665/336656/9789240015128-eng.pdf?sequence=1 (Accessed 2024-01-06).

Google Scholar

28. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention . National health and nutrition examination survey 2013–2014 data documentation, codebook, and frequencies: cognitive functioning (cfq_h). Available at: https://wwwn.cdc.gov/Nchs/Nhanes/2013-2014/CFQ_H.htm (Accessed 2024-01-06).

Google Scholar

29. Moms, JC, Heyman, A, Mohs, RC, Hughes, JP, van Belle, G, Fillenbaum, G, et al. The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (cerad). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. (1989) 39:1159. doi: 10.1212/WNL.39.9.1159

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Sotaniemi, M, Pulliainen, V, Hokkanen, L, Pirttilä, T, Hallikainen, I, Soininen, H, et al. Cerad-neuropsychological battery in screening mild Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurol Scand. (2012) 125:16–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2010.01459.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hailpern, SM, Melamed, ML, Cohen, HW, and Hostetter, TH. Moderate chronic kidney disease and cognitive function in adults 20 to 59 years of age: third national health and nutrition examination survey (NHANES III). J Am Soc Nephrol. (2007) 18:2205–13. doi: 10.1681/ASN.2006101165

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Canning, SJ, Leach, L, Stuss, D, Ngo, L, and Black, SE. Diagnostic utility of abbreviated fluency measures in Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia. Neurology. (2004) 62:556–62. doi: 10.1212/wnl.62.4.556

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Henry, JD, Crawford, JR, and Phillips, LH. Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia. (2004) 42:1212–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Clark, LJ, Gatz, M, Zheng, L, Chen, YL, Mccleary, C, and Mack, WJ. Longitudinal verbal fluency in normal aging, preclinical, and prevalent Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Dement. (2009) 24:461–8. doi: 10.1177/1533317509345154

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Jaeger, J . Digit symbol substitution test: the case for sensitivity over specificity in neuropsychological testing. J Clin Psychopharmacol. (2018) 38:513–9. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0000000000000941

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Bailey, RL, Jun, S, Murphy, L, Green, R, Gahche, JJ, Dwyer, JT, et al. High folic acid or folate combined with low vitamin b-12 status: potential but inconsistent association with cognitive function in a nationally representative cross-sectional sample of us older adults participating in the nhanes. Am J Clin Nutr. (2020) 112:1547–57. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqaa239

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Dowllah, IM, Lopez-Alvarenga, J, Maestre, GE, Karabulut, U, Lehker, M, and Karabulut, M. Relationship between cognitive performance, physical activity, and socio-demographic/individual characteristics among aging Americans. J Alzheimers Dis. (2023) 92:975–87. doi: 10.3233/JAD-221151

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Kroenke, K, Spitzer, RL, and Williams, JB. The phq-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med. (2001) 16:606–13. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Miao, H, Liu, Y, Tsai, TC, Schwartz, J, and Ji, JS. Association between blood lead level and uncontrolled hypertension in the us population (NHANES 1999-2016). J Am Heart Assoc. (2020) 9:e015533. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.119.015533

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Huang, X, Zhu, W, Gao, X, Yu, D, Jin, H, Huang, J, et al. Relationship between old-aged preferences regarding various types of physical activity and chronic disease status: a cross-sectional study in shanghai, China. Front Public Health. (2022) 10:865328. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.865328

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. White, RL, Babic, MJ, Parker, PD, Lubans, DR, Astell-Burt, T, and Lonsdale, C. Domain-specific physical activity and mental health: a meta-analysis. Am J Prev Med. (2017) 52:653–66. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2016.12.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Asztalos, M, Wijndaele, K, De Bourdeaudhuij, I, Philippaerts, R, Matton, L, Duvigneaud, N, et al. Specific associations between types of physical activity and components of mental health. J Sci Med Sport. (2009) 12:468–74. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2008.06.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Ryu, J, Jeong, A, Min, J, Lee, DH, Lee, J, Song, IH, et al. The relationship between domain-specific physical activity and depressive symptoms in Korean adults: analysis of the Korea national health and nutrition examination survey. J Affect Disord. (2022) 302:428–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.01.097

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Franks, KH, Rowsthorn, E, Bransby, L, Lim, YY, Chong, TT, and Pase, MP. Association of self-reported psychological stress with cognitive decline: a systematic review. Neuropsychol Rev. (2022) 33:856–70. doi: 10.1007/s11065-022-09567-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Rehm, J, Hasan, OSM, Black, SE, Shield, KD, and Schwarzinger, M. Alcohol use and dementia: a systematic scoping review. Alzheimers Res Ther. (2019) 11:1. doi: 10.1186/s13195-018-0453-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Swan, GE, and Lessov-Schlaggar, CN. The effects of tobacco smoke and nicotine on cognition and the brain. Neuropsychol Rev. (2007) 17:259–73. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9035-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Lee, SY, Pang, B, Lau, LK, Jabbar, KA, Seah, WT, Chen, KK, et al. Cross-sectional associations of housework with cognitive, physical and sensorimotor functions in younger and older community-dwelling adults: the Yishun study. BMJ Open. (2021) 11:e052557. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2021-052557

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Grellier, J, White, MP, de Bell, S, Brousse, O, Elliott, LR, Fleming, LE, et al. Valuing the health benefits of nature-based recreational physical activity in England. Environ Int. (2024) 187:108667. doi: 10.1016/j.envint.2024.108667

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Nie, Y, Richards, M, Kubinova, R, Titarenko, A, Malyutina, S, Kozela, M, et al. Social networks and cognitive function in older adults: findings from the hapiee study. BMC Geriatr. (2021) 21:570. doi: 10.1186/s12877-021-02531-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Mogic, L, Rutter, EC, Tyas, SL, Maxwell, CJ, O'Connell, ME, and Oremus, M. Functional social support and cognitive function in middle- and older-aged adults: a systematic review of cross-sectional and cohort studies. Syst Rev. (2023) 12:86. doi: 10.1186/s13643-023-02251-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Barha, CK, Galea, LA, Nagamatsu, LS, Erickson, KI, and Liu-Ambrose, T. Personalising exercise recommendations for brain health: considerations and future directions. Br J Sports Med. (2017) 51:636–9. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2016-096710

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Liu-Ambrose, T, Nagamatsu, LS, Graf, P, Beattie, BL, Ashe, MC, and Handy, TC. Resistance training and executive functions a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Arch Intern Med. (2010) 170:170–8. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.494

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Erickson, KI, Voss, MW, Prakash, RS, Basak, C, Szabo, A, Chaddock, L, et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2011) 108:3017–22. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Song, S, Gaynor, AM, Gazes, Y, Lee, S, Xu, Q, Habeck, C, et al. Physical activity moderates the association between white matter hyperintensity burden and cognitive change. Front Aging Neurosci. (2022) 14:945645. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.945645

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Lund, T, Labriola, M, Christensen, KB, Bültmann, U, and Villadsen, E. Physical work environment risk factors for long term sickness absence: prospective findings among a cohort of 5357 employees in Denmark. BMJ. (2006) 332:449–52. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38731.622975.3A

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Salvi, A, Liu, H, and Salim, S. Involvement of oxidative stress and mitochondrial mechanisms in air pollution-related neurobiological impairments. Neurobiol Stress. (2020) 12:100205. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2019.100205

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Sallis, JF, Floyd, MF, Rodríguez, DA, and Saelens, BE. Role of built environments in physical activity, obesity, and cardiovascular disease. Circulation. (2012) 125:729–37. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.110.969022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Ferrucci, L, Baroni, M, Ranchelli, A, Lauretani, F, Maggio, M, Mecocci, P, et al. Interaction between bone and muscle in older persons with mobility limitations. Curr Pharm Des. (2014) 20:3178–97. doi: 10.2174/13816128113196660690

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Clarkson, MJ, Conway, L, and Warmington, SA. Blood flow restriction walking and physical function in older adults: a randomized control trial. J Sci Med Sport. (2017) 20:1041–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.04.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Vega-ávila, GC, Afanador-Restrepo, DF, Rivas-Campo, Y, García-Garro, PA, Hita-Contreras, F, Carcelén-Fraile, M, et al. Rhythmic physical activity and global cognition in older adults with and without mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:19. doi: 10.3390/ijerph191912230

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Venegas-Sanabria, LC, Cavero-Redondo, I, Martínez-Vizcaino, V, Cano-Gutierrez, CA, and Álvarez-Bueno, C. Effect of multicomponent exercise in cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Geriatr. (2022) 22:617. doi: 10.1186/s12877-022-03302-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Meng, X, Li, G, Jia, Y, Liu, Y, Shang, B, Liu, P, et al. Effects of dance intervention on global cognition, executive function and memory of older adults: a meta-analysis and systematic review. Aging Clin Exp Res. (2020) 32:7–19. doi: 10.1007/s40520-019-01159-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Colcombe, S, and Kramer, AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci. (2003) 14:125–30. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Lemaitre, H, Goldman, AL, Sambataro, F, Verchinski, BA, Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Weinberger, DR, et al. Normal age-related brain morphometric changes: nonuniformity across cortical thickness, surface area and gray matter volume? Neurobiol Aging. (2012) 33:617–617.e9. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Fjell, AM, Mcevoy, L, Holland, D, Dale, AM, and Walhovd, KB. What is normal in normal aging? Effects of aging, amyloid and Alzheimer’s disease on the cerebral cortex and the hippocampus. Prog Neurobiol. (2014) 117:20–40. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.02.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Domingos, C, Pêgo, JM, and Santos, NC. Effects of physical activity on brain function and structure in older adults: a systematic review. Behav Brain Res. (2021) 402:113061. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.113061

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Yang, Y, Wang, D, Hou, W, and Li, H. Cognitive decline associated with aging. Adv Exp Med Biol. (2023) 1419:25–46. doi: 10.1007/978-981-99-1627-6_3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Mcgrattan, AM, Zhu, Y, Richardson, CD, Mohan, D, Soh, YC, Sajjad, A, et al. Prevalence and risk of mild cognitive impairment in low and middle-income countries: a systematic review. J Alzheimers Dis. (2021) 79:743–62. doi: 10.3233/JAD-201043

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Li, X, Xia, J, Ma, C, Chen, K, Xu, K, Zhang, J, et al. Accelerating structural degeneration in temporal regions and their effects on cognition in aging of mci patients. Cereb Cortex. (2020) 30:326–38. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz090

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Tan, ZS, Spartano, NL, Beiser, AS, Decarli, C, Auerbach, SH, Vasan, RS, et al. Physical activity, brain volume, and dementia risk: the Framingham study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2017) 72:789–95. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw130

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Shiqiang, W, Danlin, G, Dan, L, Yijie, W, Shaoxi, W, Zhihan, Y, et al. The effect of physical activity level on frailty in Chinese older adults – a multi-period data study based on the China health and aging tracking survey. J Shaanxi Normal Univ. (2023) 51:91–103. doi: 10.15983/j.cnki.jsnu.2023302

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Kiefer, F, Batra, A, Petersen, KU, Ardern, IS, Tananska, D, Bischof, G, et al. German guidelines on screening, diagnosis, and treatment of alcohol use disorders: update 2021. Eur Addict Res. (2022) 28:309–22. doi: 10.1159/000522335

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Webber, K, Davies, AN, Leach, C, and Bradley, A. Alcohol and drug use disorders in patients with cancer and caregivers: effects on caregiver burden. BMJ Support Palliat Care. (2020) 10:242–7. doi: 10.1136/bmjspcare-2019-002127

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Zhang, R, Shen, L, Miles, T, Shen, Y, Cordero, J, Qi, Y, et al. Association of low to moderate alcohol drinking with cognitive functions from middle to older age among us adults. JAMA Netw Open. (2020) 3:e207922. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.7922

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Blain, B, Schmit, C, Aubry, A, Hausswirth, C, Le Meur, Y, and Pessiglione, M. Neuro-computational impact of physical training overload on economic decision-making. Curr Biol. (2019) 29:3289–3297.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.08.054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: physical activity, older adult, cognitive function, executive function, MET, NHANES

Citation: Wu S, Wang L, Liu S, Qi J, Shi F, Zhuang H, Qian Y, Mei L and Zhang M (2024) Relationship between domain-specific physical activity and cognitive function in older adults – findings from NHANES 2011–2014. Front. Public Health. 12:1390511. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1390511

Received: 27 February 2024; Accepted: 08 July 2024;
Published: 24 July 2024.

Edited by:

Richard Kreider, Texas A&M University, United States

Reviewed by:

Megan Leonard, Texas A&M University, United States
Roy James Hardman, Swinburne University of Technology, Australia

Copyright © 2024 Wu, Wang, Liu, Qi, Shi, Zhuang, Qian, Mei and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lin Wang, wanglin123@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.