Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Public Health, 10 June 2024
Sec. Substance Use Disorders and Behavioral Addictions

Nudge theory and gambling: a scoping review

Marie-ve FortierMarie-Ève Fortier1Sophie Audette-ChapdelaineSophie Audette-Chapdelaine2Anne-Marie AugerAnne-Marie Auger2Magaly Brodeur
Magaly Brodeur2*
  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada
  • 2Department of Family and Emergency Medicine, University of Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada

Background: Gambling disorder (GD) is a pressing public health concern with significant societal costs. The recently developed nudge theory, which is rooted in behavioral economics, aims to influence the decision-making behaviors of individuals by implementing changes in the environment.

Aim: This scoping review aims to synthesize the literature on nudge theory as it relates to gambling.

Methods: This scoping review accords with the Arksey and O’Malley framework, as refined by Levac et al. It includes only articles from peer-reviewed journals that focus, as main themes, on both nudge theory and gambling. The final study selection includes six articles.

Results: The scoping review process led to studies explaining how (1) nudges aim to prod people toward healthier gambling choices, fostering the adoption of more responsible gambling practices, and (2) some gambling features, called dark nudges (or sludges), exploit and harm the decision-making processes of people who gamble.

Conclusion: This scoping review highlights the fact that many stakeholders are involved in the field of gambling, and that better cooperation between them would promote safer and more responsible gambling practices. Future research is also needed to empirically test nudges to develop a better understanding of their impact on those who gamble.

1 Introduction

In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) (1), gambling disorder (GD) is classified in the “Substance-Related and Addictive Disorders” category and is defined as “persistent and recurrent problematic gambling behavior leading to clinically significant impairment or distress” (1). Previous studies have concluded that other mental health disorders are often comorbid with GD, such as substance use disorders, depressive disorders, anxiety disorders, and personality disorders (13).

GD is a major public health issue (4), giving rise to many harms that often impact both those who gamble and the people close to them, such as family and friends (5). The classification of gambling harms by Langham et al. (5) illustrates that GD can lead to numerous problems: financial harms, relationship disruptions, emotional or psychological distress, decrements in health, cultural harms, reduced performance at work or school, and increased criminal activity.

Over the last few decades, many measures have been implemented to reduce the harms associated with GD. In many sectors, such as the healthcare (6, 7) and substance abuse (8) systems, the efficacy of nudges has been evidenced. The concept of nudge was introduced in 2008 by Thaler and Sunstein (9), and has been useful in promoting public health while preserving individual freedom and free choice. The authors described nudges as “any aspect of the choice architecture that alters people’s behavior in a predictable way without forbidding any options or significantly changing their economic incentives” (9). More precisely, according to choice architecture the decision-making process is influenced by the way that the choices are displayed (10). This school of thought falls under behavioral economics, which is a field at the intersection of economics and psychology that aims to understand and explain human decision-making processes (11). While nudges are supposed to influence positively the decision-making process, another side of nudge theory, called dark nudges or sludges, tends to be prejudicial (12). Moreover, these kinds of nudges are frequently applied in the gambling field (12).

Sylvester and Booch (8) conducted a realist review of the efficacy of nudge theory in reducing alcohol and tobacco consumption. Despite a limited number of studies, the review identified four effective interventions, indicating a positive impact, although these offer no guarantees (8). However, the application of nudges in the gambling sector remains unexplored. What is the current understanding of the potential effectiveness of nudges in this context?

Given the significance of GD as a critical public health concern and the promising potential of nudge theory in addressing addictive behaviors, the objective of this scoping review was to comprehensively examine the existing peer-reviewed scientific literature that focuses on the intersection of nudge theory and gambling. The following research question guided this scoping review: “What is the current state of scientific knowledge on the relationship between nudge theory and gambling?”

2 Methods

This scoping review is based on the methodological framework of Arksey and O’Malley (13) as refined by Levac et al. (14). It includes the following five steps: (1) identifying the research question, (2) identifying relevant studies, (3) selecting the studies, (4) charting the data, and (5) collating, summarizing, and reporting results (14).

2.1 Data sources and search strategy

A literature search was performed on August 3, 2023, in nine electronic databases via the EBSCOhost platform: APA PsycINFO, Academic Search Complete, APA PsycExtra, Business Source Complete, CINAHL Plus with Full Text, EconLit with Full Text, MEDLINE with Full Text, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Research Topic, and SocINDEX with Full Text. The search strategy included terminology from the concepts of both “nudge” and “gambling” (see Figure 1) and was developed with the help and support of an academic health librarian from the University of Sherbrooke. In addition, the terms “nudge” and “gambling” were searched on Google Scholar and the references of all selected articles were closely examined.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Search strategy employed to perform a literature search across nine electronic databases.

2.2 Study selection

The following inclusion criteria were applied during article selection for this scoping review: (1) articles are directly about the concept of nudge and gambling, (2) articles are published in a peer-reviewed journal, and (3) articles are in English.

The sorting of the resulting 978 articles began with the withdrawal of duplicates, leading M-EF to screen the titles and abstracts of 931 articles. M-EF then reviewed the remaining 46 full-text articles and classified each according to our level of certainty about its relevance for our scoping review and its accordance with the inclusion criteria. During the selection process, disagreements were discussed by all team members. M-EF, SA-C, and MB debated the inclusion or exclusion of the last four articles under evaluation. This concluded the final selection of the six articles included in our study. All team members approved the final selection (Figure 2).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. PRISMA flow diagram summarizing identification, screening and evaluation of articles resulting from the initial literature search.

2.3 Data extraction and data analysis

M-EF conducted the data extraction of the following characteristics for the six articles: authors’ names, year, title, country, objective, methodology, population studied, and main conclusions of the article (see Table 1). Table 1 was revised by SA-C and MB. The data collected in the six articles were grouped by themes and are presented according to the narrative synthesis approach, which consists of concise “findings from multiple studies that rely primarily on the use of words and text to summarize and explain the findings” (21).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Description of the studies included in the scoping review.

3 Results

3.1 Study characteristics

All selected articles were published between 2018 and 2021, and four of the six were from Australia (11, 15, 19, 20). The other two articles were from Canada (17) and the United Kingdom (18).

3.2 Article type

There was wide variety within our selection of articles, comprising a theoretical article (11), a commentary (17), an editorial (18), a perspective (15), a letter to the editor (19), and one original research study (20) (see Table 1).

Regarding the methodology and the characteristics of the populations studied, only the original research study provided such information (20). As this is mixed-method research, it adopts a mixed quantitative and qualitative design. The data analyzed came mostly from observations performed in sports bars, as well as interviews with customers. The mean age of the studied group was 33.3 years old, and the sample was composed principally of men (70%).

3.3 Behavioral economics

The article by Gainsbury et al. (11) explains behavioral economics in the most detail. Indeed, the authors mention that, according to behavioral economics, individuals tend to use shortcuts known as heuristics in complex and uncertain situations. Heuristics enable individuals to make prompt decisions with less mental effort. However, this can also generate unconscious errors, called biases. The authors also report that many of these heuristics and biases are exploited in gambling. Two articles in our selection (11, 18) explicitly link gambling features to heuristics and/or biases, giving examples as to how gambling games are conceptualized with strategies such as “losses disguised as wins” or “near-misses.” These strategies can take the form, for example, of a slot machine displaying a congratulatory message for a win that is worth less than the bet (11). Murch and Clark (17) also note that “losses disguised as wins” harm the decision-making process for people who practice gambling. Two articles in our study selection also briefly mention the field of behavioral science (15, 19), while one makes no mention of it (20).

3.4 Nudges

All six selected articles define the nudge concept as deriving from choice architecture (see Table 1). As mentioned by Gainsbury et al. (11), nudges “involve making subtle alterations in the choice environment to encourage behavior change.” Unlike dark nudges, nudges aim to prompt people into making healthier choices (17, 19, 20). Nudges are also characterized by not restricting people, who remain free to choose other options (15, 17).

In the gambling context, the authors mention that nudges could be used to lead individuals to adopt healthier (15) and safer (19) gambling practices. The effectiveness of nudges in gambling lies in their cost-effectiveness and subtlety (11). Nudges in gambling are most effective when they resonate with individuals’ own understanding of what is important (11).

As seen in Table 2, some examples of nudges in gambling involve the use of normative feedback to compare one’s gambling behaviors with that of others. Such feedback could also include incentives (such as smiley faces, words of encouragement, etc.), dynamic messages during gambling (“Do you need to take a break?”) (11), or a monthly summary of all gambling activities (11). Murch and Clark (17) suggest that it would be beneficial to use nudge theory to modify slot machines, such as by suppressing the sound normally made for “losses disguised as wins.”

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Examples of nudges and dark nudges in the literature.

Newall and Rockloff identify a nuance about nudges in gambling (19). While they agree that nudges could prompt safely gambling practices, they consider that it would be more urgent to reduce the dark nudges present in gambling environments.

Finally, while all of the articles discussed nudges, two of them defined the concept of nudges only briefly, after which they focused on dark nudges (18, 20).

3.5 Dark nudges and sludges in gambling

In four of the six selected articles, the authors explicitly question the dark nudges features of gambling games, as these impair the decision-making processes of those who gamble, thereby taking advantage of their biases (see Table 2) (1720).

Murch and Clark (17) consider “losses disguised as wins” to be dark nudges. Newall and Rockloff (19) also give examples of these features in gambling, such as suggesting high deposit limits. Pennay et al. (20) observed elements in sports bars that could also be considered dark nudges, such as the “visibility, lighting, scale and proximity to the entrance” of the electronic gaming machines. In addition, Gainsbury et al. (15) mention that some features in gambling, such as “infinity scrolling,” nudge users into “continued gambling.” These words dovetail with the concept of dark nudges, even though the term itself is not used by the authors.

3.6 Stakeholders

Many stakeholders are involved in gambling, and some of the articles in our selection discuss their roles (15, 18, 19), with some reporting on the contradictions or conflicts of interest that exist among them (11, 15). Two articles—Gainsbury et al. (11, 15) and Newall (18)—report that stakeholders should work together to reduce gambling harms. More precisely, Gainsbury et al. (15) provide an in-depth description of the roles played by individual users, community groups, gambling industry operators, government actors and regulators, financial institutions, and researchers to establish safer online gambling. Newall (18) discusses briefly the dynamics between stakeholders and notes that “gamblers, gambling firms, regulators and researchers” are all implicated in the current state of the gambling environment.

Gainsbury et al. (11) and Newall (18) note that while gambling is a source of government income, it comes with a big societal cost, and this cost causes a conflict of interest (11). Newall (18) adds that the presence of dark nudges in gambling is a mark of contradiction from gambling officials, since “the gambling industry claims to support responsible gambling” (18).

3.7 Future of nudges in gambling

All of the articles in our selection advise that actions be taken to encourage responsible gambling or to reduce gambling harms. In accord with the fact that their articles mostly address dark nudges, the recommendations of Newall (18) and Pennay et al. (20) do not include nudges. However, according to Newall (18), researchers in the field of gambling should pay close attention to electronic gambling machines because they constitute a large part of “gambling’s public health costs.” He also highlights the importance of informing people who gamble about the real odds of winning. For their part, and in concordance with the subject of their article, Pennay et al. (20) suggest that researchers should study various populations and sports bar environments to guide forthcoming public health interventions.

The other four selected articles suggest future uses of nudges in gambling. Gainsbury et al. (11, 15) highlight the role of researchers who study nudges in gambling. The 2018 article mentions that researchers should complete more studies to evaluate nudges in gambling and the effectiveness of interventions on different groups (11). The 2020 article suggests that researchers identify and investigate persuasive designs in gambling and nudges (15). Also, Murch and Clark (17) consider that nudges can establish a bridge between research findings and gambling policies. Gainsbury et al. (15) recommend that operators in the gambling industry detect people who gamble at “moderate-risk” and then send them personalized dynamic messages “about consumer protection tools.” According to Newall and Rockloff (19), operators could also redesign the deposit limit tool “by removing the dropdown list’s suggested limits and by getting gamblers to input their own limits.”

4 Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first scoping review whose aim that synthesizes scientific peer-reviewed studies focusing directly on both nudge theory and gambling. Conducting this scoping review revealed that the relationship between nudge theory and gambling is frequently related to the presence of dark nudges in gambling. These are harmful to people who gamble—particularly those who have a gambling disorder—as they obstruct their decision-making process. In contrast, the development and implementation of beneficial nudges could reduce gambling harms and promote responsible gambling. While nudges are relatively easy to implement due to their low cost and subtlety, the fact that they often involve many gambling stakeholders can complicate the task.

Two authors in our study selection each have two articles in which they are the first author (11, 15, 18, 19), thereby limiting our exposure to diverse views about nudges in gambling. As for article type, our selection is composed of only one original research study, which mainly concerns dark nudges (20). For that matter, two of the six articles are mostly about dark nudges (18, 20), while four of the six articles are from Australia (11, 15, 19, 20).

These limitations related to diversity in our study selection indicate a deficiency of studies (especially regarding peer-reviewed original research) on nudges and gambling. Future studies should test nudges on real people who gamble, including experts by experience and/or people with a gambling disorder (22), using realistic machines or platforms to ensure ecological validity (17). Maximizing the positive effects of this process would necessitate involving different stakeholders (11, 15, 18).

In addition, it is our opinion that ensuring a mutual understanding of the different concepts related to nudge theory requires that the scientific vocabulary be more uniform. Indeed, while developing our scoping review search strategy we observed that some articles use the terms “nudges” or “nudging” to discuss factors that harm the decision-making process, while these factors are in fact dark nudges. Examples include the articles of Carter and Hall (23) and Deans et al. (24), which use the term “nudge” when discussing features that aim to influence people to continue gambling. To avoid confusion, we believe that the terms “dark nudge” or “sludge” could be used instead in this context. We also observed that while some articles mention gambling features that could be considered nudges or dark nudges, none mention nudge theory or the concept of nudge itself. An example is the article of Graydon et al. (16), whose research about “losses disguised as wins” never mentions dark nudges. It is the commentary of Murch and Clark (17), included in our study selection, that links the two concepts. We also found original research articles that we consider to be related to the definition of nudges, but which do not mention this theoretical link. They address the concepts of dynamic warning messages (25), volatility warnings (26), and behavioral feedback (27) in gambling. However, it is important to highlight that nudge theory is a recent development, which may explain the lack of terminology, uniformity, and knowledge of these concepts within the scientific community. In conclusion, our review emphasizes the connection between nudge theory and gambling, emphasizing the detrimental effects of dark nudges. While beneficial nudges can mitigate gambling’s harms and contribute to harm minimization, the involvement of myriad stakeholders adds complexity to their implementation. The study’s limitations underscore the need for diverse and peer-reviewed research. A standardized vocabulary is crucial for clarity in discussions and research about nudges and gambling.

4.1 Strengths and limitations

This scoping review draws a systematic portrait of, and synthetizes the literature regarding, nudges and gambling. It also distinguishes terminology within nudge theory (i.e., nudge, sludge, dark nudge) and highlights the need for uniformity in the vocabulary of this emerging field. However, as this scoping review included only published articles in peer-reviewed journals, gray literature was not included. Furthermore, only articles written in English were included, which may have led to an underestimation of the number of articles linking nudges and gambling.

5 Conclusion

This scoping review asserts the promising utility of nudges to lead people who gamble toward safer and more responsible gambling practices. It also addresses the harmful side of nudges, dark nudges, which are used in gambling to exploit gamblers. Future research should empirically examine the effectiveness of nudges to gain a deeper understanding of their impact on individuals who engage in gambling. Such insights are essential for the crafting of well-informed policies that efficiently promote responsible gambling. Future research should also focus on the impacts of dark nudges, and test whether reducing dark nudges (or adding nudges) fosters safer gambling. With this objective in mind, it is imperative that all stakeholders implicated in gambling contribute to this process.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author contributions

M-ÈF: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology. SA-C: Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. A-MA: Writing – review & editing. MB: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Ministry of Economy and Innovation of the Government of Quebec, as part of a call for social innovation projects (project #57352). The funding body played no role in the design of the study, data collection, analysis, interpretation of data, writing of the manuscript, or decision to submit the article for publication. The researchers involved in this study operate independently.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the health library services of the University of Sherbrooke for their assistance with the development of the search strategy, and Natalia Muñoz Gómez, research professional, for her involvement in the revision and submission of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

2. Kessler, RC, Hwang, I, LaBrie, R, Petukhova, M, Sampson, NA, Winters, KC, et al. DSM-IV pathological gambling in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychol Med. (2008) 38:1351–60. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708002900

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Nicholson, R, Mackenzie, C, and Afifi TOSareen, J. Effects of gambling diagnostic criteria changes from DSM-IV to DSM-5 on mental disorder comorbidity across younger, middle-aged, and older adults in a nationally representative sample. J Gambl Stud. (2019) 35:307–20. doi: 10.1007/s10899-018-9801-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Browne, M. The social cost of gambling to Victoria. Vic Responsible Gambl Found (2017). Available at: http://responsiblegambling.vic.gov.au/resources/publications/the-social-cost-of-gambling-to-victoria-121/ (accessed August 4, 2023).

Google Scholar

5. Langham, E, Thorne, H, Browne, M, Donaldson, P, Rose, J, and Rockloff, M. Understanding gambling related harm: a proposed definition, conceptual framework, and taxonomy of harms. BMC Public Health. (2016) 16:80–23. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-2747-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Harrison, JD, and Patel, MS. Designing nudges for success in health care. AMA J Ethics. (2020) 22:E796–801. doi: 10.1001/amajethics.2020.796

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Last, BS, Buttenheim, AM, Timon, CE, Mitra, N, and Beidas, RS. Systematic review of clinician-directed nudges in healthcare contexts. BMJ Open. (2021) 11:e048801. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2021-048801

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Sylvester, B, and Gooch, S. A realist review of nudge interventions to decrease alcohol and tobacco use. Ottawa: Editorial Office of Defence Research and Development Canada (2021).

Google Scholar

9. Thaler, RH, and Sunstein, CR. Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness. New York, New York, U.S.A.: Penguin Books (2009).

Google Scholar

10. Johnson, EJ, Shu, SB, Dellaert, BGC, Fox, C, Goldstein, DG, Häubl, G, et al. Beyond nudges: tools of a choice architecture. Mark Lett J Res Mark. (2012) 23:487–04. doi: 10.1007/s11002-012-9186-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Gainsbury, SM, Tobias-Webb, J, and Slonim, R. Behavioral economics and gambling: a new paradigm for approaching harm-minimization. Gaming Law Rev. (2018) 22:608–17. doi: 10.1089/glr2.2018.22106

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Newall, PWS . What is sludge? Comparing Sunstein’s definition to others’. Behav Public Policy. (2023) 7:851–7. doi: 10.1017/bpp.2022.12

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Arksey, H, and O’Malley, L. Scoping studies: towards a methodological framework. Int J Soc Res Methodol. (2005) 8:19–32. doi: 10.1080/1364557032000119616

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Levac, D, Colquhoun, H, and O’Brien, KK. Scoping studies: advancing the methodology. Implement Sci. (2010) 5:69–77. doi: 10.1186/1748-5908-5-69

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Gainsbury, SM, Black, N, Blaszczynski, A, Callaghan, S, Clancey, G, Starcevic, V, et al. Reducing internet gambling harms using behavioral science: a stakeholder framework. Front Psych. (2020) 11:11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.598589

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Graydon, C, Stange, M, and Dixon, MJ. Losses disguised as wins affect game selection on multiline slots. J Gambl Stud. (2018) 34:1377–90. doi: 10.1007/s10899-018-9773-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Murch, WS, and Clark, LCommentary on Graydon. Realistic simulations and nudging gambling policy. Addiction. (2019) 114:125–6. doi: 10.1111/add.14493

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Newall, PWS . Dark nudges in gambling. Addict Res Theory. (2019) 27:65–7. doi: 10.1080/16066359.2018.1474206

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Newall, PW, and Rockloff, MJ. Promoting safer gambling via the removal of harmful sludge: a view on how behavioural science’s ‘nudge’concept relates to online gambling. Addiction. (2021) 117:838–9. doi: 10.1111/add.15700

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Pennay, A, Livingston, M, Cook, M, Room, R, Dwyer, R, MacLean, S, et al. Sports bars: environmental design, drinking, and sports betting. Addict Res Theory. (2020) 29:316–26. doi: 10.1080/16066359.2020.1830071

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Popay, J, Roberts, H, Sowden, A, Petticrew, M, Arai, L, Rodgers, M, et al. Guidance on the conduct of narrative synthesis in systematic reviews: a product from the ESRC methods Programme. Front Psychiatry. (2006). doi: 10.13140/2.1.1018.4643,

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Gainsbury, S, Russell, A, and Blaszczynski, A. Are psychology university student gamblers representative of non-university students and general gamblers? A Comparative Analysis J Gambl Stud. (2014) 30:11–25. doi: 10.1007/s10899-012-9334-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Carter, A, and Hall, W. Avoiding selective ethical objections to nudges. Am J Bioeth. (2012) 12:12–4. doi: 10.1080/15265161.2011.634488

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Deans, EG, Thomas, SL, Daube, M, and Derevensky, J. “I can sit on the beach and punt through my mobile phone”: the influence of physical and online environments on the gambling risk behaviours of young men. Soc Sci Med. (2016) 166:110–9. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.08.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Gainsbury, S, Aro, D, Ball, D, Tobar, C, and Russell, A. Determining optimal placement for pop-up messages: evaluation of a live trial of dynamic warning messages for electronic gaming machines. Int Gambl Stud. (2015) 15:141–58. doi: 10.1080/14459795.2014.1000358

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Newall, PWS, Walasek, L, and Ludvig, EA. Risk communication improvements for gambling: house-edge information and volatility statements. Psychol Addict Behav. (2022) 36:358–63. doi: 10.1037/adb0000695

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Wood, RTA, and Wohl, MJA. Assessing the effectiveness of a responsible gambling behavioural feedback tool for reducing the gambling expenditure of at-risk players. Int Gambl Stud. (2015) 15:1–16. doi: 10.1080/14459795.2015.1049191

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: gambling, nudge, dark nudge, behavioral economics, public health, scoping review

Citation: Fortier M-&, Audette-Chapdelaine S, Auger A-M and Brodeur M (2024) Nudge theory and gambling: a scoping review. Front. Public Health. 12:1377183. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1377183

Received: 26 January 2024; Accepted: 29 May 2024;
Published: 10 June 2024.

Edited by:

Annie Olivia Campbell, Queen's University Belfast, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Johanna Järvinen-Tassopoulos, Finnish Institute for Health and Welfare, Finland
Frank Houghton, Limerick Institute of Technology, Ireland

Copyright © 2024 Fortier, Audette-Chapdelaine, Auger and Brodeur. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Magaly Brodeur, bWFnYWx5LmJyb2RldXJAdXNoZXJicm9va2UuY2E=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.