Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 05 April 2024
Sec. Environmental Health and Exposome

Cultural worldviews and waste sorting among urban Chinese dwellers: the mediating role of environmental risk perception

Lin Cai,Lin Cai1,2Qingjun LiQingjun Li2Erya WanErya Wan3Menglin LuoMenglin Luo4Siwen Tao
Siwen Tao1*
  • 1School of Community for the Chinese Nation, Southwest Minzu University, Chengdu, China
  • 2School of Marxism, Sichuan Institute of Industrial Technology, Deyang, China
  • 3School of Global Health, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai, China
  • 4School of Pharmaceutical, Southern Medical University, Guangzhou, China

Objective: Waste sorting has received considerable attention in recent decades. However, research on the mechanisms underlying the relationships among cultural worldview, environmental risk perception, and waste sorting is rather scarce. This study aims to explore the cultural worldviews, environmental risk perception, and waste sorting among urban Chinese and their mechanisms.

Methods: This was a cross-sectional study involving 744 urban Chinese residents (371 men and 373 women). A questionnaire was utilized to measure cultural worldviews, environmental risk perception, and waste sorting. Pearson correlation analysis and structural equation modeling were used to examine the relationship between cultural worldviews, perceptions of environmental risk, and waste sorting.

Results: Waste sorting had a relatively insignificant negative relationship with fatalism and individualism. The correlation between environmental risk perception and cultural worldviews was negative except for egalitarianism, and the correlation between hierarchy and environmental risk perception was higher than the others, while individualism was higher than fatalism. Heightened environmental risk perception mediates the relationship between egalitarianism and waste sorting. Reduced environmental risk perception mediates the relationship between hierarchy and waste sorting, and mediates the relationship between individualism and waste sorting.

Conclusion: These new findings provide initial support for the mediating role of environmental risk perception in the relationship between cultural worldviews and waste sorting. Both theoretical and practical implications for understanding the psychological mechanisms of waste sorting are discussed.

Introduction

As the global population grows and urbanization accelerates, the generation of municipal solid waste is increasing at an unprecedented rate. A study published by the World Bank estimates that the annual generation of municipal solid waste will reach 340 million tons worldwide by 2050 (1). This challenge not only puts enormous pressure on the environment but also poses a serious test for public health and resource recycling. Waste sorting, as an effective waste management strategy, has become a key measure to address this issue in several countries and regions around the world (2).

Japanese scholars first started to study the waste-sorting behavior of residents in the 1960s (3). Subsequently, the United States, Germany, and other countries also began to research the factors affecting urban dwellers’ waste-sorting behavior (4, 5). In 1992, the concept of waste sorting was first introduced with the “Notice on several opinions on solving the problem of urban waste in China,” which was issued by the Ministry of Construction of China along with other three departments. The first leading cities to implement waste sorting, eight in total, including Beijing and Shanghai, have studied waste sorting management methods, and have gained some practical experience. However, there was a lack of public awareness regarding waste sorting, and the majority of the cities’ sorting work was formalistic, making it difficult to establish a systematic and long-term management mechanism. Thus, waste reduction has been implemented at the source to alleviate the challenge of garbage encircling cities.

In China, municipal waste is still mainly disposed of in landfills and incinerated (6). Landfills occupy a large area and have a limited lifespan, which can easily result in conflicts between urban dwellers and landowners, while waste incineration is prone to producing exhaust gasses, such as particulate matter, sulfur dioxide, and sulfur trioxide, which can cause air pollution (7). Hence, the “Implementation Plan for the Domestic Waste Sorting System,” was issued in March 2017 by the China National Development and Reform Commission and the Ministry of Housing and Urban–Rural Development, making it mandatory for residents to separate domestic waste for the first time. In addition, the Ministry of Construction also issued the “Implementation Plan for the Household Waste Sorting System,” making it mandatory for residents to sort their household waste for the first time (8). In fact, in addition to being driven by macro factors, such as government policies and infrastructure (9), residents’ engagement in household waste sorting is also influenced by individual-level variables (10). Risk perception is identified by risk perception theory as an important factor influencing individual behavior (11). The Value-Belief-Norm (VBN) theory suggests that personal values are important factors in influencing pro-environmental behavior intentions (12). However, few studies have tested the cultural theory of risk in the Asian context including China. Compared to Western countries, Chinese culture, politics, economy, and community social support differ significantly, so there is a need to determine whether the standard account of cultural theory is applicable in Asia. Therefore, this study attempts to examine the influence of two individual-level variables, cultural worldview, and environmental risk perception, on urban Chinese waste sorting and their mechanisms of action from the VBN theory and risk perception theory, and further proposes the optimization of urban dwellers’ waste sorting.

Theory and hypotheses

Cultural worldviews and waste sorting

Cultural Theory (CT), also known as Grid-Group Cultural Theory (GGCT), is a widely used framework for analyzing culture (13). It was developed by Durkheim and expanded by Douglas, Wildavsky, and others (1421). CT uses two dimensions - Grid and Group - to represent a culture’s reliance on standardized rules and the integration of individual interests into group interests (22). These dimensions can be positive or negative, creating four quadrants: Hierarchy (grid+ and group+), individualism (grid- and group–), egalitarianism (grid– and group+), and fatalism (grid+ and group–) (23). Each quadrant represents a different approach to addressing social challenges and risks. Hierarchy emphasizes rules and order within a group (24). Egalitarianism focuses on equality and social justice (25, 26). Individualism values self-regulation over authority (24, 27). Fatalism believes that challenges are beyond one’s control (28).

Waste sorting refers to the process of recycling and treating garbage according to its different components, attributes, utility value, environmental impact, and the requirements of different treatment processes (29). From the early days of simple sorting to today’s sophisticated separation, the concept and practice of waste sorting have continuously developed and improved. Many countries and regions have formulated relevant laws and regulations to promote the popularization and implementation of waste sorting (30). Waste sorting helps reduce pollution and promotes the balance and restoration of the ecosystem (31). Waste sorting promotes the development of a circular economy, improves resource utilization efficiency, and reduces production costs (32). In addition, waste sorting promotes the development and innovation of related industries, providing a new impetus for economic growth (33). In terms of culture, waste sorting has raised public awareness of environmental protection (34), promoted the popularization of green lifestyles, and helped build a social and cultural atmosphere in which human beings live in harmony with nature.

The specific presentation of cultural worldviews in social life forms a cultural way of life. This cultural lifestyle reflects individual cultural worldviews (35). Mary Douglas’s cultural symbolic analysis of pollution and taboos points out that when something is considered unclean, it is due to the misplacement of people’s cognitive classification, which is related to the construction of social order (36). This view inspired the later research on garbage, that is, attention to the cultural connotation and value of garbage. Thompson proposed the Rubbish Theory (RT). According to Thompson, our worldview determines our actions. Wang et al. (37) proposed that people develop behavioral habits that correspond to their cultural worldview. Each of the four cultural worldviews will influence decision-making and behavior including waste sorting (38). Zeng et al. showed that egalitarianism was significantly positively correlated with pro-environmental behavior, and individualism was not significantly correlated with pro-environmental behavior (39). Toorzani and Rassafi showed that egalitarianism, hierarchy, and pro-environmental behavior were significantly positively correlated, individualism was significantly negatively correlated with pro-environmental purchasing behavior, and fatalism was not significantly correlated with pro-environmental purchasing behavior (40). Jung and Cho showed that individualism and pro-environmental purchasing behavior were significantly positively correlated (41). Waste sorting, as a type of pro-environmental behavior, has certain commonalities with other pro-environmental behaviors in terms of the mechanism of influence (42, 43). Based on the above theories and the relationships between the variables, the following hypotheses were formulated in this study.

Hypothesis 1a: Higher egalitarianism and hierarchy are related to higher waste sorting.

Hypothesis 1b: Higher individualism is related to lower waste sorting.

Hypothesis 1c: Fatalism is non-significantly related to waste sorting.

Environmental risk perception and waste sorting

Beck introduced the concept of risk society in 1980. He argued that every period of human history has been a risk society, facing different threats at different times, and that in modern society, humans have gradually become the main creators of risk (44). The rapid development of technology has solved many challenges that were previously considered high-risk by humans and has made life easier. However, it has also resulted in numerous environmental hazards, disasters, and social inequalities, in addition to a significant number of unpredictable but far-reaching unknown risks (45, 46). Blaylock further noted that the more uncertainty an individual perceives, the greater the perceived risk (47). Maartensson and Loi’s empirical study of Australian adults found a significant positive correlation between environmental risk perception and pro-environmental behaviors (48). Han et al. also found a significant positive correlation between environmental risk perception and pro-environmental behavior in a survey study of Chinese adults (49). The more aware individuals are of environmental risks, the more likely they are to engage in environmentally-friendly behavior (39). When individuals perceive environmental risks in their surroundings, they may tend to modify their behavior in order to promote environmental well-being. Furthermore, waste sorting, as a form of pro-environmental behavior (42, 43), may be influenced by the perception of environmental risks. Thus, this study proposes the following hypothesis.

Hypothesis 2: An individual who perceives environmental risk more strongly has a greater tendency to sort waste.

Cultural worldview, environmental risk perception, and waste sorting

Attitudes toward risk differ among individuals who hold different values (50). Research has shown that egalitarianism is positively associated with environmental risk perception and policy support (24, 51), while individualism and hierarchy are negatively associated with these factors (5254). A number of studies showed that fatalism is negatively correlated with risk perception (55). Fatalism and environmental risk perception are not significantly correlated in most studies (21).

The VBN theory, proposed by Stern et al., suggests that environmental attitude variables are influenced by an individual’s value system (56). The theory proposes that individual values influence beliefs, which in turn result in different behaviors. Beliefs are seen as an important link between values and behaviors. Therefore, the influence of cultural worldview on behavior may not be entirely direct (57) but it may instead affect people’s behavior through changes in their perceptions (39, 58). Lacroix and Gifford’s Canadian study found that climate change risk perceptions play a mediating role between cultural worldviews and barrier perceptions (59). The study by Zeng et al. revealed that risk perception plays a mediating role between cultural worldview and pro-environmental behavior (39). Based on the existing relationship between the four dimensions of cultural worldview and environmental risk perception, waste sorting, this study proposes the following hypotheses.

Hypothesis 3a: The relationship between egalitarianism and waste sorting is mediated by an increased environmental risk perception.

Hypothesis 3b: The relationship between hierarchy and waste sorting is mediated by a reduced environmental risk perception.

Hypothesis 3c: The relationship between individualism and waste sorting is mediated by a reduced environmental risk perception.

Methods

Participants and procedures

A convenience sampling method was used to conduct the survey. Data were collected online from 15 March 2021, to 25 March 2021, using the Wenjuanxing, a widely accepted online questionnaire survey platform in China. In nine provinces and cities, 783 questionnaires were distributed to urban dwellers, including Sichuan, Chongqing, Yunnan, Guangdong, Hainan, Liaoning, Shandong, Shaanxi, and Inner Mongolia. Four undergraduate students majoring in psychology were trained as examiners to be familiar with the issues to be addressed in the questionnaire, which was administered both in batches and collectively. After the survey, the questionnaires were collected on the spot. The survey was rigorously designed and conducted in adherence with national and international ethical guidelines including ethical approval, informed consent, and data integrity. In addition, our study strictly respected the participants and protected their privacy and confidentiality. This research was approved by the Research Committee of Sichuan Institute of Industrial Technology (SCGKY-002) in accordance with international ethical standards.

The average response time was 8 min and 23 s, with questionnaires with a response time of fewer than 3 min, regular responses (e.g., always as a pattern of 1, 2, 3, 4, 5), and straight liners deleted. Thus, 744 questionnaires were valid, with an effective rate of 95.02%. There were 371 males (49.87%) and 373 females (50.13%). The ages of the participants ranged from 18 to 70 years (mean age 35.45 years, SD 15.91 years). The average monthly household income was less than RMB 2,000 for 233 people (31.32%), RMB 2,000–4,000 for 311 people (41.80%), RMB 4,001–6,000 for 116 people (15.59%), RMB 6,001–8,000 for 39 people (5.24%), RMB 6,001–8,000 for 23 people (3.09%) and more than RMB 10,000 for 22 people (2.96%).

Measures

Waste sorting behavior scale

Waste sorting was measured using the Waste Sorting Behavior Scale developed by Han et al. (60) which consists of four items. This scale measures residents’ waste sorting behavior in relation to food waste, recyclable waste, and other waste sorting behaviors. Higher scores indicate higher waste sorting behavior according to a 5-point Likert scale. The Cronbach’s coefficient here was 0.81. The goodness of fit indices for this scale showed a reasonably good fit, χ2/df = 2.63, CFI = 0.995, TLI = 0.974, and RMSEA = 0.06.

Cultural worldview scale

Scholars of cultural theory debate whether individual cultural biases should be measured by four separate indices or classified into four quadrants (61). This paper argues for treating individuals as hybrids of the four cultural types and proposes the use of separate indices to measure each culture (62). Scholars have also suggested that combining measures of worldview and relational statements provides better validity for measuring culture (63). The Cultural Worldview Scale developed by Zeng et al. (39) was used in this study to measure cultural worldview. The scale has four dimensions: fatalism, individualism, hierarchy, and egalitarianism. It consists of four items that measure the following dimensions: (1) no motivation and irrelevant, (2) self-interest, (3) laws and rules, and (4) respect for nature as a human being, measuring fatalism, individualism, hierarchy, and egalitarianism. Item number 4 is a ranking question: “Sequencing responsibility for environmental protection in Government/Business Corporations/Everyone/Other“with 5, 4, 3, and 2 points for the first to fourth place, and 1 point for the unranked option. The Cronbach’s α coefficient for the egalitarianism, hierarchy, individualism, and fatalism indices was found to be 0.23, 0.11, 0.10, and 0.30, respectively. The goodness of fit indices for this scale showed an acceptable fit: χ2/df = 3.28, CFI = 0.913, TLI = 0.926, and RMSEA = 0.07. Despite the plausible validity of our cultural measures, the Cronbach’s coefficient for each of the four cultural worldviews is very low, which may lead to unreliable measurement (64).

Environmental risk perception scale

The Environmental Risk Perception Scale developed by Wang (65) was used in this study to measure environmental risk perception. It consists of six items, such as “How serious is the problem of domestic waste pollution in your area?” Higher scores indicate a higher perception of environmental risk. The Cronbach’s coefficient here was 0.89. The goodness of fit indices for this scale showed a reasonably good fit, χ2/df = 2.78, CFI = 0.991, TLI = 0.963, and RMSEA = 0.06.

Statistical analysis

The data were statistically analyzed using SPSS 24.0 for descriptive statistics, correlation analysis, and tests of variance. Additionally, MPLUS 7.0 was used for structural equation modeling to examine the role of environmental risk perception in the relationship between fatalism, individualism, hierarchy, egalitarianism, and waste sorting.

Results

Common method deviation test

When data are collected using the self-report method, the issue of common method bias may arise. Following the recommendations of Zhou and Long (66), we implemented relevant controls in the test. These controls included using reverse presentation for some items and emphasizing that there is no right or wrong answer. Additionally, a common method bias test was conducted using the Harman one-way test prior to analyzing the data. The results showed that there were nine factors with eigenvalues greater than one, explaining 66.14% of the variance. The first factor explained 18.48% of the variance, which is below the 40% threshold (67). There is, therefore, no significant common method bias in this study.

Analysis of correlations

An overview of all variables is presented in Table 1 along with their zero-order correlations. Waste sorting was found to be relatively insignificantly negatively correlated with fatalism and individualism. All cultural worldviews, except egalitarianism, were negatively correlated with environmental risk perception. Hierarchy was more highly correlated than others, while individualism was more highly correlated than fatalism. Waste sorting was found to be significantly influenced by gender, while fatalism was significantly negatively associated with gender. Therefore, we chose “gender” as a control variable in the structural equation modeling.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations between waste sorting, environmental risk perception, and cultural worldview.

Structural results

Structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to test the research hypotheses and the model estimation method was maximum likelihood estimation. Structural equation analysis was conducted using fatalism, individualism, hierarchy, and egalitarianism as predictor variables, waste sorting as the outcome variable, environmental risk perception as the mediating variable, and gender as the control variable. Significance tests were conducted using the Bootstrap method of bias correction, with 5,000 replicate samples with replacement, to determine whether the mediating effect was statistically significant. The test was based on whether the 95% confidence interval included 0. The mediating effect was significant if the confidence interval did not include 0, and not significant if it did. The test results of the hypothesis model showed good overall model fit indices: χ2 = 11.652, df = 4, RMSEA = 0.074 [90% CI: 0.063, 0.085], CFI = 0.932, TLI = 0.926, SRMR = 0.039. The specific path coefficients are shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Path analysis of the model.

Mediating effect analysis

The model was further tested using the Bootstrap method of bias correction. The results (Table 2) indicate that all mediating effects are non-zero, except for the 95% confidence interval for the mediating effect of fatalism→environmental risk perception→ waste sorting, which includes 0, indicating a significant mediating effect. In other words, the effects of environmental risk perception, individualism, hierarchy, egalitarianism, and garbage classification were mediated by values of −0.024, −0.036, and 0.063, respectively. Hypotheses 3a, 3b, and 3c are therefore confirmed.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Bootstrap results for each path coefficient of the hypothetical model.

Discussion

The relationship between egalitarianism, hierarchy and waste sorting

The findings suggest that egalitarianism and hierarchy are positively related to waste sorting, which is consistent with the hypothesis of 1a. Egalitarianism sees the object as equal to others, views nature as fragile and resources as limited, and is more willing to participate in environmental protection and waste sorting. Hierarchy honors specialists and authority and is more acceptable to environmental specialists or government departments (68). As local governments have been increasing their efforts to publicize environmental risks in recent years (69), it is more conducive for hierarchies to engage in positive behavior in waste sorting.

The relationship between individualism and waste sorting

Individualism is non-significantly negatively related to waste sorting, which is inconsistent with the hypothesis of 1b. Individuals who hold individualistic cultural worldviews perceive environmental risks as symbolic representations of social elite status and authority. They argue that an excessive fear of environmental risks is a form of criticism of social elites, which in turn poses a threat to their cultural worldviews (37). They are more likely to devalue environmental protection and less likely to engage in pro-environmental behavior.

The relationship between fatalism and waste sorting

Fatalism is non-significantly related to waste sorting, which is consistent with hypothesis 1c. This is consistent with Dake’s study (28), which found that fatalists tend to believe that social problems are largely beyond their control. As a result, fatalists typically do not participate in individual waste sorting.

The relationship between environmental risk perception and waste sorting

Environmental risk perception is positively related to waste sorting, which supports hypothesis 2. This is in agreement with previous results (70). That is, the stronger the public’s perception of environmental risk, the more likely people are to engage in environmental protection activities and to participate more frequently in waste sorting (71). Individuals with a higher perception of environmental risk exhibit more pro-environmental behavior compared to those with a lower perception of environmental risk. And when people perceive environmental risks around them, they may be more likely to adjust their behavior to benefit the environment.

The mediating role of environmental risk perception

In this study, SEM was used to investigate how environmental risk perception mediates the relationship between waste sorting and the four cultural worldviews. In addition to directly influencing waste sorting, egalitarianism can also increase waste sorting by enhancing environmental risk perception, indicating that risk perception plays a significant role. This supports hypothesis 3a. CT suggests that individuals assess and respond to risk differently depending on their preferred social organizational structure or cultural worldviews (72). Egalitarianism may show greater fear of human-generated harm (reflected in larger positive effect sizes) than natural disasters because it provides a stronger basis for arguing for greater regulation and social reform to reduce social disparity (52). Not only is hierarchy directly related to waste sorting, but it can also increase waste sorting behavior by reducing environmental risk perceptions, suggesting that risk perceptions play a partial mediating role in this process. This supports hypothesis 3b. According to Xue et al. (53), hierarchies function to maintain power structures and disparities within society. Therefore, they may fail to recognize the dangers posed by this system. Even in the presence of high risk, hierarchical behavior is not significantly affected. The negative association between individualism and waste sorting is relatively insignificant but may increase waste sorting behavior by reducing environmental risk perceptions, suggesting that risk perceptions play a partial mediating role in this process. This supports hypothesis 3c. The preference for less regulation and more freedom may stem from the fact that individuals are less concerned about human-generated hazards than natural hazards (53). Individualistic perceptions of environmental risk are inhibited, leading to a decrease in waste-sorting behavior.

Limitations and future research

While this study contributes to our understanding of cultural worldviews in China, it has some limitations that should be acknowledged. First, urban dwellers in nine provinces and cities were sampled using convenience sampling, which may limit the generalizability of the results. Future research should consider expanding the sample to include both rural and urban dwellers from different regions of China. Second, this study relied primarily on self-reported measures, which may have introduced biases such as social desirability and recall bias. Future research should consider using alternative methods such as experience sampling and peer evaluation to supplement self-report measures. Third, Cronbach’s α coefficients for each of the four cultural worldviews were low, indicating unsatisfactory reliability. Future research can improve the validity and reliability of the study by conducting tests and making revisions to the items measuring the four cultural worldviews (64). Fourth, the dimensions of cultural worldviews may not be independent of each other, which is consistent with previous research in cultural theory. The results of our study should be replicated in future studies using other worldview scales, such as those developed by Kahan et al. (73). Additionally, it is important to consider that the worldview measures used in this study were developed in the United States and may not accurately reflect Chinese public opinion. Future studies should examine cross-cultural measurement invariance or develop new culturally appropriate measures.

Conclusion

Using RT and VBN theory, we have developed a theoretical framework that establishes a connection between cultural worldviews, environmental risk perception, and waste-sorting behavior among urban dwellers. The results showed that an increased perception of environmental risk contributes to the improvement of waste sorting practices in terms of egalitarianism. Reduced environmental risk perception is conducive to improving waste sorting practices and promoting individualism. We gained a deeper understanding of how urban dwellers sort their waste in this study. It also provides empirical evidence for the city manager to effectively intervene in waste sorting among urban dwellers.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by this research was approved by the Research Committee of Sichuan Institute of Industrial Technology. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

LC: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. QL: Writing – original draft. EW: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. ML: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. ST: Writing – review & editing, Formal analysis, Validation.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by the Sichuan Research Center for Applied Psychology Program (CSXL-202A01).

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank the editors and reviewers.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Kaza, S, Yao, LC, Bhada-Tata, P, and Woerden, FV. What a waste 2.0: a global snapshot of solid waste management to 2050. Washington, DC: The World Bank (2018).

Google Scholar

2. Zhao, Y, and Zhang, J. Literature mapping of waste sorting and recycling behavior research: a visual analysis using CiteSpace. Environ Sci Pollut Res. (2023) 30:67953–73. doi: 10.1007/s11356-023-27295-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Boulding, K . The economics of the coming spaceship earth In: H Jarrett , editor. Environmental quality in a growing economy. Baltimore, United States: Resources for the Future. Hohn Hopkins University Press (1966)

Google Scholar

4. Nevalainen, K . The use of industrial statistics to estimate the generation of recycled and waste residuals in Finland. Stat J U N Econ Comm Eur. (1985) 3:161–74. doi: 10.3233/SJU-1985-3203

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Wertz, KL . Economic factors influencing households production of refuse. J Environ Econ Manag. (1976) 2:263–72. doi: 10.1016/S0095-0696(76)80004-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Pi, T, Zheng, Y, and Xu, J. Why is garbage classification mandatory?—analysis of policy changes of municipal solid waste classification from the perspective of multi-source theory. Chin J Environmental Manag. (2021) 13:86–93. doi: 10.16868/j.cnki.1674-6252.2021.02.086

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Liu, Q, Xu, QH, Shen, X, Chen, BW, and Esfahani, SS. The mechanism of household waste sorting behaviour-a study of Jiaxing, China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:2447. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19042447

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Chen, F, Chen, H, Wu, M, Li, S, and Long, R. Research on the driving mechanism of waste separation behavior: based on qualitative analysis of Chinese urban residents. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:1859. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16101859

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Yang, S, Wei, J, and Cheng, P. Spillover of different regulatory policies for waste sorting: potential influence on energy-saving policy acceptability. Waste Manag. (2021) 125:112–21. doi: 10.1016/j.wasman.2021.02.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Wang, X, and Lin, L. The role of two social marketing strategies and communication design in Chinese households’ waste-sorting intentions and behavior: a theory of planned behavior approach. Sustain For. (2023) 15:5176. doi: 10.3390/su15065176

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Spencer, T . Risk perception: Theories and approaches. New York: Nova Science Publishers (2016).

Google Scholar

12. Ghazali, EM, Nguyen, B, Mutum, DS, and Yap, SF. Pro-environmental behaviours and value-belief-norm theory: assessing unobserved heterogeneity of two ethnic groups. Sustain For. (2019) 11:3237. doi: 10.3390/su11123237

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Maleki, A, and Hendriks, F. Grid, group, and grade: challenges in operationalizing cultural theory for cross-national research. Cross-Cult Res. (2015) 49:250–80. doi: 10.1177/1069397114555843

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Douglas, M . Purity and danger: An analysis of the concepts of pollution and taboo. London, UK: Routledge (1966).

Google Scholar

15. Douglas, M . Cultural bias. London, UK: Royal Anthropological Institute (1978).

Google Scholar

16. Douglas, M . Natural symbols: Explorations in cosmology. New York, NY: Pantheon (1982).

Google Scholar

17. Douglas, M, and Wildavsky, AB. Risk and culture: An essay on the selection of technical and environmental dangers. California: University of California (1982).

Google Scholar

18. Fardon, R . The faithful disciple: on Mary Douglas and Durkheim. Anthropol Today. (1987) 3:4–6. doi: 10.2307/3032886

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Fardon, R . Mary Douglas: An intellectual biography. London, UK: Routledge (1999).

Google Scholar

20. Fardon, R . Margaret Mary Douglas, 1921–2007. Proceed. Br. Acad. (2010) 166:135–58. doi: 10.5871/bacad/9780197264751.003.0007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Johnson, BB, and Swedlow, B. Cultural theory's contributions to risk analysis: a thematic review with directions and resources for further research. Risk Anal. (2021) 41:429–55. doi: 10.1111/risa.13299

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Chai, SK, Liu, M, and Kim, MS. Cultural comparisons of beliefs and values: applying the grid-group approach to the world values survey. Beliefs Values. (2009) 1:193–208. doi: 10.1891/1942-0617.1.2.193

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Yuan, M . Psychological perceptions and voluntary protective behaviors during COVID-19 pandemic in China: the roles of cultural worldview. Hum Ecol Risk Assess. (2022) 28:1–19. doi: 10.1080/10807039.2021.2023316

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kim, HK, and Kim, Y. Risk information seeking and processing about particulate air pollution in South Korea: the roles of cultural worldview. Risk Anal. (2018) 39:1071–87. doi: 10.1111/risa.13231

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Dake, K . Orienting dispositions in the perception of risk: an analysis of contemporary worldviews and cultural biases. J Cross-Cult Psychol. (1991) 22:61–82. doi: 10.1177/0022022191221006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Tansey, J, and O’Riordan, T. Cultural theory and risk: a review. Health Risk Soc. (1999) 1:71–90. doi: 10.1080/13698579908407008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Arpaci, I, Abdeljawad, T, Baloğlu, M, Kesici, Ş, and Mahariq, I. Mediating effect of internet addiction on the relationship between individualism and cyberbullying: cross-sectional questionnaire study. J Med Internet Res. (2020) 22:e16210. doi: 10.2196/16210

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Dake, K . Myths of nature: culture and the social construction of risk. J Soc Issues. (1992) 48:21–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1992.tb01943.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Dong, YY, Zou, DA, Liu, YX, and Ye, B. Study on characteristics of domestic waste in China and its comprehensive treatment technologies: taking Hangzhou city as an example. Acta Agricult Zhejiangensis. (2014) 28:1055–60.

Google Scholar

30. Fu, H, He, W, Feng, K, Guo, X, and Hou, C. Understanding consumers' willingness to pay for circular products: a multiple model-comparison approach. Sustain Product Consum. (2024) 45:67–78. doi: 10.1016/j.spc.2023.12.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Farahbakhsh, A, and Forghani, MA. Sustainable location and route planning with GIS for waste sorting centers, case study: Kerman, Iran. Waste Manag Res. (2019) 37:287–300. doi: 10.1177/0734242X18815950

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Nainggolan, D, Pedersen, AB, Smed, S, Zemo, KH, Hasler, B, and Termansen, M. Consumers in a circular economy: economic analysis of household waste sorting behaviour. Ecol Econ. (2019) 166:106402. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2019.106402

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Liuj, K, Li, YX, and Wang, ZS. The potential for carbon reduction in construction waste sorting: a dynamic simulation. Energy. (2023) 275:127477. doi: 10.1016/j.energy.2023.127477

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Yan, Q, Shen, HJ, and Hu, Y. Assessing the learning effects of host communications on the green knowledge and behavior of festival attendees—evidence from compulsory garbage sorting in China. Sustain For. (2021) 13:1839. doi: 10.3390/su13041839

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Hindsley, PR, and Morgan, OA. The role of cultural worldviews in willingness to pay for environmental policy. Environ Resour Econ. (2022) 81:1–27. doi: 10.1007/s10640-021-00622-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Douglas, M . Purity and danger: An analysis of concepts of pollution and taboo. London: Routledge (2002).

Google Scholar

37. Wang, X, Zhang, H, Wu, D, and Lv, X. Cultural influences on individual risk perception: cultural cognition theory's explanation. Adv Psychol Sci. (2017) 25:1251–60. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2017.01251

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Schwartz, SH . A theory of cultural values and some implications for work. Appl Psychol. (1999) 48:23–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.1999.tb00047.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Zeng, JJ, Jiang, MQ, and Yuan, M. Environmental risk perception, risk culture, and pro-environmental behavior. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:1750. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17051750

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Toorzani, AA, and Rassafi, AA. The effect of cultural values on pro-environmental attitude in the context of travel mode choice: a hierarchical approach. Transport Res F: Traffic Psychol Behav. (2022) 88:291–308. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2022.05.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Jung, J, and Cho, SY. How do individualism and collectivism influence pro-environmental purchasing behavior based on environmental self-identity? Sustain For. (2023) 15:16075. doi: 10.3390/su152216075

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Broers, VJV, Van Scharrenburg, M, Fredrix, L, Lataster, J, Löhr, AJ, and Jacobs, N. Individual and situational determinants of plastic waste sorting: an experience sampling method study protocol. BMC Psychol. (2021) 9:92. doi: 10.1186/s40359-021-00596-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Gong, YC, Li, Y, and Sun, Y. Waste sorting behaviors promote subjective well-being: a perspective of the self-nature association. Waste Manag. (2023) 157:249–55. doi: 10.1016/j.wasman.2022.12.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Ulrich, B, and Wang, W. From industrial society to risk society: questions of survival social structure and ecological enlightenment. Marxism Reality. (2003) 14:26–45. doi: 10.15894/j.cnki.cn11-3040/a.2003.03.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Cai, L, Mei, ZH, Fan, TL, Li, QJ, Yao, Y, Yan, HX, et al. Impact of risk perception about COVID-19 on the coping behavior of the elderly: mediating role of self-efficacy. Psychos Med Res. (2021) 3:61–8. doi: 10.53388/psmr2021-0620-044

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Zhang, C . The dilemma of public opinion in environmental risk regulation and its solution. J Central South Univ. (2021) 27:65–75. doi: 10.11817/j.issn.1672-3104.2021.01.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Blaylock, BK . Risk perception: evidence of an interactive process. J Bus Res. (1985) 13:207–21. doi: 10.1016/0148-2963(85)90027-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Maartensson, H, and Loi, NM. Exploring the relationships between risk perception, behavioural willingness, and constructive hope in pro-environmental behaviour. Environ Educ Res. (2021) 28:1–14. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2021.2015295

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Han, R, and Xu, J. A comparative study of the role of interpersonal communication, traditional media and social media in pro-environmental behavior: a China-based study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:1883. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17061883

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Dal Maso, L, Gianfagna, L, Maglione, F, and Lattanzi, N. Going green: Environmental risk management, market value and performance. Corp Soc Responsib Environ Manag. (2023) 31:122–32. doi: 10.1002/csr.2556

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Yang, J . The inflfluence of culture on Koreans’ risk perception. J Risk Res. (2015) 18:69–92. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2013.879490

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Kahan, DM . Cultural cognition as a conception of the cultural theory of risk In: S Roeser, R Hillerbrand, P Sandin, and M Petersen, editors. Handbook of risk theory: Epistemology, decision theory, ethics, and social implications of risk. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer (2012). 725–59.

Google Scholar

53. Xue, W, Hine, DW, Loi, NM, Thorsteinsson, EB, and Phillips, WJ. Cultural worldviews and environmental risk perceptions: a meta-analysis. J Environ Psychol. (2014) 40:249–58. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.07.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Chen, X, Orom, H, Kiviniemi, MT, Waters, EA, Schofield, E, Li, Y, et al. Cultural worldviews and perceived risk of colon cancer and diabetes. Health Risk Soc. (2020) 22:324–45. doi: 10.1080/13698575.2020.1827142

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Younge, SN, Salem, D, and Bybee, D. Risk revisited: the perception of HIV risk in a community sample of low-income African American women. J Black Psychol. (2008) 36:49–74. doi: 10.1177/0095798408320633

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Wynveen, CJ, Wynveen, BJ, and Sutton, SG. Applying the value-belief-norm theory to marine contexts: implications for encouraging pro-environmental behavior. Coast Manag. (2015) 43:84–103. doi: 10.1080/08920753.2014.989149

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Yuan, M, and Swedlow, B. Chinese cultural biases, value congruence, and support for and compliance with protective policies during the COVID-19 pandemic. Rev Policy Res. (2022) 39:449–71. doi: 10.1111/ropr.12515

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Annika, M, and Nordlund, JG. Effects of values, problem awareness, and personal norm on willingness to reduce personal car use. J Environ Psychol. (2003) 23:339–47. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(03)00037-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Lacroix, K, and Gifford, R. Psychological barriers to energy conservation behavior: the role of worldviews and climate change risk perception. Environ Behav. (2018) 50:749–80. doi: 10.1177/0013916517715296

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Han, H, Zhang, Z, and Peng, W. An analysis of the influence mechanism of social capital on Households' waste separation. J Zhejiang Univ. (2016) 46:164–79. doi: 10.3785/j.issn.1008-942X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Swedlow, B, Ripberger, JT, Liu, L, Silva, CL, Jenkins-Smith, H, and Johnson, BB. Construct validity of cultural theory survey measures. Soc Sci Q. (2020) 101:2332–83. doi: 10.1111/ssqu.12859

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Johnson, BB, and Swedlow, B. Comparing cultural theory and cultural cognition theory survey measures to each other and as explanations for judged risk. J Risk Res. (2020) 23:1278–1300. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2019.1646310

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Johnson, BB, Swedlow, B, and Mayorga, M. Cultural theory and cultural cognition theory survey measures: confirmatory factoring and predictive validity of factor scores for judged risk. J Risk Res. (2019) 23:1467–1490. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2019.1687577

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Yuan, M, Zeng, JJ, Swedlow, B, and Qi, R. Environmental concern among Chinese youth: the roles of knowledge and cultural bias. Environ Educ Res. (2022) 28:1472–89. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2022.2033705

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Wang, X . The influence path of public environmental risk perception on behavior choice. J Jishou Univ. (2019) 40:114–23. doi: 10.13438/j.cnki.jdxb.2019.04.013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Zhou, H, and Long, L. Statistical remedies for common method biases. Adv Psychol Sci. (2004) 6:942–50. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1671-3710.2004.06.018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Podsakoff, PM, Mackenzie, SB, Lee, JY, and Podsakoff, NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J Appl Psychol. (2003) 88:879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Parsons, M, and Lykins, AD. Cultural worldviews and the perception of natural hazard risk in Australia. Environ Hazards. (2022) 22:13–9. doi: 10.1080/17477891.2022.2050668

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Liu, H, Zhu, G, and Li, Y. Research on the impact of environmental risk perception and public participation on evaluation of local government environmental regulation implementation behavior. Environ Chall. (2021) 5:100213. doi: 10.1016/j.envc.2021.100213

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Yan, B, and Liu, T. Information service, information literacy and farmers' adoption of green control technology. J Arid Land Resour Environ. (2022) 36:46–52. doi: 10.13448/j.cnki.jalre.2022.118

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Steg, L, and Vlek, C. Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: an integrative review and research agenda. J Environ Psychol. (2009) 29:309–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.10.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Kemper, NP, Popp, JS, Nayga, RM, and Kerr, JB. Cultural worldview and genetically modified food policy preferences. Food Policy. (2018) 80:68–83. doi: 10.1016/j.foodpol.2018.09.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Kahan, DM, Braman, D, Gastil, J, Slovic, P, and Mertz, CK. Culture and identity-protective cognition: explaining the white male effect in risk perception. J Empir Leg Stud. (2007) 4:465–505. doi: 10.1111/j.1740-1461.2007.00097.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cultural worldview, waste sorting, environmental risk perception, urban dwellers, cultural theory

Citation: Cai L, Li Q, Wan E, Luo M and Tao S (2024) Cultural worldviews and waste sorting among urban Chinese dwellers: the mediating role of environmental risk perception. Front. Public Health. 12:1344834. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1344834

Received: 26 November 2023; Accepted: 18 March 2024;
Published: 05 April 2024.

Edited by:

Jingkuang Liu, Guangzhou University, China

Reviewed by:

Usman Talat, The University of Manchester, United Kingdom
Hanliang Fu, Xi'an University of Architecture and Technology, China

Copyright © 2024 Cai, Li, Wan, Luo and Tao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Siwen Tao, tswxnmd@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.