- 1College of Forestry and Grassland Science, Jilin Agricultural University, Changchun, China
- 2Jilin Provincial Key Laboratory of Tree and Grass Genetics and Breeding, College of Forestry and Grassland Science, Jilin Agricultural University, Changchun, China
The forest experience is good for people’s physical and mental health. However, few studies on the effects of pure forest based on the duration and way of experience on people’s physical and mental recovery. In this study, we took 180 first-year college students as research objects and conducted experiments in Pinus sylvestris and Betula platyphylla and the control group of grass plot. The changes of physiological and psychological indexes of the subjects were compared by two perception methods (onsite perception, video perception) and three perception duration (10 min, 20 min, 30 min). The results indicated that: (1) Differences between the two pure forests were mainly reflected in short-term recovery of diastolic blood pressure (DBP) and long-term recovery of total mood disorder (TMD). (2) Video perception was more conducive to short-term recovery of systolic blood pressure (SBP) and diastolic blood pressure (DBP). (3) Viewing the Pinus sylvestris for 20 min in different ways was the best way to relieve stress. It is suggested that, Pinus sylvestris can be used as the rehabilitation perception material, and reasonable path length or perception time can be selected for landscape construction in future. These results can provide scientific reference for landscape design based on forest health and environmental perception.
1 Introduction
With the progress and development of the times, the process of urbanization has shown an unprecedented dynamic trend. More than half of human beings live in urban areas (1). People encounter more and more psychological pressure from financial, family, occupational and other stressors (2–5). Increased stress may lead to a series of adverse reactions such as memory loss, attention loss, and insomnia (6–8), which affect people’s normal life and work, hinder people’s development, and even cause depression, diabetes, cancer, cardiac arrest and other diseases (9–12). The forest environment has been confirmed to have a positive impact on human physiological and psychological health, a short and leisurely forest trip, that is, “forest bathing,” can not only bring positive changes in physiological health for people living in urban environments, but also improve emotional anxiety and depression (13–15). Although scientific and technological progress has increased the way people perceive and experience nature, the opportunities and time for people to contact nature are still very limited, and the scientific and reliability of perception methods are still uncertain. The cross effects of perception method and perception durations is also inconclusive, which leads to the mismatch between the public’s demand for the natural environment and the actual landscape design (16). Therefore, it is necessary to study the scientific perceiving methods and the effective perceiving durations.
1.1 Perception of different forests
Studies have found that the natural environment can reduce stress, give people a sense of psychological satisfaction and stability, and even improve immunity and resist disease (17–20). Forests is an important part of the natural environment. The forests environment is naturally quiet. According to the theory of natural therapy, forests areas are beneficial to health and can relax the mood (21). Slow walking in the forest can relieve stress and bring mental stability (22, 23). Perception of forests landscape can improve mental state (24), help people free themselves from the pressure of daily living space (25–29). However, forests are not all the same, and the potential of forests to relieve stress cannot be evenly distributed among different forests (30). Sandro et al. confirmed in a study that coniferous forests have better stress relief for humans in winter (31). Wang et al. found that evergreen tree species have a significant effect on human psychological recovery in spring, autumn and winter (32). Different forests may lead to different restoration effects due to their vertical structure density, canopy closure and other structural differences within the forests (33–35). Therefore, it is of great significance to explore the effect of different forests on the physical and psychological recovery of human body for the construction of health forest with the goal of health care.
1.2 Perception in different methods
When people find that they can relieve stress and fatigue by contacting the forests environment, they begin to be more eager to perceive the forests landscape. However, the heavy pressure of work and life forces people to stay indoors for 70–90% of the day (36). Therefore, how to effectively perceive forests landscape has become a research hotspot in the field of forests therapy and environmental perception. Previous studies have shown that onsite perception and off-site perception are two main ways of landscape perception. Onsite perception means that the perception of subjects is affected by environmental factors and onsite landscape. Zhao et al. found that middle-aged and older adult people can obtain physiological and psychological benefits through onsite viewing of peony activities (37). Duan et al. have shown that onsite perception of plant communities has a greater healing effect on subjects than video appreciation of plant communities (38). Off-site perception means that the subject’s perception is completed indoors, mainly through visual perception images to achieve a healing effect, basically not affected by environmental factors. Wang et al. found that watching a neat bamboo shrub video can have a more beneficial physical and mental impact on the human body (39). Duan et al. showed that the use of perceptual plant communities alternately or superimposed with videos and photos can maximize the healing effect (40). Although the two perception methods have been widely used and studied, it is necessary to explore the differences between the perception methods of different forests because the research results of landscape perception are inconsistent, and there are few perception studies based on different forests.
1.3 Perception in different durations
The effect of landscape perception is not only affected by the way of perception, but also varies with the length of perception. Jo’s research showed that exposure to a green environment will have an immediate effect on physical and mental recovery, self-esteem and emotions show the greatest changes within 5 min (41). Ning et al.’s research showed that 5 min of short-term environmental exposure is beneficial to the physical and mental recovery of young people (42). A field experiment in Japan also proved that 15 min of observation and walking in the forest can induce physiological relaxation (43). An Australian study had shown that people who spend 30 min or more in the green space have a lower probability of depression and hypertension (44). Although studies had shown that long-term environmental exposure still has a positive effect on human health recovery, most of them are based on the periodic accumulation of short-term recovery effects (45–47). Therefore, the short-term perception of green environment on the physical and mental recovery benefits of visitors cannot be ignored, but the optimal time dose of short-term perception remains to be studied.
In view of this, this study completed the relationship between landscape perception and public health benefits of two pure forests (Pinus sylvestris, Betula platyphylla) and a control group (grass plot) by means of onsite perception and video perception, explored the differences in short-term physiological and psychological recovery based on different time doses, to provide scientific basis for future landscape perception and forest health evaluation. Our research aims to address the following issues:
(1) Are there differences of two pure forests on the recovery of physiological and psychological indicators?
(2) How do the perceiving methods affect the subjects’ physiological and psychological recovery?
(3) How does the interaction of perception methods and perception durations affect the physiological and psychological recovery of subjects?
2 Materials and methods
2.1 Sample space selection
Forests areas of China ranks first among 48 Asian countries (48), and pure forests are abundant. In this study, we focus on pure forests with high ornamental value that widely found in the Northeast China. The experimental plot is located on the campus of Jilin Agricultural University (43°51′N, 125°18′E) in Changchun City, Jilin Province, Northeast China. Previous studies have shown that understory space is more popular with visitors (49). Therefore, in the sample selection, we chose the Pinus sylvestris forests (PS), the Betula platyphylla forests (BP) and the Grass plot (GP) as the control group for study (Figure 1).
Figure 1. Map of experimental location and study area. (A) BP, Betula platyphylla. (B) PS, Pinus sylvestris. (C) GP, Grass plot.
In this study, we selected two stimulation methods, namely, onsite perception and video perception. Both onsite perception and video perception experiments were completed between 1 October 2022 and 7 October 2022. The onsite perception test was carried out in sunny weather and at a suitable temperature. Before the test, non-testers in the site were emptied, and the distance between the test area and the forest edge was ensured to be 8–10 meters, thereby reducing the interference of external factors. The video perception test material was filmed by the EOS70D Canon camera, which is carried out at 8:00–12:00 with sufficient light and sunny weather. To obtain a better immersive feeling for later viewing, two shooting methods are adopted: tripod fixed shooting and pan-tilt assisted walking shooting. A total of 16 videos were taken by the five-point sampling method. Under the guidance and advice of landscape design experts, these videos were edited, and finally the three spatial videos required for the experiment were selected for this experiment.
2.2 Selecting subjects and grouping
The subjects recruited in this experiment were first-year students from the College of Forestry and Grassland Science of Jilin Agricultural University. The freshmen are in a certain unknown state of the campus environment, which can minimize the experimental errors caused by familiarity with the experimental plots and exclude patients with heart disease or other medical history. Subjects were also told to forbid any strenuous physical activity, smoking, drinking throughout the experiment, before and during the experiment, who disagreed or did not want to continue participating in the experiment was excluded. All procedures in this study are consistent with the “Declaration of Helsinki.” The experiment initially recruited 185 participants, of which five participants failed to participate in the whole experiment due to the conflict of student community activities. A total of 180 subjects participated in the experiment. During the experiment, 180 subjects were randomly divided into six groups, which are“Onsite perception Pinus sylvestris forest (OPS), Video perception Pinus sylvestris forest (VPS), Onsite perception Betula platyphylla forest (OBP), Video perception Betula platyphylla forest (VBP), Onsite perception Grass plot (OGP) and Video perception Grass plot (VGP),” 30 subjects in each group, and 30 subjects were randomly divided into 3 teams, 10 subjects in each team, the experiments are organized in teams (Figure 2). In addition, through interviews and physiological and psychological indicators tests, we confirmed that the subjects were under the pressure of the new living environment and learning courses, and the initial values of the physiological indicators of each group were not significantly different through analysis of variance (Table 1).
2.3 Experimental design
2.3.1 Test introduction
We designed a random experiment to study the effects of two pure forests and control group on the physiological and psychological recovery of the subjects through the interaction between two perception methods (Onsite perception, Video perception) and three perception durations (10 min, 20 min, 30 min). Each group was randomly assigned to one of the six scenarios. Only one onsite perception team and one video perception team were tested within a single test day (Figure 2). Subjects were subjected to a stress test before entering the test plot or sensing video to induce mental stress, then experienced and relaxed in each scenery. To evaluate and compare their physiological and psychological changes based on different perception durations, we tested the subjects’ SBP, DBP, HR during the experiment for physiological recovery every 10 min and used the POMS scale to test the situation of psychological recovery at the same time.
2.3.2 Experimental process
2.3.2.1 Onsite perception
We choose a sunny day with pleasant temperature from 1 October 2022 to 7 October 2022 as the measurement day. In the preparation stage, the subjects were guided to enter the vicinity of the sample site (isolated test environment), and the test process was described. When the subjects returned to calm, they tested the baseline of physiological indicators and filled in the first part of the psychological index questionnaire, and then pressured the subjects to complete 5 min national English level 6 listening test. During this process, the test instrument was continuously worn. After the pressure was applied, SBP, DBP, and HR were tested, and the second part of the POMS scale was filled in (Ppressure). The subjects were guided to enter the sample environment to relax. The volunteers recorded the SBP, DBP and HR of the subjects every 10 min and guided the subjects to fill in the POMS scale again (P10, P20). After the onsite perception, the participants completed the third and last physiological index test and psychological scale filling (P30), and then removed the wrist sphygmomanometer, and the subjects left the test site.
2.3.2.2 Video perception
The video perception test and the onsite perception test were carried out at the same time because the indoor temperature in October was more comfortable, and the indoor environment with normal light and no visual interference was selected. The subjects wore wrist band sphygmomanometers and explained the experimental procedures and precautions. The subjects were taken to the designated experimental site, and the subjects began to test physiological indicators and fill in the first part of the questionnaire after the sit-in was stable, and then the subjects were pressured before watching the video. The subjects were asked to complete 5 min national English level 6 listening test. After the pressure was applied, the SBP, DBP and HR were tested, and the second part of the POMS scale psychological questionnaire was filled out (Ppressure). Volunteers began to play videos of the corresponding forest types, and the subjects watched the videos. During the 30 min viewing period, the volunteers recorded the SBP, DBP and HR of the volunteers every 10 min and guided the subjects to fill out the POMS scale again (P10, P20). After the subjects completed the third index test (P30), the experimental process was completed and the whole experiment was completed.
2.4 Measurement
2.4.1 Physiological measures
Biofeedback measurement was used to measure physiological changes, including diastolic blood pressure (DBP), systolic blood pressure (SBP) and heart rate (HR). During the test, the Sean wrist electronic sphygmomanometer was selected to collect the physiological indexes of the subjects. To minimize the measurement error, three repeated measurements were performed during the experimental data collection of the subjects, and the average value of the data was finally taken.
2.4.2 Psychological measures
The psychological test was based on the Chinese conventional POMS scale, and the reliability was between 0.60–0.82 (50). The scale is divided into three dimensions (Restlessness, Interested, Relaxed) and including 12 adjectives (Interest, Refresh, Energy, Vigor, Relax, Happy, Quiet, Calm, Insecure, Confusion, Sullen, Worry) describing mood and emotion as secondary indicators. From “not at all” to “very” rating scale, the subjects independently choose to describe their current emotions. Total mood disorder (TMD) is calculated according to the above three dimensions, namely TMD = Restlessness (Insecure + Confusion + Sullen + Worry) - Interested (Interest + Refresh + Energy + Vigor) – Relaxed (Relax + Happy + Quiet + Calm). The higher the value is, the worse the mental health is, and vice versa.
2.5 Data analysis
The agreement and data collection for this study were approved by Jilin Agricultural University Ethical Committee. All data were processed by SPSS 26.0 (IMB SPSS Statistics). Continuous data conforming to normal distribution or approximate normal distribution were statistically described by mean and standard deviation. The timeline is divided into 3 parts:
In the formula, P is the mean value of each index, Ppressure is the mean value of each index after pressure application, P10 is the mean value of each index after 10 min of recovery, P20 is the mean value of each index after 20 min of recovery, P30 is the mean value of each index after 30 min of recovery, △P is the change value of each index, P10 is the change value of each index after 10 min of recovery, P20 is the change value of each index after 20 min of recovery, P30 is the change value of each index after 30 min of recovery. When △P is negative, it indicates that the index after different recovery time is greater than the index after pressure. When △P is positive, it indicates that the index after different recovery time is less than the index after pressure.
One-way ANOVA was used to compare the differences between different forests. Paired Sample T Test was used to compare the changes of physiological and psychological indicators under different perception methods. One-way ANOVA and Two-ways repeated-measures ANOVA were used to compare the differences between different groups under the interaction of perception methods and perception durations. p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant in all comparisons.
3 Results
3.1 Effects of forests on health recovery
After visitors perceive different forests and grass plot, the recovery amounts of DBP and TMD is significantly different (Table 2). Further analysis shows that the perception of PS is the most effective for alleviating the DBP of subjects. The short-term healing effect of DBP on GP is better than that of BP. The TMD recovery amounts of PS and BP were significantly greater than GP. With the increase of perception time, the TMD recovery amounts of PS decreased, while that of BP was the opposite. The results show that PS has obvious advantages in the recovery of DBP and TMD, GP for the recovery of DBP is also very good, BP for the recovery of TMD showed potential.
3.2 Effects of perception methods on health recovery
The analysis of the recovery of each indicator after the subjects perceived the two pure forests and grass plot showed that the perception methods had a significant effect on the short-term recovery of SBP and DBP. It had no significant effects on the short-term recovery and long-term recovery of HR and TMD (Table 3).
Further analysis found that the SBP recovery effect of subjects’ video perception of different forests was better than that of onsite perception of different forests. For the recovery of DBP, video perception was more conducive to the recovery of DBP. The results show that video perception is conducive to the short-term recovery of physiological health, the perception method has no significant difference on the long-term recovery of physiological health and psychological stress relief.
3.3 Effects of the interaction of perception methods and perception durations on health recovery
3.3.1 Change of SBP indicator
The SBP change value of visitors after perceiving the forests landscape indicated that the interaction between perception methods and perception durations had a significant effect on the SBP recovery effect of visitors after 10 min (Table 4). By further pairwise comparison, it was found that there were significant differences between OBP and VBP (Table 5). △P10 of SBP were: VPS (8.73) > VGP (8.10) > VBP (7.56) > OPS (5.70) > OGP (4.97) > OBP (3.70), which indicated that video perception was more conducive to the rapid recovery of SBP, and the recovery effect of video watching PS was the best (Figure 3).
Table 4. Effects of the interaction of perception methods and perception durations on physiological and psychological indicators.
Table 5. Pairwise comparison between sites based on the significant effects of the interaction between perception methods and perception durations on SBP, DBP, and TMD.
Figure 3. Comparison of differences in physiological and psychological aspects with six scenarios. Recovery amounts of SBP (A), recovery amounts of DBP (B), recovery amounts of HR (C), and recovery amounts of TMD (D).
3.3.2 Change of DBP indicator
By analyzing the DBP change value of visitors after perceiving different forest types on the spot (Table 4), it was found that under the interaction of perception methods and perception durations, the DBP recovery effect of subjects in different forests was significantly different. Further analysis found that based on the different duration of short-term recovery, the DBP change of perceiving of the forests landscape were significant differences between VPS and VBP, OBP, OGP (Table 5). By comparing the amount of recovery found that subjects’ perception of VPS for 10 min and 20 min had the best effect on DBP recovery. The subjects’ perception of OPS for 30 min were significant different from VPS, OBP, VBP, OGP (Table 5). By comparing the amount of recovery found that 30 min perception of OPS had the best effect on DBP recovery. The results showed that VPS is conducive to the short-term recovery of subjects’ DBP, while OPS is more conducive to the long-term recovery of DBP (Figure 3).
3.3.3 Change of HR indicator
The HR changes of visitors after perceiving different forest types, showed that the interaction between perception methods and perception durations had no significant effect on the heart rate recovery effect of visitors (Table 4). This showed that under the same recovery time, there is no significant difference in heart rate recovery between the two forests and grass plot in any way (Figure 3).
3.3.4 Change of TMD indicator
By analyzing the TMD changes of the two pure forests and the grass plot (Table 4), it was found that the interaction between the perception methods and the perception durations had a significant effect on the TMD recovery of the subjects.
Further analysis showed that after the subjects perceived the two pure forests and the grass plot for 10 min, VPS was significant different from OPS, VBP, OGP, and VBP was significant different from OBP, VGP (Table 5). △P10 of the OGP was the most obvious. After 20 min of perception of the two forests and the grass plot, the recovery effect of VPS was significant different from VBP, OGP (Table 5). By comparing the amount of recovery (Table 4), the TMD recovery effect of VPS was the best, and the TMD recovery effect of OGP was significantly weakened. After 30 min of perception of the two forests and the grass plot, the TMD recovery effect of OPS and OBP was significant from OGP (Table 5), and the recovery amounts of OPS was the best (Table 4). The results showed that under the interaction of perception methods and perception durations, the influence and difference of pure forests on the recovery of TMD for subjects gradually appear with the increase of recovery time. Video perception showed the advantage of TMD recovery for subjects in a smaller time dose, but onsite perception needs more time for psychological recovery (Figure 3).
In summary, the difference in the pressure relief effect of the two pure forests and grass plot on the human body under the interaction of perception methods and perception durations is mainly reflected in the changes of physiological index DBP and psychological index TMD. The recovery effect of subjects’ DBP is better after 10 min of video perception of two forests, and the video perception grass plot also has a good recovery effect. The recovery advantage of forest for subjects’ DBP began to appear after 20 min of sensing forests, and the recovery effect of VPS was the best. For the TMD index, the recovery effect of video perception reached the best after 20 min, and the recovery effect of onsite perception showed a cumulative effect. With the increase of recovery time, the recovery effect was enhanced. The recovery effect of TMD was better after 20 min of video perception and 30 min of onsite perception.
4 Discussion
The purpose of this study is to explore the differences in human stress relief between different pure forests under the interaction of perception methods and perception durations, and to provide scientific basis for the construction of healthy forests and the evaluation of environmental perception preference in the future. Firstly, different forests have different effects on the physiological and psychological health recovery of subjects. PS is a good choice for subjects’ physiological and psychological recovery. Secondly, there is a big difference between the two methods of perception, and video perception is more conducive to the rapid relief of stress. Finally, no matter what kind of perception method is adopted, experiencing different forests under the perception duration of 20 min is more conducive to the physiological and psychological recovery of subjects.
4.1 Effects of forests on health recovery
After the visitors perceive the forests landscape, the physiological and psychological indicators show a downward trend, and the physiological and psychological health has been restored to a certain extent. This is consistent with the results of previous studies, which proves that the forest environment is conducive to the visitors to relieve stress (51, 52). At the same time, we also noticed that there were significant differences in the short-term recovery of DBP among subjects. This may be because the recovery of physiological indicators is controlled by parasympathetic nerves, and changes can appear quickly (53). As for the impact of different forests on the recovery effect, the recovery effect of PS is the best, followed by GP, and BP is the worst. It may be because the green environment can relieve people’s tension and anxiety and stabilize emotions, while the large area of white trunks in BP weakens the advantages of the green environment (54). The restoration effect of PS is better than that of GP, probably because people’s landscape preference for trees is higher than that of the grass plot (55). The results also show that there are significant differences in the long-term recovery of visitors’ TMD, which may be since the relief of psychological stress is not controlled by the nervous system, but depends on the mobilization of positive emotions, so the recovery has certain long-term benefits (56). It may also be that the subjects’ preference for green landscape leads to different effects of psychological stress relief, but the recovery advantage of BP for mental health gradually appears with the increase of recovery time. This conclusion is consistent with the previous research results (57).
4.2 Effects of perception methods on health recovery
The two perception methods have a positive effect on the physical and mental health of visitors and can relieve stress. This conclusion is in line with previous research results (38, 58). Previous studies have shown that there is no significant difference between non-immersive image perception and onsite perception for stress relief (59). However, this study found that there is a significant difference in the decompression difference between SBP and DBP in the early stage of perception between the two perception methods, and the effect of video perception on different forests is greater than that of onsite perception. This may be due to the fact that perceived short natural video can provide easy-to-obtain low-cost green landscape healing effect, which is helpful to improve physiological and psychological health (60). It may also be because video perception is a single visual stimulus. Onsite perception is the result of visual and other sensory factors, and single stimulation is more direct and rapid than multi-sensory stimulation (21). The long-term recovery of physiological health and the long-term and short-term recovery of psychological health are not different due to different perception methods. This result is consistent with the previous research conclusions. More than a certain length of time, the perception method is no longer the main reason affecting the physical and mental health recovery effect of visitors (61). Therefore, visitors who want to relief from stress quickly, short-term video perception of the forest landscape can provide better healing effects.
4.3 Effects of the interaction between perception methods and perception durations on health recovery
4.3.1 Onsite perception in 3 durations
The physiological indications of perceiving different forests and grass plot at 10 min, 20 min, and 30 min were recovered to a certain extent, and the recovery of OPS and OBP showed an upward trend, and the recovery of OGP showed a downward trend. This may be due to the absorption of CO2 and the release of O2 by photosynthesis of green plants. The secondary metabolites (VOCs) produced in the reaction process can reduce blood pressure and heart rate, while the photosynthesis of trees is higher than that of grass plot. Therefore, the recovery effect of OPS and OBP on physiological indexes is better than that of OGP in three recovery durations (62). For the recovery of SBP and DBP after onsite perception, the recovery of OPS under three different recovery durations showed obvious advantages, and the recovery of SBP at 30 min reached the highest, and the recovery of DBP at 20 min was the best. This may be due to the fact that PS has a denser crown, and the high-density crown can be more fully perceived by subjects, which has a better effect on human stress relief (63). Previous studies have shown that not any length of forest bath has a significant effect on human stress relief, because secondary metabolites need to accumulate to a certain amount to restore physiological health (64). This is consistent with the conclusion of this study. The OPS for 20 min–30 min is more conducive to human physiological health. Subjects onsite perception of different forests and grass plot after 10 min, 20 min, 30 min, the psychological pressure has a certain degree of relief, which is in line with the previous research conclusions, people have subjective expectations for the forest environment, subjective expectations to mobilize the perception of emotions, and thus achieve the effect of psychological healing (65). The recovery difference of the TMD onsite perception of the grass plot for 10 min is higher than that of the forests. This may be due to the fact that although the high-density forest space is private, the psychological security of the strange environment visitors is reduced, and the lookout attribute of the grass plot is magnified (66). Therefore, after people perceive 10 min on the spot, the grass plot can make people freer from tension and anxiety. With the increase of time, the sense of shelter of trees appeared, so the recovery difference of TMD indicator of OPS and OBP was higher than that of OGP.
4.3.2 Video perception in 3 durations
The physiological indexes of 10 min, 20 min, and 30 min of video perception of different forests and grass plot have a certain degree of recovery. For the three indexes of SBP, DBP and HR, the recovery difference of VPS and VGP in different duration is higher than that of VBP. This may be since people appreciate different forests through video in the indoor environment, visual stimulation is the only way of perception, and people’s preference for landscape elements such as green and sky is prominent. Therefore, VPS and VGP are most conducive to the recovery of physiological health (67). With the increase of perception time, the recovery trend of each physiological index decreases. It may be due to long time watching the screen, resulting in physical fatigue. The TMD recovery amounts of different forests and grass plot were restored after 10 min, 20 min, and 30 min of video perception, and the effect of VPS and VGP on relieving psychological pressure was still better than that of VBP. This may be the result of the long-term effect of psychological recovery and the visual preference of visitors, and the psychological recovery also showed a downward trend after 30 min of video perception, indicating that the fatigue caused by video perception was not only visual, but also caused psychological boredom.
5 Limitations
This study examined the effects of two pure forests on the physiological and psychological health of the subjects under the interaction of perception methods and perception durations. Although we have made careful preparations, there are still some shortcomings in this study. Firstly, the subjects are college students. The number and age composition of the subjects have limitations. The research results cannot represent all kinds of social groups. In the future, people of different ages, different genders and different health conditions can be further studied. Secondly, there are only two pure forests in the study, and the seasonal changes of forests are not considered. The perceived preference of the subjects may affect the results of the study. Therefore, in the future research, the number of pure forests can be increased, and the experiment can be carried out in different seasons. Finally, future research can also consider adding more sophisticated instruments, such as EEG, skin inductors, etc., to facilitate more scientific and multidimensional display of physiological and psychological recovery effects.
6 Conclusion
This study explored the effects of different pure forests on the recovery of people’s physiological and psychological health through different perception methods and different durations of short-term recovery, aiming to provide scientific support for the construction of healthy forests and environmental perception evaluation. The results show that, firstly, the forest environment is conducive to people’s physiological and psychological recovery, and the PS shows obvious recovery advantages. Secondly, onsite perception and video perception have their own advantages in short recovery effect. Video perception is more conducive to the short-term recovery of SBP and DBP. Onsite perception shows potential for the long-term recovery of TMD. With the increase of recovery time, the recovery effect of the two methods tends to be consistent. Finally, based on the perception of forest environment at different duration, DBP and TMD showed significant differences among two pure forests. The recovery of DBP reached the maximum after 20 min of perception of PS in different ways. The perception of OPS showed a cumulative effect, and the recovery amount was the largest at 30 min. The recovery amount of TMD reached the maximum after 20 min of VPS, and gradually lost the advantage of forests with the increase of recovery time. Last not the least, based on the above conclusions, using different perception methods to perceive PS for 20 min has a better recovery effect.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Author contributions
WN: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Writing – original draft. QL: Visualization, Writing – original draft. JY: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. JH: Investigation, Writing – original draft. YZ: Formal analysis, Writing – original draft. XZ: Resources, Writing – original draft. MS: Data curation, Writing – original draft. XS: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was supported by Jilin Provincial Natural Science Foundation, grant number 20210101039JC.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Lederbogen, F, Kirsch, P, Haddad, L, Streit, F, Tost, H, Schuch, P, et al. City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature. (2011) 474:498–501. doi: 10.1038/nature10190
2. Dudzic-Gyurkovich, K . Urban development and population pressure: the case of Młynówka Królewska Park in Krakow, Poland. Sustain For. (2021) 13:1–25. doi: 10.3390/su13031116
3. Teychenne, M, Stephens, LD, Costigan, SA, Olstad, DL, Stubbs, B, and Turner, AI. The association between sedentary behaviour and indicators of stress: a systematic review. BMC Public Health. (2019) 19:1357–15. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-7717-x
4. Ahmed, MZ, Ahmed, O, Aibao, Z, Hanbin, S, Siyu, L, and Ahmad, A. Epidemic of COVID-19 in China and associated psychological problems. Asian J Psychiatr. (2020) 51:102092. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102092
5. Gutowski, E, White, AE, Liang, B, Diamonti, A-J, and Berado, D. How stress influences purpose development: the importance of social support. J Adolesc Res. (2017) 33:571–97. doi: 10.1177/0743558417737754
6. De Vente, W, Olff, M, Van Amsterdam, JGC, Kamphuis, JH, and Emmelkamp, PMG. Physiological differences between burnout patients and healthy controls blood pressure, heart rate, and cortisol responses. Occup Environ Med. (2003) 60:54i–561i. doi: 10.2307/27732132
7. Potter, GG, Hartman, M, and Ward, T. Perceived stress and everyday memory complaints among older adult women. Anxiety Stress Coping. (2009) 22:475–81. doi: 10.1080/10615800802449610
8. Dolling, A, Nilsson, H, and Lundell, Y. Stress recovery in forest or handicraft environments – An intervention study. Urban For Urban Green. (2017) 27:162–72. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2017.07.006
9. Batelaan, NM, Seldenrijk, A, van den Heuvel, OA, van Balkom, AJLM, Kaiser, A, Reneman, L, et al. Anxiety, mental stress, and sudden cardiac arrest: epidemiology, possible mechanisms and future research. Front Psych. (2022) 12:813518. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.813518
10. Kamarck, T, and Jennings, JR. Biobehavioral factors in sudden cardiac death. Psychol Bull. (1991) 109:42–75. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.1.42
11. Albert, CM, Chae, CU, Rexrode, KM, Manson, JE, and Kawachi, I. Phobic anxiety and risk of coronary heart disease and sudden cardiac death among women. Circulation. (2005) 111:480–7. doi: 10.1161/01.Cir.0000153813.64165.5d
12. Kawachi, I, Sparrow, D, Vokonas, PS, and Weiss, ST. Symptoms of anxiety and risk of coronary heart disease. The normative aging study. Circulation. (1994) 90:2225–9. doi: 10.1161/01.Cir.90.5.2225
13. Vujcic, M, Tomicevic-Dubljevic, J, Zivojinovic, I, and Toskovic, O. Connection between urban green areas and visitors’ physical and mental well-being. Urban For Urban Green. (2019) 40:299–307. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2018.01.028
14. Li, Q, Kobayashi, M, Kumeda, S, Ochiai, T, Miura, T, Kagawa, T, et al. Effects of Forest bathing on cardiovascular and metabolic parameters in middle-aged males. Evid Based Complement Alternat Med. (2016) 2016:1–7. doi: 10.1155/2016/2587381
15. Chun, MH, Chang, MC, and Lee, S-J. The effects of forest therapy on depression and anxiety in patients with chronic stroke. Int J Neurosci. (2017) 127:199–203. doi: 10.3109/00207454.2016.1170015
16. Sevenant, M, and Antrop, M. Landscape representation validity: a comparison between on-site observations and photographs with different angles of view. Landsc Res. (2011) 36:363–85. doi: 10.1080/01426397.2011.564858
17. Bratman, GN, Daily, GC, Levy, BJ, and Gross, JJ. The benefits of nature experience: improved affect and cognition. Landsc Urban Plan. (2015) 138:41–50. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.02.005
18. Jiang, B, Chang, C-Y, and Sullivan, WC. A dose of nature: tree cover, stress reduction, and gender differences. Landsc Urban Plan. (2014) 132:26–36. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2014.08.005
19. Berman, MG, Kross, E, Krpan, KM, Askren, MK, Burson, A, Deldin, PJ, et al. Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. J Affect Disord. (2012) 140:300–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.03.012
20. Mennis, J, Mason, M, and Ambrus, A. Urban greenspace is associated with reduced psychological stress among adolescents: a geographic ecological momentary assessment (GEMA) analysis of activity space. Landsc Urban Plan. (2018) 174:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2018.02.008
21. Bielinis, E, Takayama, N, Boiko, S, Omelan, A, and Bielinis, L. The effect of winter forest bathing on psychological relaxation of young polish adults. Urban For Urban Green. (2018) 29:276–83. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2017.12.006
22. Lee, D-G, Lee, M-M, Jeong, Y-M, Kim, J-G, Yoon, Y-K, and Shin, W-S. Influence of Forest visitors’ perceived Restorativeness on social–psychological stress. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:6328–8. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18126328
23. Li, Q . Forest bathing enhances human natural killer activity and expression of anti-cancer proteins. Int J Immunopathol Pharmacol. (2007) 20:3–8. doi: 10.1177/03946320070200s202
24. Kobayashi, H, Ikei, H, Song, C, Kagawa, T, and Miyazaki, Y. Comparing the impact of forest walking and forest viewing on psychological states. Urban For Urban Green. (2021) 57:126920. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2020.126920
25. Sonntag-Öström, E, Nordin, M, Lundell, Y, Dolling, A, Wiklund, U, Karlsson, M, et al. Restorative effects of visits to urban and forest environments in patients with exhaustion disorder. Urban For Urban Green. (2014) 13:344–54. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2013.12.007
26. Morita, E, Fukuda, S, Nagano, J, Hamajima, N, Yamamoto, H, Iwai, Y, et al. Psychological effects of forest environments on healthy adults: Shinrin-yoku (forest-air bathing, walking) as a possible method of stress reduction. Public Health. (2007) 121:54–63. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2006.05.024
27. Sop, SW, Sik, YP, Wha, YR, and Seob, SC. Forest experience and psychological health benefits the state of the art and future prospect in Korea. Environ Health Prev Med. (2010) 15:38–47. doi: 10.1007/s12199-009-0114-9
28. Park, B-J, Furuya, K, Kasetani, T, Takayama, N, Kagawa, T, and Miyazaki, Y. Relationship between psychological responses and physical environments in forest settings. Landsc Urban Plan. (2011) 102:24–32. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2011.03.005
29. Song, C, Ikei, H, Kobayashi, M, Miura, T, Li, Q, Kagawa, T, et al. Effects of viewing forest landscape on middle-aged hypertensive men. Urban For Urban Green. (2017) 21:247–52. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2016.12.010
30. Grilli, G, and Sacchelli, S. Health benefits Derived from Forest: a review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:6125. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17176125
31. Sacchelli, S, Grilli, G, Capecchi, I, Bambi, L, Barbierato, E, and Borghini, T. Neuroscience application for the analysis of cultural ecosystem services related to stress relief in Forest. Forests. (2020) 11:190. doi: 10.3390/f11020190
32. Wang, R, and Zhao, J. Effects of evergreen trees on landscape preference and perceived restorativeness across seasons. Landsc Res. (2020) 45:649–61. doi: 10.1080/01426397.2019.1699507
33. Stoltz, J, Lundell, Y, Skärbäck, E, van den Bosch, MA, Grahn, P, Nordström, E-M, et al. Planning for restorative forests: describing stress-reducing qualities of forest stands using available forest stand data. Eur J For Res. (2016) 135:803–13. doi: 10.1007/s10342-016-0974-7
34. Nielsen, AB, Heyman, E, and Richnau, G. Liked, disliked and unseen forest attributes: relation to modes of viewing and cognitive constructs. J Environ Manag. (2012) 113:456–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvman.2012.10.014
35. Tomao, A, Secondi, L, Carrus, G, Corona, P, Portoghesi, L, and Agrimi, M. Restorative urban forests: exploring the relationships between forest stand structure, perceived restorativeness and benefits gained by visitors to coastal Pinus pinea forests. Ecol Indic. (2018) 90:594–605. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolind.2018.03.051
36. Chen, J, and Xiao, L. Research and application of intelligent household environment monitoring system. Glob Electr China. (2011) 11:62–5.(in Chinese with English abstract). doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1006-7604.2011.11.054
37. Zhao, R-L, Zhang, G, Wang, X, Zhang, B-T, Guo, L-N, Niu, L-X, et al. Psycho-physiological effects of a Peony-viewing program on middle-aged and elderly individuals at different Phenological stages. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:439–9. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16030439
38. Duan, Y, and Li, S. Effects of plant communities on human physiological recovery and emotional reactions: a comparative onsite survey and photo elicitation study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:721–1. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19020721
39. Wang, Y, Jiang, M, Huang, Y, Sheng, Z, Huang, X, Lin, W, et al. Physiological and psychological effects of watching videos of different durations showing urban bamboo forests with varied structures. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:3434. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17103434
40. Yifan, D, and Shuhua, L. Effects of viewing forms of plant community landscapes on human physiological responses. Landscape Architect. (2022) 29:105–11.(in Chinese with English abstract). doi: 10.14085/j.fjyl.2022.03.0105.07
41. Barton, J, and Pretty, J. What is the best dose of nature and green exercise for improving mental health a multi-study analysis. Environ Sci Technol. (2010) 44:3947–55. doi: 10.1021/es903183r
42. Ning, W, Yin, J, Chen, Q, and Sun, X. Effects of brief exposure to campus environment on students' physiological and psychological health. Front Public Health. (2023) 11:1051864. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1051864
43. Park, BJ, Tsunetsugu, Y, Kasetani, T, Kagawa, T, and Miyazaki, Y. The physiological effects of Shinrin-yoku (taking in the forest atmosphere or forest bathing): evidence from field experiments in 24 forests across Japan. Environ Health Prev Med. (2009) 15:18–26. doi: 10.1007/s12199-009-0086-9
44. Shanahan, DF, Bush, R, Gaston, KJ, Lin, BB, Dean, J, Barber, E, et al. Health benefits from nature experiences depend on dose. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:28551. doi: 10.1038/srep28551
45. Zhou, C, Yan, L, Yu, L, Wei, H, Guan, H, Shang, C, et al. Effect of short-term Forest bathing in urban parks on perceived anxiety of young-adults: a pilot study in Guiyang, Southwest China. Chinese Geogr Sci. (2018) 29:139–50. doi: 10.1007/s11769-018-0987-x
46. Zeng, C, Lyu, B, Deng, S, Yu, Y, Li, N, Lin, W, et al. Benefits of a three-day bamboo Forest therapy session on the physiological responses of university students. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:3238. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17093238
47. Kim, JG, Khil, TG, Lim, Y, Park, K, Shin, M, and Shin, WS. The psychological effects of a campus Forest therapy program. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:3409. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17103409
48. Chen, H, Hong, W, Lin, S, and Liu, J. Comparison of forest resources in Asian countries based on the global Forest resources assessment project. For Sci Technol. (2023) 5:9–14. (in Chinese with English abstract). doi: 10.13456/j.cnki.lykt.2022.06.13.0003
49. Zhang, T, Zhang, W, Meng, H, and Zhang, Z. Analyzing visitors’ preferences and evaluation of satisfaction based on different attributes, with Forest trails in the Akasawa National Recreational Forest, Central Japan. Forests. (2019) 10:431. doi: 10.3390/f10050431
50. Beili, Z . Brief introduction of POMS scale and its model for China. J Tianjin Univ Sport. (1995) 10:35–7.(in Chinese with English abstract). doi: 10.13297/j.cnki.issn1005-0000
51. Jones, MV, Gidlow, CJ, Hurst, G, Masterson, D, Smith, G, Ellis, N, et al. Psycho-physiological responses of repeated exposure to natural and urban environments. Landsc Urban Plan. (2021) 209:104061. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2021.104061
52. Kobayashi, H, Song, C, Ikei, H, Park, B-J, Kagawa, T, and Miyazaki, Y. Combined effect of walking and Forest environment on salivary cortisol concentration. Front Public Health. (2019) 7:376. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2019.00376
53. Hartig, T, Evans, GW, Jamner, LD, Davis, DS, and Gärling, T. Tracking restoration in natural and urban field settings. J Environ Psychol. (2003) 23:109–23. doi: 10.1016/s0272-4944(02)00109-3
54. Park, B-J, Tsunetsugu, Y, Ishii, H, Furuhashi, S, Hirano, H, Kagawa, T, et al. Physiological effects of Shinrin-yoku (taking in the atmosphere of the forest) in a mixed forest in Shinano town, Japan. Scand J For Res. (2008) 23:278–83. doi: 10.1080/02827580802055978
55. Kenwick, RA, Shammin, MR, and Sullivan, WC. Preferences for riparian buffers. Landsc Urban Plan. (2009) 91:88–96. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2008.12.005
56. Mayer, FS, Frantz, CM, Bruehlman-Senecal, E, and Dolliver, K. Why is nature beneficial? Environ Behav. (2008) 41:607–43. doi: 10.1177/0013916508319745
57. Guan, H, Wei, H, He, X, Ren, Z, and An, B. The tree-species-specific effect of forest bathing on perceived anxiety alleviation of young-adults in urban forests. Ann For Res. (2017):327–41. doi: 10.15287/afr.2017.897
58. Jo, S-H, Park, J-S, and Yeon, P-S. The effect of Forest video using virtual reality on the stress reduction of university students focused on C University in Korea. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:12805. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182312805
59. Gao, T, Liang, H, Chen, Y, and Qiu, L. Comparisons of landscape preferences through three different perceptual approaches. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:4754. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16234754
60. Moscoso, C, Matusiak, B, Svensson, UP, and Orleanski, K. Analysis of stereoscopic images as a new method for daylighting studies. ACM Trans Appl Percept. (2015) 11:1–13. doi: 10.1145/2665078
61. Wu, L, Luo, S, Li, D, Chen, Q, Li, J, and Wen, J. Effects of deciduous forests on adolescent emotional health in urban areas: An example from the autumn Ginkgo Forest in Chengdu. Forests. (2023) 14:1099. doi: 10.3390/f14061099
62. Song, X, Li, H, Li, C, Xu, J, and Hu, D. Effects of VOCs from leaves of Acer truncatum Bunge and Cedrus deodara on human physiology and psychology. Urban For Urban Green. (2016) 19:29–34. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2016.06.021
63. Gerstenberg, T, and Hofmann, M. Perception and preference of trees: a psychological contribution to tree species selection in urban areas. Urban For Urban Green. (2016) 15:103–11. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2015.12.004
64. An, B-Y, Wang, D, Liu, X-J, Guan, H-M, Wei, H-X, and Ren, Z-B. The effect of environmental factors in urban forests on blood pressure and heart rate in university students. J For Res. (2018) 24:27–34. doi: 10.1080/13416979.2018.1540144
65. Köteles, F, Babulka, P, Szemerszky, R, Dömötör, Z, and Boros, S. Inhaled peppermint, rosemary and eucalyptus essential oils do not change spirometry in healthy individuals. Physiol Behav. (2018) 194:319–23. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.06.022
66. Lis, A, and Iwankowski, P. Why is dense vegetation in city parks unpopular? The mediative role of sense of privacy and safety. Urban For Urban Green. (2021) 59:126988. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2021.126988
Keywords: forests bathing, physiological health, psychological health, perception methods, perception durations
Citation: Sun X, Li Q, Zhang X, Sun M, Yin J, He J, Zhong Y and Ning W (2024) Physiological and psychological recovery in two pure forests: interaction between perception methods and perception durations. Front. Public Health. 12:1296714. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1296714
Edited by:
Álvaro I. Langer, San Sebastián University, ChileReviewed by:
Quan Qiu, South China Agricultural University, ChinaChanjuan Sun, University of Shanghai for Science and Technology, China
Qinghua Lei, Shenzhen University, China
Copyright © 2024 Sun, Li, Zhang, Sun, Yin, He, Zhong and Ning. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Wei Ning, bmluZ3dlaUBqbGF1LmVkdS5jbg==
†These authors have contributed equally to this work