Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 20 February 2024
Sec. Public Health Education and Promotion

A study on the factors influencing the intention to receive booster shots of the COVID-19 vaccine in China based on the information frame effect

Qizhen Zhu,Qizhen Zhu1,2Yunyun GaoYunyun Gao1Qingyuan HuQingyuan Hu3DeHua Hu
DeHua Hu1*Xusheng Wu
Xusheng Wu4*
  • 1Department of Biomedical Informatics, School of Life Sciences, Central South University, Changsha, China
  • 2School of Medical Information Engineering, Jining Medical University, Rizhao, China
  • 3Third Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
  • 4Shenzhen Health Development Research and Data Management Center, Shenzhen, Guangdong, China

Introduction: In the response to and prevention and control of the Novel coronavirus pneumonia, the COVID-19 vaccine does not provide lifelong immunity, and it is therefore important to increase the rate of booster shots of the COVID-19 vaccine. In the field of information health science, research has found that information frames have an impact in changing individual attitudes and health behaviors.

Objective: This study focuses on the effects of different influencing factors on the public’s willingness to receive the booster shots of the COVID-19 vaccine under two information frameworks.

Methods: An online questionnaire was conducted to explore the effects of demographic characteristics, personal awareness, social relationships, risk disclosure, perceived booster vaccination protection rate, and duration of protection under the assumption of an information framework. T test and one-way analysis were used to testing the effect of variables.

Results: (1) The persuasion effect under the gain frame is higher than that under the loss frame (B = 0.863 vs. B = 0.746); (2) There was no significant difference in subjects’ intention of booster vaccination in terms of gender, age, income, occupation, educational background and place of residence. Whether family members received booster vaccination was strongly correlated with their intention of vaccination under the loss framework (p = 0.017, M = 4.63, SD = 0.664). (3) The higher the understanding of COVID-19, the higher the degree of compliance with the government’s COVID-19 prevention and control measures, and the higher the willingness to strengthen vaccination; (4) Risk disclosure has a significant impact on people’s willingness to receive COVID-19 booster shots (M = 2.48, under the loss framework; M = 2.44, under the gain framework); (5) Vaccine protection rate and duration of protection have an impact on people’s willingness to vaccinate. Increased willingness to vaccinate when the protection rate of booster vaccine approaches 90% (M = 4.76, under the loss framework; M = 4.68, under the gain framework). When the vaccine protection period is 2 years, people are more willing to receive a booster vaccine; and the willingness to receive a booster shot is stronger under the loss framework (M = 4.60, SD = 0.721, p = 0.879).

Conclusion: The impact of the information framework on COVID-19 vaccination intentions is different, and the disclosure of relevant health information should focus on the impact of the information framework and content on the public’s behavior toward strengthening vaccination. Therefore, in the face of public health emergencies, public health departments, healthcare institutions, and other sectors can consider adopting the Gainful Information Framework tool to disseminate health information to achieve better persuasion and promote public health behavior change enhancing public health awareness, and promoting universal vaccination.

1 Introduction

Novel coronavirus pneumonia (COVID-19), named “2019 coronavirus disease” by the World Health Organization, refers to pneumonia caused by the new 2019 coronavirus (severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2, SARS-Co V-2) infection causing pneumonia (1, 2). China Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) suggests that the new coronavirus variant currently circulating globally is still highly infectious (3). To cope with and prevent and control new coronavirus pneumonia, experts have developed the COVID-19 vaccine, which is a vaccine against the new coronavirus. However, the COVID-19 vaccine does not provide lifelong immunity, and the immune function decreases significantly after 6 months of vaccination, while the antibody level increases significantly after 6 months of the second dose of vaccine and then the booster shot, which increases more than 10 times the original level, and remains at a relatively high level after another 6 months of vaccination. And despite the fact that COVID-19 vaccination has been underway for an extended period, over 50% of the global population remains unvaccinated (4). On 13 December 2022, the Comprehensive Group of the Joint Prevention and Control Mechanism of the State Council issued the Implementation Plan for the Second Dose of Booster Immunization with the New Coronavirus Vaccine, which called for the active implementation of booster vaccination with the New Coronavirus Vaccine in China. How to better appeal to the public to receive booster shots and increase the public’s willingness to be vaccinated have become hot topics (5).

In the field of information health science, some research has found that information frames have an impact on changing individual attitudes and health behaviors. The concept of “framing effects” was first introduced by Kahneman and Tversky with the help of the Asian disease problem, where the risk preferences of individuals often depend on the way the problem is framed (6). Rothman et al. (7) suggest, based on an extensive review and evidence from previous studies, that the loss frame in disease detection behavior is a good indicator of the risk of disease. Proposed that the loss framework is more persuasive in disease detection behaviors, while the gain framework is more persuasive in disease prevention behaviors. Lee et al. (8) demonstrated through six experiments that the gain framework is more persuasive when information is dedicated to promoting behavior, and the loss framework is more persuasive when information is designed to prevent a phenomenon. These studies mentioned that information framing can have an impact on the public’s willingness and that the effects of gain and loss framing are different.

The proposed information framework provides a new theoretical perspective for the study of information behavior and a new research idea for exploring the role of information in influencing behavioral decisions. It was found that the gain frame has a stronger persuasive effect than the loss frame in prevention-biased domains such as using sunscreen to prevent skin cancer (9, 10) and quitting smoking (1113). In contrast, vaccination is a disease prevention behavior in which the gain frame has a stronger persuasive effect than the loss frame, i.e., the gain frame has a stronger effect on people’s willingness to vaccinate (14). So, to explore the impact of information frameworks on the public’s willingness to receive COVID-19 vaccine booster shots and to understand the key factors influencing the willingness to receive COVID-19 vaccine booster shots, this paper will explore the impact of demographic characteristics, personal awareness, social relationships, risk disclosure, perceived vaccine efficacy, and duration of protection on the willingness to receive COVID-19 vaccine booster shots under different information frameworks.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Investigation method

This study investigated the effect of information frames on people’s willingness to receive COVID-19 vaccine booster shots and was conducted by the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. Two different types of questionnaires (loss frame (Paper A) and gain frame (Paper B)) were distributed through an online questionnaire platform, and links to the questionnaires were distributed to participants through social software such as WeChat and QQ. The questionnaire stated “If you have already filled out the volume A, please do not fill out the volume B” to exclude the possibility of cross-filling.

The questionnaire first introduced the current status and progress of epidemic prevention and control in China and the concept and role of booster shots (booster immunization). The questionnaire consisted of three main parts, firstly, basic information about the subject, such as gender, age, occupation, place of residence, education level, income, and the subject’s and subject’s family members’ information about the COVID-19 vaccination booster shot and knowledge about COVID-19 and compliance with government COVID-19 prevention and control measures. In the second part, the questionnaire provided four stimulus messages designed according to the characteristics of gain and loss frames, and subjects continued to respond to the intention to receive the COVID-19 vaccine booster after carefully reading the stimulus messages. In the last part, subjects continued to read and respond to questions about COVID-19 vaccine booster effectiveness, side effects, duration of protection, and family member vaccination rates.

All participants gave informed consent and volunteered to participate in this study. Individuals with mental illness or cognitive impairment and those who refused to participate in the study were excluded (When investigating the basic information of the subject, there are relevant questions asking whether he/she has a mental illness or not) (15).

We distributed questionnaires nationwide from January 2023 until June 2023.A combination of random distribution and snowball sampling for online survey distribution to expand the sample size. 594 questionnaires were sent out, 540 valid questionnaires were obtained, after removing invalid questionnaires, and the response rate was 90.45%. Including 273 questionnaires based on the gain framework and 267 questionnaires based on the loss framework.The collected data were analyzed by SPSS 21.0, and the Cronbach’s alpha values of the two groups were 0.769 and 0.753, both greater than 0.7, which proved that the questionnaire had good reliability, and the KMO values were 0.751 and 0.765, both greater than 0.6, which proved that the questionnaire had good validity and could be analyzed formally.

2.2 Variable measurement and statistical analysis

This study involved measures of intention to vaccinate against COVID-19 and in various framing scenarios, all of which used the Likert 5 scale, the higher the value, the higher the effect of information framing, and the higher the intention to vaccinate against COVID-19. All measures of relevant factors were adapted from existing studies and finalized as follows.

1. The vaccination intention for COVID-19 was adapted from Fishbein and Ajzen (16)“What is your intention for COVID-19 vaccine booster vaccination?”; “If COVID-19 booster vaccination is available for a fee, how likely are you to receive it?”; “If there is a waiting list for the COVID-19 booster vaccination, how likely are you to get the vaccination?”

2. Intention to get vaccinated was measured in four specific combinations with different frameworks of information, including vaccine effectiveness, safety, duration of protection, and family member vaccination rates, such as “If the COVID-19 booster is 60% effective, how likely are you to get the vaccine booster?; “If the COVID-19 booster vaccine causes side effects such as swelling and pain at the vaccination site, itching, generalized fever, and malaise, how likely is it that you will receive the booster?”; “If the duration of protection for the COVID-19 vaccine booster is 2 years (24 months), how likely is it that you will receive the vaccination?” What is the likelihood that you will receive the booster vaccine if 90% of the family members around you receive it?” etc.;

3. Based on the existing research base, this study involved nine control variables: gender, age, occupation, place of residence, literacy, income, whether family members received COVID-19 vaccine booster shots, knowledge of COVID-19, and compliance with government COVID-19 prevention and control measures.

Two variables (gender and place of residence) were subjected to an independent samples t-test and four variables (age, occupation, income, and education) were subjected to one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA).Finally, we conducted regression analyses between different information frames and the public’s willingness to receive the COVID-19 vaccination booster.

2.3 Information framing

There are two types of information frames: the benefits of taking action - i.e., gain-frame information (e.g., getting a New Crown booster vaccination is effective in preventing COVID-19 infection), and the costs of refusing to take action - i.e., loss-frame information (e.g., by not receive a New Crown vaccine booster, you are likely to become infected with COVID-19). The loss-framed information is more compelling in disease detection behaviors, while the gain-framed information is more compelling in disease prevention behaviors (17).

A study showed that gain-frame messages were more persuasive when the message was intended to promote a behavior, whereas loss-frame messages were more persuasive when the message was intended to prevent a phenomenon (14). It has been found that gain-frame messages are more effective than their loss-frame counterparts in areas that favor prevention. Whereas vaccination is a disease prevention behavior, it is known that gain-frame messages are more persuasive than loss-frame messages in the preventive behavior of vaccination, i.e., gain-frame messages have a stronger effect on people’s willingness to receive booster shots (18).

However, when the degree of risk of taking, and refusing a behavior varies, people’s willingness is affected as a result: the persuasive effect of gain-framing information is stronger when the risk of behavioral consequences is low, and the effect of loss-framing information is stronger when the risk of behavioral consequences is high (19). Compared with general preventive behaviors, the risk of booster vaccination may be higher and may cause a series of reactions such as swelling and pain at the vaccination site, itching, generalized fever and malaise, cerebral thrombosis and stroke, and even life-threatening, i.e., booster vaccination is a high-risk behavior. In summary, booster vaccination is both a preventive and a high-risk behavior. Therefore, which framing message has a stronger effect on the intention to receive booster vaccine needs further study.

All information covered in this study was designed based on the characteristics of the gain and loss frames from the literature review, resident interviews, and expert consultations. In addition, the number of words between the two frames was similar in order to avoid the influence of the amount of information. The stimulus information is shown in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Stimulation information used in the questionnaire.

2.4 Personal cognition and social relationships

Personal cognition is a mental function that involves the storage, selection, organization, and action planning of information. In the field of cognitive epidemiology, it is believed that there is an association between individual cognition and mortality, disease, and health. In our study, personal cognition refers to people’s knowledge of COVID-19 and their awareness of compliance with government prevention and control measures, which may be associated with willingness to receive the COVID-19 booster vaccine. Also, a study showed that whether an individual’s loved ones, family members, or public figures they trust and respect receive booster shots influences that person’s willingness to receive booster vaccines (20). That is, booster vaccination of family members affects an individual’s willingness to receive a booster vaccine.

2.5 Risk disclosure

In the field of health communication, risk disclosure refers to the decision to accept or reject a medical intervention or medication by providing patients with important risk information (21). A Study of Intention to Vaccinate Against Seasonal Influenza Virus Shows that a Communication model framed by risk disclosure information leads to lower public intention to receive influenza vaccine (22). Therefore, disclosure of information on the risks of the COVID-19 vaccine booster may affect the public’s willingness to receive the vaccine.

2.6 Perceived vaccine effectiveness and duration of protection

Perceived vaccine effectiveness refers to the “effectiveness of the vaccine” and is the main consideration for individuals to receive vaccines (23). A study of vaccination behavior showed that information framing and vaccination intentions were moderated by perceived vaccine effectiveness (24), suggesting that target framing information would increase people’s belief in vaccine effectiveness, thereby increasing their intentions to receive a booster vaccine. Duration of immunity also has a positive impact on the public’s willingness to be vaccinated.

3 Results

3.1 Influence of demographic characteristics on intention to receive booster vaccinations

3.1.1 Effect of demographic characteristics on vaccination intentions based on a gaining framework

According to Table 2, there was no significant difference between male and female subjects in the case of receiving the gaining frame information by statistical analysis, and there was no significant difference between male and female subjects in terms of elevated intention to receive booster vaccination. Gender, place of residence, average monthly income occupation, and education level did not reach significant levels for booster vaccination. In addition, the willingness to receive booster vaccination was greater in the group aged 50–59 years than in other age groups; the willingness to receive booster vaccination was greater in the group with an average monthly income of RMB 3001–5,000 than in other income levels.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Effect of demographic characteristics on booster vaccination intention based on a gaining framework (N = 267).

3.1.2 Effect of demographic characteristics on vaccination intentions based on a loss framework

According to the following Table 3, the willingness to vaccinate was statistically higher in men than in women in the case of loss of frame information, and male subjects had an increased willingness to vaccinate when receiving loss of frame information, which may be related to the higher risk of COVID-19 complications and death in men (25). Place of residence, age, mean monthly income, occupation, and education did not reach significant levels for booster vaccination. In addition, booster vaccination intentions were greater in groups younger than 18 years than in all other age groups; booster vaccination intentions were greater in groups with an average monthly income of 3,001–5,000 yuan than in groups with other income levels.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Effect of demographic characteristics on booster vaccination intention based on a loss framework (N = 273).

3.2 Influence of personal cognition and social relationships on intention to receive booster vaccinations

3.2.1 Influence of individual cognition on intention to receive booster vaccinations

According to Table 4, by one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), we found that knowledge of COVID-19 (p = 0.000 < 0.05) and compliance with government COVID-19 control measures (p = 0.002 < 0.05) were significantly associated with intention to receive booster vaccination in a gain framework. Knowledge of COVID-19 (p = 0.001 < 0.05) and compliance with government COVID-19 control measures (p < 0.001 < 0.05) were also significant in the loss frame. This implies that higher knowledge of COVID-19 is associated with greater compliance with government COVID-19 control measures and higher willingness to receive booster vaccinations.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Influence of individual awareness on intention to receive booster vaccinations.

3.2.2 Effect of social relationships on intention to receive booster vaccinations

According to the data in Table 5, we conducted a comparative analysis of changes in the public’s willingness to receive COVID-19 vaccine booster across different information frames at 30 per cent, 60 per cent and 90 per cent vaccination rates for family members.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Effect of booster vaccination rate among acquaintances on intention to receive booster vaccination.

Vaccination rates of acquaintances were significantly correlated with vaccination intentions under all information framework assumptions (p < 0.001), and after receiving information about vaccination rates of acquaintances, subjects’ vaccination intentions increased with the increase in vaccination rates of acquaintances. With 30% of the acquaintances around them vaccinated, people are holding a wait-and-see attitude.

As seen in Table 6 there was a significant difference between the change in vaccination intention by whether family members were vaccinated or not in the loss framework (p = 0.017 < 0.05). When family members were vaccinated with the new crown vaccine (mean = 4.63, standard deviation = 0.664), their willingness to vaccinate was much higher than those without family members vaccinated (mean = 4.10, standard deviation = 1.101). With the gain frame information, there was no significant difference between whether family members/friends were vaccinated or not on subjects’ willingness to be vaccinated (p = 0.114 > 0.05), which did not reach statistical significance.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Effect of family members’ vaccination on intention to receive booster shots.

3.3 Impact of information framework on intention to receive booster vaccination

The information frames involved in this study were divided into loss-frame and gain-frame groups. As shown in Table 7, the mean intention to receive COVID-19 vaccine was lower in the loss-frame group (mean = 4.62, standard deviation = 0.595; mean = 4.11, standard deviation = 0.959) than in the gain-frame group (mean = 4.72, standard deviation = 0.550; mean = 4.23, standard deviation = 0.960). The information frame was significantly associated with the intention to vaccinate with COVID-19 (p < 0.001). However, the comparison between the two groups was not significant.

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Linear regression analysis of vaccination intention for booster shots in an information framework.

Further analysis of the linear regression analysis revealed that for booster vaccination intention, We conducted regression analyses using different information frames used as independent variables, public willingness to receive the COVID-19 vaccine booster vaccination as well as public willingness to receive the COVID-19 booster vaccine in a fee-paying situation as dependent variables, the absolute value of the standardized beta coefficient under the loss framework was smaller than under the gain framework (B = 0.746 < B = 0.863, p < 0.001), suggesting that the gain framework has a greater effect on intention to receive COVID-19 vaccination than the loss framework. With an emphasis on booster vaccination charges, the willingness to receive booster vaccination decreases in both frameworks. The introduction of a free booster vaccination policy in China has, to some extent, increased the coverage of booster vaccination. In contrast, the absolute values of the standardized Beta coefficients in the loss frame were smaller than those in the gain frame (B = 0.492 < B = 0.504, p < 0.001) for whether or not they intended to charge for booster vaccination, indicating that the gain frame had a larger effect on intention to charge for COVID-19 vaccine booster vaccination than the loss frame and that the standardized Beta coefficients between the loss and gain frames The comparison of absolute values was not particularly significant.

3.4 Impact of risk disclosure on intention to receive booster vaccinations

According to the data in the following Table 8, compares differences in public willingness to be inoculated under different information frameworks when faced with disclosed risk factors, the intention of the subjects changed significantly after receiving information about the side effects of the COVID-19 vaccine. People may opt for booster vaccination in the loss frame (mean = 3.48, standard deviation = 1.122, p < 0.001) but tend to wait and see in the gain frame (mean = 3.34, standard deviation = 1.163, p < 0.001) if the side effects of the vaccine are swelling and pain, itching, generalized fever, and malaise. In addition, when vaccination may lead to cerebral thrombosis and stroke, the effect of the two information frames on people’s willingness to receive booster vaccination was similar (mean = 2.22, standard deviation = 1.190, p < 0.001; mean = 2.23, standard deviation = 1.191, p < 0.001) and people’s willingness to receive booster vaccination compared to side effects were “swelling and pain, itching, generalized fever, and weakness.” And if vaccination affects life safety and has a risk of death, people mostly tend to refuse (mean = 1.75, standard deviation = 1.221, p < 0.001; mean = 1.77, standard deviation = 1.265, p < 0.001). Different levels of risk disclosure led to a significant decrease in vaccination intention compared to no disclosure of these risks (mean = 4.61, standard deviation = 0.689, p < 0.001; mean = 4.73, standard deviation = 0.569, p < 0.001).

Table 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Impact of risk disclosure on intention to receive booster vaccinations.

3.5 Effect of perceived booster vaccine protection rate on vaccination intention

Based on the data in Table 9, explored the impact of the public’s perceived protection rate of booster vaccines on vaccination intentions under different information frameworks, the perceived booster vaccine protection rate was significantly associated with intention to receive the booster vaccine under all information framework assumptions (p < 0.001). After receiving information about the booster shot protection rate of the COVID-19 vaccine, subjects’ intentions decreased in the first two degrees (30, 60%) and increased in the third degree (90%). When the booster vaccine protection rate was 30%, there was a tendency to adopt a wait-and-see attitude (mean = 3.69, standard deviation = 1.112, p < 0.001; mean = 3.58, standard deviation = 1.181, p < 0.001). When the protection rate of the booster vaccine was 60%, people were likely to be willing to receive the booster vaccine (mean = 4.24, standard deviation = 0.898, p < 0.001; mean = 4.16, standard deviation = 0.910, p < 0.001). People tended to be very willing to receive the booster vaccine when the protection rate of the booster vaccine was 90% (mean = 4.76, standard deviation = 0.706, p < 0.001; mean = 4.68, standard deviation = 0.781, p < 0.001). This suggests that when the protection rate of booster vaccine approaches 90%, the promotion of vaccine protection rate can appropriately increase people’s willingness to receive booster vaccine (mean = 4.61 vs. mean = 4.76; mean = 4.73 vs. mean = 4.68).

Table 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Effect of perceived booster vaccine protection rate on vaccination intention.

3.6 Effect of perceived duration of booster vaccine protection on vaccination intention

According to the data in Table 10, after receiving information about the duration of protection for COVID-19 vaccine booster shots, subjects’ intentions were slightly lower in the first two degrees (6 months, 1 year) and the third degree (2 years) than before receiving the hypothesis (mean = 4.60 vs. mean = 4.61; mean = 4.58 vs. mean = 4.73). When the duration of protection was 6 months, people were likely to choose wait-and-see vaccination in both the loss and gain frames (mean = 3.92, standard deviation = 0.967, p < 0.001; mean = 3.92, standard deviation = 1.010, p < 0.001); when the duration of protection was 1 year, people were likely to choose booster vaccination in both the loss and gain frames (mean = 4.29, standard deviation = 0.828, p < 0.001; mean = 4.29, standard deviation = 0.861, p < 0.001); when the duration of protection was 2 years, people were more likely to receive booster vaccine and were slightly more likely to receive a booster shot in the loss frame (mean = 4.60, standard deviation = 0.721, p = 0.879) than in the gain frame (mean = 4.58, standard deviation = 0.758, p = 0.002).

Table 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Effect of perceived booster vaccine protection rate on vaccination intention.

In summary, the minimum acceptable standard of protection for the COVID-19 vaccine is at least 1 year. People are more inclined to protect themselves and choose wait-and-see rather than vaccinate with a protection period of less than 1 year.

3.7 Multi-factor impact analysis under different information frameworks

From the data in Table 11, multivariate analysis Of Variance, we used a multifactorial analysis of variance to analyse the multifactorial effects on the willingness to receive a booster shot of the New Crown vaccine across different information frames, also we include gender as a control variable to exclude its confounding effect. Under the loss framework, social relationships, Level of knowledge of novel coronaviruses, and Risk Disclosure have a significant effect on willingness to receive booster shots under a multifactorial mixture of influences (p < 0.05), this is consistent with previous analyses. Under the gain framework, Level of knowledge of novel coronaviruses, Degree of compliance with government COVID-19 precautionary measures, and Risk Disclosure have an impact on willingness to receive booster shots (p < 0.05).

Table 11
www.frontiersin.org

Table 11. Multifactorial effects on vaccination intention.

4 Discussion

4.1 Influence of demographic characteristics on willingness to vaccinate booster shoot

Previous research has extensively explored the impact of socio-demographic factors on vaccine uptake, yet there is a dearth of studies elucidating the differential effects of socio-demographic characteristics within the context of a gain-loss framework. Men exhibit a greater willingness to receive the COVID-19 booster shot in a loss framework, possibly due to the elevated risk of complications and mortality in men (26). This heightened persuadability aligns with men’s lower trust in vaccine-related rumors and greater awareness of the infectious disease risk (27, 28).Contrary to expectations, age showed no significant association with vaccination willingness in the information framework, though prior studies suggest older individuals are more inclined to vaccinate (29, 30). This discrepancy may be attributed to the study’s focus on the 18–49 age group, limiting representation of older individuals. While previous research indicates a positive link between high income and vaccination willingness (31, 32), our findings diverge within the loss framework. High-income groups (> $10,000 monthly) demonstrate lower willingness, possibly due to their cautious approach, access to information, and financial stability for protective measures. Additionally, the higher-educated group (master’s degree and above) exhibits lower vaccination willingness, likely stemming from their ability to process more information and prioritize safety and efficacy considerations. This nuanced response highlights the multifaceted influences on vaccination attitudes.

4.2 Impact of personal cognition on willingness to vaccinate booster shoot

This study establishes a positive correlation between higher knowledge of COVID-19 and increased willingness to vaccinate it (3335). Alignment with government-mandated prevention measures also positively influences vaccination intent. Trust in the government, particularly in countries like Korea and China, where vaccine uptake is associated with governmental trust, plays a crucial role (34). Moreover, individuals exhibiting high trust in their country’s health system or information disseminated by public health authorities express greater willingness to receive the vaccine (36, 37).

4.3 Impact of social relationships on willingness to vaccinate booster shoot

The study indicates that a higher vaccination rate among close contacts positively influences individuals’ willingness to get vaccinated. About 78% of participants cited the influence of family and friends in their vaccination decision (38). Having vaccinated family or friends significantly increases vaccination intent in the loss framework, with inconsistent results in the gain framework (19).

China’s effective epidemic control may lead to complacency and reduced risk perception, potentially causing hesitancy (39). However, a loss-framed approach emphasizes the consequences of not vaccinating, highlighting continued disease spread and challenges in achieving herd immunity. This may prompt individuals to vaccinate, especially if their social circle is actively doing so.

However, a 60% acquaintance vaccination rate is still not effective in persuading people to receive the vaccine in either a loss or gain information framework, so there is some difficulty in achieving a herd immunization rate by people consciously receiving the vaccine.

Beliefs in individual and community benefits positively impact vaccination intent (40). Emphasizing communal advantages could motivate hesitant individuals to vaccinate. Targeting those with low vaccination intentions, persuading their social network, or engaging influential community figures may enhance overall vaccination intentions.

4.4 Impact of information framing on willingness to vaccinate booster shoot

The information frame can influence willingness to receive a booster vaccination with the COVID-19 vaccine, with the gain frame outweighing the loss frame for both intention to receive a booster vaccination and intention to charge for a booster vaccination. While the intention to receive booster vaccination generally declines when we emphasize the cost of booster vaccination for a fee. A study indicates that the provision of free COVID-19 vaccination services is crucial for low- and middle-income countries (41), a principle that holds true in China as well. It is noteworthy that the comparison of vaccination impacts between the two frames is similar when booster vaccination is charged for.

4.5 Risk disclosure effect of willingness to vaccinate booster shoot

Disclosure of potential side effects of booster shots for the COVID-19 vaccine significantly reduces vaccination willingness, particularly within a loss framework. This conclusion is largely consistent with the perspective of the Prospect Theory, wherein individuals choose vaccination to mitigate greater risks. However, when the act of vaccination itself entails substantial risks, exceeding the risk of disease from refusal, people tend to decline vaccination. Some studies indicate that individuals perceiving higher infection risks are more likely to accept the vaccine (4245). This inconsistency may be related to effective epidemic control in our country, resulting in a relatively lower infection risk. Therefore, excessive transmission of risk information regarding booster shots may lead the public to perceive the risks of not vaccinating as lower than the vaccine’s side effects.

4.6 Effect of perceived vaccine effectiveness and duration of protection

In a study assessing the vaccination willingness of Hong Kong residents against COVID-19, a correlation was observed between perceived vaccine effectiveness and the duration of protection, a finding congruent with our own research (46). The expeditious development of the COVID-19 vaccine, influenced by the unique spread of the virus, has engendered uncertainty regarding immunization effectiveness and its duration, thereby diminishing perceived vaccine efficacy (47).

The United States Food and Drug Administration has established a minimum acceptable effectiveness threshold of 50% for the COVID-19 vaccine (48). Achieving universal immunity necessitates a high population coverage exceeding 70% (49). Notably, a study underscores that, even with an 80% vaccine effectiveness rate, a minimum vaccination coverage of 75% is indispensable for pandemic control (50). Given the prevalent low perception of the efficacy of the newly available COVID-19 vaccine, it becomes imperative to incorporate information on the vaccine’s effective protection rate in booster dose promotion efforts to enhance public willingness to undergo vaccination.

4.7 Multifactorial effect under different information frameworks

Multi-factor impact analysis of two different information frameworks, we found that in the loss frame, the degree of compliance with government COVID-19 precautionary measures did not have an impact on the willingness to step up vaccination. Possible reasons for this are that the public, after receiving information about the costs of refusing booster shots, will be less interested in complying with the Government’s new crown precautionary measures and will be more concerned about social relations, knowledge of novel coronaviruses and risk disclosure. On the contrary, under the benefit framework, the public receives various information related to the benefits of receiving booster shots, and the thoughts of those around them may converge, so the influence of social ties on the willingness to receive booster shots of the new crown vaccine may not be as prominent as compared with other factors under the influence of the multiple factors.

5 Conclusion

This study explored the factors influencing the intention to receive COVID-19 vaccine booster shots based on information framing. Firstly, concerning sociodemographic characteristics, there is a strong correlation between household members receiving booster vaccine shots and individual willingness to receive them under the gain framework. Other factors such as gender, age, income, occupation, educational background, and place of residence did not show significant differences. Secondly, a higher level of knowledge about COVID-19 and greater adherence to government COVID-19 prevention and control measures were associated with a higher willingness to receive booster vaccination. Additionally, the persuasive effect of the gain framework was found to be higher than that of the loss framework in the context of COVID-19 vaccine booster shots. Finally, risk disclosure significantly influenced individuals’ willingness to receive COVID-19 vaccine booster shots.

Based on the findings above, we propose the following recommendations to increase the COVID-19 vaccine booster shot coverage: Firstly, public health departments and healthcare institutions should raise awareness about COVID-19 among the public, and the government should enhance the dissemination of preventive measures. Secondly, public health departments, healthcare institutions, and other relevant sectors should focus on utilizing the gain framework to promote health behaviors. Thirdly, withholding risk disclosure information during information dissemination can effectively enhance individuals’ willingness to receive booster vaccine shots. Therefore, when administering booster shots, medical information of residents should be kept as confidential as possible, accompanied by privacy protection statements, to address public concerns and thereby expand booster vaccination coverage.

6 Limitations and directions for future research

The results of this study provide theoretical and practical insights in the area of promoting public health behaviors. The gain framework had a greater persuasive effect than the loss framework in terms of willingness to vaccinate for new crowns. The concept that risk disclosure has a large negative impact on vaccination intention may be a meaningful predictor for testing vaccine promotion information strategies. However, shortcomings remain in the study:

On the one hand, this study used a convenience sampling method, and despite the randomness of the sample, most of the participants were a young and middle-aged group, the age distribution of the sample is more restricted, and the information framing effect on the willingness to vaccinate against COVID-19 vaccine in older age groups needs to be further studied.

On the other hand, the study mainly explored the effects of various factors on the public’s willingness to receive the New Corona booster vaccination under different information frameworks, however, the factors affecting the public’s willingness to vaccinate are multifaceted, and only some of them have been selected for study in this paper. In future studies, other factors can be explored to influence vaccination intentions, such as the influence of third-party organizations’ interventions. Also, future research could compare a range of behavioral changes in the public under different information frameworks to examine the relationships and underlying mechanisms between these behaviors.

There is still much room for exploring the application of information frameworks in the health field, and there are various forms of research methods to broaden the research and application of information frameworks in the health field, in addition to the gain and loss frameworks involved in this study.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the College of Life Sciences, Central South University (Reference No. 2022-1-45). The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable data included in this article.

Author contributions

QZ: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. YG: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – original draft. QH: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft. DH: Conceptualization, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. XW: Data curation, Project administration, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research work is supported by the National Social Science Fund of China (Grant No.20BTQ081), Key International Cooperation Projects of Hunan Province of China (Grant No. (30) WK2003), Project of Theory, Practice, and Popularization of Scientific and Technological Novelty Search by Shenzhen Health Development Research and Data Management Center (Grant No. H202111120250001).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Lingxi, H, Po, C, and Rongqi, W. Progress in studying the mechanism of liver injury in patients with novel coronavirus pneumonia. J Prac Liver Dis. (2022) 25:453–6. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1672-5069.2022.03.038

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Randolph, HE, Barreiro, LB. Herd immunity: understanding COVID-19. Immunity. (2020), 52:737–741, doi: 10.1016/j.immuni.2020.04.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. China Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2023) https://www.chinacdc.cn/jkzt/crb/zl/szkb_11803/202307/t20230705_267608.html. Accessed May, 5 2023

Google Scholar

4. Hassan, W, Kazmi, SK, Tahir, MJ, Ullah, I, Royan, HA, Fahriani, M, et al. Global acceptance and hesitancy of COVID-19 vaccination: A narrative review. Narra J. (2021) 1:e57. doi: 10.52225/narra.v1i3.57

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Zhang, R, Yan, J, Jia, H, Luo, X, Liu, Q, and Lin, J. Policy endorsement and booster shot: exploring politicized determinants for acceptance of a third dose of COVID-19 vaccine in China. Vaccines. (2023) 11:421. doi: 10.3390/vaccines11020421

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Tversky, A, and Kahneman, D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science. (1981) 211:453–8. doi: 10.1126/science.7455683

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Xie, H, Jiayin, D, and Wenting, H. Mechanisms of influence of information framework on the initial vaccination intentions of residents for the new crown vaccine - a moderated mediation model. Mod Inf. (2022) 42:70–80. doi: 10.1037/hea0000101

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lee, AY, and Aaker, JL. Bringing the frame into focus: the influence of regulatory fit on processing fluency and persuasion. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2004) 86:205–18. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.205

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Nandakumar, N, Sivakumaran, B, Kalro, A, and Sharma, P. Threat, efficacy and message framing in consumer healthcare. Mark Intell Plan. (2017) 35:442–57. doi: 10.1108/MIP-07-2016-0117

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Reyna, VF, and Brainerd, CJ. Fuzzy-trace theory and children's acquisition of mathematical and scientific concepts. Learn Individ Differ. (1991) 3:27–59. doi: 10.1016/1041-6080(91)90003-J

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Elliot, AJ, and Thrash, TM. Approach-avoidance motivation in personality: approach and avoidance temperaments and goals. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2002) 82:804–18. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.5.804

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Higgins, ET. Promotion and prevention as a motivational duality: implications for evaluative processes In: S Chaiken and Y Trope, editors. Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: The Guilford Press (1999). 503–25.

Google Scholar

13. Gerend, MA, and Shepherd, JE. Using message framing to promote acceptance of the human papillomavirus vaccine. Health Psychol. (2007) 26:745–52. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.6.745

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Nan, X. Communicating to young adults about HPV vaccination: consideration of message framing, motivation, and gender. Health Commun. (2012) 27:10–8. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2011.567447

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Peng, L, Guo, Y, and Hu, D. Information framing effect on Public's intention to receive the COVID-19 vaccination in China. Vaccines. (2021) 9:995. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9090995

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Fishbein, M, and Ajzen, I. Predicting and changing behavior: the reasoned action approach. Psychology Press. Mahwah, NJ, USA, (2010).

Google Scholar

17. Rothman, AJ, and Salovey, P. Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behavior: the role of message framing. Psychol Bull. (1997) 121:3–19. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Updegraff, J, Brick, C, and Emanuel, AS. Message framing for health: moderation by perceived susceptibility and motivational orientation in a diverse sample of Americans. Health Psychol. (2015) 34:20–9. doi: 10.1037/hea0000101

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Feleszko, W, Lewulis, P, and Czarnecki, A. Flattening the curve of COVID-19 vaccine rejection—an international overview. Vaccine. (2021) 9:44. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9010044

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Mazur, DJ, and Hickam, DH. Patients0 preferences for risk disclosure and role in decision making for invasive medical procedures. J Gen Intern Med. (1997) 12:114–7. doi: 10.1007/s11606-006-5006-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Kim, S, Pjesivac, I, and Jin, Y. Effects of message framing on influenza vaccination: understanding the role of risk disclosure, perceived vaccine efficacy, and felt ambivalence. Health Commun. (2019) 34:21–30. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2017.1384353

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Nan, X, Xie, B, and Madden, K. Acceptability of the H1N1 vaccine among older adults: the interplay of message framing and perceived vaccine safety and efficacy. Health Commun. (2012) 27:559–68. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2011.617243

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Ferguson, E, and Gallagher, L. Message framing concerning decisions about vaccination: the roles of frame valence, frame method, and perceived risk. Br J Psychol. (2007) 98:667–80. doi: 10.1348/000712607X190692

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Igarashi, A, Nakano, Y, and Yoneyama-Hirozane, M. Public preferences and willingness to accept a hypothetical vaccine to prevent a pandemic in Japan: a conjoint analysis. Expert Rev Vaccines. (2022) 21:241–8. doi: 10.1080/14760584.2022.2016402

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Galbadage, T, Peterson, BM, and Awada, J. Systematic review and meta-analysis of sex-specific COVID-19 clinical outcomes. Front Med. (2020) 7:348. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2020.00348

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Sallam, M, Dababseh, D, and Eid, H. High rates of COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and its association with conspiracy beliefs: a study in Jordan and Kuwait among other Arab countries. Vaccine. (2021) 9:42. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9010042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Sallam, M. COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy worldwide: a concise systematic review of vaccine acceptance rates. Vaccine. (2021) 9:160. doi: 10.3390/vaccines9020160

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Detoc, M, Bruel, S, Frappe, P, et al. Intention to participate in a COVID-19 vaccine clinical trial and to get vaccinated against COVID-19 in France during the pandemic. Vaccine. (2020) 38:7002–6. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2020.09.041

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Gagneux-Brunon, A,Detoc, M,Bruel, S, Tardy, B, Rozaire, O, Frappe, P, et al. Intention to get vaccinations against COVID-19 in French healthcare workers during the first pandemic wave:a crosssectional survey. J Hosp Infect, (2021), 108:168–173, doi: 10.1016/j.jhin.2020.11.020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Coustasse, A, Kimble, C, and Maxik, K. COVID-19 and vaccine hesitancy. J Ambul Care Manag. (2021) 44:71–5. doi: 10.1097/JAC.0000000000000360

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Danchin, M, Biezen, R, and Manski-Nankervis, J-A. Preparing the public for COVID-19 vaccines: how can general practitioners build vaccine confidence and optimise uptake for themselves and their patients? Aust J Gen Pract. (2020) 49:625–9. doi: 10.31128/AJGP-08-20-5559

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Chen, T, Dai, M, Xia, S, and Zhou, Y. Do messages matter? Investigating the combined effects of framing, outcome uncertainty, and number format on COVID-19 vaccination attitudes and intention. Health Commun. (2021) 37:944–51. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2021.1876814

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Chen, M, Li, Y, and Chen, J. An online survey of the attitude and willingness of Chinese adults to receive COVID-19 vaccination. Hum Vaccin Immunother. (2021) 17:2279–88. doi: 10.1080/21645515.2020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Lazarus, JV, Ratzan, SC, and Palayew, A. A global survey of potential acceptance of a COVID-19 vaccine. Nat Med. (2021) 27:225–8. doi: 10.1038/s41591-020-1124-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Biasio, LR, Bonaccorsi, G, and Lorini, C. Assessing COVID-19 vaccine literacy: a preliminary online survey. Hum Vaccin Immunother. (2021) 17:1304–12. doi: 10.1080/21645515.2020.1829315

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Al-Mohaithef, M, and Padhi, BK. Determinants of COVID-19 vaccine acceptance in Saudi Arabia: a web-based national survey. J Multidiscip Healthc. (2020) 13:1657–63. doi: 10.2147/JMDH.S276771

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Grüner, S, and Krüger, F. The intention to be vaccinated against COVID-19: stated preferences before vaccines were available. Appl Econ Lett. (2020) 28:1847–51. doi: 10.1080/13504851.2020.1854445

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Seale, H, Heywood, AE, and Leask, J. Examining Australian public perceptions and behaviors towards a future COVID-19 vaccine. BMC Infect Dis. (2021) 21:1–9. doi: 10.1186/s12879-021-05833-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Chu, H, and Liu, S. Integrating health behavior theories to predict American’s intention to receive a COVID-19 vaccine. Patient Educ Couns. (2021) 104:1878–86. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2021.02.031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Guidry, JPD, Laestadius, LI, and Vraga, EK. Willingness to get the COVID-19 vaccine with and without emergency use authorization. Am J Infect Control. (2020) 49:137–42. doi: 10.1016/j.ajic.2020.11.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Sallam, M, Anwar, S, Yufika, A, Fahriani, M, Husnah, M, Kusuma, HI, et al. Willingness-to-pay for COVID-19 vaccine in ten low-middle-income countries in Asia, Africa and South America: A cross-sectional study. Narra J. (2022) 2:e74. doi: 10.52225/narra.v2i1.74

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Head, KJ, Kasting, ML, and Sturm, LA. A national survey assessing SARS-CoV-2 vaccination intentions: implications for future public health communication efforts. Sci Commun. (2020) 42:698–723. doi: 10.1177/1075547020960463

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Harapan, H, Wagner, AL, and Yufika, A. Acceptance of a COVID-19 vaccine in Southeast Asia: a crosssectional study in Indonesia. Front Public Health. (2020) 8:381. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.00381

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Reiter, PL, Pennell, ML, and Katz, ML. Acceptability of a COVID-19 vaccine among adults in the United States: how many people would get vaccinated? Vaccine. (2020) 38:6500–7. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2020.08.043

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Singh, T, and Smith, D. Direct-to-consumer prescription drug advertising: a study of consumer attitudes and behavioral intentions. J Consum Mark. (2005) 22:369–78. doi: 10.1108/07363760510631101

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Yanqiu, Y, Lau, JTF, and Lau, MMC. Understanding the prevalence and associated factors of behavioral intention of COVID-19 vaccination under specific scenarios combining effectiveness, safety, and cost in the Hong Kong Chinese general population. Int J Health Policy Manag. (2020) 11:1090–101. doi: 10.34172/ijhpm.2021.02

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Freeman, D, Loe, BS, and Chadwick, A. COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in the UK: the Oxford coronavirus explanations, attitudes, and narratives survey (OCEANS) II. Psychol Med. (2020) 52:3127–41. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720005188

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Wang, K, Wong, ELY, Ho, KF, Cheung, AWL, Chan, EYY, Yeoh, EK, et al. Intention of nurses to accept coronavirus disease 2019 vaccination and change of intention to accept seasonal influenza vaccination during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic: a cross-sectional survey. Vaccine. (2020) 38:7049–56. doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2020.09.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Parletta, N, Aljeesh, Y, and Baune, BT. Health behaviors, knowledge, life satisfaction, and wellbeing in people with mental illness across four countries and comparisons with normative sample. Front Psych. (2016) 7:145. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00145

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Kwok, KO, Li, KK, Chan, HHH, Yi, YY, Tang, A, Wei, WI, et al. Community responses during early phase of COVID-19 epidemic, Hong Kong. Emerg Infect Dis. (2020) 26:1575–9. doi: 10.3201/eid2607.200500

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19 vaccine booster shot, information framing, framing effect, risk disclosure, perceived vaccine potency

Citation: Zhu Q, Gao Y, Hu Q, Hu D and Wu X (2024) A study on the factors influencing the intention to receive booster shots of the COVID-19 vaccine in China based on the information frame effect. Front. Public Health. 12:1258188. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1258188

Received: 14 July 2023; Accepted: 31 January 2024;
Published: 20 February 2024.

Edited by:

Ritthideach Yorsaeng, Chulalongkorn University, Thailand

Reviewed by:

Mari Kannan Maharajan, University of Nottingham Malaysia, Malaysia
Shuang-jun Pan, Zhejiang Taizhou Hospital, China

Copyright © 2024 Zhu, Gao, Hu, Hu and Wu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: DeHua Hu, aHVkZWh1YUBjc3UuZWR1LmNu; Xusheng Wu, MTI1NzQ1MTg2OUBxcS5jb20=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.