- 1School of Psychology, Weifang Medical University, Weifang, China
- 2Neuropsychology and Applied Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, CAS Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Beijing, China
- 3Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
Introduction: Mindfulness reflects attention to the present moment in a non-judgmental way and has been linked to individual autonomy and motivation, but conclusions are inconsistent. The purpose of this review was to summarize previous studies to explore the relationship between mindfulness and motivation and its intervention effects.
Methods: Literature searches were conducted in five electronic databases. Both correlational studies assessing the association between motivation and mindfulness and experimental studies to verify the effect of intervention were included.
Results: Six papers with seven intervention studies and twenty-three papers with twenty-seven correlational studies met the inclusion criteria. Meta-analysis showed that mindfulness was positively correlated with intrinsic motivation (r = 0.28, p < 0.0001) and total motivation (r = 0.37, p < 0.0001) but had no significant correlation with extrinsic motivation (r = 0.01, p = 0.93) or amotivation (r = −0.17, p = 0.14). Effect-size estimates suggested that mindfulness intervention was beneficial to motivation promotion, but the effect was at a low level (g = 0.12).
Conclusion: We found consistent support for mindfulness practice relating to motivation promotion, especially on intrinsic motivation development. However, there was still a portion of heterogeneity that could not be explained and needed to be identified in future studies.
1. Introduction
Motivation, defined as the psychological tendency or internal drive that stimulates and maintains the action of an organism, lies at the heart of all behavior of human being and the focus topic of psychology and pedagogy (1). An individual with a high level of motivation is more productive during their study or work. For example, academic achievement has been reported to be influenced by different types of motivation that stem from external incentives, ego involvement, personal value, and intrinsic interest (2). Motivation is also associated with individual participation in physical activity or maintaining a healthy lifestyle (3–5). Lack of motivation, on the other hand, is typically categorized as the syndrome of anhedonia (6, 7), which is often identified a central feature of some mental disorders (8), such as schizophrenia or major depressive disorder, and is strongly linked to individual poor functional outcomes (9). Finding from Barch et al. (10) showed that patients with strong anhedonia often exhibited motivation deficits. Therefore, how to improve individual motivation in order to maintain high work and study results or maintain a healthy lifestyle is of great significance for personal development.
Traditional psychologists defined motivation as the psychological disposition or drive that inspires and sustains an individual to perform an activity and leads to that activity toward a goal (11). As Ryan and Deci found in the 1970s that imposed extrinsic rewards had a debilitating effect on individuals’ interest, attention has been paid to the distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (12). Subsequently, many researchers proposed different theories about the relationship between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. For example, Deci et al. (13) obtained consistent results from laboratory experiments with different subjects using different procedures and stimuli: extrinsic stimuli weakened pre-existing intrinsic motivation (14). Simon argued that the most important function of motivation is the control of attention, and the difference between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation can be seen as the difference between distraction and concentration of attention (14).
Although there is no conclusive theory on the relationship between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, most researchers agreed that extrinsic motivation drives individuals when they engage in activities for the pleasure of obtaining external objects (e.g., money), whereas intrinsic motivation drives individuals when they engage in activities for the pleasure of the activity itself and to satisfy basic human psychological needs (e.g., autonomy) (12). Accordingly, why do people experience amotivation, perhaps because they do not see the connection between their behavior and the expected result, and/or feel incapable of doing the work. Amotivation is thus associated with theories concerning low expectancy and/or value (15), low self-efficacy (16), and learned helplessness (17).
Mindfulness reflects attention to the present moment in a non-judgmental, non-reactive manner (18, 19). Conceptually related to mindfulness is the construct of savoring (20), which means the attention to, appreciation, and enhancement of positive experiences in the moment (21). Some scholars further divide mindfulness into trait and state, with the former thought to be a personality trait and the latter thought to be cultivable (18, 22, 23). Through mindfulness practice, people learn to observe sensations and be more motivated in their daily life. Ryan and Deci (24) proposed that “mindfulness, defined as the open and receptive awareness of what is occurring both within people and within their context, facilitates greater autonomy and integrated self-regulation” (p. 268). Through attention-directed training, the trainers’ thought patterns can be changed which in turn leads to changes in attitudes and behaviors (25).
In everyday life, there is evidence of enhancements in both psychological and physical aspects of well-being and emotion regulation, following mindfulness training (26). Some studies have found that mindfulness-based intervention can significantly improve depressive mood, anxiety (27–29), sleep disturbance (30), cognitive function in older adults (31), even hedonic capacity among patients with chronic pain (32). However, some studies have found no such effect (33–35). Liu et al. (36) conducted one systematic review and found that mindfulness intervention had significant improvement effect on negative symptom, such as amotivation and anhedonia, among patients with schizophrenia. However, the question of whether mindfulness practices can increase levels of motivation and decrease amotivation in the broader population is unclear.
In addition, although some studies have found that mindfulness was significantly related to both extrinsic and intrinsic (5), the results of some experimental studies have only found that mindfulness improved intrinsic motivation (35–38). There are even studies that have found the opposite effect. For example, Marion-Jetten et al. (39) have found that higher levels of dispositional mindfulness had lower controlled goal motivation. Therefore, it is not clear whether mindfulness practice has a positive effect on both intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Which motivation, if any, works better.
Taken together, although some previous studies have found that mindfulness practice has an effect on the improvement of motivation, some studies have not found this effect. In addition, there is no consensus on whether extrinsic motivation or intrinsic motivation is more closely related to mindfulness practice, and whether both can be improved by mindfulness practice.
Therefore, in this study, we conducted a meta-analysis combining existing correlational and intervention studies in an attempt to explore the relationship between mindfulness and motivation. Based on the existing studies (35–38, 40), we hypothesized that mindfulness would be significantly associated with motivation. We also hypothesized that mindfulness could improve amotivation and increase the level of motivation, especially in the case of intrinsic motivation.
2. Methods
2.1. Eligibility criteria
This systematic review was conducted and reported in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines (41). To be included, studies needed to meet the following criteria: (a) studies described as quantitative, not qualitative, measures of mindfulness and motivation; (b) studies examined mindfulness and motivation; (c) studies reported the relations between mindfulness and motivation, including either an effect size (e.g., Cohen’s d), or sufficient information to compute an effect size; (d) studies were included only if they were written in English and published as full-text articles in peer-reviewed professional journals; (e) studies used an intervention (with or without a control condition) or correlational design. The exclusion criteria were as follows: (a) mindfulness was not considered an element of intervention; (b) studies were not published in a peer-review journal in English; (c) data was unavailable to compute.
2.2. Search strategies
Literature searched were conducted in five databases, including PubMed, Embase, Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, PsycINFO, Web of Science. The search was performed for articles published from the earliest available date to 30 September 2022.
We used the search terms “mindfulness” and “motivation”/“anhedonia.” The following search terms example were initially searched in PubMed: [Mindfulness*(Title/Abstract)] OR [MBSR(Title/Abstract)] OR [MBCT(Title/Abstract)] OR “[Mindfulness”(Mesh)] AND [“Anhedonia”(Mesh)] OR [“Motivation”(Mesh)] OR [anhedonia(Title/Abstract)] OR [amotivation(Title/Abstract)] OR [motivation(Title/Abstract)] OR [“negative symptom”(Title/Abstract)].
Titles and abstracts were screened. Four authors independently screened titles and abstracts of all the studies to exclude duplicate records, review papers, conference abstract and case studies. To confirm the inclusion, the same four authors assessed the eligibility of these full-texts and reasons for exclusion of publications of eligible studies were recorded. The details of the selection process were recorded to generate the PRISMA flow diagram (Figure 1).
2.3. Data extraction
Four researchers independently extracted data using self-designed data extraction forms from 29 papers included in this review. The forms included the following information: (a) title; (b) author(s); (c) publication year; (d) study design; (e) country of the participants; (f) participants’ demo-graphics; (g) number of participants (in each group, if intervention); (h) average age of participants; (i) proportion of female participants; (j) instrument used to measure mindfulness and motivation; (k) relevant outcomes; (l) intervention and control details (if intervention).
2.4. Synthesis
In this review, 27 correlation studies and 7 intervention studies were included, therefore outcomes were pooled separately. For correlational studies, all summary measures were converted to Fisher’s z and all analyses were performed in Fisher’s z. For intervention studies, all summary measures were converted to Hedges’ g (42), which corrects for biases due to small sample size from Cohen’s d. Appropriate data were pooled by using a random effects model and calculating the standardized mean difference. When studies report more than one time point, the latest time point which was chosen for analysis. All analyses were conducted in the R environment [Version 4.2.1 (43)] and meta-analyses were conducted using the meta package (44).
2.4.1. Heterogeneity analysis
Heterogeneity was systematically assessed as the following steps: the generation of forest plots, the I2 statistic and chi-square [Q test (45)] and. I2 scores greater than 75% would be considered high heterogeneity, which could be explained by moderating factors, whereas 50% indicates moderate heterogeneity and lower than 25% indicates low heterogeneity (46).
2.4.2. Assessment of publication biases
Publication bias was visually judged by the funnel plot (47). A symmetric distribution funnel illustrates no publication bias, while an asymmetrical funnel illustrates potential publication bias of the included studies. We further performed Egger’s test of the intercept (48) to further explore the publication bias of asymmetric funnel plots.
2.4.3. Assessment of risk of bias
For correlational designs included in the review, we drew upon the methods outlined in the PRISMA statement. Risk of bias criteria were: (a) description of participants demographic; (b) evidence that the sample is representative of the population which it selected; (c) a valid measurement instrument of mindfulness; (d) a valid measurement instrument of motivation. Four researchers independently assigned 0 (absent described) and 1 (present described). A total score of 1 or less was considered high risk, 2–3 was considered moderate risk and 4 was considered low risk. For intervention designs, risk of bias was assessed by using JADAD scale. Four criteria from this scale were adapted and four researchers independently assigned 0 (absent described), 1 (inadequately described) or 2 (present described). A total score of 1–3 was considered high risk and 4–7 was considered low risk. Publication bias was also visually judged by the funnel plot (47). The overall level of quality evidence was assessed for each outcome by grading evidence according to the Grade of Recommendation, Assessment, Development and Evaluation Working Group (GRADE) (49).
2.4.4. Sensitivity analysis
Sensitivity analysis (50) was performed to examine the extent to which studies judged to be at high risk of bias influenced the results. Both random effects and fixed effects models were rerun to assess how outcomes were affected. Pooled estimated effect sizes were calculated by removing one study individually to assess how each study affects the pooled estimates.
3. Results
3.1. Study selection
A total of 1,846 records were identified from five databases. After removing 903 duplicates, 943 records were screened out by titles and abstracts. 706 irrelevant studies, 135 reviews, 15 case studies and 17 protocols were excluded. The full-text publications were retrieved for the remaining 71 papers and were assessed for the eligibility criteria. This resulted in the identification of 29 papers for inclusion.
3.2. Study characteristics
Of the 29 papers included in the meta-analysis, 6 papers with 7 studies were intervention studies and 23 papers with 27 studies were correlational studies. Of the correlational studies, since different measurement instruments were selected by each study, thirteen studies measured intrinsic motivation, eleven studies measured extrinsic motivation, six studies measured amotivation and thirteen studies measured total motivation. Further information on each study, including sample size, sex and measurement instruments of mindfulness and motivation used, could be found in Table 1.
Of the intervention studies, the studies were published from 2015 to 2021. For the study design, seven studies were two-arm RCTs with inactive-controlled design, four studies were two arm RCTs with a parallel intervention group and one study was one-group pretest-posttest design. Further information on each study could be found in Table 2.
3.3. Risk of bias
The risk of bias summary was presented in Tables 1, 2. In sum, among correlational studies, one study was high risk (3.70%), thirteen studies were moderate risk (48.25%) and thirteen were low risk (48.25%). Among intervention studies, two of seven studies were high risk (28.57%) and five studies were low risk (71.43%). The overall level of quality evidence in intervention studies shows that three studies were high quality, three were moderate quality and one was low quality. All intervention outcome measures for each study is listed in Supplementary materials.
3.4. Publication bias
The funnel plots for correlational studies appear in Figure 2 and for intervention studies in Figure 3. Among the plots for correlational studies, the symmetry of the funnel plot was found to be good by examination of total motivation, however, there were clearly missing effect sizes for intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation and amotivation. Of the plots for intervention studies, there was a good symmetry, suggesting the likelihood of a risk of publication bias was low.
Figure 2. The funnel plots for correlational studies. (A) The funnel plot for total motivation in correlational studies; (B) The funnel plot for intrinsic motivation in correlational studies; (C) The funnel plot for extrinsic motivation in correlational studies; (D) The funnel plot for amotivation in correlational studies.
Based on the results of the visual inspections, we further performed an Egger linear regression test. For correlational studies, these tests indicated low levels of bias across all pooled effects: intrinsic (t = −2.43, p = 0.0335), extrinsic (t = −0.70, p = 0.5002), total motivation (t = −1.26, p = 0.2322). It was not possible to run Egger linear regression test for correlational amotivation studies and intervention studies because there were less than ten data points. Therefore, it can be concluded that there is no publication bias in this study.
3.5. Synthesis of results
3.5.1. Correlational effects
In the results of total motivation, thirteen studies that measured the correlation between total motivation and mindfulness. These studies were found to be high heterogeneity (Q = 347.23, p < 0.0001, I2 = 96.5%). Therefore, the choice of random effects model is reasonable. The results showed that mindfulness had a moderate correlation with total motivation (r = 0.37, 95% CI = 0.23 to 0.50, p < 0.0001). The forest diagram is shown in Figure 4A.
Figure 4. The forest diagrams for correlational studies. (A) The forest diagram for total motivation in correlational studies; (B) The forest diagram for intrinsic motivation in correlational studies; (C) The forest diagram for extrinsic motivation in correlational studies; (D) The forest diagram for amotivation in correlational studies.
In the results of intrinsic motivation, thirteen studies that measured the correlation between intrinsic motivation and mindfulness. These studies were found to be high heterogeneity (Q = 240.27, p < 0.0001, I2 = 95.0%). The results showed that mindfulness had a small correlation with intrinsic motivation (r = 0.28, 95% CI = 0.15 to 0.40, p < 0.0001). The forest diagram is shown in Figure 4B.
In the results of extrinsic motivation and amotivation, eleven studies measured the correlation between extrinsic motivation and mindfulness and six studies measured the correlation between amotivation and mindfulness. These studies were found to be high heterogeneity in extrinsic motivation (Q = 745.46, p < 0.0001, I2 = 98.7%) and amotivation (Q = 133.78, p < 0.0001, I2 = 96.3%). The results showed that a non-significant correlation between mindfulness with extrinsic motivation (r = 0.01, 95% CI = −0.27 to 0.29, p = 0.93) and amotivation (r = −0.17, 95% CI = −0.38 to 0.06, p = 0.14), respectively. The forest diagram is shown in Figures 4C,D.
The further subgroup analysis found that age and participant characteristics (i.e., clinical patients or not) could be considered as potential moderators of extrinsic motivation, the participants who were clinical patients (g = −0.16) and beyond 40 years (g = −0.24) evidenced lower effect size. A detailed description of subgroup analyses is in Section 3 of the Supplementary material.
3.5.2. Intervention effects
We included seven studies of mindfulness interventions in this review to test whether mindfulness promotes motivation. Figure 5 shows the pooled effects from studies of mindfulness interventions on motivation. These studies were found to be a low heterogeneity (Q = 4.14, p = 0.66, I2 = 0.0%). However, due to the small number of included studies, a random effect model still needs to be selected. We observed a low effect of mindfulness intervention on motivation promotion with a g-value of 0.12 (95% CI = 0.01 to 0.22, p < 0.05). A detailed description of full results is presented in Table 3.
3.6. Sensitivity analysis
Results from these models are reported in Supplementary materials and are summarized here. For correlational studies, no significant difference was found in the results of total motivation, intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation. In the results of amotivation, we found that Yusainy et al. (5) had an extreme value. If this study was omitted, the pooled correlation coefficient of mindfulness with amotivation would be −0.28 (95% CI = −0.34 to −0.22, p < 0.0001). In the other studies, no significant difference was found. The results of sensitivity analysis can be found in Supplementary Figures S1, S2.
4. Discussion
In the present review, we identified both correlational and intervention studies, a total of 34 studies, to investigate the relationship between mindfulness and different types of motivation and the effect of mindfulness training on motivational enhancement. After integrating the results of 27 correlational studies, we found that mindfulness was moderately correlated with total motivation. Across the dimensions of motivation, we found that mindfulness had a small positively correlation with intrinsic motivation, but no statistically significant with extrinsic motivation and amotivation. The analysis results of the experimental study further found the promotional effect of mindfulness training on motivation, although the effect size was at a small level. After visual inspections and Egger linear regression test, there was no publication bias in this review. However, due to the small sample size and the low level of evidence among intervention studies, the conclusions should be proposed with caution.
One of our main findings was mindfulness was moderately associated with motivation. This finding is consistent with some previous studies (3, 51), but there are still some studies that found no significant correlation between mindfulness and motivation (33, 81). This difference may be caused by several reasons. Firstly, there are many different theories about motivation. No uniform measurement instrument of motivation was accepted. Studies would be focus on different motivation scales, thus creating a gap between studies. Secondly, motivation can also be classified into different categories based on different theoretical backgrounds. Mindfulness may be effective in enhancing only one of these categories of motivation, but not all of them. Therefore, these studies may not have categorized different motivations when measuring motivation, leading to different results.
In our study, we explored the relationship between mindfulness on intrinsic and extrinsic motivation separately, and found that mindfulness was more closely associated with intrinsic motivation than extrinsic motivation. This supports our hypothesis and is consistent with some previous findings (39, 40). Compared with extrinsic motivation, intrinsic motivation is more emphasized individual autonomy, curiosity and interest (82). Autonomy/self-determination is the core element of intrinsic motivation (12). Extrinsic motivation, on the other hand, is caused by external rewards, such as money, material goods, and honors (12). Some studies argued that intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation are opposed to each other (14, 83). While only a weak negative correlation between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation was found in some studies (83). Mindfulness is considered as an intraindividual factor to support autonomous engagement in activities (24). The primary role of mindfulness was to enhance the individual’s attention and awareness to internal experience, including awareness of emotions, somatic states and psychological needs (84, 85). This is in line with the characteristics of intrinsic motivation. We believe that individuals with a high level of mindfulness are better able to become aware of their needs and thus improve their motivation level by increasing their autonomy. The closer association of mindfulness with intrinsic motivation further suggested the potential role of mindfulness practice on emphasizing the arousal of individual needs rather than the desire for external rewards.
Another main finding of our study was that mindfulness-based intervention could effectively improve individuals’ levels of motivation. Previous studies on the possible causes of mindfulness intervention on motivation can be summarized as follows. Firstly, the mindfulness-based intervention was found to be effective in improving the attention of individuals (86, 87). Smart et al. (86) found that after 8 weeks of mindfulness-based intervention, the subjects showed better self-awareness and improved immediate regulation of attention. This helps individuals to improve their self-determination, pay more attention to internal feelings, and reduce the interference of extraneous factors, thus promoting higher levels of motivation. Secondly, some researchers think that the effect of mindfulness-based intervention may be mediated by improving emotions to increase motivation levels. For example, unpleasant emotional experiences, such as anxiety, depression and nervousness, has been confirmed to seriously affect an individual’s social functioning and cause loss of interest in daily life (88). Some studies showed that mindfulness-based intervention was more effective in controlling anxiety symptoms in people with anxiety disorders (89–91). Mindfulness-based intervention can also reduce symptoms associated with major depressive disorder and increase an individual’s interest in life, thereby promoting increased motivation (92). Finally, some brain imaging studies have also found possible effects of mindfulness interventions on brain function. For example, Zhou and Liu (93) found that mindfulness-based intervention can enhance individual left-sided brain activity. In the study of Wang and Huang (94), mindfulness-based intervention was found to be related with the thickened gray matter of some brain areas, such as the hippocampus, insula and cingulate gyrus. Therefore, the intervention effect of mindfulness practice on motivation enhancement may have its cognitive neural mechanism, but relevant studies are relatively few, let alone the lack of intervention studies on the long-term effect on brain function.
This study has several limitations. Firstly, due to the limitation of the number of articles, we failed to investigate the correlation between different categories of mindfulness and different categories of motivation. Secondly, among the correlational studies, there was unexplained heterogeneity in pooled effects of mindfulness on some types of motivation, which may affect the analysis results. Thirdly, some articles did not select existing and accepted scales for motivation measurement and the degree of consistency of the instruments was not reported in the articles, which may lead to inaccurate motivation measures. Fourthly, only studies publication in English was included. Then, in the intervention study, we found by sensitivity analysis that if the non-RCT study was removed, the overall effect size became non-significant, although the g-value did not change significantly. This is likely to be due to the small number of studies that could be included. Lastly, the risk of bis assessment found a high risk of bias for two of seven intervention studies. Both were nonrandomization of participants, non-concealment of allocation and nonblinding of participants and researchers. These potential methodological deficiencies may affect the conclusions of the intervention studies in this review.
Despite the above limitations, this study is the first to analyze the relationship between mindfulness and motivation and the effect of intervention by integrating correlation studies and intervention studies. Although our findings may indicate that trait mindfulness and systematic mindfulness intervention could improve the level of individual motivation, further more intervention designs still need to be studied in different type of motivation to verify this finding. In addition, Mindfulness was more closely associated with intrinsic motivation than extrinsic motivation. Our study further confirms that mindfulness practice, a low-cost, easy-to-implement and productive daily training, has the potential to increase motivation levels in individuals’ daily lives and thus improve the quality of life. Future studies should pay more attention to the long-term effect of mindfulness intervention on motivational enhancement and its intrinsic neural mechanism.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Author contributions
Y-yW and YW designed the study and deeply modified the manuscript. L-yL performed the statistical analysis and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. XM and W-tH analyzedthe data andmodified the manuscript.J-sG, T-hC, J-cL, M-yH, and H-yL read, selected, and evaluated previous articles. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This study was supported by a grant from the Natural Science Foundation of Shandong Province (ZR2021MC103), the Humanities and Social Science Research Project, Ministry of Education, China (19YJA190006), the Postgraduate Tutor Guidance Ability Improvement Project of Shandong Province (SDYKC20147) and the Science and Technology Project of Weifang Medical University.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2023.1159902/full#supplementary-material
References
1. Zhang, M, Li, Y, Li, J, and Liu, X. The influence of extrinsic and intrinsic motivation on memory in adolescents and the underlying neural mechanisms. Adv Psychol Sci. (2023) 31:1–9. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2023.00001
2. Howard, JL, Bureau, J, Guay, F, Chong, JXY, and Ryan, RM. Student motivation and associated outcomes: a meta-analysis from self-determination theory. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2021) 16:1300–23. doi: 10.1177/1745691620966789
3. Amemiya, R, and Sakairi, Y. The effects of passion and mindfulness on the intrinsic motivation of Japanese athletes. Personal Individ Differ. (2019) 142:132–8. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.01.006
4. Hutmacher, D, Eckelt, M, Bund, A, Melzer, A, and Steffgen, G. Uncovering the role of mindfulness in autonomous motivation across physical education and leisure time: extending the trans-contextual model. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:12999. doi: 10.3390/ijerph192012999
5. Yusainy, C, Chan, DKC, Hikmiah, Z, and Anggono, CO. Physical activity in Indonesian university students: the contradictory roles of dispositional mindfulness and self-control. Psychol Health Med. (2019) 24:446–55. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2018.1546015
6. Robert, P, Onyike, CU, Leentjens, AF, Dujardin, K, Aalten, P, Starkstein, S, et al. Proposed diagnostic criteria for apathy in Alzheimer’s disease and other neuropsychiatric disorders. Eur Psychiatry. (2009) 24:98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.09.001
7. Starkstein, SE, and Leentjens, AF. The nosological position of apathy in clinical practice. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2008) 79:1088–92. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.136895
8. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental health disorders. 5 160–164, Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
9. Fervaha, G, Foussias, G, Agid, O, and Remington, G. Impact of primary negative symptoms on functional outcomes in schizophrenia. Eur Psychiatry. (2014) 29:449–55. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.01.007
10. Barch, DM, Treadway, MT, and Schoen, N. Effort, anhedonia, and function in schizophrenia: reduced effort allocation predicts amotivation and functional impairment. J Abnorm Psychol. (2014) 123:387–97. doi: 10.1037/a0036299
11. Hull, CL. Principles of behavior: an introduction to behavior theory. New York: Appleton-Century (1943).
12. Ryan, RM, and Deci, EL. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: classic definitions and new directions. Contemp Educ Psychol. (2000) 25:54–67. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1020
13. Deci, EL, Koestner, R, and Ryan, R. A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological bulletin. (1999) 125:627–700. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.627
14. Lepper, MR, and Henderlong, J. Turning “play” into “work” and “work” into “play”: 25 years of research on intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation In: C Sansone and JM Harackiewicz, editors. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: the search for optimal motivation and performance. San Diego, CA: Academic Press (2000). 257–307.
15. Wigfield, A, Rosenzweig, E, and Eccles, J. Achievement values In: AJ Elliott, CS Dweck, and DS Yeager, editors. Handbook of competence and motivation: theory and application. New York: The Guilford Press (2017). 116–34.
16. Schunk, DH, and DiBenedetto, MK. Self-efficacy theory in education In: Handbook of motivation at school. 2nd ed. London: Routledge (2016). 34–52.
17. Abramson, LY, Seligman, ME, and Teasdale, JD. Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation. J Abnorm Psychol. (1978) 87:49–74. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.87.1.49
18. Brown, KW, and Ryan, RM. The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2003) 84:822–48. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
19. Kabat-Zinn, J. Full catastrophe living: using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. 15th ed. New York: Bantam Dell (2005).
20. Bryant, FB, and Smith, JL. Appreciating life in the midst of adversity: savoring in relation to mindfulness, reappraisal, and meaning. Psychol Inq. (2015) 26:315–21. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2015.1075351
21. Bryant, FB, and Verhoff, J. Savoring: a new model of positive experience. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers (2007).
22. Baer, RA, Smith, GT, and Allen, KB. Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: the Kentucky inventory of mindfulness skills. Assessment. (2004) 11:191–206. doi: 10.1177/1073191104268029
23. Kiken, LG, Garland, EL, Bluth, K, Palsson, OS, and Gaylord, SA. From a state to a trait: trajectories of state mindfulness in meditation during intervention predict changes in trait mindfulness. Personal Individ Differ. (2015) 81:41–6. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.044
24. Ryan, RM, and Deci, EL. Self-determination theory: basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. New York: The Guilford Press (2017).
25. Isbel, BD, Lagopoulos, J, Hermens, DF, and Summers, MJ. Mental training affects electrophysiological markers of attention resource allocation in healthy older adults. Neurosci Lett. (2019) 698:186–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2019.01.029
26. Fountain-Zaragoza, S, and Prakash, RS. Mindfulness training for healthy aging: impact on attention, well-being, and inflammation. Front Aging Neurosci. (2017) 9:11. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00011
27. Geiger, PJ, Boggero, IA, Brake, CA, Caldera, CA, Combs, HL, Peters, JR, et al. Mindfulness-based interventions for older adults: a review of the effects on physical and emotional well-being. Mindfulness. (2016) 7:296–307. doi: 10.1007/s12671-015-0444-1
28. Li, SYH, and Bressington, D. The effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on depression, anxiety, and stress in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Ment Health Nurs. (2019) 28:635–56. doi: 10.1111/inm.12568
29. Zhang, R, Sun, L, and Shi, J. A study of the clinical effects of psychotherapy on patients with anxiety disorders. Chin Remed Clin. (2017) 17:83–4. doi: 10.11655/zgywylc2017.01.030
30. Rusch, HL, Rosario, M, Levison, LM, Olivera, A, Livingston, WS, Wu, T, et al. The effect of mindfulness meditation on sleep quality: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2019) 1445:5–16. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13996
31. Lenze, EJ, Hickman, S, Hershey, T, Wendleton, L, Ly, K, Dixon, D, et al. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for older adults with worry symptoms and co-occurring cognitive dysfunction. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2014) 29:991–1000. doi: 10.1002/gps.4086
32. Thomas, EA, and Garland, EL. Mindfulness is associated with increased hedonic capacity among chronic pain patients receiving extended opioid pharmacotherapy. Clin J Pain. (2017) 33:166–73. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0000000000000379
33. Montani, F, Courcy, F, Battistelli, A, and de Witte, H. Job insecurity and innovative work behaviour: a moderated mediation model of intrinsic motivation and trait mindfulness. Stress Health. (2021) 37:742–54. doi: 10.1002/smi.3034
34. Neace, SM, Hicks, AM, DeCaro, MS, and Salmon, PG. Trait mindfulness and intrinsic exercise motivation uniquely contribute to exercise self-efficacy. J Am Coll Heal. (2022) 70:13–7. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2020.1748041
35. Tekin, MS, Özdemir, N, and Şahin, ŞK. Effect of attachment styles, emotional regulation difficulty and mindful attention levels on treatment motivation in patients with substance use disorder. J Subst Abus. (2021) 26:441–8. doi: 10.1080/14659891.2020.1846807
36. Liu, YC, Li, IL, and Hsiao, FH. Effectiveness of mindfulness-based intervention on psychotic symptoms for patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Adv Nurs. (2021) 77:2565–80. doi: 10.1111/jan.14750
37. Elphinstone, B, Egan, P, and Whitehead, R. Greater autonomous motivation for study and basic psychological need satisfaction by being presently aware and ‘letting go’: an exploration of mindful attention and nonattachment. Motiv Emot. (2020) 45:1–12. doi: 10.1007/s11031-020-09836-4
38. Ruffault, A, Bernier, M, Juge, N, and Fournier, JF. Mindfulness may moderate the relationship between intrinsic motivation and physical activity: a cross-sectional study. Mindfulness. (2016) 7:445–52. doi: 10.1007/s12671-015-0467-7
39. Marion-Jetten, AS, Taylor, G, and Schattke, K. Mind your goals, mind your emotions: mechanisms explaining the relation between dispositional mindfulness and action crises. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. (2022) 48:3–18. doi: 10.1177/0146167220986310
40. Donald, JN, Bradshaw, EL, Ryan, RM, Basarkod, G, Ciarrochi, J, Duineveld, JJ, et al. Mindfulness and its association with varied types of motivation: a systematic review and meta-analysis using self-determination theory. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. (2020) 46:1121–38. doi: 10.1177/0146167219896136
41. Page, MJ, McKenzie, JE, Bossuyt, PM, Boutron, I, Hoffmann, TC, Mulrow, CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. (2021) 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71
42. Hedges, JI, Cowie, GL, Ertel, JR, Barbour, RJ, and Hatcher, PG. Degradation of carbohydrates and lignins in buried woods. Geochim Cosmochim Acta. (1985) 49:701–11. doi: 10.1016/0016-7037(85)90165-6
43. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2019) Available at: https://www.R-project.org/.
44. Schwarzer, G, Carpenter, JR, and Rücker, G. Meta-analysis with R. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing (2015).
45. Huedo-Medina, TB, Sánchez-Meca, J, Marín-Martínez, F, and Botella, J. Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index? Psychol Methods. (2006) 11:193–206. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.2.193
46. Higgins, JP, Thompson, SG, Deeks, JJ, and Altman, DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. (2003) 327:557–60. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557
47. Duval, S, and Tweedie, R. Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. (2000) 56:455–63. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x
48. Egger, M, Davey Smith, G, Schneider, M, and Minder, C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. (1997) 315:629–34. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629
49. Atkins, D, Best, D, Briss, PA, Eccles, M, Falck-Ytter, Y, Flottorp, S, et al. Grading quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. BMJ. (2004) 328:1490. doi: 10.1136/bmj.328.7454.1490
50. Higgins, J.P.T., and Green, S., The Cochrane Collaboration (2011) Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Version 5.1.0. Available at: http://handbook-5-1.cochrane.org
51. Ali, M, Khan, AN, Khan, MM, Butt, AS, and Shah, SHH. Mindfulness and study engagement: mediating role of psychological capital and intrinsic motivation. J Prof Cap Commun. (2021) 7:144–58. doi: 10.1108/jpcc-02-2021-0013
52. Lubatkin, MH, Simsek, Z, Ling, Y, and Veiga, JF. Ambidexterity and performance in small-to medium-sized firms: the pivotal role of top management team behavioral integration. J Manag. (2006) 32:646–72. doi: 10.1177/0149206306290712
53. Höfling, V, Moosbrugger, H, Schermelleh-Engel, K, and Heidenreich, T. Mindfulness or mindlessness?: A modified version of the mindful attention and awareness scale (MAAS). Eur J Psychol Assess. (2011) 27:59–64. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000045
54. Ying, T, Tan, X, Ye, S, Ka, X, and Zhou, Y. Examining tourist mindfulness in museums: the roles of familiarity and motivation. Asia Pac J Tour. Res. (2020) 25:981–96. doi: 10.1080/10941665.2020.1819835
55. Guay, F, Vallerand, RJ, and Blanchard, C. On the assessment of situational intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: the situational motivation scale (SIMS). Motiv Emot. (2000) 24:175–213. doi: 10.1023/A:1005614228250
56. Vallerand, RJ, Pelletier, LG, Blais, MR, Briere, NM, Senecal, C, and Vallieres, EF. The academicmotivation scale: a measure of intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivation in education. Educ Psychol Meas. (1992) 52:1003–17. doi: 10.1177/0013164492052004025
57. Frauman, E, and Norman, WC. Mindfulness as a tool for managing visitors to tourism destinations. J Travel Res. (2004) 42:381–9. doi: 10.1177/0047287504263033
58. Pan, M, and Liu, J. Chinese English as a foreign language teachers’ wellbeing and motivation: the role of mindfulness. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:906779. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.906779
59. Dweik, BS, and Awajan, NW. Factors that enhance English language teachers’ motivation in Jordanian secondary schools. Engl Linguist Res. (2013) 2:33–42. doi: 10.5430/elr.v2n1p33
60. Böge, K, Pollex, F, Bergmann, N, Hahne, I, Zierhut, MM, Mavituna, S, et al. Mindfulness, cognitive fusion, and self-compassion in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders-a cross-sectional study. Front Psychiatry. (2022) 13:959467. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.959467
61. Chen, J, Lin, G, and Lyu, Y. The impact of self-compassionate mindfulness on online learning behavioral engagement of international students during COVID-19: positive emotion and self-improvement motivation as mediators. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:969657. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.969657
62. Breines, JG, and Chen, S. Self-compassion increases self-improvement motivation. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. (2012) 38:1133–43. doi: 10.1177/0146167212445599
63. Ghanizadeh, A, Makiabadi, H, and Navokhi, S. Relating EFL university students’ mindfulness and resilience to self-fulfilment and motivation in learning. Issues Educ Res. (2019) 24:695–714. Available at: http://www.iier.org.au/iier29/ghanizadeh.pdf
64. Schmidt, R, Boraie, D, and Kassabgy, O. Foreign language motivation: internal structure and external connections In: RL Oxford, editor. Language learning motivation: pathways to the new century. Honolulu, HI: The University of Honolulu Press (1996). 14–87. Available at: https://nflrc.hawaii.edu/publications/view/TR11/
65. Dust, SB, Liu, H, Wang, S, and Reina, CS. The effect of mindfulness and job demands on motivation and performance trajectories across the workweek: an entrainment theory perspective. J Appl Psychol. (2022) 107:221–39. doi: 10.1037/apl0000887
66. Wanberg, CR, Zhu, J, Kanfer, R, and Zhang, Z. After the pink slip: applying dynamic motivation frameworks to the job search experience. Acad Manag J. (2012) 55:261–84. doi: 10.5465/amj.2010.0157
67. Kimmes, JG, Jaurequi, ME, May, RW, Srivastava, S, and Fincham, FD. Mindfulness in the context of romantic relationships: initial development and validation of the relationship mindfulness measure. J Marital Fam Ther. (2018) 44:575–89. doi: 10.1111/jmft.12296
68. Levesque, C, and Brown, KW. Mindfulness as a moderator of the effect of implicit motivational self-concept on day-to-day behavioral motivation. Motiv Emot. (2007) 31:284–99. doi: 10.1007/s11031-007-9075-8
69. Torok, E, and Keri, S. The relationship among mentalization, mindfulness, working memory, and schizotypal personality traits in the general population. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:682889. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.682889
70. Wu, CH, Nien, JT, Lin, CY, Nien, YH, Kuan, G, Wu, TY, et al. Relationship between mindfulness, psychological skills, and mental toughness in college athletes. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:6802. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18136802
71. Bernstein, A, Tanay, G, and Vujanovic, AA. Concurrent relations between mindful attention and awareness and psychopathology among trauma-exposed adults. J Cogn Psychother. (2011) 25:99–113. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.25.2.99
72. Gagné, M. The role of autonomy support and autonomy orientation in pro-social behavior engagement. Motiv Emot. (2003) 27:199–223. doi: 10.1023/A:1025007614869
73. Sheldon, KM, Osin, EN, Gordeeva, TO, Suchkov, DD, and Sychev, OA. Evaluating the dimensionality of self-determination theory’s relative autonomy continuum. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. (2017) 43:1215–38. doi: 10.1177/0146167217711915
74. Tabak, NT, Horan, WP, and Green, MF. Mindfulness in schizophrenia: associations with self-reported motivation, emotion regulation, dysfunctional attitudes, and negative symptoms. Schizophr Res. (2015) 168:537–42. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.07.030
75. Brown, K, Goodman, RJ, Ryan, RM, and Anālayo, B. Mindfulness enhances episodic memory performance: evidence from a multimethod investigation. PLoS One. (2016) 11:e0153309. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153309
76. Cox, AE, Ullrich-French, S, and Austin, B. Testing the Role of State Mindfulness in Facilitating Autonomous Physical Activity Motivation. Mindfulness. (2020) 11:1018–1027. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01311-y
77. Moir, F, Henning, M, Hassed, C, Moyes, SA, and Elley, CR. A Peer-Support and Mindfulness Program to Improve the Mental Health of Medical Students. Teaching and learning in medicine. (2016) 28:293–302. doi: 10.1080/10401334.2016.1153475
78. Oberleiter, S, Wainig, H, Voracek, M, and Tran, US. No Effects of a Brief Mindfulness Intervention on Controlled Motivation and Amotivation, but Effect Moderation Through Trait Mindfulness: a Randomized Controlled Trial. Mindfulness. (2022) 13:2434–2447. doi: 10.1007/s12671-022-01968-7
79. Smyth, A, and Milyavskaya, M. Mindfully motivated: Can a brief session of mindfulness meditation enhance motivation towards personal goals?. European Journal of Social Psychology. (2021) 51:758–772. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2770
80. Zanesco, AP, King, BG, and Saron, CD. Executive control and felt concentrative engagement following intensive meditation training. TFrontiers in human neuroscience. (2013) 7:566. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00566
81. Mihelič, KK, and Culiberg, B. Reaping the fruits of another’s labor: the role of moral meaningfulness, mindfulness, and motivation in social loafing. J Bus Ethics. (2018) 160:713–27. doi: 10.1007/s10551-018-3933-z
82. Di Domenico, SI, and Ryan, RM. The emerging neuroscience of intrinsic motivation: a new frontier in self-determination research. Front Hum Neurosci. (2017) 11:145. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00145
83. Lepper, MR, Sethi, S, Dialdin, D, and Drake, M. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: a developmental perspective In: SS Luthar, JA Burack, D Cicchetti, and JR Weisz, editors. Developmental psychopathology: perspectives on adjustment, risk, and disorder. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (1997). 23–50.
84. Hanley, AW, Mehling, WE, and Garland, EL. Holding the body in mind: interoceptive awareness, dispositional mindfulness and psychological well-being. J Psychosom Res. (2017) 99:13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.05.014
85. Shapiro, SL, Carlson, LE, Astin, JA, and Freedman, B. Mechanisms of mindfulness. J Clin Psychol. (2006) 62:373–86. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20237
86. Smart, CM, Segalowitz, SJ, Mulligan, BP, Koudys, J, and Gawryluk, JR. Mindfulness training for older adults with subjective cognitive decline: results from a pilot randomized controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis. (2016) 52:757–74. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150992
87. Wong, WP, Coles, J, Chambers, R, Wu, DB, and Hassed, C. The effects of mindfulness on older adults with mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis Rep. (2017) 1:181–93. doi: 10.3233/ADR-170031
88. Ferrari, AJ, Charlson, FJ, Norman, RE, Patten, SB, Freedman, G, Murray, CJ, et al. Burden of depressive disorders by country, sex, age, and year: findings from the global burden of disease study 2010. PLoS Med. (2013) 10:e1001547. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001547
89. Ahmadpanah, M, Akbari, T, Akhondi, A, Haghighi, M, Jahangard, L, Sadeghi Bahmani, D, et al. Detached mindfulness reduced both depression and anxiety in elderly women with major depressive disorders. Psychiatry Res. (2017) 257:87–94. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.07.030
90. Ghahari, S, Mohammadi-Hasel, K, Malakouti, SK, and Roshanpajouh, M. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for generalised anxiety disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. East Asian Arch Psychiatry. (2020) 30:52–6. doi: 10.12809/eaap1885
91. Hoge, EA, Bui, E, Marques, L, Metcalf, CA, Morris, LK, Robinaugh, DJ, et al. Randomized controlled trial of mindfulness meditation for generalized anxiety disorder: effects on anxiety and stress reactivity. J Clin Psychiatry. (2013) 74:786–92. doi: 10.4088/JCP.12m08083
92. Vignaud, P, Donde, C, Sadki, T, Poulet, E, and Brunelin, J. Neural effects of mindfulness-based interventions on patients with major depressive disorder: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 88:98–105. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.03.004
93. Zhou, R, and Liu, L. Eight-week mindfulness training enhances left frontal EEG asymmetry during emotional challenge: a randomized controlled trial. Mindfulness. (2017) 8:181–9. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0591-z
Keywords: mindfulness, motivation, intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, amotivation
Citation: Li L-y, Meng X, Hu W-t, Geng J-s, Cheng T-h, Luo J-c, Hu M-y, Li H-y, Wang Y and Wang Y-y (2023) A meta-analysis of the association between mindfulness and motivation. Front. Public Health. 11:1159902. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1159902
Edited by:
Lei Feng, National University of Singapore, SingaporeReviewed by:
Patricia Silveira Martins, Independent Researcher, Belo Horizonte, BrazilHiroto Narimatsu, Kanagawa Cancer Center, Japan
Copyright © 2023 Li, Meng, Hu, Geng, Cheng, Luo, Hu, Li, Wang and Wang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Yan-yu Wang, wangyanyu@wfmc.edu.cn