Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 15 February 2023
Sec. Aging and Public Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Novel Indicators and Strategies for Prevention and Management of Physical and Cognitive Frailty in Aging Population View all 14 articles

Incidence of cognitive impairment after hypothetical interventions on depression, nighttime sleep duration, and leisure activity engagement among older Chinese adults: An application of the parametric g-formula

\nNan ZhangNan Zhang1Fenghui ChenFenghui Chen2Cui WangCui Wang3Ping Yan
Ping Yan1*
  • 1Department of Surgical Nursing, School of Nursing, Xinjiang Medical University, Urumqi, Xinjiang, China
  • 2Department of Internal Medicine Nursing, School of Nursing, Xinjiang Medical University, Urumqi, Xinjiang, China
  • 3Department of Health Science, School of Nursing, Peking University, Beijing, China

Background: Cognitive impairment is an age-relevant intermediate stage where cognition declines to a state between the normal aging process and dementia. Earlier studies reported that depression, inappropriate nighttime sleep duration (NSD), and limited leisure activity engagement are cognitive impairment risk factors among older adults. Thus, we postulated that interventions on depression, sleep duration, and leisure activity engagement can reduce cognitive impairment risk. However, no earlier research ever explored this.

Methods: The data of 4,819 respondents aged 60 years and above, without cognitive impairment at baseline and with no prior history of memory-related diseases, including Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease, and encephalatrophy, were obtained from the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS) between 2011 and 2018. The parametric g-formula, an analytic tool for estimating standardized outcome distributions using covariate (exposure and confounders)-specific estimates of the outcome distribution, was used to estimate 7-year cumulative cognitive impairment risks among older Chinese adults, under independent hypothetical interventions on depression, NSD, and leisure activity engagement, which was subdivided into social activity (SA) and intellectual activity (IA) for the different intervention combinations.

Results: The observed cognitive impairment risk was 37.52%. Independent intervention on IA was the most effective factor in reducing incident cognitive impairment, with a risk ratio (RR) of 0.75 (95% confidence interval [CI]: 0.67–0.82), followed by depression (RR: 0.89, 95% CI: 0.85–0.93) and NSD (RR: 0.88, 95% CI: 0.80–0.95). The joint intervention combining depression, NSD, and IA interventions could reduce the risk by 17.11%, with an RR of 0.56 (95% CI: 0.48–0.65). In subgroup analyses, independent interventions on depression and IA had analogously significant effects on men and women. However, interventions on depression and IA had stronger effects on literate than illiterate individuals.

Conclusions: Hypothetical interventions on depression, NSD, and IA reduced cognitive impairment risks among older Chinese adults, both independently and jointly. The findings of the present study suggest that the intervention measures on depression, inappropriate NSD, limited intellectual activities, and their combination may prove to be effective strategies for preventing cognitive impairment among older adults.

Introduction

Cognitive impairment is an age-relevant intermediate stage of cognitive decline between the normal aging process and dementia, featuring cognition declines in memory, visuospatial ability, orientation, calculation, execution, and comprehension (1, 2). Older adults with cognitive impairment tend to have a significantly higher risk of dementia, with a progression rate of 10%−30% per year, whereas those adults without cognitive impairment have a progression rate of 1%−2% annually (3). Accordingly, it is important to identify older adults with cognitive impairment not only to develop interventions that alleviate individual suffering but also because this represents a population that is at an increased risk of developing dementia. Among older adults aged 60 years and above, the global prevalence of cognitive impairment ranges from 5.1% to 41.0% (4). In China, it is reported that the prevalence of cognitive impairment varies from 2.40% to 39.88% among older adults in different provinces, and approximately 36,000 additional cases of cognitive impairment occur annually (2, 5). The Chinese population is aging dramatically. In 2021, the population of people aged 60 years and above was 267 million, but it is projected to surpass the 400 million mark by 2035 (6). Therefore, China faces several challenges with regard to cognitive impairment among older adults, and early screening and intervention for cognitive impairment should receive more attention.

Evidence shows that women and individuals with low education levels are common risk factors for cognitive impairment (7, 8). An earlier study showed that overall cognitive impairment is more serious in women than in men (9). Consistent with this result, another study confirmed that Chinese women are significantly disadvantaged when it comes to cognitive functioning in old age (10). As regards education, a Chinese-based longitudinal study revealed that a low education level is associated with a high risk of cognitive impairment among older adults (5). As well as gender and education level, depression, inappropriate nighttime sleep duration (NSD), and limited leisure activity engagement are reported as other risk factors for cognitive impairment among older adults (1113). Obvious issues with addressing the immutable characteristic of gender, and difficulty with changing the education level, especially in older adults, are prevalent. However, numerous studies revealed that treatments or interventions on depression, inappropriate NSD, and limited leisure activity engagement can not only ameliorate the symptoms themselves but may also be beneficial for improving cognitive performance and reducing the risk of cognitive impairment, although the results of these studies are mixed. For depression, reviews by Motter et al. (14) and Culpepper et al. (15), which examined the effectiveness of cognitive remediation therapy (CRT) in improving depression and cognition, revealed that CRT is effective in reducing depressive symptoms and improving cognitive functions. However, Wong et al. (16) and Adler et al. (17) demonstrated that, in cognitively impaired patients with depression, treatments are only effective in improving depression and not cognitive impairment. As regards the NSD, earlier studies reported that changes from long (≥9 h) to moderate (6–9 h) sleep duration (18), or from short (< 6 h) to moderate (6–9 h) sleep duration (19), were negatively associated with cognitive impairment risks. Nevertheless, three other studies demonstrated that, compared with unchanged sleep durations in baseline and follow-ups, any increases or decreases in sleep durations were associated with worse cognitive performances or increased cognitive impairment risks, despite using the different sleep duration references (2022). Participation in leisure activity and its subtypes, namely social activity (SA), physical activity (PA), and intellectual activity (IA), has been proven to be beneficial for the preservation of cognitive function (23). Several studies examining the association between leisure activity engagement and cognitive function in older adults demonstrated that SA and/or IA are relevant to decreased cognitive deficit and risks of dementia (2426). Notably, changes in leisure activity engagement also have effects on subsequent trajectories of age-related cognitive performance, so individuals with more leisure activity engagement than before will have slower cognitive decline rates as they age (13). Although some, mostly observational, studies confirmed the risk factors of cognitive impairment to a certain extent, many limitations are prevalent with regard to not properly controlling confounding variates such as time-varying confounders in observational studies and a lack of, or inability to, estimate the causal effect of risk factors.

The standard for evaluating the comparative effectiveness and the causal effect of an intervention is a randomized controlled trial (RCT). However, when RCTs are not feasible or timely, the impact of potential treatment strategies can be informed empirically using observational data to emulate a target trial (27). Target trial emulation is the application of design principles from RCTs to the analysis of observational data, thereby explicitly tying the analysis to the trial it is emulating (28). This requires a clear specification of the trial protocol elements and, when assessing interventions sustained over time, an analytic method known as the g-formula is required to appropriately account for time-dependent confounding variate (27). The g-formula, first described by Robins (29) in 1986, is used to estimate the causal effect of arsenic on heart disease in an occupationally exposed cohort and is an analytic tool for estimating standardized outcome distributions using covariate (exposure and confounders)-specific estimates of the outcome distribution (30). It can be viewed as a generalized form of standardization of the conditional hazard under each treatment strategy to the joint distribution of the time-varying covariates so that it can appropriately adjust for time-varying confounders affected by prior exposures. It is especially well-suited to estimating effects when the intervention involves multiple factors (joint interventions) or decisions that depend on the value of evolving time-dependent factors (31). Moreover, while observational analyses are restricted to a framework where one can only test interventions that have been explicitly carried out in the data, the g-formula enables one to simulate treatment strategies (also called hypothetical intervention) and to estimate their effect, even if those strategies have not been fully carried out in the data used to construct the model (32). Earlier studies using the parametric g-formula were conducted to measure hypothetical interventions on the levels of blood pressure, cholesterol, weight, PA, smoking, and alcohol intake for preventing stroke (33) and to estimate the risk of fall under single and joint interventions on sleep duration, SA, smoking, drinking, body mass index (BMI), systolic blood pressure, vision, depression, and activities of daily living scores (ADLs) (34). However, research using the parametric g-formula to investigate the risks of cognitive impairment under hypothetical interventions is limited. Therefore, the present study aimed at estimating the 7-year cumulative risks of cognitive impairment under independent interventions on depression, NSD, SA, and IA, as well as joint interventions consisting of different combinations thereof, using the parametric g-formula on the basis of a nationwide representative cohort of the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS). Such an estimation can help guide and provide intervention strategies and alternative treatments for the prevention of cognitive impairment among community-dwelling older Chinese adults.

Materials and methods

Data sources and participants

Data were extracted from four waves of CHARLS conducted in 2011 (wave 1), 2013 (wave 2), 2015 (wave 3), and 2018 (wave 4). CHARLS is a national longitudinal survey aimed at investigating and evaluating the aging issue of adults from 150 country-level units distributed in 28 provinces of China. It has been conducted by the Institute of Social Science Survey of Peking University since 2011, which updates the aforementioned survey every 2 years. The participants were informed of the research purpose and signed written informed consent forms to participate. Ethical approval was obtained from the Biomedical Ethics Review Committee of Peking University (IRB00001052-11015) (35).

In total, 17,954 respondents were included in the first wave (2011), and these data were used as the baseline. To emulate hypothetical interventions on cognitive impairment risks, the inclusion and exclusion criteria were set according to the g-formula principle. The inclusion criteria of the present study were as follows: (1) participants aged 60 years and above, (2) participants who had no cognitive impairment at baseline, and (3) participants who had completed at least one follow-up. The exclusion criteria of the present study were as follows: (1) participants' key variables were missing and (2) participants were diagnosed with memory-related diseases including Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease, and encephalatrophy at baseline. Finally, a total of 4,819 respondents were included in the analysis. The sample selection flowchart for this study is presented in Figure 1. For each participant, the follow-up was ended at the time of onset of cognitive impairment, when the patient was lost to follow-up, or when the patient was part of the examination of wave 4 (2018), whichever occurred first.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Sample selection flowchart based on the 2011–2018 China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS) data.

Cognitive impairment

The cognitive function was assessed by four dimensions comprising orientation, attention, episodic memory, and visuospatial ability. Cognition scores were calculated using the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS-10), word recall, and picture redrawing. An overall cognition score (36) was considered the primary outcome of interest. The sum of all three TICS-10 scores (orientation and attention), word recall (episodic memory), and figure drawing (visual spatial ability) was then calculated to assess the global cognitive function, with the score ranging from 0 to 21. A higher score indicated a better cognitive function. Cronbach's α was 0.84 across all four waves.

Due to the discordancy of the criteria among multiple measurements, we used the concept of aging-associated cognitive decline (AACD) to define cognitive impairment, requiring at least one standard deviation (SD) below age-appropriate norms (10, 37). AACD—which is recommended by the International Psychogeriatric Association in collaboration with the World Health Organization (the WHO)—seeks evidence of cognitive decline within a broader range of cognitive domains, covering all criteria for estimating cognitive impairment. The AACD has been widely used as diagnostic criteria for estimating cognitive decline by both clinicians and scholars, therefore fitting the aim of this study (38). Respondents were divided into age groups, with each group representing a 5-year interval, and individuals with cognition scores lower than 1 SD from their group mean were defined as having cognitive impairment.

Intervention variables

Depressive symptoms were assessed using the 10-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D-10) Scale which comprised 10 depressive effects: bothered by things that usually did not bother them, trouble in concentration, feeling depressed, effort exerted to complete daily tasks, feeling fearful, restless sleep, loneliness, inability to “get going,” feeling hopeful about the future, and feeling happy. Each of the 4-option responses to the item was scored from 0 to 3, and the total score was the sum of points for all 10 items. A total score of 10 or higher indicated clinical depression (39).

Nighttime sleep duration was obtained from the question “During the past month, how many hours of actual sleep did you get at night?” As reported in earlier studies, we took an NSD of 6 h as the reference duration (12) and classified participants into a 6-hr group and a non-6-hr group.

Leisure activity engagement was assessed through SA and IA (24). The SA items included: interacting with friends; going to a sport, social, or any other kind of club; taking part in a community-related organization; and doing volunteer or charity work. The IA items included: playing Mahjong, chess, or cards; attending an education or training course; stock investment; and surfing the Internet. The frequency of each activity was rated as never (score = 0), not regularly (score = 1), almost every week (score = 2), and almost daily (score = 3). All activity types were synthesized to a sum score ranging from 0 to 12 based on the frequency level. Participants whose sum score was > 1 were categorized as “participating in SA/IA”; otherwise, they were regarded as “not participating in SA/IA.”

Covariates

Baseline covariates included gender (male/female), age (continuous), education level (literate/illiterate), and marital status (married/other). Time-varying covariates included ADLs (restricted/unrestricted), instrumental activities of daily living scores (IADLs, restricted/unrestricted), chronic diseases (0/1–2 diseases/ ≥ 3 diseases), smoking status (none/quit/still smokes), drinking status (none of these/drinks but less than once a month/drinks more than once a month), and PA (continuous).

Functional disability was measured based on ADLs and IADLs. ADLs included six aspects: dressing, bathing or showering, eating, getting into or out of bed, using the toilet, and controlling urination and defecation. IADLs comprised five aspects: doing household chores, preparing hot meals, shopping, taking medications, and managing money. The response scale contained four options: “no, I do not have any difficulty,” “I have difficulty but can still do it,” “yes, I have difficulty and need help,” and “I cannot do it.” Participants whose responses were “no, I do not have any difficulty” for all items of ADLs or IADLs were classified as “unrestricted” to ADLs or IADLs; otherwise, they were classified as “restricted.” Chronic diseases were determined by asking “Have you been diagnosed with the following conditions by a doctor?” and comprised the following 13 diseases: hypertension, dyslipidemia, diabetes, or hyperglycemia; cancer or malignant tumor; chronic lung disease; liver disease; heart attack; coronary heart disease; angina; congestive heart failure; stroke; kidney disease; stomach, emotional, nervous or psychiatric problems; arthritis or rheumatism; and asthma. Participants were classified as having no diseases, one to two chronic diseases, and over two chronic diseases based on their responses. PA comprised the amount of time a person spent on vigorous activities, moderate activities, and walking in a usual week. According to the responses, we indexed the amount of PA per day as 1 (< 0.5 h), 2 (0.5–2 h), 3 (2–4 h), and 4 (>4 h). The weekly PA duration score was calculated by multiplying the number of days the activity was performed and the daily PA duration index for each activity. Finally, we generated the PA score variables using metabolic equivalent (MET) multipliers as follows: PA score = 8.0 × total vigorous activity weekly duration score + 4.0 × total moderate activity weekly duration score + 3.3 × total walking weekly duration score (40).

Hypothetical interventions

The parametric g-formula was used to estimate cognitive impairment risks under each of the following hypothetical interventions. Single interventions were as follows:

1. Depression: Intervention resolved all depressive symptoms in all participants.

2. NSD: Intervention resulted in all participants sleeping for 6 h per night.

3. SA: Intervention ensured that all participants took part in SA.

4. IA: Intervention ensured that all participants took part in IA.

Joint interventions consisted of combinations of single interventions, which were significant in the last stage of analysis.

Statistical analysis

The 7-year cumulative risks of cognitive impairment under hypothetical interventions were estimated by applying the parametric g-formula. The g-formula directly models probabilities for a given outcome conditional upon covariates and exposures/interventions (32). For real-world datasets, directly modeling all conditional probabilities is not feasible, especially in the presence of continuous covariates. The parametric g-formula is an extension of the g-formula, where parametric models are used to model probabilities, instead of direct calculations. Under the assumptions of no unmeasured confounding and no model misspecification, this method can provide an estimate of the risk of outcomes under full adherence to different hypothetical sustained interventions (27). The standardized risk is estimated by a weighted average of the risks of cognitive impairment conditional on the given intervention and observed confounder history. The weights are probability distribution functions of the time-varying confounders estimated using parametric regression models. The weighted average is approximated through the Monte Carlo simulation (41). We used cognitive impairment as the outcome and simulated cognitive impairment risks under each single and joint intervention using the parametric g-formula with the following four-step algorithm (42):

1. Fit a parametric regression model for each time-varying covariate, as a function of the baseline covariate and covariate history among participants followed up to time k.

2. Estimate the conditional probability of cognitive impairment, as a function of conditional on intervention and covariate history and surviving and remaining uncensored among participants followed up to time k, using a pooled over-time logistic regression model to approximate time-to-failure risk.

3. Perform a Monte Carlo simulation to generate life histories for a pseudo-population of 10,000 simulated individuals, in which baseline covariates are randomly sampled with replacements from the original population. Time-varying covariates are drawn from the parametric distribution in Step 1, and intervention covariates are set based on the defined strategy, except for the natural course (no intervention) strategy.

4. Compute the intervention risk estimate of cognitive impairment at 7 years in the pseudopopulation. The risk ratio (RR)/risk difference (RD) can be constructed between each strategy and the natural course. Ninety-five percent confidence intervals (CIs) are computed by repeating the entire aforementioned steps in 500 bootstrap samples.

The aforementioned steps were repeated for each single and joint intervention to compute cognitive impairment risks, RRs, and RDs. We also conducted subgroup analyses to assess different effects of each single intervention according to gender (male and female) and education (illiteracy and literacy). To test the model reliability, sensitivity analyses were performed by changing the order of time-varying covariates. All analyses were conducted using the R 4.1.2 statistical package for the g-formula for Windows.

Results

Baseline characteristics

Table 1 summarizes the sample characteristics at baseline. The mean age of respondents was 68.0 years, with an SD of 6.6 years. The proportion of men was larger than that of women: 54.9% and 45.1%, respectively. Among the population, 71.4% were literate and 81.6% were married. Over 75% were classified as being without restricted ADLs (79.4%) and IADLs (77.3%) at baseline. As regards chronic diseases, 52.0% of the individuals had 1–2 such diseases and 22.0% of them had more than two. Significantly more participants had never smoked (53.9%) or drank (66.9%) than participating in those actions. The mean CES-D-10 score and NSD were 8.5 points and 6.3 h, respectively. Accordingly, the prevalence of individuals without depression and of those who sleep 6 h at night was 37.9% and 22.6%, respectively. As regards leisure activity engagement, the mean score of PA was 103.5. The prevalence of older adults participating in SA and IA was 40.7% and 19.4%, respectively.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The distribution of variables at baseline.

During the 7-year follow-up, 1,808 participants developed cognitive impairment, with a significantly lower prevalence in men than in women (28.9% vs. 48.1%); similarly, this tendency was observed in the literate and illiterate groups, with 25.3% and 68.1% prevalence, respectively.

Interventions in total population

The 7-year cumulative risks of cognitive impairment under four single hypothetical interventions, as estimated by the parametric g-formula, are shown in Table 2. The observed cognitive impairment risk was 37.52%, and the simulated risk in the natural course was 39.28% (95% CI: 35.99–44.12). Among the independent hypothetical interventions, participants who were in the IA intervention group showed the greatest risk reduction, with an RR of 0.75 (95% CI: 0.67–0.82). Notably, independent interventions for depression and NSD showed similar effects, with RRs of 0.89 (95% CI: 0.85–0.93) and 0.88 (95% CI: 0.80–0.95), respectively. However, participants who were in the SA intervention group did not show a decreased risk of cognitive impairment, with an RR of 1.00 (95% CI: 0.95–1.04).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Cognitive impairment risks at 7-year follow-up under natural course and single hypothetical interventions.

Table 3 depicts the cognitive impairment risks under different combinations of joint interventions, after exclusion of the SA intervention due to its ineffectiveness. All joint interventions could significantly reduce cognitive impairment risks among older adults. Due to the similar effects of independent interventions on depression and NSD, the combination of “Depression + IA” and “NSD + IA” showed similar effects, with RRs of 0.66 (95% CI: 0.58–0.73) and 0.65 (95% CI: 0.58–0.70), respectively. The joint intervention of “Depression + NSD” could reduce the risk by 8.93%, with an RR of 0.77 (95% CI: 0.69–0.84). The “All factors” joint intervention, that combined interventions on depression, NSD, and IA, reduced the risk by 17.11%, with an RR of 0.56 (95% CI: 0.48–0.65).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Cognitive impairment risks at 7-year follow-up under natural course and joint hypothetical interventions.

Interventions in subgroups

In subgroup analyses, intervention on SA was excluded once more due to its lack of significance. The results showed a difference in independent intervention when stratified by gender (Table 4) and education level (Table 5). The cognitive impairment risk in men was lower than that in women (33.83% vs. 49.48%) in the natural course, similar to the literate and illiterate groups (26.79% vs. 72.62%). For gender subgroups, in men, only independent interventions on depression and IA had significant effects on reducing the risks of cognitive impairment, with RRs of 0.88 (95% CI: 0.83–0.94) and 0.74 (95% CI: 0.63–0.86), respectively. In contrast, for women, all three independent interventions on depression, NSD, and IA had significant effects, with RRs of 0.89 (95% CI: 0.83–0.94), 0.84 (95% CI: 0.74–0.94), and 0.76 (95% CI: 0.65–0.85), respectively (Figure 2). For education subgroups, in the literate group, all three independent interventions on depression, NSD, and IA had significant effects, with RRs of 0.80 (95% CI: 0.74–0.86), 0.85 (95% CI: 0.73–0.99), and 0.69 (95% CI: 0.59–0.79), respectively. Similarly, in the illiterate group, all three independent interventions on depression, NSD, and IA had significant effects, with RRs of 0.96 (95% CI: 0.92–1.00), 0.90 (95% CI: 0.81–1.00, and 0.80 (95% CI: 0.66–0.93), respectively (Figure 3).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Cognitive impairment risks at 7-year follow-up under natural course and single hypothetical interventions in subgroups stratified by gender.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Cognitive impairment risks at 7-year follow-up under natural course and single hypothetical interventions in subgroups stratified by education.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Forest plot of RR for depression risk in subgroups stratified by gender (NSD, nighttime sleep duration; IA, intellectual activity, RR, risk ratio; CI, confidence interval).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Forest plot of RR for depression risk in subgroups stratified by education level (NSD, nighttime sleep duration; IA, intellectual activity, RR, risk ratio; CI, confidence interval).

Discussion

We estimated the 7-year cumulative risks of cognitive impairment under single and joint interventions on depression, sleep duration, and leisure activity engagement among older Chinese community-dwelling adults by applying the parametric g-formula to the nationally representative CHARLS cohort. The results of the present study suggest that single interventions on depression, NSD, and IA could reduce cognitive impairment risks, and that different combinations of these interventions reduced the risk further. Similar effects were observed in gender subgroups, with interventions on depression and IA reducing cognitive impairment risks. However, the effects of interventions on depression and IA were greater in literate than illiterate individuals.

The results of the present study showed that independent hypothetical interventions on depression, NSD, and IA could reduce cognitive impairment risks by 4.33%, 4.79%, and 9.94%, respectively, while the joint hypothetical intervention of all three factors could significantly reduce cognitive impairment risk by 17.11%. These findings confirmed the conclusions of earlier studies, wherein depression, inappropriate NSD, and limited IA were found to be risk factors for cognitive impairment among older adults (1113). As the g-formula is a causal-effect analytic method, the results of the present study showed the causal effects of depression, inappropriate NSD, and limited IA engagement on the incidence of cognitive impairment. Thus, the social and public health implications of this finding are that it can provide both a theoretical and data-driven basis for future RCTs of interventions on depression, inappropriate NSD, and limited IA engagement to prevent cognitive impairment among older adults. Moreover, our result is also helpful for community health service institutions to provide timely intervention for community-dwelling older adults with depression, inappropriate NSD, and limited IA engagement to reduce the risk of cognitive impairment. Nonetheless, it is unclear how these factors might increase cognitive impairment risks. Currently, researchers proposed several depression-relevant mechanisms as follows: (1) depressive symptoms can generate hyperactivity of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis, thus increasing glucocorticoids, possibly leading to hippocampal damage and development of cognitive impairment (43). (2) Depression can cause hippocampal atrophy and accelerate the loss in hippocampal volume in women via the primary mediating mechanism of glucocorticoids (44). (3) Depression may also contribute to a cognitive decline through other pathways such as vascular disease, inflammation, impact on nerve growth factors, or by increasing β-amyloid accumulation (45). As regards the impact of NSD on cognition, evidence shows that the mechanisms of short and long sleep durations are different. Short sleep duration contributes to cognitive impairment via several different pathologies such as impaired β-amyloid clearance, pathological tau, impaired synaptic plasticity, atrophy of the cortex, and circadian rhythm disturbances. In contrast, long sleep duration is relevant to sleep fragmentation and chronic inflammation, which are linked to lower cognition (46). Moreover, Spira et al. (47) demonstrated that, among individuals with a normal cognitive function, sleep durations of < 7 h and of >7 h may accelerate frontotemporal gray matter atrophy and subsequently increase the cognitive impairment risk. This result is not consistent with the results from both this study and an earlier one, which took 6 h as the reference sleep duration (12). These differences may have arisen due to differences in the study population such as race, study sample size, and population age bracket. The beneficial effects of IA engagement on impaired cognition risk reduction may be due to improved cognitive reserves. Excess β-amyloid can accumulate in the brain as plaques that block the gaps between synapses, which in turn affects the electrical signals used by the neurons to communicate with each other; moreover, the chemistry-imbalanced tau protein can entangle with other tau proteins to form tangles, which will destroy microtubules and prevent necessary nutrients from reaching nerve endings of neurons, thus causing the entire cell to become dysfunctional. All of these alterations can cause cognitive impairment (48). According to the cognitive reserve theory, IA can strengthen the functioning and plasticity of neural circuits, further increasing the cognitive reserve and decreasing the risk of cognitive impairment (25).

Cognitive impairment risks did not decrease under the SA intervention. A similar result was reported by Li et al. (48) who found that a higher participation in SA did not improve cognitive function. Moreover, an RCT conducted by Park et al. (49) also found that engagement in SA had limited cognitive benefits. Interpretations for this phenomenon may be attributed to the fact that reduced social participation is an early manifestation of cognitive impairment rather than a cause (48). Additionally, simple SA is not particularly helpful in optimizing cognition in the long term because of the relatively passive participation and low cognitive demand. Such passivity may not be particularly beneficial for enhancing cognitive reserve (50). Therefore, monitoring changes of engagement in SA, rather than intervening to increase SA participation, may be beneficial to prevent cognitive impairment among older adults via timely identification and intervention.

Notably, in subgroup analyses, although similar effects of interventions on depression and IA for cognitive impairment risks were observed in gender subgroups, interventions on depression and IA displayed stronger effects in literate than illiterate participants. Several studies reported that education years and high education levels could predict and alleviate the severity of depression (51, 52). Accordingly, the intervention on depression was effective for literate individuals, reducing cognitive impairment risks. As regards different effectiveness levels of interventions on IA for literate and illiterate individuals, as far as we know, we are the first to find a difference. We speculate that older adults with high education levels get more training and application during engagement because IA subtypes need more comprehend ability. As a result, intervention on IA was more effective for literate than for illiterate individuals. Recently, studies investigated the effectiveness of continuing education for the prevention of cognitive impairment and dementia. Thow et al. (53) reported that older adults who attended university courses over a period of 12 months showed significant improvement in their language processing capacity compared with healthy adult controls, but there was no change in their fluid cognitive function. However, another study, by Lenehan et al. (54), found that 92.5% of older individuals who undertook further education in 12-month part- or full-time university courses displayed a significantly linear increase in cognitive reserve over the 4 years of study, indicating that continuing education could improve cognitive function and offset cognitive decline. Because of these contradicting results, more studies focusing on the effectiveness of continuing education for the prevention of cognitive impairment and dementia are essential. If the possible preventive effects are confirmed, then interventions based on continuing education for older adults should be established and promoted in communities and nursing facilities.

The strengths of the present study are the population-based longitudinal design, standardized survey methods, rigorous case validation, and long follow-up time. We used the parametric g-formula to appropriately adjust for time-varying confounders and estimate the effects of independent and joint interventions on depression, NSD, and IA for a reduction in cognitive impairment risks. Thus, the estimates are more directly relevant and provide more information to guide public health and clinical decisions. However, the present study has several limitations. First, the validity of the g-formula approach relies on three common assumptions for observational research: no model misspecification, no measurement error, and no unmeasured confounding. We adjusted for many potential risk factors to alleviate the “no measurement error” and “no unmeasured confound issues,” which were inevitable for this observational study. Evidence that simulated data under no intervention using the parametric g-formula were analogous to the observed data, indicating the condition of no model misspecification. Our sensitivity analyses also revealed that our results were robust across different specifications. Second, the precondition of generalizing our results to other populations is based on the condition that the target population should have the same distribution of effect modifiers and interference patterns as our study population because the calculation principle of the g-formula is based on the standardized risk to the distribution of risk factors. Due to the advantage of a population-based sample, we consider that the conclusion of this study has a certain degree of generalizability to some extent. Finally, we implicitly assumed that the counterfactual outcome for all scenarios should be the same as the observed outcome under the observed exposure history. It should be noted that, in real life, idealized intervention effects and complete adherence of participants are hard to accomplish; therefore, our estimates purely correspond to the best-case scenario, rather than the specific circumstances of each program.

Conclusions

By applying the parametric g-formula to a longitudinal sample from CHARLS, we found that hypothetical interventions on depression, NSD, and IA can reduce cognitive impairment risks among older Chinese adults, both independently and jointly. Furthermore, we observed similar effects of interventions on depression and IA in gender subgroups, but interventions on depression and IA had stronger effects in literate than illiterate individuals. Thus, the findings of the present study suggest that intervention measures for depression, inappropriate NSD, and limited intellectual activities and any combination thereof may prove to be effective strategies for the prevention of cognitive impairment among older adults.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Biomedical Ethics Committee, Peking University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

NZ: conceptualization, data curation, statistical analysis, methodology, drafting, manuscript editing, and graphics production. FC and CW: methodology and reviewing. PY: conceptualization, methodology, supervision, reviewing, and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the Regional Collaborative Innovation Special Project of Autonomous Region—Science and Technology Aid Xinjiang Plan (No. 2022E02119) and the Xinjiang Medical University Doctoral Research Start-up Grant.

Acknowledgments

All authors thank CHARLS for providing data. They are grateful to those who designed, conducted, and participated in this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Xiong S, Liu S, Qiao Y, He D, Ke C, Shen Y. Estimation of losses of quality-adjusted life expectancy attributed to the combination of cognitive impairment and multimorbidity among Chinese adults aged 45 years and older. BMC Public Health. (2021) 21:24. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-10069-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Xue J, Li J, Liang J, Chen S. The prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in China: a systematic review. Aging Dis. (2018) 9:706–15. doi: 10.14336/ad.2017.0928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Falck RS, Davis JC, Best JR, Chan PCY Li LC, Wyrough AB, et al. Effect of a multimodal lifestyle intervention on sleep and cognitive function in older adults with probable mild cognitive impairment and poor sleep: a randomized clinical trial. J Alzheimers Dis. (2020) 76:179–93. doi: 10.3233/jad-200383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Pais R, Ruano L, Ofélia Ofélia PC, Barros H. Global cognitive impairment prevalence and incidence in community dwelling older adults-a systematic review. Geriatrics (Basel). (2020) 5:84. doi: 10.3390/geriatrics5040084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Zhang Q, Wu Y, Han T, Liu E. Changes in cognitive function and risk factors for cognitive impairment of the elderly in China: 2005–2014. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:2847. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16162847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. National Health Commission of the People's Republic of China. Report of the State Council on the Progress of Strengthening and Promoting the Work on Aging. National Health Commission of the People's Republic of China (2022). Available online at: https://www.nhc.gov.cn/wjw/mtbd/202209/2ca97b572ee34a38819cceaf5207dd1a.shtml (accessed November 3, 2022).

7. Millán-Calenti JC, Tubío J, Pita-Fernández S, González-Abraldes I, Lorenzo T, Maseda A. prevalence of cognitive impairment: effects of level of education, age, sex and associated factors. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2009) 28:455–60. doi: 10.1159/000257086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Alley D, Suthers K, Crimmins E. Education and cognitive decline in older Americans: results from the ahead sample. Res Aging. (2007) 29:73–94. doi: 10.1177/0164027506294245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Lee Y, Jean Yeung WJ. Gender matters: productive social engagement and the subsequent cognitive changes among older adults. Soc Sci Med. (2019) 229:87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.08.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Zhang K, Zhang W. Adverse childhood experiences and mild cognitive impairment in later life: exploring rural/urban and gender differences using CHARLS. J Appl Gerontol. (2022) 41:1454–64. doi: 10.1177/07334648211064796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Mukku SSR, Dahale AB, Muniswamy NR, Muliyala KP, Sivakumar PT, Varghese M. Geriatric depression and cognitive impairment—an update. Indian J Psychol Med. (2021) 43:286–93. doi: 10.1177/0253717620981556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Wei Y, Lin JL, Chen G, Pei LJ. Association between sleep duration and cognitive impairment in Chinese people aged 65 years and older. Zhonghua Liu Xing Bing Xue Za Zhi. (2021) 42:2138–42. doi: 10.3760/cma.j.cn112338-20201106-01313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Armstrong NM, Tom SE, Harrati A, Casaletto K, Pa J, Arce Rentería M, et al. Longitudinal relationship of leisure activity engagement with cognitive performance among non-demented, community-dwelling older adults. Gerontologist. (2022) 62:352–63. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnab046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Motter JN, Pimontel MA, Rindskopf D, Devanand DP, Doraiswamy PM, Sneed JR. Computerized cognitive training and functional recovery in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2016) 189:184–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Culpepper L, Lam RW, McIntyre RS. Cognitive impairment in patients with depression: awareness, assessment, and management. J Clin Psychiatry. (2017) 78:1383–94. doi: 10.4088/JCP.tk16043ah5c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Wong MM, Chan CF Li SW, Lau YM. Six-month follow-up of cognitive impairment and depressive symptoms in late-onset depression. East Asian Arch Psychiatry. (2015) 25:146–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

17. Adler G, Chwalek K, Jajcevic A. Six-month course of mild cognitive impairment and affective symptoms in late-life depression. Eur Psychiatry. (2004) 19:502–5. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2004.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Zhu Q, Fan H, Zhang X, Ji C, Xia Y. Changes in sleep duration and 3-year risk of mild cognitive impairment in Chinese older adults. Aging. (2020) 12:309–17. doi: 10.18632/aging.102616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Hua J, Sun H, Shen Y. Improvement in sleep duration was associated with higher cognitive function: a new association. Aging. (2020) 12:20623–44. doi: 10.18632/aging.103948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Ferrie JE, Shipley MJ, Akbaraly TN, Marmot MG, Kivimäki M, Singh-Manoux A. Change in sleep duration and cognitive function: findings from the Whitehall II study. Sleep. (2011) 34:565–73. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Lian Y, Zhang J, Jia CX. Sleep duration change and cognitive function: a national cohort study of Chinese people older than 45 years. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2020) 208:498–504. doi: 10.1097/nmd.0000000000001159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Wu J, Cheng GH, Sheng LT, Feng L, Yuan JM, Chee M, et al. Prospective associations between change in sleep duration and cognitive impairment: findings from the Singapore Chinese health study. J Affect Disord. (2021) 281:125–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Wion RK, Hill NL, DePasquale N, Mogle J, Whitaker EB. The relationship between subjective cognitive impairment and activity participation: a systematic review. Act Adapt Aging. (2020) 44:225–45. doi: 10.1080/01924788.2019.1651188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Li H, Li C, Wang A, Qi Y, Feng W, Hou C, et al. Associations between social and intellectual activities with cognitive trajectories in Chinese middle-aged and older adults: a nationally representative cohort study. Alzheimers Res Ther. (2020) 12:115. doi: 10.1186/s13195-020-00691-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Lee ATC, Richards M, Chan WC, Chiu HFK, Lee RSY, Lam LCW. Association of daily intellectual activities with lower risk of incident dementia among older Chinese adults. JAMA Psychiatry. (2018) 75:697–703. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.0657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Su X, Huang X, Jin Y, Wan S, Han Z. The relationship of individual social activity and cognitive function of community Chinese elderly: a cross-sectional study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2018) 14:2149–57. doi: 10.2147/ndt.s160036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Rojas-Saunero LP, Hilal S, Murray EJ, Logan RW, Ikram MA, Swanson SA. Hypothetical blood-pressure-lowering interventions and risk of stroke and dementia. Eur J Epidemiol. (2021) 36:69–79. doi: 10.1007/s10654-020-00694-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Labrecque JA, Swanson SA. Target trial emulation: teaching epidemiology and beyond. Eur J Epidemiol. (2017) 32:473–5. doi: 10.1007/s10654-017-0293-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Robins J, A. New approach to causal inference in mortality studies with sustained exposure periods—application to control of the healthy worker survivor effect. Mathemat Modell. (1986) 7:1393–512. doi: 10.1016/0270-0255(86)90088-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Keil AP, Edwards JK, Richardson DB, Naimi AI, Cole SR. The parametric G-formula for time-to-event data: intuition and a worked example. Epidemiology. (2014) 25:889–97. doi: 10.1097/ede.0000000000000160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Taubman SL, Robins JM, Mittleman MA, Hernán MA. Intervening on risk factors for coronary heart disease: an application of the parametric G-formula. Int J Epidemiol. (2009) 38:1599–611. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyp192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Johnson KW, Glicksberg BS, Hodos RA, Shameer K, Dudley JT. Causal inference on electronic health records to assess blood pressure treatment targets: an application of the parametric G formula. Pac Symp Biocomput. (2018) 23:180–91. doi: 10.1142/9789813235533_0017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Vangen-Lønne AM, Ueda P, Gulayin P, Wilsgaard T, Mathiesen EB, Danaei G. Hypothetical interventions to prevent stroke: an application of the parametric G-formula to a healthy middle-aged population. Eur J Epidemiol. (2018) 33:557–66. doi: 10.1007/s10654-017-0344-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Ren J, Li G, Zhang L, Zhang N, Ren J. Hypothetical Interventions for falls among older adults: an application of the parametric G-formula. Front Med (Lausanne). (2021) 8:732136. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2021.732136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Zhao Y, Hu Y, Smith JP, Strauss J, Yang G. Cohort profile: the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS). Int J Epidemiol. (2014) 43:61–8. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Wang J, Zhu WH Li YF, Zhu WW. Temporal precedence of cognitive function and functional abilities: a latent difference score model of the Chinese community-dwelling elders. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2019) 34:1892–9. doi: 10.1002/gps.5206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Gilles C. Age-related mild cognitive deficit: a ready-to-use concept? Dialog Clin Neurosci. (2003) 5:61–76. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2003.5.1/cgilles

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Richards M, Touchon J, Ledesert B, Richie K. Cognitive decline in ageing: are AAMI and AACD distinct entities? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (1999) 14:534–40. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1166(199907)14:7<534::aid-gps963>3.0.co;2-b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Liu H, Fan X, Luo H, Zhou Z, Shen C, Hu N, et al. Comparison of depressive symptoms and its influencing factors among the elderly in urban and rural areas: evidence from the China health and retirement longitudinal study (CHARLS). Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:3886. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18083886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Zhang N, Wang C, Li Y, Yang M, Yang J, Chen F, et al. Gender-specific association between nighttime sleep duration and falls among community-dwelling middle-aged and older adults in China. J Appl Gerontol. (2022) 41:2511–9. doi: 10.1177/07334648221118904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Yang YI, Hodge AM, Dugué PA, Williamson EJ, Gardiner PA, Barr ELM, et al. Mortality effects of hypothetical interventions on physical activity and TV viewing. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2021) 53:316–23. doi: 10.1249/mss.0000000000002479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. McGrath S, Lin V, Zhang Z, Petito LC, Logan RW, Hernán MA, et al. Gformula: an R package for estimating the effects of sustained treatment strategies via the parametric g-formula. Patterns (NY). (2020) 1:100008. doi: 10.1016/j.patter.2020.100008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Vreeburg SA, Hoogendijk WJ, van Pelt J, Derijk RH, Verhagen JC, van Dyck R, et al. Major depressive disorder and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity: results from a large cohort study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2009) 66:617–26. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.50

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Brenowitz WD, Zeki Al Hazzouri A, Vittinghoff E, Golden SH, Fitzpatrick AL, Yaffe K. Depressive symptoms imputed across the life course are associated with cognitive impairment and cognitive decline. J Alzheimers Dis. (2021) 83:1379–89. doi: 10.3233/jad-210588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Byers AL, Yaffe K. Depression and risk of developing dementia. Nat Rev Neurol. (2011) 7:323–31. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2011.60

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hua J, Zhuang S, Shen Y, Tang X, Sun H, Fang Q. Exploring the bidirectional associations between short or long sleep duration and lower cognitive function: a 7-year cohort study in China. Front Aging Neurosci. (2021) 13:727763. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.727763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Spira AP, Gonzalez CE, Venkatraman VK, Wu MN, Pacheco J, Simonsick EM, et al. Sleep duration and subsequent cortical thinning in cognitively normal older adults. Sleep. (2016) 39:1121–8. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Li X, Zhang J, Hou R, Zheng M, Singh M, Li H, et al. Bidirectional associations of intellectual and social activities with cognitive function among middle-aged and elderly adults in China. J Affect Disord. (2022) 319:83–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.09.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Park DC, Lodi-Smith J, Drew L, Haber S, Hebrank A, Bischof GN, et al. The impact of sustained engagement on cognitive function in older adults: the synapse project. Psychol Sci. (2014) 25:103–12. doi: 10.1177/0956797613499592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Lam LC, Ong PA, Dikot Y, Sofiatin Y, Wang H, Zhao M, et al. Intellectual and physical activities, but not social activities, are associated with better global cognition: a multi-site evaluation of the cognition and lifestyle activity study for seniors in Asia (CLASSA). Age Ageing. (2015) 44:835–40. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afv099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Bień B, Bień-Barkowska K. Objective drivers of subjective well-being in geriatric inpatients: mobility function and level of education are general predictors of self-evaluated health, feeling of loneliness, and severity of depression symptoms. Qual Life Res. (2016) 25:3047–56. doi: 10.1007/s11136-016-1355-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Chrzastek Z, Guligowska A, Soltysik B, Pigłowska M, Borowiak E, Kostka J, et al. Association of lower nutritional status and education level with the severity of depression symptoms in older adults-a cross sectional survey. Nutrients. (2021) 13:515. doi: 10.3390/nu13020515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Thow ME, Summers MJ, Saunders NL, Summers JJ, Ritchie K, Vickers JC. Further education improves cognitive reserve and triggers improvement in selective cognitive functions in older adults: the Tasmanian healthy brain project. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). (2018) 10:22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2017.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Lenehan ME, Summers MJ, Saunders NL, Summers JJ, Ward DD, Ritchie K, et al. Sending your grandparents to university increases cognitive reserve: the Tasmanian Healthy brain project. Neuropsychology. (2016) 30:525–31. doi: 10.1037/neu0000249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: older adults, cognitive impairment, sleep duration, depression, leisure activity, g-formula

Citation: Zhang N, Chen F, Wang C and Yan P (2023) Incidence of cognitive impairment after hypothetical interventions on depression, nighttime sleep duration, and leisure activity engagement among older Chinese adults: An application of the parametric g-formula. Front. Public Health 11:1088833. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1088833

Received: 03 November 2022; Accepted: 19 January 2023;
Published: 15 February 2023.

Edited by:

Jing Liao, Sun Yat-sen University, China

Reviewed by:

Jiaming Liang, University of Southern California, United States
Kourosh Zarea, Ahvaz Jundishapur University of Medical Sciences, Iran

Copyright © 2023 Zhang, Chen, Wang and Yan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ping Yan, yes yanping@xjmu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.