Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 15 August 2022
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Adaption to Change and Coping Strategies: New Resources for Mental Health View all 29 articles

Adult attachment and trait anxiety among Chinese college students: A multiple mediation model

  • 1Center for Counseling and Psychological Services, Guangdong University of Petrochemical Technology, Maoming, China
  • 2Department of Psychology, School of Education Science, Jiangsu Normal University, Xuzhou, China

The present study discusses the relationship between adult attachment and trait anxiety of Chinese college students based on the “internal working models” theory. Six hundred and seventy-two valid data were collected using a self-reported questionnaire. The mediating roles of self-esteem, perceived social support, and security in the relationship between adult attachment orientations (anxiety and avoidance) and trait anxiety were investigated using the structural equation model and bootstrap methods. The results showed that: self-esteem and security play mediating roles in the relationship between attachment anxiety and trait anxiety. self-esteem, perceived social support, and security play mediating roles in the relationship between attachment avoidance and trait anxiety. In addition, there were different mediation mechanisms in the correlation between different attachment dimensions and trait anxiety. This study provided empirical data for exploring the formation and maintenance of trait anxiety in college students and had a specific significance for the early prevention and clinical intervention of anxiety-related disorders.

Introduction

The further development of COVID-19 has hurt people's mental health, leading to anxiety and depression among some college students (1). Trait anxiety reflects the relatively persistent and stable differences in individual anxiety tendencies and is a critical predisposition factor for anxiety-related disorders (2). College students leave their families of origin, come to the university campus, and establish an attachment mode dominated by adult attachment. Therefore, exploring the relationship and mediating mechanism between adult attachment and trait anxiety in the COVID-19 pandemic is essential. The results of this study had implications for the prevention and intervention of anxiety-related disorders among college students.

Attachment, an intimate relationship between individuals and caregivers formed in the early years, is significant to individual psychological development and emotional regulation. The internal work model (IWMs) is an individual's internal mental representation and cognitive schema about self and others formed in early parent-child interaction (3). This cognitive schema works on an individual's early attachment objects and can extend to new interpersonal relationships with age growth (4). With age, the individual's different cognitive schemata about self and others develop into different adult attachment orientations and become an essential basis of individual psychological security (5). Brennan et al. (6) divided adult attachment orientations into two dimensions of “attachment anxiety” and “attachment avoidance” based on the differences between self-representation and other-representation in IWMs. Individuals with higher levels of attachment anxiety adopt “hyperactivating strategy,” while individuals with higher levels of attachment avoidance adopt “deactivating strategy” (7). “Hyperactivating strategy” is a kind of emotion regulation strategy to obtain security by constantly trying to establish relationships with others. “Deactivating strategy” is a kind of emotion regulation strategy to avoid emotional investment by suppressing the need for attachment through self-protection.

Emotional dysregulation is an important factor affecting individuals' trait anxiety (8). A few studies on the relationship between adult attachment and anxiety levels have also proved that emotional dysregulation was a key mechanism linking attachment and state anxiety [e.g., (9)]. However, there are few studies on the effect of adult attachment on individual trait anxiety. Therefore, this study intends to establish the following hypotheses based on IWMs and adult attachment emotion regulation strateies: attachment anxiety and avoidance correlated with trait anxiety (H1a); Attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance have different mechnisems in predicting trait anxiety (H1b).

Self-esteem is the core self-evaluation and emotional experience of individuals. Greenberg et al. (10) terror Management theory regards self-esteem as a universal buffer for dealing with stress and fear in human evolution. The self-representation formed by individuals in interacting with the attachment object is the basis for developing individual self-esteem (11). Individuals with secure attachment have the highest level of self-esteem compared to insecure attachment (12), but research on the relationship between insecure attachment and self-esteem has been inconsistent. A study based on a sample of American college students found that attachment anxiety was negatively correlated with self-esteem after controlling for relevant variables. Still, attachment avoidance was not significantly correlated with self-esteem (13). This result is inconsistent with the study of Li and Zheng (14), who found that attachment anxiety and avoidance are negatively correlated with self-esteem in Chinese culture. In addition, it has been found that self-esteem is an essential factor affecting trait anxiety, and childhood parental bondings can affect trait anxiety in adulthood through individual self-esteem (15). Therefore, this study proposed the hypothesis: self-esteem mediates the relationship between attachment anxiety and trait anxiety (H2a). In addition, considering that this study was conducted in Chinese culture, we also hypothesized that self-esteem does not mediate the relationship between attachment avoidance and trait anxiety (H2b).

As an essential psychological resource possessed by individuals, social support plays a buffering effect between external pressure and individual mental health (16). Perceived social support refers to an individual's expectation and evaluation of social support or belief that it is possible to obtain it (17). Previous studies have shown that perceived social support, as a stable feature with individual differences, is closely related to personal subjective feelings and emotional satisfaction and affects the mental health level of college students (18). Furthermore, Calvo et al. (19) found that attachment anxiety and avoidance may negatively influence perceived social support. Therefore, this study proposed that perceived social support mediates the relationship between attachment orientations (anxiety and avoidance) and trait anxiety (H3a and H3b).

According to attachment theory, the acquisition and satisfaction of security is a critical threshold to trigger individual emotion regulation behavior (5). Security is an individual's strong sense of coping with all kinds of possible dangers, expressed as the sense of prediction, determination, and control of life (20). Insecurity is the typical personality basis of various neuroses, and many neurotic behaviors of human beings are caused by the excessive compensatory pursuit of security (21). When the level of security is reduced, individuals feel that they are facing more significant risks and may be unable to cope with them, resulting in worry and nervousness. Attachment is the basis for individual security formation and change (22). Meanwhile, existing studies have shown that self-identification (22) and perception of external support (23) are important sources of individual security. Therefore, we propose the following hypotheses: security mediates the relationship between attachment orientations (anxiety and avoidance) and trait anxiety (H4a and H4b); self-esteem and security play serial roles in the relationship between attachment orientations (anxiety and avoidance) and trait anxiety (H4c and H4d); perceived social support and security play serial roles in the relationship between attachment orientations (anxiety and avoidance) and trait anxiety (H4e and H4f).

Based on the review of existing research and IWMs theory, this study established a multi-mediating model based on these assumptions to investigate the relationship between adult attachment orientations (anxiety or avoidance) and trait anxiety. The hypothesis model is as follows (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Research hypotheses framework.

Materials and methods

Participants

During the COVID-19 pandemic, we recruited 693 undergraduates from two universities in Jiangsu and Guangdong province using a cluster random sampling method. Participants voluntarily completed the paper-and-pencil version during the class break. This study was conducted under applicable ethics regulations. Participants signed the informed consent and were given gifts to those who completed the survey. After removing 15 uncompleted and six regular questionnaires, 672 valid questionnaires were collected, with an effective rate of 97%. The distribution of the subjects was as follows: 360 male and 312 female; Age ranged from 17 to 23 years (M = 19.39; SD = 1.05). Among them, 326 freshmen (48.5%), 231 sophomores (34.40%), and 115 juniors (17.10%).

Measures

Adult attachment

Adult attachment was measured using the revised version of the Experiences in Close Relationships Scale (ECR-R) developed by Fraley et al. (24). The ECR-R has 36 items and uses a 7-point Likert scale (1 = “strongly disagree,” 7 = “strongly agree”) to measure the two dimensions of “attachment anxiety” (e.g., “I worry that I won't measure up to other people.”) and “attachment avoidance” (e.g., “I tell my partner just about everything.”). This scale has been widely adopted and has demonstrated good reliability in Chinese participants (25). In this study, the internal consistency coefficients of the Chinese version were 0.89 (attachment anxiety) and 0.87 (attachment avoidance), respectively.

Self-esteem

Self-esteem was measured using the self-esteem Scale (SES) developed by Rosenberg (26). The scale is a single-dimensional self-rating scale, consisting of 10 items (e.g., “I take a positive attitude toward myself.”), using Likert scale 4 (1 = “strongly disagree”, 4 = “strongly agree”). This scale has been widely adopted and has demonstrated good reliability in Chinese participants (27). In this study, the internal consistency coefficient was 0.86.

Perceived social support

Perceived social support was measured using the published Perceived Social Support Scale (PSSS) (17). The scale consists of 12 self-rated items on a 7-Likert scale (1 = “strongly disagree,” 7 = “strongly agree”). The Chinese version is divided into three subscales of family support (e.g., “My family really tries to help me.”), friend support (e.g., “My friends really try to help me.”), and other support (e.g., “There is a special person with whom I can share my joys and sorrows.”). The Chinese version demonstrated good reliability in Chinese participants (28). In this study, the α coefficients of “family support,” “friend support,” and “other support” subscales were 0.84, 0.87, and 0.86, respectively.

Security

The security questionnaire (SQ) prepared by Cong and An (20) was used to evaluate the safety score of neurotic patients and normal people. The scale has 16 items, using a 5-point Likert scale (1 = “very consistent”, 5 = “very inconsistent”), including two dimensions of “interpersonal security” (eight items; e.g., “I never dare to volunteer my opinion”) and “sense of definite control” (eight items; e.g., “I always worry that something bad will happen”). In this study, the internal consistency coefficients of the “interpersonal security” and “sense of definite control” were 0.77 and 0.78, respectively.

Trait anxiety

Trait anxiety was measured using the trait anxiety subscale (TAI) of the State-Trait Anxiety Scale (STAI) developed and revised by Spielberger (29). The TAI subscale includes 11 items of negative emotion and nine items of positive emotion (e.g., “I feel pleasant”), using a 4-point Likert scale (1 = “almost never”, 4 = “almost always”). TAI subscale is mainly used to evaluate stable personality traits such as anxiety and tension. This TAI scale has been widely adopted and has demonstrated good reliability in Chinese participants (30). In this study, the internal consistency coefficient of the revised Chinese version was 0.84.

Procedures

The data analysis of this study was carried out in two steps. The first step was to conduct a descriptive statistical analysis of variables and test the correlation between variables and gender differences. Second, structural equation modeling was used to examine further the relationship between adult attachment dimensions and trait anxiety and the mediating roles of self-esteem, perceived social support, and security. Maximum likelihood estimation (ML) and bootstrap method with bias correction were used to establish the structural equation model and evaluate the mediating effects. Item parceling method was used to solve the problem of too many observation indicators of individual latent variables. R 4.0 and Mplus8.0 were used for all statistical processing in this study.

Results

Descriptive statistics

The means, standard deviations, and correlations of all variables are presented in Table 1. The skewness coefficient of the data is between −0.59 and 0.50, and the kurtosis coefficient is between −0.58 and 0.61, indicating that the data meet the normality requirement for structural equation analysis (31). The variance inflation factor (VIF) is between 1.40 and 1.90, indicating no multicollinearity problem in the data. Independent sample T-test results showed that there were gender differences in attachment avoidance [t(670) = 3.80, p < 0.001] and perceived social support [t(670) = −3.23, p < 0.005]. These differences indicated that women have higher attachment avoidance levels and lower social support than men. Therefore, gender as the control variable is included in the model analysis in the following mediation effect analysis.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations among the variables.

Common method biases test

Exploratory factor analysis without rotation was performed using Harman single factor test on all measurement items. The results showed that there were six factors with characteristic roots >1, among which the first factor accounted for 19.15% of the total variation, far less than the critical value of 40%, indicating that there was no serious common method bias in this study (32).

Mediation analyses

The results of mediation analysis show that the model fits well (χ2 = 289.89, df = 117, χ2/df = 2.48, p ≤ 0.001; RMSEA = 0.05; CFI = 0.98; TLI = 0.97; SRMR = 0.04). As shown in Figure 2, factor loads of each measurement model range from 0.78 to 0.89, and all are significant at 0.001 level.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Multiple mediation model. Dashed lines indicate nonsignificant paths. AN1–AN3 = three parcels of attachment anxiety; AV1–AV3 = three parcels of Attachment avoidance; SE1–SE3 = three parcels of self-esteem; PSS1 = family support; PSS2 = friend support; PSS3 = other support; SQ1–SQ2 = two parcels of security; TA1–TA3 = three parcels of trait anxiety. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

As shown in Table 2 and Figure 2, the direct effect of attachment anxiety on trait anxiety is significant (β7), and the total mediating effects in the relationship between attachment anxiety and trait anxiety are significant (β1β12 + β2β13 + β5β11 + β1β9β11 + β2β10β11). Self-esteem (β1β12) and security (β5β11) play mediating roles in the relationship between attachment anxiety and trait anxiety, respectively. Self-esteem and security play serial mediating roles in the relationship between attachment anxiety and trait anxiety (β1β9β11). The direct effect of attachment avoidance on trait anxiety was not significant, but the total mediating effects in the relationship between attachment avoidance and trait anxiety are significant (β3β12 + β4β13 + β6β11 + β3β9β11 + β4β10β11) are significant. Self-esteem (β3β12) plays mediating role in the relationship between attachment avoidance and trait anxiety. Self-esteem and security play serial mediating roles in the relationship between attachment avoidance and trait anxiety (β3β9β11). Perceived social support and security play serial mediating roles in the relationship between attachment avoidance and trait anxiety (β4β10β11).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Direct and indirect effects of adult attachment on trait anxiety.

By setting up auxiliary variables in the model, we investigated the different internal mechanism of adult attachment predicting trait anxiety. The results show that (non-standard results): attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance have significant differences to predict trait anxiety ((β7 + β1β12 + β2β13 + β5β11 + β1β9β11 + β2β10β11) − (β8 + β3β12 + β4β13 + β6β11 + β3β9β11 + β4β10β11) = 0.11, 95% CI = [0.05, 0.17]); there is no significant difference in the serial mediating effects of self-esteem via security ((β1β9β11 – β3β9β11) = −0.01, 95% CI = [−0.03, 0.00]); the serial mediating effects of perceived social support via security are significantly different ((β2β10β11) − (β4β10β11) = −0.02, 95% CI =[− 0.03, −0.01]).

Discussion

This study's descriptive statistical results showed that both attachment anxiety and avoidance positively correlated with trait anxiety. At the same time, effect comparison results showed that the total effect of attachment anxiety predicts trait anxiety is significantly greater than that of attachment avoidance. These results verified H1a and H1b and suggest that there may be different mediating pathways or effects between attachment orientations and relationships. In the following analysis, we further analyzed the mechanism of the difference between attachment anxiety and avoidance and trait anxiety.

Mediating effect analysis found that self-esteem plays mediating role between attachment anxiety and trait anxiety. This result verified H2a and indicated that college students with higher attachment anxiety negatively evaluated themselves. The lower levels of self-esteem were also associated with trait anxiety. In addition, self-esteem also plays mediating role between attachment avoidance and trait anxiety. This result verified H2b and indicated that individuals with higher scores in attachment avoidance might suppress and isolate negative self-cognition by a “defensive mechanism” (33). This result was inconsistent with previous studies with U.S. samples. They found that attachment avoidance was not associated with self-esteem [e.g., (13)]. It was a puzzling and meaningful result that we suspect may be related to differences in the samples collected in the study. It needs to be further deepened and clarified in future studies.

At the same time, we found that perceived social support mediates attachment avoidance and trait anxiety but not between attachment anxiety and trait anxiety. These results verified the H3b, but it was inconsistent with H3a. On the one hand, these results indicated that although college students with high attachment anxiety obtain security by constantly seeking others to establish intimacy, they do not experience the corresponding care and support during this process. These results also explained the intrinsic motivation that leads individuals with high attachment anxiety to adopt a “hyperactivating strategy” (34). Individuals with high attachment anxiety expect to gain support and care through establishing intimate relationships with others, but they do not experience adequate outside support during this process (35). On the other hand, These results indicated that college students with higher scores in attachment avoidance adopt “deactivation strategy” to selectively filter external support and care from others (36). This isolation strategy of intimate relationships leads to higher trait anxiety.

This study found that the mediating effect of security and the serial mediating effect of self-esteem via security in the relationship between attachment anxiety and trait anxiety were significant. These results supported H4a and H4c and suggest security plays a critical mediating role in the relationship between attachment anxiety and trait anxiety. In other words, individuals with high attachment anxiety generally lack a sense of security (37), so they easily become a susceptible group to anxiety disorder. At the same time, the serial mediating effect of self-esteem via security and the serial mediating effect of perceived social support via security in the relationship between attachment avoidance and trait anxiety are significant. These results supported H4D and H4F and indicated that although individuals with high attachment avoidance adopt interpersonal withdrawal strategies (38), they also have a lower sense of security, which explains the core mediating factor of high trait anxiety levels of individuals with attachment avoidance.

Conclusion

This study reveals the relationship and mediating mechanism between adult attachment and trait anxiety in college students and guided the prevention and intervention of anxiety-related disorders. Specifically, individuals with higher scores in attachment anxiety need to improve their social support skills in interaction with attachment objects while enhancing their self-positive evaluation. While providing adequate social support for individuals with high scores in the attachment avoidance dimension, self-worth training should also be strengthened.

Of course, there are still many deficiencies in this study. First of all, although the mediation model established in this study systematically reveals the relationship between adult attachment and trait anxiety and its internal mediation mechanism, the mediation model fits well. However, the model has many variables and complex relationships, which leads to an insufficient discussion of the relationship between some variables. Secondly, since the structural equation model was built by collecting cross-sectional data in this study, the model results cannot directly support causal inferences. Further analysis can use the longitudinal mediation model to verify and explore the conclusion of this study. In addition, the influence of attachment avoidance on self-representation in the context of Chinese culture needs further research. Thirdly, the relationship between attachment avoidance and self-representation in the context of Chinese culture needs further investigation.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Academic Ethics Review Committee of Guangdong University of Petrochemical Technology. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

XL designed, wrote, and approved all contributions to the study. PS helped to design the study and edit the manuscrip. LL participated in collecting the data. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The research was supported by the Key Scientific Research Platform Project of Guangdong Education Department (2018WTSCX091), the Key Projects of the 13th Five-Year Plan for Education Science of Jiangsu Province (B-a/2020/01/11), the Humanity and Social Science Youth Foundation of Ministry of Education of China (21YJC190014), the Education science Planning project of Guangdong Province (2021GXJK036).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2022.912128/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Yldrm M, Akgül M, Geer E. The effect of COVID-19 anxiety on general health: the role of COVID-19 coping. Int J Ment Health Addict. (2022) 20:1110–21. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00429-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Spielberger CD, editor. Theory and research on anxiety. In: Anxiety and Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press (1966), p. 3–19. doi: 10.1016/B978-1-4832-3131-0.50006-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Attachment in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics, and Change. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2007).

Google Scholar

4. Nichols O, Vaughn BE, Lu T, Krzysik L, El-Sheikh M. Scripted attachment representations and adaptive functioning during early childhood. Attach Hum Dev. (2019) 21:289–306. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2019.1575551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Attachment orientations and emotion regulation. Curr Opin Psychol. (2019) 25:6–10. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Brennan KA, Clark CL, Shaver PR. Self-report measurement of adult attachment: an integrative overview. In: Simpson JA, Rholes WS, editors. Attachment Theory and Close Relationships. New York, NY: Guilford Press (1998), p. 46–76.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

7. Zheng L, Luo Y, Chen X. Different effects of attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance on depressive symptoms: a meta-analysis. J Soc Pers Relat. (2020) 37:3028–50. doi: 10.1177/0265407520946482

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Liu B, Wang Y, Li X. Implicit emotion regulation deficits in trait anxiety: an ERP study. Front Hum Neurosci. (2018) 12:382. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Nielsen SKK, Lønfeldt N, Wolitzky-Taylor KB, Hageman I, Vangkilde S, Daniel SIF. Adult attachment style and anxiety–the mediating role of emotion regulation. J Affect Disord. (2017) 218:253–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.04.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Greenberg J, Solomon S, Pyszczynski T, Rosenblatt A, Burling J, Lyon D, et al. Why do people need self-esteem? Converging evidence that self-esteem serves an anxiety-buffering function. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1992) 63:913–22. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Branden N. The Power of Self-Esteem. New York, NY: Bantam (1992).

Google Scholar

12. Doinita NE. Adult attachment, self-esteem and emotional intelligence. Procedia Soc Behav Sci. (2015) 187:570–4. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.03.106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Han S. Attachment insecurity and openness to diversity: the roles of self-esteem and trust. Pers Individ Dif. (2017) 111:291–6. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.02.033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Li X, Zheng X. Adult attachment orientations and subjective well-being: emotional intelligence and self-esteem as moderators. Soc Behavior Pers. (2014) 42:1257–65. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2014.42.8.1257

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Shimura A, Takaesu Y, Nakai Y, Murakoshi A, Ono Y, Matsumoto Y. Childhood parental bonding affects adulthood trait anxiety through self-esteem. Compr Psychiatry. (2017) 74:15–20. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Uchino BN. Social support and health: a review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J Behav Med. (2006) 29:377–87. doi: 10.1007/s10865-006-9056-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Zimet GD, Powell SS, Farley GK, Werkman S, Berkoff KA. Psychometric characteristics of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. J Pers Assess. (1990) 55:610–7. doi: 10.1080/00223891.1990.9674095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Reid GM, Holt MK, Bowman CE, Espelage DL, Green JG. Perceived social support and mental health among first-year college students with histories of bullying victimization. J Child Fam Stud. (2016) 25:3331–41. doi: 10.1007/s10826-016-0477-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Calvo V, Cusinato M, Meneghet N, Miscioscia M. Perceived social support mediates the negative impact of insecure attachment orientations on internalized homophobia in gay men. J Homosex. (2021) 68:2266–84. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2020.1734378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Cong Z, An L. Developing of security questionnaire and its reliability and validity. Chin Mental Health J. (2004) 18:97–9. doi: 10.3321/j.issn:1000-6729.2004.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Schneider K. Psychopathic Personalities. Los Angeles, CA: Cassell (1958).

Google Scholar

22. Yirmiya K, Motsan S, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. Human attachment triggers different social buffering mechanisms under high and low early life stress rearing. Int J Psychophysiol. (2020) 152:72–80. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Zhou W, Hong Z, Hu R, Zhu T, Liu S, Zhang L. Parent-child support and elders'depression: roles of sense of security and emotional expression. Psychol Dev Educ. (2020) 36:249–56. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.02.14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Fraley RC, Waller NG, Brennan KA. An item response theory analysis of self-report measures of adult attachment. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2000) 78:350–65. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Lan Y, Zheng X. Revision of the “experiences in close relationship-revised” scale. China J Health Psychol. (2011) 19:379−81. doi: 10.13342/j.cnki.cjhp.2011.03.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Rosenberg M. Rosenberg self-esteem scale (RSE). In: Rosenberg M, editor. Conceiving the Self. New York, NY: Basic Books (1965), p. 201–5. doi: 10.1037/t01038-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Liu Y, Chen J. The relationship between undergraduates'self-esteem and social adaptation: the chain mediation analysis of positive core schema and peer attachment. Psychol Dev Educ. (2020) 36:694–9. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.06.07

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Xiang G, Xian H, Guo C, Wang Y, Li Q. The relationship between perceived social support and hope: a cross-lagged study. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2020) 28:1246–9. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2020.06.034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Spielberger CD. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory STAI (form Y). Monterey, CA: Counsulting Psychologists Press Inc. (1983). doi: 10.1037/t06496-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Wang X, Wang X, Ma H. Rating Scales for Mental Health (revised edition) (In Chinese). Beijing: Chinese Mental Health Journal Press (1999).

Google Scholar

31. Muthén B, Kaplan D. A comparison of some methodologies for the factor analysis of non-normal Likert variables. Br J Math Stat Psychol. (1985) 38:171–89. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.1985.tb00832.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Zhou H, Long L. Statistical remedies for common method biases (in Chinese). Adv Psychol Sci. (2004) 12:942–50. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1671-3710.2004.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Baumeister RF, Dale K, Sommer KLJ. Freudian defense mechanisms and empirical findings in modern social psychology: reaction formation, projection, displacement, undoing, isolation, sublimation, and denial. J Pers. (1998) 66:1081–124. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. The attachment behavioral system in adulthood: activation, psychodynamics, and interpersonal processes. In Zanna M, edirtor. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 35. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press (2003), p. 53–152. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(03)01002-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Collins NL, Ford MB, Guichard AC, Allard LM. Working models of attachment and attribution processes in intimate relationships. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. (2006) 32:201–19. doi: 10.1177/0146167205280907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Girme YU, Overall NC, Simpson JA, Fletcher GJ. “All or nothing”: attachment avoidance and the curvilinear effects of partner support. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2015) 108:450–75. doi: 10.1037/a0038866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Ryan RM, Brown KW, Creswell JD. How integrative is attachment theory? Unpacking the meaning and significance of felt security. Psychol Inq. (2007) 18:177–82. doi: 10.1080/10478400701512778

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Emery LF, Gardner WL, Carswell KL, Finkel EJ. You can't see the real me: attachment avoidance, self-verification, and self-concept clarity. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. (2018) 44:1133–46. doi: 10.1177/0146167218760799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: adult attachment, self-esteem, perceived social support, security, trait anxiety

Citation: Li X, Sun P and Li L (2022) Adult attachment and trait anxiety among Chinese college students: A multiple mediation model. Front. Public Health 10:912128. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.912128

Received: 04 April 2022; Accepted: 29 July 2022;
Published: 15 August 2022.

Edited by:

María del Carmen Pérez-Fuentes, University of Almeria, Spain

Reviewed by:

Wu Hao, Guangzhou Medical University, China
Xudong Zhao, Shanghai Normal University, China

Copyright © 2022 Li, Sun and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xu Li, cHN5bGl4dSYjeDAwMDQwO2ZveG1haWwuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.