Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 28 July 2022
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Workplace Health Promotion, Volume II View all 14 articles

Leave Me Alone With Your Symptoms! Social Exclusion at the Workplace Mediates the Relationship of Employee's Mental Illness and Sick Leave

  • 1Department of Work and Organizational Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Philipps University Marburg, Marburg, Germany
  • 2Department of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Philipps University Marburg, Marburg, Germany
  • 3Department of Special Needs Educational and Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Sports Science, Justus-Liebig-University Giessen, Giessen, Germany
  • 4Department of Medical Biometry, Institute of Medical Biometry and Informatics, Heidelberg University, Heidelberg, Germany
  • 5Department of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-University München, Munich, Germany
  • 6Department of Educational Psychology, Faculty of Education, Psychology, and Sociology, Institute of Psychology, Technical University Dortmund, Dortmund, Germany

Although a substantial part of employees suffers from a mental illness, the work situation of this population still is understudied. Previous research suggests that people with a mental illness experience discrimination in the workplace, which is known to have detrimental effects on health. Building on the stereotype content model and allostatic load theory, the present study investigated whether employees with a mental illness become socially excluded at the workplace and therefore show more days of sick leave. Overall, 86 employees diagnosed with a mental disorder were interviewed and completed online-surveys. Path analyses supported the hypotheses, yielding a serial mediation: The interview-rated severity of the mental disorder had an indirect effect on the days of sick leave, mediated by the symptomatic burden and the social exclusion at the workplace. In the light of the costs associated with absenteeism the present paper highlights the harmfulness of discrimination. Organizations and especially supervisors need to be attentive for signs of exclusion within their teams and try to counteract as early as possible.

Introduction

Mental illnesses are one of the main causes of disability worldwide (1, 2). Estimates indicate that more than one in six people across the European countries (17.3%) experienced mental health problems in 2016 (3). Besides severe cognitive, emotional, and behavioral impairments for the affected individuals (46), mental health problems pose a substantial economic burden on health care systems (7, 8). Since a substantial part of the working population suffers from mental disorders (9, 10), mental illnesses furthermore interfere with the functioning of employees and organizations as well: Several studies provide evidence for a significant relationship between employees' mental health status and their performance (1113). Thus, mental disorders contribute to a substantial amount of indirect costs organizations spend, arising from reduced productivity or increased absenteeism of their employees with a mental illness (14, 15).

Despite these consequences of mental disorders on the well-being of employees and organizations, the work situation of employees with a mental illness (EMI) still is understudied (16). Investigations suggest that EMI are faced with various barriers at their jobs (17, 18), which in turn could worsen their health status (19). Stigmatization and discrimination of people with a mental illness for instance are not only common in the general public (20, 21), but also appear in organizations, for instance in form of the exclusion from work-related events or the denial of conversation in general (2224). Discriminative actions as this social exclusion are known to be detrimental for health (2529) and thus social exclusion might result in increased absenteeism (30). However, to the best of our knowledge, the influence of the mental illness on social exclusion and absenteeism has not been investigated jointly yet. This study is intended to close this research gap and thereby to contribute to a better understanding of the negative effects mental disorders and social exclusion have on employees and organizations, hoping to provide starting points for their mitigation.

In a recent review, Follmer and Jones (16) not only highlight the existence of negative stereotypes about EMI among supervisors and employees, but also the discrimination they experience in the workplace. An explanation why and how such negative stereotypes about people with a mental disorder end up in discriminative behavior provides the stereotype content model (SCM) (31) and its extension, the behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes (BIAS) map (32). The SCM proposes that two dimensions are central for the emergence of different group stereotypes: warmth, defined as the perception whether the intent of a certain group toward one self or one's ingroup is either beneficial or malevolent and competence, defined as the perceived capability of the group to pursue and enact those intentions (31, 33). Groups are classified on both dimensions, yielding either thoroughly positive stereotypes (i.e.„ classified as warm and competent) as e.g., the middle class, thoroughly negative stereotypes (i.e., cold and incompetent) as e.g., poor people, or ambivalent stereotypes (i.e., warm and incompetent or cold and competent) as e.g., elderly or rich people (32, 33). Each of those stereotype-combinations elicits specific emotional reactions toward the classified group (31, 33): Warm and competent groups are admired, cold and incompetent groups elicit contempt, warm but incompetent groups induce pity, and cold but competent groups envy. Those emotional reactions finally end up in specific behavioral tendencies toward the classified group, mediating the effect of the stereotype on the behavior as the BIAS map proposes (32, 33): While admired groups induce active and passive facilitation, the opposite is true for resented groups (i.e., incompetent and cold) which provoke active and passive harm. Envied or pitied groups evoke mixed behavioral patterns (passive facilitation and active harm or active facilitation and passive harm, respectively). A multiplicity of studies investigated the SCM and the BIAS map and found support for their assumptions, also across various nations, including Germany [e.g., (31, 32, 3436)].

Hence, according to the SCM, emotional reactions and behavioral tendencies toward people with a mental illness depend on the perception whether (a) they want to help or harm oneself and whether (b) they are able to do so. In a systematic literature review, Parcesepe and Cabassa (21) sum up that people with a mental illness are often perceived as being e.g., incompetent, dangerous and criminal, indicating a rather low evaluation on the two dimensions warmth and competence. In a more proximal study on the SCM, Sadler et al. (37) asked participants to rate the warmth and competence of people with a mental disorder as seen by Americans in general: Results confirmed the indirect evidence, showing that people with a mental illness are perceived as equally incompetent and cold as poor people. Accordingly, people tend to react with active harm, e.g., segregation (38, 39), or passive harm, e.g., social distance (20, 21, 38), toward them, as predicted by the SCM.

Although research on employees with a mental illness in general still is scarce [see Follmer and Jones (16) for a recent review], the existing literature indicates that likewise discriminatory behaviors toward people with a mental illness also exist in the workplace [e.g., (17, 18, 22, 24, 40, 41)]. Follmer et al. (23) for instance found that lower ratings of warmth and competence of EMI predict a higher desire to socially distance oneself from a fictious coworker with a mental illness. This confirms investigations on the experiences of EMI, reporting that people at work avoid them due to their mental health problems (24). Thus, people with a mental illness do not only experience discriminatory behavior as social exclusion in the general public, but also in their workplaces.

The evaluation that a specific colleague has a mental health problem and the subsequent exclusion of this colleague by the coworkers however, do not appear out of the blue: First, the coworkers have to notice corresponding peculiar behavior (or in other words: symptoms) that marks the colleague accordingly. Only if the colleague acts strangely (i.e., displays symptoms), an appraisal as being (more or less) mentally ill on the part of the coworkers is possible—followed by the above mentioned perception that the colleague is cold and incompetent [c.f. (37)] and the corresponding behavioral reactions (e.g., social exclusion) toward her/him [c.f. (33)]. Thus, EMI with a higher symptomatic burden (i.e., more and/or stronger symptoms) should be perceived as being more mentally ill—and thus experience more social exclusion at the workplace.

The symptomatic burden on the other hand is inevitably linked to the severity of the mental illness itself: The level of disability and distress patients experience in various life domains, including occupation, increases as the mental health status gets more severe (42, 43). More depressed people for instance spend less time in groups, use more negative emotion words, and feel lonely more often than their less depressed counterparts (44, 45). Thus, an increasing severity of the mental illness should go along with more and/or stronger symptoms—or in other words a higher symptomatic burden. This in turn will be noticed by the coworkers, leading to social exclusion of the EMI.

Based on this line of thought we therefore test the following hypotheses:

Hypothesis 1: The severity of the mental disorder leads to a higher symptomatic burden.

Hypothesis 2: The severity of the mental disorder leads to an increase of experienced social exclusion at the workplace via a higher symptomatic burden.

Social stressors as social exclusion are not only uncomfortable to endure but unfortunately also could have detrimental effects on human body and thus health according to the allostatic load model (4649). The allostatic load model assumes that physiological reactions mediate the effect of (job) stressors on health outcomes (48, 50). According to the model, different physiological systems in the human body initiate an adaptive response if exposed to a stressor, that is the physiological markers increase (48, 49). This response persists until the stressor vanishes (49). Now the physiological response is stopped and the markers decrease – thus recovery takes place (48, 49). This process of adjustment in order to cope with a stressor fulfilled by different physiological systems is defined as allostasis (50, 51).

The described switching on and off of the physiological response is an adaptive and beneficial mechanism—however it can become overstrained with potential detrimental effects on the human body in the long run, a status called allostatic load (4649). The allostatic system for instance may have difficulties to habituate to the same stressor (i.e., the physiological response to the stressor is always equally high) or problems to end the response adequately (i.e., it continues even after the stressor disappears)—both resulting in a hyperactivation of the system (48, 49). McEwen (49) calls these subtypes of allostatic load “lack of adaption” and “prolonged response” and further postulates that the effects of a chronic hyperarousal add up over time and finally result in diseases (4749).

Unfortunately, social stressors are designated to cause those subtypes of allostatic load: Various psychophysiological studies indicate that even when people are exposed to the same social stressor for several times, the sympathetic response does not significantly change, suggesting poor habituation of the associated system to the stressor [e.g., (5255)]—or (in the sense of allostatic load theory) a “lack of adaption.” Furthermore, employees tend to ruminate in their leisure time when they are confronted with social stressors at work (5658). Such pondering about a stressor can lead to an extended physiological reaction, as experimental laboratory studies suggest (59)—or in other words can cause a ”prolonged response” in the sense of allostatic load theory. Thus, if people are exposed to the same social stressor over and over again—as it can be the case for employees confronted with social exclusion at the workplace—both mentioned subtypes of allostatic load might occur and finally end up in sickness.

The proposed impact of social exclusion on human health was supported by research from various fields, indicating that isolation in general affects the functioning of the immune system and even mortality rates (2729). But there also is according evidence in the work environment since socially excluded employees have a higher risk for a long-term sick leave spell (30), just as victims of workplace bullying do [for a recent review and meta-analysis see (60)]. Thus, it is possible that the influence of the mental illness goes beyond the symptoms a patient suffers from and the social exclusion (s)he experiences thereof at the workplace: The isolation could furthermore lead to an increase of days EMI are sick leaving, yielding a serial mediation of the severity of the mental illness on sick leave via the symptomatic burden and social exclusion at the workplace. While the debarment of EMI as well as the impact of social exclusion on absenteeism has already been under examination (23, 24, 30), no study has ever combined those research lines and investigated the influence of the mental illness on sick leave via social exclusion.

Based on this line of thought we therefore test the following hypothesis:

Hypothesis 3: The severity of the mental disorder leads to an increase of sick leave, sequentially via a higher symptomatic burden and more social exclusion at the workplace.

Materials and Methods

Design and Procedure

The data of the present study originate from the COMPARE-consortium (61) standing for “children of mentally ill parents at risk evaluation.” The consortium investigates why children of parents with a mental illness are at higher risk of developing mental illnesses themselves and whether a preventive intervention may interrupt this malicious transmission of the parental mental state to the child (61). It consists of a clinical study “COMPARE family” [see (62)] as well as four subprojects named “COMPARE emotion,” “COMPARE interaction,” “COMPARE work” and “COMPARE school” [see (61)]. In the following we only report the aspects of the COMPARE-consortium which are relevant for the paper at hand. For more details on the consortium and the subprojects we refer to Christiansen et al. (61) and for more information on the clinical study to Stracke et al. (62)1.

The recruitment for the used partial data set took place from January 2018 to May 2020 in different university outpatient clinics throughout Germany. The clinical study was advertised with e.g., flyers, newspaper articles and information meetings for professionals (e.g., physicians). Persons contacting the clinics asking for psychotherapeutic help were screened for eligibility and interest in participating in the clinical study [c.f. (62)]. After deciding to participate, they signed the informed consent and fulfilled the first assessment, from which the data used in this paper originate.

Every assessment was split in multiple occasions: First, structured interviews were conducted in two sessions on site by trained study personal. After the second interview session, patients received a sheet with a link to the online questionnaire assessed by “COMPARE family” and were asked to answer it at home [c.f. (62)]. If participants also agreed to take part in “COMPARE work” (or one of the other subprojects) they furthermore received links to the corresponding online questionnaires.

Different criteria had to be met for patients to participate in the clinical study: (1) they had to search for outpatient psychotherapeutic care, (2) they had to fulfill the diagnostic criteria for a DSM-5 disorder (63), and (3) they had to care for at least one child aged between 1.5 and 16 years [c.f. (62)]. Patients were not included in the clinical study if (1) the patient already had been in psychotherapeutic treatment at the present time, (2) the patient needed an acute inpatient treatment, (3) all children fulfilled the criteria for a severe mental illness and furthermore were in need of an immediate treatment, (4) the patient used benzodiazepines regularly (an intermittent use less than once every 2 weeks was allowed) or (5) the family had insufficient German language skills [c.f. (62)]. For the present paper we furthermore excluded patients without a current (self-employment) as well as patients being on a sick leave for longer than 6 weeks since any item with regard to the current work situation could presumably not have been reasonably answered.

Measures

All data used in this paper have been collected and managed with REDCap (64), standing for “research electronic data capture”. As already mentioned, the data—although belonging to one assessment—were partly measured at different events. Thus, in addition to describing the variables used in the present paper we also mention the occasion they were collected in.

Variables Assessed in Interview Sessions

Severity of Mental Illness

The Diagnostic Interview for Mental Disorders (DIPS) (65, 66) was conducted with the patients in the first interview session. Trained study personnel executed the DIPS [c.f. (62)] taking between 60 and 120 min [c.f. (65)]. The structured interviews of the DIPS-family are a reliable and valid method for diagnosing mental disorders across the lifespan (65). At the end of the interview, the assessor rates the severity of the main diagnosis on a scale from 0 to 8 with digits between 0 and 3 standing for a subclinical diagnosis and digits between 4 and 8 for a clinical diagnosis [c.f. (65, 66)]. This rating was used as an indicator for the severity of the patients' mental illness in the present study. Since subclinical diagnoses were not sufficient for being enrolled into the clinical study, the rating only varied between 4 and 8 in the present paper.

Variables Assessed in the Questionnaire of “COMPARE Family”

Symptomatic Burden

The Brief Symptom Inventory (67) was used to assess the symptomatic burden of the patient. The inventory is a short form of the Symptom Check-List-90-R (68) and measures the subjective impairment caused by the symptoms of the patient (67). Various studies indicate the reliability and validity of the inventory, especially if the psychopathology is under study (67, 69). The 53 items ask the participant how much (s)he suffered from different symptoms within the last seven days as, for example, the symptom of “feeling no interest in things” (Cronbach's α = 0.96). Answers ranged from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very much) on a Likert-scale with labeled intermediate steps.

Variables Assessed in the Questionnaire of “COMPARE Work”

Social Exclusion

We measured social exclusion with the corresponding scale from Zapf and Holz (70) which is an adaption and further development of Frese's and Zapf (71) scale and has proven to be reliable and valid (71, 72). It consists of four items as “you are being ignored and excluded” (Cronbach's α = 0.82). Answers ranged from 1 (does not apply at all) to 6 (completely applies) on a Likert scale with labeled intermediate steps.

Sick Leave

Patients indicated the number of days they were on sick leave by answering the following question: “On how many days have you been absent from work for health reasons in the last 4 weeks (without taking into account the days on which you were missing due to an illness of your child / children)?”

Statistics

To test hypothesis one and two—which stated that patients with a more severe mental illness suffer from more and/or stronger symptoms (H1), and as a result become socially excluded at work more often, yielding an indirect effect (H2)—we conducted a path analysis using the PROCESS macro version 3.4.1 (73) in IBM SPSS Statistics 26 executing Model 4 (simple mediation): First, we regressed the symptomatic burden on the severity of mental illness. Second, we regressed social exclusion on the severity of mental illness and the symptomatic burden simultaneously. Third, we calculated the indirect effect of the severity of the mental illness on social exclusion via the symptomatic burden. Lastly, we regressed social exclusion solely on the severity of mental illness to test for the total effect of the predictor.

To test whether the social exclusion of the patients at work finally results in an increase of absenteeism—yielding a serial mediation with two mediators (H3)—we further conducted a path analysis using Model 6 (serial mediation, two mediators) of the Process macro (73): After rehearsing the first two steps just described we further regressed sick leave on the severity of mental illness, the symptomatic burden and social exclusion simultaneously. Then, we calculated the indirect effect of the severity of mental illness on sick leave via the symptomatic burden and social exclusion. Finally, we regressed sick leave on the severity of mental illness alone to again obtain the total effect model. The significance level (α) was 0.05 for every hypothesis.

As recommended by Hayes (73) the confidence intervals of any indirect effect presented in this paper are percentile bootstrap confidence intervals calculated on the basis of 10,000 bootstrap-samples. We do not report p-values of indirect effects since the normal theory approach used for their calculation has different statistical drawbacks and therefore cannot be recommended [e.g., (73)].

Results

Overall, N = 86 patients met the inclusion criteria and answered the questionnaires from “COMPARE work” and “COMPARE family.” Details of the sample can be found in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample details.

Preliminary Analysis

Means, standard deviations and correlations between the investigated variables are presented in Table 2. As expected, the severity of mental illness had a positive relation with the symptomatic burden of the patient, which in turn correlated positively with social exclusion. Furthermore, sick leave was positively correlated with social exclusion. Thus, the bivariate relations showed the expected pattern.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means, standard deviations, and correlations between investigated constructs.

Test of Hypotheses

The results of our first path analysis which tested the simple mediation model are presented in Figure 1: Patients with a more severe mental illness suffered from more and/or stronger symptoms (b = 0.186, p = 0.003, 95% CI [0.064, 0.307]) supporting hypothesis 1. Furthermore, employees with a stronger symptomatic burden reported more experienced social exclusion (b = 0.628, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.321, 0.935]). There was no direct effect of the severity of the mental illness on social exclusion – neither with (b = −0.117, p = 0.191, 95% CI [−0.294, 0.060]) nor without (b = −0.001, p = 0.995, 95% CI [−0.184, 0.182]) controlling for the symptomatic burden. However, there was an indirect effect: Patients with a more severe mental disorder suffered from a stronger symptomatic burden and in turn reported more experienced social exclusion at work (b = 0.117, 95% CI [0.028, 0.251]), supporting hypothesis 2.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mediation model showing the impact of the severity of the mental illness on social exclusion via the symptomatic burden (n = 82). The total effect of the severity of the mental illness on social exclusion without controlling for the symptomatic burden is shown in parentheses. Unstandardized regression coefficients. Two-sided testing of significance. **p < 0. 01, ***p < 0.001.

Figure 2 presents the results of the second path analysis which tested the serial mediation model. In addition to Figure 1, it shows that patients who reported more experienced social exclusion were absent from work more often (b = 2.652, p = 0.006, 95% CI [0.800, 4.504]). Neither the symptomatic burden (b = −1.034, p = 0.464, 95% CI [−3.829, 1.761]) nor the degree of the mental illness (no matter whether directly (b = 0.048, p = 0.948, 95% CI [−1.431, 1.528]) or in total (b = −0.145, p = 0.842, 95% CI [−1.584, 1.294])) were associated with the amount of days a patient was sick leaving at work. Instead, as can be seen in Table 3, the severity of the mental illness had an indirect effect on sick leave: Patients with a more severe mental disorder suffered from a stronger symptomatic burden and reported in turn more experienced social exclusion at work which went along with an increase of days a patient was sick leaving (b = 0.309, 95% CI [.022, 0.777]), supporting hypothesis 3. The indirect effects of the severity of the mental illness on absenteeism via the symptomatic burden or social exclusion reclusively were both statistically negligible (see Table 3).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mediation model showing the impact of the severity of the mental illness on sick leave via the symptomatic burden and social exclusion (n = 82). The total effect of the severity of the mental illness on sick leave without controlling for the symptomatic burden and social exclusion is shown in parentheses. Unstandardized regression coefficients. Two-sided testing of significance. **p < 0. 01, ***p < 0.001.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Indirect effects of the severity of the mental illness on sick leave.

Additional Analysis

Since the acquisition of patients for the present paper lasted until May 2020, a part of the participants undertook their assessment during the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany. To rule out the possibility that the far-reaching influences of the pandemic on the society did affect the presented results we excluded any patient having participated in the year 2020 and reran the analyses, yielding similar results (c.f. Appendix A of the Supplementary Material).

Furthermore, some patients completed the questionnaire of “COMPARE work” (i.e., the questions regarding social exclusion and sick leave) before answering the questionnaire of “COMPARE family” (i.e., the questions regarding the symptomatic burden) resulting in a mixed temporal precedence. Thus, we excluded every participant who answered to “COMPARE work” one or more days before answering to “COMPARE family”, which resulted in patients completing the questionnaire of “COMPARE work” 12 days after the questionnaire of “COMPARE family”, on average. Afterwards we reran the analyses and obtained similar results (c.f. Appendix B of the Supplementary Material).

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to contribute to the scarce literature on employees with a mental illness (EMI) by investigating the impact of the mental illness on sick leave. More specifically, we hypothesized and tested whether the severity of the mental illness has an indirect effect on the days of sick leave sequentially via the symptomatic burden and social exclusion at the workplace. Regression and path analysis supported our hypothesis, indicating that patients with a more severe mental disorder suffer from a stronger symptomatic burden. This in turn increases their experienced exclusion at the workplace, yielding an indirect effect of the severity of the mental illness on social exclusion. Finally, the isolation EMI experience at the workplace leads to an increase of absenteeism, resulting in the hypothesized serial mediation.

Although stigmatization and discrimination of people with a mental illness in general has been the scope of several investigations [for reviews on the topic see e.g., (21, 75, 76)], the work situation of EMI still is understudied (16). Previous investigations indicate that social distancing from people with a mental illness exists – not only in the general public but also in the work context [e.g., (21, 24)]. The resulting isolation is known to have detrimental effects on health [e.g., (2729)] and thus, social exclusion can contribute to sick leave (30). The present study is the first that connects those findings, demonstrating that EMI show higher rates of absenteeism due to the social exclusion they experience at the workplace. It therefore adds to a better understanding of the experiences people with a mental illness make at the workplace and the consequences evoked thereof.

Deeper insights on the work situation of EMI are necessary since many of the published investigations on this population are descriptive in nature, limiting the possible conclusions drawn from those findings [c.f. (16)]. Thus, the current paper contributes to the scarce literature by applying inferential statistics, allowing more reliable conclusions about the specific work environment of EMI.

The study's findings underline the harmfulness of discrimination in general and social exclusion in particular. Various studies indicate the deleterious effects isolation can have on human health (2729)—thus it is not surprising that socially excluded employees have a higher risk for a long-term sick leave spell (30). The present study replicates those findings in the population of employees with a mental illness, indicating that the social exclusion of EMI increases the days they are sick leaving. Besides the detrimental effects on the health of the discriminated individual, those results also imply negative consequences for organizations and the society as a whole given the amount of costs employers and states across the European Union spend in relation to absenteeism (77).

Although discriminative actions as social distancing toward EMI have been the scope of investigations before [e.g., (23)], this study is the first to portend that the symptomatic burden caused by the mental illness is crucial for the degree of exclusion EMI experience at their workplaces. Seen through the lens of the stereotype content model (SCM) (31) this finding makes sense: The model and its extension, the BIAS Map (32), propose that groups who are evaluated as being cold and incompetent as e.g., people with a mental illness (37), elicit active and passive harm (e.g., exclusion) (3133). Thus, employees who are perceived as having more mental health problems should experience more social exclusion. However, to evaluate that a certain coworker has a mental health problem, the colleagues first have to notice corresponding peculiar behavior, that is, symptoms of a mental disorder. The more symptoms the colleagues notice (or the stronger they are), the higher the attributed mental health problems of the coworker and hence the elicited behavioral reaction will be. Thus, EMI suffering from a high symptomatic burden will be perceived as being more mentally ill by their coworkers in comparison to EMI with a low symptomatic burden, and thus experience more social exclusion. Experimental evidence supports the suggested importance of the symptomatic burden: Muschalla et al. (78) were able to show that the announced willingness of a fictious coworker with a mental illness to work on her mental health problems led to a lower desire for social distance toward that fictious coworker. Thus, even the anticipation of lower symptoms seems to mitigate the desire to socially distance oneself from the EMI.

Limitations

The present paper has several limitations. First of all, cross-sectional data assessed at one point of time cannot be interpreted causally, since the temporal precedence remains unclear [c.f. (79)]. In the present study however, this concern can partly be thwarted: Although the variables belong to the same assessment, they have not all been measured at the same time. The predictor (severity of the mental disorder) has been rated by trained study personnel in the first interview session which always took part before the online-questionnaires were dispensed [c.f. (62)]. Furthermore, we found similar results when we reanalyzed the data without participants who answered to the questionnaire of “COMPARE work” one or more days before answering the questionnaire of “COMPARE family,” yielding an average of 12 days between the assessment of the first mediator (the symptomatic burden) and the second mediator/the outcome (social exclusion and sick leave). Thus, temporal precedence could at least partly be established.

Despite these efforts to ensure temporal precedence, the results still have to be interpreted with caution: Different investigations suggest that experienced exclusion is not only a consequence of a mental illness but also may contribute to its development [c.f. (80, 81)], making a reversed causation of the presented results possible. Furthermore, mental illnesses go along with an increased risk for physical disorders (82, 83). Thus, it is possible that comorbid physical impairments might have contributed to social exclusion and sick leave as well, confounding the results of the present study. Therefore, future replications applying a cross-lagged panel design and incorporating possible confounders are recommended to gain more certainty about the direction and validity of the presented effects [c.f. for instance (84)].

As in other clinical trials (8587) recruitment of patients was difficult, yielding a small sample size in the present paper which bears the risk of a non-representative sample [c.f. for instance (84)]. In the German population, the most prevalent groups of mental disorders are anxiety disorders followed by affective disorders (88, 89). Similar results are found in non-German representative studies on mental illnesses in the working population, showing that simple phobia is the most prevalent mental disorder among the workforce followed by depression [c.f. (10)]. Although the present sample also mostly consists of patients with affective or anxiety disorders, the former are clearly dominating (40 diagnoses of affective disorders vs. 20 diagnoses of anxiety disorders). Thus, our sample is not an optimal representation of the population of employees with a mental illness. Future investigations applying a net online-questionnaire, including online screening instruments instead of on-site clinical interviews, could be able to establish a better representation of EMI by lowering the effort for participation.

Besides restrictions in the representativeness, the small sample size also constitutes a problem for the conducted analyses: Complex path models often require more participants than investigated in the present paper [c.f. (90)]. Unfortunately, although recruitment maintained for 17 months, it was not possible to acquire a larger sample—a problem different clinical trials are confronted with [c.f. (8587)]. Since previous literature already provided support for the individual paths of the mediation model, indicating that a higher severity of the mental illness is associated with a higher symptomatic burden (4245), that mental illnesses go along with social exclusion [e.g., (20, 21, 24, 38)] and that social exclusion in turn increases the risk for sick leave (30), we are confident that the depicted mediation model represents not merely a chance finding. However, future investigations should try to replicate our results within a larger sample.

Multiple sources of common method biases can operate in any given study and limit the trustworthiness of the results [c.f. (91)]. In the present paper we tried to confine this problem by several ways: First of all, we were able to draw on different assessment-methods (interviews and questionnaires), which might delimitate the risk of a common method bias to some degree [c.f. (91)]. Furthermore, the problem could partly be mitigated by the different contexts some of the variables were assessed in (more specifically, the different questionnaires of “COMPARE family” and “COMPARE work”) [c.f. (91)]. At last, the response format used to assess sick leave has been different from the response formats of its predictors which might reduce the risk of common method bias as well (91).

Besides actually being ill, employees may also call in sick for other non-health related reasons, as for instance caring for their ill children. To assure that we only measure the time employees were sick leaving because of their ill health, we explicitly asked for the days they were absent from work due to health reasons and appealed to exclude any days they were missing due to an illness of their children.

Lastly, the majority of the participants were female (77.9%). This can be explained by the applied inclusion criteria, only allowing people to participate if they fulfilled the criteria for a mental disorder [c.f. (62)]. In Germany, however, common mental illnesses as affective disorders or anxiety disorders have a higher prevalence in women than in men [c.f. (88, 89)]. Thus, the reason for the predominantly female composition of the sample probably lies in the applied inclusion criteria and mirrors the gender differences in the prevalence of common mental illnesses in Germany.

Practical Implications and Future Research

The results of the present study indicate that the social exclusion of EMI can increase the days they are sick leaving. Thus, not only active forms of discrimination, which are [according to the extension of the SCM (31, 32)] executed with the blatant aim to affect the target group (e.g., bullying), have detrimental effects on the individual [e.g., (60, 92)], but also more passive forms that are marked by less directed effort as e.g., neglecting, ignoring or excluding [c.f. (32)], can affect individuals health and therefore also the organization they work in, e.g., due to indirect costs associated with absenteeism (15). While active forms of discrimination might at first view seem to be more threatening for the organizational health and probably—due to their more directed effort (32)—attract more attention in organizations, it is hence also necessary that supervisors pay attention whether the more passive forms of discrimination take place in their teams and, if so, try to counteract them.

One possibility to mitigate the social exclusion of EMI could be the appliance of workplace interventions. The Mental Health First Aid training (93, 94) for instance can increase the intentions to provide help to a person with mental health problems in general and might also reduce the desire for social distance, as a recent meta-analysis across several settings indicates (95). Although not specifically developed for the workplace (94), a recent randomized controlled trial shows that the training may also increase the willingness to help a fictious coworker with a mental illness among public servants (96). Future randomized controlled trials might investigate whether the training is able to prevent the detrimental process described in this paper, e.g., by reducing the social exclusion EMI experience in the workplace.

Practitioners, responsible for occupational health policies in their organization, should furthermore focus on reducing stigma toward EMI, since the stereotype, that people suffering from a mental illness are cold and incompetent, is responsible for their exclusion, as the SCM proposes [c.f. (3133)]. According to a recent meta-analysis, the most promising approach to reduce stigma toward people with a severe mental disorder is establish contact and education [c.f. (97)]. Thus, practitioners can implement existing workplace programs as “The Working Mind” (98), which entails elements of education and contact (98, 99) and has proven to reduce mental health stigma (99), in order to counteract social exclusion of EMI at the workplace.

While the group “people with a mental illness” as a whole is perceived as being cold and incompetent in the sense of the SCM, research indicates that the perception on the two SCM-dimensions warmth and competence varies across different psychological disorders (23, 37). Follmer and Jones (23) for instance let participants rate how warm and competent different disorders are perceived in the workplace by society and found that individuals suffering from an anxiety disorder are perceived as being warmer and more competent than people with a depression or a bipolar disorder. As a result, the evoked behavioral response toward people with a mental illness might differ, depending on the disorder [c.f. (32)]: Employers for instance would rather dismiss an employee developing a schizophrenia than an employee developing a depression (17). Thus, it would be interesting to explore whether the presented results vary across different mental illnesses, more specifically, whether the type of disorder (for instance depression vs. anxiety disorder) moderates the effect of the symptomatic burden on social exclusion in the demonstrated process. Yet, when we checked our data, we were not able to find a significant interaction (c.f. Appendix C of the Supplementary Material), which might be caused by the small sample size. However, a better understanding of which subgroups of people with a mental illness are at special risk for social exclusion and the subsequent absenteeism at the workplace would help, e.g.„ for constructing more precise interventions, which is why we encourage further investigations in this regard.

Conclusion

In 2016, about every sixth person in the European Union suffered from mental health problems. Besides the detrimental effects that go along with the disorder itself, people with a mental illness also face various hindrances imposed by society, that are known to worsen the overall health status. The present paper demonstrated that those effects also take place within the workforce. Employers therefore need to implement an integrative climate where employees with a mental illness can feel as safe and valued as every other employee – for the sake of their staff's but also their organization's well-being.

Data Availability Statement

Data share of all primary data of the COMPARE-consortium is planed after the completion of the projects [see (61)]. Participants were also assured that data would not be shared until the projects were completed. Therefore, since assessments and projects are not yet completed, the data cannot yet be made available.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee at the department of psychology at the Philipps-University Marburg. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Members of the COMPARE-Family Research Group

Kristin Gilbert, Department of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Philipps University Marburg, Marburg, Germany; Markus Stracke, Department of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Philipps University Marburg, Marburg, Germany; Christina Klose, Department of Medical Biometry, Institute of Medical Biometry and Informatics, Heidelberg University, Heidelberg, Germany; Johannes Krisam, Department of Medical Biometry, Institute of Medical Biometry and Informatics, Heidelberg University, Heidelberg, Germany; Moritz Pohl, Department of Medical Biometry, Institute of Medical Biometry and Informatics, Heidelberg University, Heidelberg, Germany; Claudia Buntrock, Department of Psychology, Chair for Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Friedrich-Alexander University Erlangen-Nüremberg, Erlangen, Germany; David Daniel Ebert, Department of Psychology and Digital Mental Health Care, Faculty of Sports and Health Science, Technical University München, Munich, Germany; Jürgen Margraf, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Psychology, Ruhr-University Bochum, Bochum, Germany; Silvia Schneider, Department of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Ruhr-University Bochum, Bochum, Germany; Rudolf Stark, Department of Psychotherapy and Systems Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Sports Science, Justus-Liebig-University Gießen, Gießen, Germany; Julia Metzger, Department of Psychotherapy and Systems Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Sports Science, Justus-Liebig-University Gießen, Gießen, Germany; Julia Glombiewski, Department of Biopsychology, Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Psycholgy, University Koblenz-Landau, Landau, Germany; Anette Schröder, Department of Biopsychology, Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Psycholgy, University Koblenz-Landau, Landau, Germany; Jens Heider, Department of Biopsychology, Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Psycholgy, University Koblenz-Landau, Landau, Germany; Winfried Rief, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Psychology, Philipps University Marburg, Marburg, Germany; Pia Eitenmüller, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Psychology, Philipps University Marburg, Marburg, Germany.

Author Contributions

BF, CT, KO, and NB generated the idea for the present paper. BF and CT performed the statistical analysis. BF wrote the first draft of the manuscript. CT contributed to sections of the manuscript. CR, CS, HC, KO, LW, MK, and RS designed the COMPARE-Consortium from which the used data originate and thus substantially contributed to the design of the present study. BF and NB were responsible for managing the project COMPARE work. The COMPARE-family research group was responsible for managing the project COMPARE family, for the clinical study monitoring as well as the data acquisition and data management of both projects. All authors revised the manuscript and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The projects COMPARE work (grant number: 01GL1748A) and COMPARE family (grant number: 01GL1748B), from which the data used in this study originate, are entirely funded by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2022.892174/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1. ^In the present paper we used data from the clinical study “COMPARE family” [see (62)] and the subproject “COMPARE work” [see (61)]. Since the acquisition of participants in the clinical study is still ongoing, two partial data sets were provided in May 2020 and in February 2021 with the aim to facilitate in advance analysis. The partial data sets contain final, not changeable and completely refined data. In this present paper we used data from the first partial data set provided in May 2020.

References

1. Chaudhury PK, Deka K, Chetia D. Disability associated with mental disorders. Indian J Psychiatry. (2006) 48:95–101. doi: 10.4103/0019-5545.31597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Ormel J, Petukhova M, Chatterji S, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alonso J, Angermeyer MC, et al. Disability and treatment of specific mental and physical disorders across the world. Br J Psychiatry. (2008) 192:368–75. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.039107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. OECD/European Union. Health at a Glace: Europe 2018: State of Health in the EU Cycle. Paris: OECD Publishing (2018).

4. Marwaha S, Parsons N, Broome M. Mood instability, mental illness and suicidal ideas: results from a household survey. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2013) 48:1431–7. doi: 10.1007/s00127-013-0653-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Qin P, Nordentoft M. Suicide risk in relation to psychiatric hospitalization: evidence based on longitudinal registers. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2005) 62:427–32. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.4.427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Schaefer J, Giangrande E, Weinberger DR, Dickinson D. The global cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: consistent over decades and around the world. Schizophr Res. (2013) 150:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Oliva-Moreno J, López-Bastida J, Montejo-González AL, Osuna-Guerrero R, Duque-González B. The socioeconomic costs of mental illness in Spain. Eur J Health Econ. (2009) 10:361–9. doi: 10.1007/s10198-008-0135-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Rice DP, Kelman S, Miller LS. The economic burden of mental illness. Hosp Community Psychiatry. (1992) 43:1227–32. doi: 10.1176/ps.43.12.1227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Dewa CS, Lesage A, Goering P, Caveen M. Nature and prevalence of mental illness in the workplace. Healthcare Papers. (2004) 5:12–25. doi: 10.12927/hcpap..16820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Sanderson K, Andrews G. Common mental disorders in the workforce: Recent findings from descriptive and social epidemiology. Can J Psychiatry. (2006) 51:63–75. doi: 10.1177/070674370605100202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Adler DA, McLaughlin TJ, Rogers WH, Chang H, Lapitsky L, Lerner D. Job performance deficits due to depression. AJP. (2006) 163:1569–76. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.9.1569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Asami Y, Goren A, Okumura Y. Work productivity loss with depression, diagnosed and undiagnosed, among workers in an Internet-based survey conducted in Japan. J Occup Environ Med. (2015) 57:105–10. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0000000000000310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Wright TA, Bonett DG, Sweeney DA. Mental health and work performance: results of a longitudinal field study. J Occup Organ Psychol. (1993) 66:277–84. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8325.1993.tb00539.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Stewart WF, Ricci JA, Chee E, Hahn SR, Morganstein D. Cost of lost productive work time among US workers with depression. JAMA. (2003) 289:3135–44. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.23.3135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Greenberg PE, Fournier AA, Sisitsky T, Pike CT, Kessler RC. The economic burden of adults with major depressive disorder in the United States (2005 and 2010). J Clin Psychiatry. (2015) 76:155–62. doi: 10.4088/JCP.14m09298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Follmer KB, Jones KS. Mental illness in the workplace: An interdisciplinary review and organizational research agenda. J Manage. (2018) 44:325–51. doi: 10.1177/0149206317741194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Manning C, White PD. Attitudes of employers to the mentally ill. Psychiatr bull. (1995) 19:541–3. doi: 10.1192/pb.19.9.541

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Thornicroft G, Brohan E, Rose D, Sartorius N, Leese M. Global pattern of experienced and anticipated discrimination against people with schizophrenia: a cross-sectional survey. Lancet. (2009) 373:408–15. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61817-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Dhanani LY, Beus JM, Joseph DL. Workplace discrimination: A meta-analytic extension, critique, and future research agenda. Pers Psychol. (2018) 71:147–79. doi: 10.1111/peps.12254

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Angermeyer MC, Matschinger H. The stigma of mental illness: Effects of labelling on public attitudes towards people with mental disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2003) 108:304–9. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.00150.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Parcesepe AM, Cabassa LJ. Public stigma of mental illness in the United States: a systematic literature review. Adm Policy Ment Health. (2013) 40:384–99. doi: 10.1007/s10488-012-0430-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Baldwin ML, Marcus SC. Perceived and measured stigma among workers with serious mental illness. Psychiatric services. (2006) 57:388–92. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.57.3.388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Follmer KB, Jones KS. Stereotype content and social distancing from employees with mental illness: the moderating roles of gender and social dominance orientation. J Appl Soc Psychol. (2017) 47:492–504. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12455

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Reavley NJ, Jorm AF, Morgan AJ. Discrimination and positive treatment toward people with mental health problems in workplace and education settings: Findings from an Australian national survey. Stigma and Health. (2017) 2:254–65. doi: 10.1037/sah0000059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Pavalko EK, Mossakowski KN, Hamilton VJ. Does perceived discrimination affect health? Longitudinal relationships between work discrimination and women's physical and emotional health. J Health Soc Behav. (2003) 44:18. doi: 10.2307/1519813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Pascoe EA, Smart Richman L. Perceived discrimination and health: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. (2009) 135:531–54. doi: 10.1037/a0016059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Friedmann E, Thomas SA, Liu F, Morton PG, Chapa D, Gottlieb SS. Relationship of depression, anxiety, and social isolation to chronic heart failure outpatient mortality. Am Heart J. (2006) 152:940. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2006.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Garner W, Speicher C, Penn GM, Holliday J, Glaser R. Psychosocial modifiers of immunocompetence in medical students. Psychosom Med. (1984) 46:7–14. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198401000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Luo Y, Hawkley LC, Waite LJ, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness, health, and mortality in old age: a national longitudinal study. Soc Sci Med. (2012) 74:907–14. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.11.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Josephson M, Lindberg P, Voss M, Alfredsson L, Vingård E. The same factors influence job turnover and long spells of sick leave—a 3-year follow-up of Swedish nurses. Eur J Public Health. (2008) 18:380–5. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckn009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Fiske ST, Cuddy AJC, Glick P, Xu J. A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2002) 82:878–902. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.82.6.878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Cuddy AJC, Fiske ST, Glick P. The BIAS map: behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2007) 92:631–48. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Cuddy AJC, Fiske ST, Glick P. “Warmth and Competence as Universal Dimensions of Social Perception: The Stereotype Content Model and the BIAS Map”. In: Zanna MP, editor. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. London, UK: Academic Press (2008). p. 61–149.

34. Durante F, Fiske ST, Gelfand MJ, Crippa F, Suttora C, Stillwell A, et al. Ambivalent stereotypes link to peace, conflict, and inequality across 38 nations. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2017) 114:669–74. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1611874114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Cuddy AJC, Fiske ST, Glick P. When professionals become mothers, warmth doesn't cut the ice. J Soc Iss. (2004) 60:701–18. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-4537.2004.00381.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Cuddy AJC, Fiske ST, Kwan VSY, Glick P, Demoulin S, Leyens J-P, et al. Stereotype content model across cultures: towards universal similarities and some differences. Br J Soc Psychol. (2009) 48:1–33. doi: 10.1348/014466608X314935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Sadler MS, Meagor EL, Kaye KE. Stereotypes of mental disorders differ in competence and warmth. Soc Sci Med. (2012) 74:915–22. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Boysen GA, Isaacs RA, Tretter L, Markowski S. Evidence for blatant dehumanization of mental illness and its relation to stigma. J Soc Psychol. (2020) 160:346–56. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2019.1671301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Clark K. The effect of mental illness on segregation following institutional misconduct. Crim Justice Behav. (2018) 45:1363–82. doi: 10.1177/0093854818766974

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Baldwin ML, Marcus SC. Labor market outcomes of persons with mental disorders. Ind Relat. (2007) 46:481–510. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-232X.2007.00478.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Yoshimura Y, Bakolis I, Henderson C. Psychiatric diagnosis and other predictors of experienced and anticipated workplace discrimination and concealment of mental illness among mental health service users in England. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2018) 53:1099–109. doi: 10.1007/s00127-018-1561-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. García-Mieres H, de Jesús-Romero R, Ochoa S, Feixas G. Beyond the cognitive insight paradox: Self-reflectivity moderates the relationship between depressive symptoms and general psychological distress in psychosis. Schizophr Res. (2020) 222:297–303. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2020.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Ormel J, von Korff M, Ustun B, Pini S, Korten A, Oldehinkel T. Common mental disorders and disability across cultures: Results from the WHO collaborative study on psychological problems in general health care. JAMA. (1994) 272:1741–8. doi: 10.1001/jama.1994.03520220035028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Lasgaard M, Goossens L, Elklit A. Loneliness, depressive symptomatology, and suicide ideation in adolescence: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2011) 39:137–50. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9442-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Baddeley JL, Pennebaker JW, Beevers CG. Everyday social behavior during a major depressive episode. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. (2013) 4:445–52. doi: 10.1177/1948550612461654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Seeman TE, Singer BH, Rowe JW, Horwitz RI, McEwen BS. Price of adaptation-Allostatic load and its health consequences: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Arch Intern Med. (1997) 157:2259–68. doi: 10.1001/archinte.1997.00440400111013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. McEwen BS, Stellar E. Stress and the individual: Mechanisms leading to disease. Arch Intern Med. (1993) 153:2093–101. doi: 10.1001/archinte.1993.00410180039004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. McEwen BS, Seeman TE. Protective and damaging effects of mediators of stress: elaborating and testing the concepts of allostasis and allostatic load. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (1999) 896:30–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08103.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. McEwen BS. Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. N Engl J Med. (1998) 338:171–9. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199801153380307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Ganster DC, Rosen CC. Work stress and employee health: a multidisciplinary review. J Manage. (2013) 39:1085–122. doi: 10.1177/0149206313475815

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Sterling P, Eyer J. “Allostasis: A new paradigm to explain arousal pathology”. In: Fisher S, Reason J, editors. Handbook of Life Stress, Cognition, and Health. New York, NY: Wiley (1988). p. 629–49.

52. Jönsson P, Wallergård M, Österberg K, Hansen AM, Johansson G, Karlson B. Cardiovascular and cortisol reactivity and habituation to a virtual reality version of the trier social stress test: a pilot study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2010) 35:1397–403. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Schommer NC, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. Dissociation between reactivity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system to repeated psychosocial stress. Psychosom Med. (2003) 65:450–60. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000035721.12441.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. von Känel R, Kudielka BM, Preckel D, Hanebuth D, Fischer JE. Delayed response and lack of habituation in plasma interleukin-6 to acute mental stress in men. Brain Behav Immun. (2006) 20:40–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2005.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Gerra G, Zaimovic A, Mascetti GG, Gardini S, Zambelli U, Timpano M, et al. Neuroendocrine responses to experimentally-induced psychological stress in healthy humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2001) 26:91–107. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(00)00046-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Pereira D, Müller P, Elfering A. Workflow interruptions, social stressors from supervisor(s) and attention failure in surgery personnel. Ind Health. (2015) 53:427–33. doi: 10.2486/indhealth.2013-0219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Chen Y, Li S. Relationship between workplace ostracism and unsafe behaviors: The mediating effect of psychological detachment and emotional exhaustion. Psychol Rep. (2020) 123:488–516. doi: 10.1177/0033294118813892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Pereira D, Elfering A. Social stressors at work and sleep during weekends: The mediating role of psychological detachment. J Occup Health Psychol. (2014) 19:85–95. doi: 10.1037/a0034928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Glynn LM, Christenfeld N, Gerin W. The role of rumination in recovery from reactivity: Cardiovascular consequences of emotional states. Psychosom Med. (2002) 64:714–26. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000031574.42041.23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Nielsen MB, Indregard A-MR, Øverland S. Workplace bullying and sickness absence: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the research literature. Scand J Work Environ Health. (2016) 42:359–70. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.3579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Christiansen H, Reck C, Zietlow A-L, Otto K, Steinmayr R, Wirthwein L, et al. Children of mentally III parents at risk evaluation (COMPARE): design and methods of a randomized controlled multicenter study-part I. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:128. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Stracke M, Gilbert K, Kieser M, Klose C, Krisam J, Ebert DD, et al. COMPARE family (children of mentally ill parents at risk evaluation): a study protocol for a preventive intervention for children of mentally Ill parents (Triple P, evidence-based program that enhances parentings skills, in addition to gold-standard cbt with the mentally ill parent) in a multicenter RCT-Part II. Front Psych. (2019) 10:54. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing (2013). 947 p.

64. Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. Research electronic data capture (REDCap): a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform. (2009) 42:377–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Margraf J, Cwik JC, Pflug V, Schneider S. Structured clinical interviews for mental disorders across the lifespan: psychometric quality and further developments of the DIPS open access interviews. [Strukturierte klinische Interviews zur Erfassung psychischer Störungen über die Lebensspanne] Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie. (2017) 46:176–86. doi: 10.1026/1616-3443/a000430

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Margraf J, Cwik JC, Suppiger A, Schneider S. DIPS Open Access: Diagnostic Interview for Mental Disorders. [DIPS Open Access: Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen] [Internet]. Bochum: Mental Health Research and Treament Center, Ruhr-Universität Bochum; (2017). 274p. doi: 10.13154/rub.100.89 (accessed February, 2022).

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Kliem S, Brähler E. Brief Symptom Inventory by L. R. Derogatis - German Version [Brief Symptom Inventory von L. R. Derogatis - Deutsche Fassung]. Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Pearson (2017). 131 p.

68. Kliem S, Brähler E. Symptom-Checklist-90-R by L. R. Derogatis - German Version [Symptom-Checkliste-90-R von L. R. Derogatis - Deutsche Fassung]. Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Pearson (2016). 334 p.

69. Franke GH. Brief Symptom Inventory by LR Derogatis - German Version [Brief Symptom Inventory von L R Derogatis - Deutsche Version]. Göttingen, Germany: Beltz Test (2000).

70. Zapf D, Holz M. Social Stressors in Organisations - The Development of an Instrument. Frankfurt am Main: JWGoethe-Universität: Institut für Psychologie (2002).

71. Frese M, Zapf D. A scale measuring social stressors at work [Eine Skala zur Erfassung von Sozialen Stressoren am Arbeitsplatz]. Zeitschrift für Arbeitswissenschaft. (1987) 41:134–41.

72. Kern M, Heissler C, Zapf D. Social job stressors can foster employee well-being: introducing the concept of social challenge stressors. J Bus Psychol. (2021) 36:771–792. doi: 10.1007/s10869-020-09702-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. 2nd ed. New York: The Guilford Press (2018). 692 p.

74. Graubner B, Auhuber TC. ICD-10-GM 2020 Systematic Catalogue. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems: 10th Revision - German modification [ICD-10-GM 2020 Systematisches Verzeichnis. Internationale statistische Klassifikation der Krankheiten und verwandter Gesundheitsprobleme: 10. Revision - German modification]. Köln, Germany: Deutscher Ärzteverlag (2020). 782 p.

75. Overton SL, Medina SL. The stigma of mental illness. J Counsel Develop. (2008) 86:143–51. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6678.2008.tb00491.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Sickel AE, Seacat JD, Nabors NA. Mental health stigma update: a review of consequences. Adv Mental Health. (2014) 12:202–15. doi: 10.1080/18374905.2014.11081898

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. European Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Condition. Absence from work. Available online at: https://www.eurofound.europa.eu/de/publications/report/2010/absence-from-work (2010). 29 p.

78. Muschalla B, Fay D, Seemann A. Asking for work adjustments or initiating behavioural changes - what makes a ‘problematic co-worker' score Brownie points? An experimental study on the reactions towards colleagues with a personality disorder. Psychol Health Med. (2016) 21:856–62. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2015.1109671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Steyer R. Theory of Causal Regression Models [Theorie kausaler Regressionsmodelle]. Stuttgart, Germany: Fischer (1992). 248 p.

80. Rudert SC, Janke S, Greifeneder R. Ostracism breeds depression: Longitudinal associations between ostracism and depression over a three-year-period. J Affect Disord Reports. (2021) 4:100118. doi: 10.1016/j.jadr.2021.100118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Fung K, Xu C, Glazier BL, Parsons CA, Alden LE. “Research in Clinical Psychology: Social Exclusion and Psychological Disorders“. In: Riva P, Eck J, editors. Social Exclusion: Psychological Approaches to Understanding and Reducing Its Impact. Cham, Switzerland: Springer (2016). p. 157–176. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-33033-4_8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Fan Z, Wu Y, Shen J, Ji T, Zhan R. Schizophrenia and the risk of cardiovascular diseases: a meta-analysis of thirteen cohort studies. J Psychiatr Res. (2013) 47:1549–56. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Fuller BE, Rodriguez VL, Linke A, Sikirica M, Dirani R, Hauser P. Prevalence of liver disease in veterans with bipolar disorder or schizophrenia. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. (2011) 33:232–7. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2011.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Eid M, Gollwitzer M, Schmitt M. Statistics and research methods [Statistik und Forschungsmethoden]. 2nd ed. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz (2011). 1024 p.

85. Hunninghake DB, Darby CA, Probstfield JL. Recruitment experience in clinical trials: literature summary and annotated bibliography. Control Clin Trials. (1987) 8:6–30. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(87)90004-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Charlson ME, Horwitz RI. Applying results of randomised trials to clinical practice: impact of losses before randomisation. Br Med J. (1984) 289:1281–4. doi: 10.1136/bmj.289.6454.1281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Howard L, de Salis I, Tomlin Z, Thornicroft G, Donovan J. Why is recruitment to trials difficult? An investigation into recruitment difficulties in an RCT of supported employment in patients with severe mental illness. Contemp Clin Trials. (2009) 30:40–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cct.2008.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Jacobi F, Höfler M, Strehle J, Mack S, Gerschler A, Scholl L, et al. Mental disorders in the general population: Study on the health of adults in Germany and the additional module mental health (DEGS1-MH)[Psychische Störungen in der Allgemeinbevölkerung: Studie zur Gesundheit Erwachsener in Deutschland und ihr Zusatzmodul Psychische Gesundheit (DEGS1-MH)] [Mental disorders in the general population: study on the health of adults in Germany and the additional module mental health (DEGS1-MH)]. Nervenarzt. (2014) 85:77–87. ger. doi: 10.1007/s00115-013-3961-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Jacobi F, Höfler M, Strehle J, Mack S, Gerschler A, Scholl L, et al. Erratum to: Mental disorders in the general population: Study on the health of adults in Germany and the additional module mental health (DEGS1-MH) [Erratum zu: Psychische Störungen in der Allgemeinbevölkerung: Studie zur Gesundheit Erwachsener in Deutschland und ihr Zusatzmodul “Psychische Gesundheit” (DEGS1-MH)] [Erratum to: Mental disorders in the general population. Study on the health of adults in Germany and the additional module mental health (DEGS1-MH)]. Nervenarzt. (2016) 87:88–90. ger. doi: 10.1007/s00115-015-4458-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Sim M, Kim SY, Suh Y. Sample size requirements for simple and complex mediation models. Educ Psychol Meas. (2022) 82:76–106. doi: 10.1177/00131644211003261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee JY, Podsakoff NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J App Psychol. (2003) 88:879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Rosenthal L, Earnshaw VA, Carroll-Scott A, Henderson KE, Peters SM, McCaslin C, et al. Weight- and race-based bullying: health associations among urban adolescents. J Health Psychol. (2015) 20:401–12. doi: 10.1177/1359105313502567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Kitchener BA, Jorm AF, Kelly C. Mental Health First Aid Manual. 4th ed. Melbourne, Australia: Mental Health First Aid Australia (2017). 184 p.

94. Kitchener BA, Jorm AF. Mental health first aid training for the public: Evaluation of effects on knowledge, attitudes and helping behavior. BMC Psychiatry. (2002) 2:10. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-2-10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Morgan AJ, Ross A, Reavley NJ. Systematic review and meta-analysis of Mental Health First Aid training: Effects on knowledge, stigma, and helping behaviour. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0197102. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Reavley NJ, Morgan AJ, Fischer J-A, Kitchener BA, Bovopoulos N, Jorm AF. Effectiveness of elearning and blended modes of delivery of mental health first aid training in the workplace: randomised controlled trial. BMC Psych. (2018) 18:312. doi: 10.1186/s12888-018-1888-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Morgan AJ, Reavley NJ, Ross A, San Too L, Jorm AF. Interventions to reduce stigma towards people with severe mental illness: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Psychiatr Res. (2018) 103:120–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Szeto A, Dobson KS, Luong D, Krupa T, Kirsh B. Workplace antistigma programs at the mental health commission of Canada: part 1. processes and projects. Can J Psychiatry. (2019) 64(Supplement 1):5S–12S. doi: 10.1177/0706743719842557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Dobson KS, Szeto A, Knaak S. The working mind: a meta-analysis of a workplace mental health and stigma reduction program. Can J Psychiatry. (2019) 64(Supplement 1):39S–47S. doi: 10.1177/0706743719842559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: workplace, mental illness, social exclusion, sick leave, discrimination, stereotype content model

Citation: Frank BP, Theil CM, Brill N, Christiansen H, Schwenck C, Kieser M, Reck C, Steinmayr R, Wirthwein L, Otto K and the COMPARE-family Research Group (2022) Leave Me Alone With Your Symptoms! Social Exclusion at the Workplace Mediates the Relationship of Employee's Mental Illness and Sick Leave. Front. Public Health 10:892174. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.892174

Received: 08 March 2022; Accepted: 22 June 2022;
Published: 28 July 2022.

Edited by:

Leah Okenwa Emegwa, Swedish Red Cross University College, Sweden

Reviewed by:

Johanna Baumgardt, Vivantes Hospital, Germany
Keming Yang, Durham University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Frank, Theil, Brill, Christiansen, Schwenck, Kieser, Reck, Steinmayr, Wirthwein, Otto and the COMPARE-family Research Group. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Benjamin Pascal Frank, benjamin.frank@uni-marburg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.