Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Public Health, 10 December 2021
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Insights in Public Mental Health: 2021 View all 25 articles

From Burnout to Occupational Depression: Recent Developments in Research on Job-Related Distress and Occupational Health

\nIrvin Sam Schonfeld
Irvin Sam Schonfeld1*Renzo BianchiRenzo Bianchi2
  • 1Department of Psychology, The City College and the Graduate Center of the City University of New York, New York, NY, United States
  • 2Institute of Work and Organizational Psychology, University of Neuchâtel, Neuchâtel, Switzerland

Job-related distress has been a focal concern in occupational health science. Job-related distress has a well-documented health-damaging and life-threatening character, not to mention its economic cost. In this article, we review recent developments in research on job-related distress and examine ongoing changes in how job-related distress is conceptualized and assessed. By adopting an approach that is theoretically, empirically, and clinically informed, we demonstrate how the construct of burnout and its measures, long favored in research on job-related distress, have proved to be problematic. We underline a new recommendation for addressing job-related distress within the long-established framework of depression research. In so doing, we present the Occupational Depression Inventory, a recently developed instrument devised to assess depressive symptoms that individuals specifically attribute to their work. We close our paper by laying out the advantages of a paradigm shift from burnout to occupational depression.

Introduction

Job-related distress, a focal concern of occupational health science, has well-documented health-damaging and life-threatening effects, not to mention economic costs. Burnout and depression have constituted two key indicators in research on job-related distress over the last decades.

Considerable evidence has accumulated to show that chronic exposure to adverse working conditions contributes to the emergence of depressive symptoms and disorders (13). Depression can culminate in suicide, including workplace suicide (4). Burnout is thought to reflect the personal impact of chronic exposure to adverse working conditions. Given the common origins of burnout and job-related depressive symptoms and disorders, it is important to examine the evidence bearing on their conceptualization.

In theory, burnout is a gauge of job-related distress. It consists of three symptom dimensions, the core dimension being (emotional) exhaustion (5). Maslach et al. (6) wrote that “[f]or use in applied settings, a prudent approach when deciding to take action on the basis of burnout scores is to give the most weight to Emotional Exhaustion [EE] scores as they are the most reliable” (p. 3), underlining the dimension's central position in burnout. EE refers to feeling emotionally drained—the result of the worker's chronic exposure to adverse job conditions. Burnout's two other dimensions are depersonalization/cynicism (DP) and a reduced sense of personal accomplishment (rPA), also known as professional inefficacy (6). DP refers to the worker being socially distant, disengaged, and possessing a cynical attitude toward coworkers and the people the worker is supposed to serve (e.g., patients, students, customers); rPA refers to the worker feeling that he or she fails to accomplish worthwhile job-related goals. Many researchers have advanced the view that burnout is distinct from depression (5, 7).

This paper has a twofold aim. First, we examine sets of research findings and observations that bear on the conceptualization of job-related distress in the context of the burnout–depression relationship, approached both dimensionally and categorically. Second, we provide a recommendation for assessing job-related distress by way of a new measure that may help occupational health specialists support individuals and organizations more effectively.

Burnout and Depression

Burnout has been conceptualized as a set of symptoms, largely fatigue-related, that are caused by adverse working conditions. An exemplary item from the most frequently used burnout measure, the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI), demonstrates that idea: “I feel emotionally drained from my work” (6). The symptom in question is explicitly attributed to the individual's job. Burnout scales like the MBI have been linked to prominent occupational risk factors such as excessive workloads and reduced control over job tasks (8, 9). From a methodological standpoint, given that burnout items reference both the dependent variable (the symptoms) and independent variable (workplace stressors), it is no surprise that burnout scales are related to perceived workplace stressors, particularly when cross-sectional designs are used [see (10)].

Depression involves a cluster of affective, cognitive, behavioral, and somatic symptoms (e.g., dysphoric mood and anhedonia). Though depression is nosologically and diagnostically defined, there is robust evidence that depression is better conceived of as a dimensional phenomenon—a continuum—with only individuals at the highest end of the continuum meeting criteria for formal diagnoses of depression (1113). Depressive symptom scales differ from burnout scales in an important way. Except for the recently developed Occupational Depression Inventory (14), described later, depressive symptom scales are “cause-neutral.” This is not to say that depressive symptoms and disorders are void of causes, only that assessment instruments tend to focus on symptom severity rather than symptom causes. Situations involving unresolvable stress, in which individuals feel helpless and trapped in the face of negative events perceived as uncontrollable and impossible to surmount, have long been identified as crucial depressogenic factors (15, 16).

Observations Bearing on Burnout's Relation to Depression

We now examine six sets of observations that bear on the problematic nature of the burnout construct, particularly regarding its relation to depression. First, although multiple factors can give rise to depression, abundant evidence from well-controlled longitudinal studies indicates that workplace stressors are related to increased levels of depressive symptoms (measured by cause-neutral symptom scales) and elevated risk of depressive disorders (2, 3, 17, 18). It is unlikely that job stressors increase the risk of burnout without commensurately increasing the risk of depressive symptoms and disorders (19, 20).

Second, burnout's core dimension, exhaustion, is highly related to depressive symptoms. Although some meta-analytic evidence suggests an average burnout/exhaustion–depression correlation in the 0.50 s (21, 22), other meta-analytic research (11, 23) indicates that the burnout/exhaustion–depression correlation can reach 0.70–0.80 and higher, particularly when measurement error is controlled. Meier and Kim (22) observed that a correlation of 0.50 has been used to advance the view that (a) burnout and depression do not overlap and (b) burnout and depression do overlap. Correlation coefficients should be understood in context. The research of Wurm et al. (24) provides some of that context. These authors obtained a burnout–depression correlation of 0.52 in a sample of 5,897 physicians, and observed that, compared to an almost-symptom-free reference group, as burnout symptoms increased stepwise from mild to moderate to severe to extremely high levels, the odds ratio for major depression increased dramatically, from 2.99 to 10.14 to 46.84 to 92.78.

Because fatigue and sleep problems are symptoms of depression (25), fatigue-related items are commonly found in depression symptom scales. Maslach and Leiter (7) argued that, because depression scales include fatigue-related symptom items, a high burnout–depression correlation may be a methodological artifact. However, empirical research in which fatigue-related items were stripped out of depressive symptom scales barely changed the correlation (23, 26).

Exploratory structural equation modeling bifactor analyses extract a general factor on which all items can, theoretically, load and specific factors (bifactors) on which items are allowed to cross-load. In several studies (11, 27, 28), depression and exhaustion items primarily loaded on the general factor. The DP and rPA items tended to load more highly on their respective specific factors than on the general factor with exemplary exceptions. For example, the rPA item “I feel very energetic,” which, understandably, reflects the opposite of exhaustion, has a strong negative loading on the general factor and a much weaker loading on the rPA-specific factor.

What construct do depression and burnout's exhaustion (sub)scales measure? Schonfeld et al. (23, 27) advanced the view that the construct is a dimension of psychopathology that can be labeled psychological distress/dysphoria. More than 40 years ago, Dohrenwend et al. (29) found that measures of dread, sadness, anxiety, helplessness/hopelessness, and poor self-esteem correlated about as highly as the scales' reliabilities permitted, suggesting that the scales reflect the same underlying construct. The investigators labeled the construct psychological distress. More recent research on psychopathology has provided evidence for a distress or internalizing dimension reflecting depressive and related (e.g., anxiety) symptoms (12, 27, 30, 31). Structural equation modeling (SEM) evidence indicates that one cohesive distress factor underlies depressive, anxiety, and exhaustion symptom items (27).

Some have argued that burnout (or depression) may mediate the impact of job stressors on depression (or burnout). However, that the discriminant validity of burnout vis-à-vis depression has not been clearly demonstrated undermines the argument. Moreover, such an argument is difficult to articulate when approaching both burnout and depression dimensionally (i.e., as continua).

Third, if burnout is a syndrome comprising EE, DP, and rPA—as indicated by Maslach and colleagues (5, 6)—one would expect the EE, DP, and rPA subscales of the MBI to be more highly correlated with each other than with non-burnout scales. Meta-analytic and SEM evidence (11, 23), however, indicates that EE is more highly related to depressive symptoms than to DP and rPA, undermining the idea that burnout is a (distinct) syndrome (11, 23). In other words, if exhaustion is part of a syndrome, it is part of a depressive syndrome (10, 11).

Interestingly, several methodological factors work against finding the magnitude of the EE–depression correlation to be stronger than the correlations among the MBI's subscales, suggesting that the EE–depression link is sturdy. First, the abovementioned “energetic” item on the rPA subscale of the MBI likely increases the magnitude of the EE–rPA correlation. Second, depression symptom items cover the previous 1 or 2 weeks whereas MBI items cover the previous year. Third, the wording of the items in the MBI subscales are substantially similar, each item referencing work, but depression items cover diverse symptoms without a common causal reference. Despite the influence of these methodological factors, the magnitude of the EE–depression correlation is greater than that of the correlations among the MBI subscales.

How are the MBI's other dimensions, DP and rPA, related to depression? They have non-zero correlations with depressive symptoms. DP supposedly reflects a strategy for coping with EE (5, 32). It is reflective of “not caring anymore,” a feature associated with depression (25). rPA is likely to be a long-term consequence of exhaustion (5, 33). It parallels the negative evaluations of one's worth associated with depression (25).

Fourth, there are no clear or consensual diagnostic criteria for burnout. Maslach et al. (6) indicated that the MBI is not a diagnostic tool. That admonition has not stopped researchers from treating burnout diagnostically. Rotenstein et al. (34) found 142 unique definitions of burnout in the literature on physicians. When attempting to treat burnout diagnostically, researchers generally define a case arbitrarily, as an individual with a score above a predetermined cutoff on a burnout (sub)scale. Bianchi, Schonfeld, and colleagues (35, 36) observed that many researchers identify individuals as cases of burnout based on surprisingly low scale scores. Such case-identification procedures fail to distinguish an individual experiencing pervasive distress from an individual confronting a day in which job stress increased but was still within the “normal range.” When Bianchi et al. (37) used an MBI cutoff corresponding to burnout symptoms experienced at least a few times a week (a frequency assumed to represent pervasive distress), they found that 90% of the individuals in the burnout group met criteria for a provisional diagnosis of depression. Schonfeld and Bianchi (20) obtained similar findings using the Shirom-Melamed Burnout Measure. Such findings suggest that if “clinical burnout” (i.e., burnout as a medical diagnosis) were to be defined someday, its differential diagnosis vis-à-vis clinical depression would be impossible, undermining burnout's clinical validity and usefulness (38).

A related set of findings indicates that as burnout symptoms increase, the risk of meeting criteria for a diagnosis of depression increases. This pattern has been replicated across occupational groups, including physician (24), teacher (20, 37), and dentist (39) samples. Ahola et al. (19) went on to observe that “burnout could be used as an equivalent to depressive symptoms in work life” (p. 35).

Fifth, “[o]ften insomnia or fatigue is the presenting complaint” when a depressed individual seeks help from a clinician [(25); p. 162]. An experienced clinician recognizes the disorder. Freudenberger (40), the first investigator to write about burnout in a research journal, observed that the burned-out individual “looks, acts and seems depressed” (p. 161). Exhaustion complaints can mask broader depressive syndromes, especially in male and younger patients. Exhaustion then acts as a metonym for depression.

Sixth, the nomological networks of burnout's exhaustion core and depression parallel each other. Research has, inter alia, identified parallels in burnout/exhaustion's and depression's relationship to the following: cognitive style [e.g., attentional, interpretational, and memory biases, rumination, pessimistic attributions; (4143)]; anxiety, workplace support, and exposure to nonwork stressful life events and job-related adversity (20, 27); and job satisfaction, illegitimate work tasks, and work-nonwork interference (44).

In this disquisition on burnout and depression, we do not advance the view that the burnout construct is identical to the construct of work-related depression. Extensive findings show that burnout's exhaustion core and fatigue-related symptoms are important symptoms of work-related depression, but not the only symptoms of work-related depression. The burnout construct captures a depressive phenomenon, but in a truncated manner.

Some depressed workers may represent themselves as “burned out” to reduce stigma. However, the sparse comparative literature on the issue suggests that both conditions are similarly stigmatizing (45). Moreover, the stigma recruiters attach to burnout is a barrier to employment and promotion (46).

We summarize the reasons for identifying burnout symptoms as depressive symptoms. First, job stressors elicit both burnout and depressive symptoms. Second, burnout's core dimension, exhaustion, correlates highly with depressive symptoms. Third, exhaustion correlates more highly with depressive symptoms than with burnout's other putative burnout dimensions, DP and rPA. Fourth, as burnout symptoms increase, the risk of a depressive diagnosis increases. Fifth, often with depression, the presenting complaint is exhaustion, the core symptom of burnout. Sixth, the nomological networks of burnout's exhaustion core and depression closely parallel each other.

Finally, we assert that the symptoms burnout scales assess are some of the symptoms that characterize depression, but not all the symptoms that characterize depression. Kasl (47) already observed that a famous burnout scale that “reflects exhaustion (physical, mental, and emotional) … can be seen as a major component of depression” (p. 396). Burnout scales miss important depressive symptoms, such as suicidal thoughts. We, next, report on a new instrument that better fills the role that burnout scales occupy in identifying workers adversely affected by their jobs.

The Occupational Depression Inventory

Depression contributes heavily to the worldwide burden of disease (48). Unresolvable stress is a primary depressogenic factor. Intractable stressors are often encountered at work. In view of the problem of job-related stressors evoking depressive symptoms in workers, we recently developed the Occupational Depression Inventory [ODI; (14, 49, 50)]. The ODI references the nine symptoms of major depression (25) and incorporates causal attributions to work. From a psychometric and structural standpoint, the ODI is stronger than burnout scales (14, 49, 50). In addition, the ODI assesses crucial symptoms such as suicidal ideation, a risk factor for work-related suicide. The construct of depression, which is deeply anchored in the ODI, has been and continues to be highly researched. A diagnosis of depression has consensual criteria, such as the widely employed criteria in the DSM-5, which contrasts sharply with Rotenstein et al.'s (34) finding that researchers have employed 142 different unique categorizations of burnout.

In contrast to other depressive symptom scales (e.g., the CES-D), the ODI explicitly asks respondents whether they attribute each depressive symptom to their job (e.g., “My experience at work made me feel like a failure”). The instrument has a protocol to help rule out symptoms attributed to nonwork sources (e.g., a conflictual spousal relationship) or a source the respondent cannot identify. The ODI can be used in two different ways. First, it can quantify work-related depressive symptoms along a continuum of severity. Second, the instrument produces provisional diagnoses of work-related depression. The ODI is currently available in English, French, and Spanish (see the Supplemental Material).

Because the ODI can help identify workers with clinically significant job-related distress, occupational health specialists (e.g., physicians, psychologists) can direct affected workers to treatment. We also recommend that specialists investigate workplace stressors that contribute to distress and take steps to ameliorate the stressful working conditions. The ODI can also help epidemiologists estimate the prevalence of job-related depression in organizations and occupational sectors. Because the instrument is brief, it reduces the burden on the respondent completing it. Unlike the MBI, the ODI is available at no cost.

Compared to the ODI, MBI items manifest redundancy (e.g., “Working with people all day is really a strain for me;” “Working with people directly puts too much stress on me)”, artificially inflating the instrument's reliability. The ODI does not exhibit this limitation because each of its nine items focuses on a specific DSM-5 symptom of major depression. Item wording shows no explicit redundancy—yet the ODI has optimal reliability.

Because the ODI deliberately assesses symptoms the individual attributes to perceived job stress, we recommend against using the instrument in cross-sectional research linking the ODI to perceived job-stressor measures. We recommend research that employs longitudinal designs or that links objective measures of job adversity to the ODI.

We created the ODI for two practical purposes: (1) to help occupational health specialists identify suffering workers and take the appropriate steps to remediate the problem and (2) to aid epidemiologists in estimating the prevalence of job-related depression such that appropriate public health policies can be established. A corollary to those two practical purposes is that the ODI can help specialists identify depressogenic organizations. For example, if within a given economic sector, the prevalence of occupational depression is, say, 2.5% but in one organization the prevalence is 25%, problematic working conditions are likely to plague that organization. The ODI would be the sentinel that signals occupational health specialists to take action to help troubled workers and improve workplaces.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

ISS wrote the initial draft of the paper. RB edited the draft and added to it. The draft went back and forth between ISS and RB several times during which the two authors iteratively shaped the paper and settled on its final form. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2021.796401/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Madsen IEH, Nyberg ST, Magnusson Hanson LL, Ferrie JE, Ahola K, Alfredsson L, et al. Job strain as a risk factor for clinical depression: systematic review and meta-analysis with additional individual participant data. Psychol Med. (2017) 47:1342–56. doi: 10.1017/S003329171600355X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Schonfeld IS, Chang C-H. Occupational Health Psychology: Work, Stress, and Health. New York: Springer Publishing Company (2017). doi: 10.1891/9780826199683

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Theorell T, Hammarström A, Aronsson G, Träskman Bendz L, Grape T, Hogstedt C, et al. A systematic review including meta-analysis of work environment and depressive symptoms. BMC Public Health. (2015) 15. doi: 10.1186/s12889-015-1954-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Howard MC, Follmer K B, Smith MB, Tucker RP. Work and suicide: An interdisciplinary systematic literature review. Journal of Organizational Behavior. (2021). doi: 10.1002/job.2519. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Maslach C, Schaufeli WB. Job burnout. Annu Rev Psychol. (2001) 52:397–422. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Maslach C, Jackson SE, Leiter MP. Maslach Burnout Inventory Manual. 4th ed. Menlo Park, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press (2016).

Google Scholar

7. Maslach C, Leiter M. Understanding the burnout experience: recent research and its implications for psychiatry. World Psychiatry. (2016) 15:103–11. doi: 10.1002/wps.20311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Demerouti E, Bakker AB, Nachreiner F. The job demands-resources model of burnout. J Appl Psychol. (2001) 86:499–512. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.499

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Hakanen JJ, Schaufeli WB. The job demands-resources model: a three-year cross-lagged study of burnout, depression, commitment, and work engagement. Work Stress. (2008) 22:224–41. doi: 10.1080/02678370802379432

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Kasl SV. Epidemiological contributions to the study of work stress. In: Cooper CL, Payne RL, Editors. Stress At Work. Chichester, United Kingdom: Wiley (1978). p. 3–38.

Google Scholar

11. Bianchi R, Verkuilen J, Schonfeld IS, Hakanen JJ, Jansson-Fröjmark M, Manzano-García G, et al. Is burnout a depressive condition? a 14-sample transnational and multi-occupational study. Clin Psychol Sci. (2021) 9:579–97. doi: 10.1177/2167702620979597

CrossRef Full Text

12. Haslam N, Holland E. Categories versus dimensions in personality and psychopathology: A quantitative review of taxometric research. Psychol Med. (2012) 42:903–20. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711001966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Wichers M. The dynamic nature of depression: a new micro-level perspective of mental disorder that meets current challenges. Psychol Med. (2014) 44:1349–60. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Bianchi R, Schonfeld IS. The occupational depression inventory: a new tool for clinicians and epidemiologists. J Psychosom Res. (2020) 138:110249. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2020.110249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Pryce CR, Azzinnari D, Spinelli S, Seifritz E, Tegethoff M, Meinlschmidt G. Helplessness: a systematic translational review of theory and evidence for its relevance to understanding and treating depression. Pharm Therap. (2011) 132:242–67. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2011.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Willner P, Scheel-Krüger J, Belzung C. The neurobiology of depression and antidepressant action. Neuroscien Biobehav Rev. (2013) 37:2331–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Melchior M, Caspi A, Milne BJ, Danese A, Poulton R. Work stress precipitates depression and anxiety in young, working women and men. Psychol Med. (2007) 37:1119–29. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Stansfeld S. Psychosocial work environment and mental health—a meta-analytic review. Scand J Work Environ Health. (2006) 32:443–62. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.1050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Ahola K, Hakanen J, Perhoniemi R, Mutanen P. Relationship between burnout and depressive symptoms: a study using the person-centred approach. Burnout Research. (2014) 1:29–37. doi: 10.1016/j.burn.2014.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Schonfeld IS, Bianchi R. Burnout and depression: two entities or one. J Clin Psychol. (2016) 72:22–37. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Koutsimani P, Montgomery A. The relationship between burnout, depression, and anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:284. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Meier ST. Meta-regression analyses of relationships between burnout and depression with sampling and measurement methodological moderators. J Occup Health Psychol. (2021). doi: 10.1037/ocp0000273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Schonfeld IS, Verkuilen J, Bianchi R. Inquiry into the correlation between burnout and depression. J Occup Health Psychol. (2019) 24:603–16. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Wurm W, Vogel K, Holl A, Ebner C, Bayer D, Mörkl S, et al. Depression-burnout overlap in physicians. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0149913. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.journals.9780890425596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Bianchi R, Schonfeld IS, Verkuilen J. A five-sample confirmatory factor analytic study of burnout-depression overlap. J Clin Psychol. (2020) 76:801–21. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Schonfeld IS, Verkuilen J, Bianchi R. An exploratory structural equation modeling bi-factor analytic approach to uncovering what burnout, depression, and anxiety scales measure. Psychol Assess. (2019) 31:1073–9. doi: 10.1037/pas0000721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Verkuilen J, Bianchi R, Schonfeld IS. Burnout-depression overlap: exploratory structural equation modeling bifactor analysis and network analysis. Assessment. (2021) 28:1583–600. doi: 10.1177./1073191120911095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Dohrenwend BP, Shrout PE, Egri G. Nonspecific psychological distress and other dimensions of psychopathology. Measures for use in the general population. Archives of General Psychiatry. (1980) 37:1229–236. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1980.01780240027003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Goldman-Mellor SJ, Harrington H, Israel S, et al. The P factor: one general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science. (2014) 2:119–37. doi: 10.1177/2167702613497473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Kotov R, Krueger RF, Watson D, Achenbach TM, Althoff RR, Bagby RM, et al. The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): a dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. J Abnorm Psychol. (2017) 126:454–77. doi: 10.1037/abn0000258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Schaufeli WB, Taris TW. The conceptualization and measurement of burnout: common ground and worlds apart. Work and Stress. (2005) 19:256–62. doi: 10.1080/02678370500385913

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Taris TW, Le Blanc PM, Schaufeli WB. Are there causal relationships between the dimensions of the maslach burnout inventory? a review and two longitudinal tests. Work and Stress. (2005) 19:238–55. doi: 10.1080/02678370500270453

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Rotenstein LS, Torre M, Ramos MA, Rosales RC, Guille C, Sen S. Prevalence of burnout among physicians: a systematic review. JAMA. (2018) 320:1131–50. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.12777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Bianchi R, Schonfeld IS. Burnout: Moving beyond the status quo. Int J Stress Manag. (2019) 26:36–45. doi: 10.1037/str0000088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Schonfeld IS, Bianchi R. Psychiatrist burnout. Am J Psychiatry. (2021) 178:204. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20071110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Bianchi R, Schonfeld IS, Laurent E. Is burnout a depressive disorder? a reexamination with special focus on atypical depression. Int J Stress Manag. (2014) 21:307–24. doi: 10.1037/a0037906

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Robins E, Guze S. Establishment of diagnostic validity in psychiatric illness: its application to schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. (1970) 126:983–7. doi: 10.1176/ajp.126.7.983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Ahola K, Honkonen T, Isometsä E, Kalimo R, Nykyri E, Aromaa A. The relationship between job-related burnout and depressive disorders–results from the finnish health 2000 study. J Affect Disord. (2005). 88:55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Freudenberger HJ. Staff burnout. Journal of Social Issues. (1974) 30:159–65.

Google Scholar

41. Bianchi R, Laurent E, Schonfeld IS, Verkuilen J, Berna C. Interpretation bias toward ambiguous information in burnout and depression. Pers Individ Differ. (2018) 135:216–21. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.07.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Bianchi R, Laurent E, Schonfeld IS, Bietti LM. Memory bias toward emotional information in burnout and depression. J Health Psychol. (2020) 25:1567–75. doi: 10.1177/1359105318765621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Bianchi R. Burnout is associated with a depressive cognitive style. Pers Individ Differ. (2016) 100:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Bianchi R, Schonfeld IS. Burnout-depression overlap: nomological network examination and factor-analytic approach. Scand J Psychol. (2018) 59:532–9. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Bianchi R, Verkuilen J, Brisson R, Schonfeld IS. Burnout and depression: Label-related stigma, help-seeking, and syndrome overlap. Psychiatry Res. (2016) 24:591–8. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Sterkens P, Baert S, Rooman C. As if it weren't hard enough already: Breaking down hiring discrimination following burnout. Econ Hum Biol. (2021) 43. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2021.101050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Kasl SV. Measuring job stressors and studying the health impact of the work environment: an epidemiolgic commentary. J Occup Heath Psychol. (1998) 3:390–401. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.3.4.390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. World Health Organization. Depression and Other Common Mental Disorders: Global Health Estimates. Geneva: Author (2017).

Google Scholar

49. Bianchi R, Schonfeld IS. The occupational depression inventory—a solution for estimating the prevalence of job-related distress. Psychiatry Res. (2021) 305:114181. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2021.114181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Bianchi R, Schonfeld IS. Is the occupational depression inventory predictive of cognitive performance? a focus on inhibitory control and effortful reasoning. Pers Individ Dif. (2022) 184:111213. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: depression, anxiety, occupational depression inventory, work stress and burnout, job-related distress, occupational health science, Maslach Burnout Inventory

Citation: Schonfeld IS and Bianchi R (2021) From Burnout to Occupational Depression: Recent Developments in Research on Job-Related Distress and Occupational Health. Front. Public Health 9:796401. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.796401

Received: 16 October 2021; Accepted: 12 November 2021;
Published: 10 December 2021.

Edited by:

Wulf Rössler, Charité University Medicine Berlin, Germany

Reviewed by:

Yuan-Pang Wang, University of São Paulo, Brazil
Francesco Chirico, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Schonfeld and Bianchi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Irvin Sam Schonfeld, ischonfeld@ccny.cuny.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.