Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 30 August 2021
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Coping with Pandemic and Infodemic Stress: a Multidisciplinary Perspective View all 32 articles

Infodemic vs. Pandemic Factors Associated to Public Anxiety in the Early Stage of the COVID-19 Outbreak: A Cross-Sectional Study in China

\nJian Xu,,Jian Xu1,2,3Cong Liu,
Cong Liu1,3*
  • 1School of Media and Communication, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai, China
  • 2China Institute for Urban Governance, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai, China
  • 3Institute of Cultural Innovation and Youth Development, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai, China

Introduction: Every outbreak of an epidemic or pandemic disease is accompanied by the tsunami of information, which is also known as the infodemic. Infodemic makes it hard for people to find trustworthy sources and reliable guidance when they need it, and causes social panic about health, widens the gaps between races and regions, and even brings the social chaos all over the world. While most researchers and related parties made efforts to control the inaccurate information spreading online during the COVID-19 pandemic, the infodemic influence caused by the overload of accurate information were almost or completely ignored, and this will hinder the control of infodemic in future public health crises. This study aims to explore the infodemic vs. pandemic influence on people's psychological anxiety across different media sources in the early stage of the COVID-19 outbreak in China.

Methods: A cross-sectional study using online survey method was conducted by a data-collection service provider in April 2020. A total of 1,117 valid samples were finally collected from 5,203 randomly invited members via webpages and WeChat. The sample distribution covered the 30 provincial administrative divisions of mainland China.

Results: Hierarchical regression analysis for the potential pandemic sources and infodemic sources of psychological anxiety showed that the infodemic factors of attention to the coronavirus information (β = 0.154, p < 0.001) and commercial media exposure (β = 0.147, p < 0.001) is positively related to the level of anxiety. Statistics indicated that influence of the infodemic factors is over and above that of the pandemic factors (ΔR2 = 0.054, F = 14.199, and p < 0.001). Mediation analysis showed that information overload (B = 0.155, Boot SE = 0.022, and 95% Boot CI [0.112, 0.198]) mediates the link between attention to coronavirus information and anxiety; both information overload (B = 0.035, Boot SE = 0.014, and 95% Boot CI [0.009, 0.062]) and media vicarious traumatization (B = 0.106, Boot SE = 0.017, and 95% Boot CI [0.072, 0.140]) mediate the link between commercial media exposure and anxiety.

Conclusion: This study suggested that the influence of infodemic with mixed accurate and inaccurate information on public anxiety does exist, which could possibly go beyond that of the pandemic. Information overload and vicarious traumatization explain how infodemic may be associated to public anxiety. Finally, commercial media could be a major source of infodemic in the Chinese media context. Implications for the related parties were discussed.

Introduction

Background

At the beginning of 2020, the whole world fell into an emergent public health crisis brought by the COVID-19 pandemic. Accompanied with the outbreak of the COVID-19 was the tsunami of the disease-related information, which is known as the infodemic (1). In this crisis, media plays an important role in people's information acquisition. All media outlets were unprecedentedly active, reaping countless high searching, reading, and forwarding volumes. However, some media outlets have also been criticized for providing false information, inflammatory views, and even unethical content. Although the pandemic has been under well-control in some countries, a series of social, psychological, and ethical issues brought about by the infodemic still worth to be reconsidered.

Infodemic is a phenomenon described as an over-abundance of information—some accurate and some not—that makes it hard for people to find trustworthy sources and reliable guidance when they need it (2). The World Health Organization declared that besides the pandemic threat originated by the COVID-19 virus, an infodemic has been generated by a large amount of information available on the matter, as well as by the difficulty to sort the veracious information from the false (2). Although the outbreak of SARS in 2003, H1N1 in 2009, and MERS in 2012 were all accompanied by rumors and false information, creating different degrees of social panic, the COVID-19 pandemic has developed unprecedented trend of infodemic with the emerging information technology, which was defined as “the first true social-media infodemic” (3). Social media is considered a powerful tool for sharing health information related to pandemic risks (4, 5). After the COVID-19 outbreak, 70–80% of the Chinese users reported an increase in the use of WeChat (6, 7).

Infodemic, including dissemination of conflicting or unclear messages, misinformation, rumors, and conspiracy theories, can profoundly cause public anxiety and social panic, affect public health communication, diminish preventive measures, impede effective crisis management, widen the gaps between races and regions, and even bring the social chaos all over the world (812). The heightened distress by the infodemic can also cause individuals' irrational behaviors in the crisis, such as health information avoidance, spread of misinformation, overuse of the healthcare services, panic purchases, incompliance with preventive measures (such as physical distancing, mask wearing, and vaccination) (1317). The World Health Organization, the United Nations, the United States Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, and many other organizations had all engaged in fighting against the infodemic by debunking of false information, stopping the spread of rumors, providing the population with reliable data and updated news about COVID-19 (1820). Technology industries including Google, Amazon, Facebook, YouTube, Microsoft, and Twitter also implemented restrictions on publishing pandemic-related content and removed medically disproved claims (2124).

Interesting enough, existing studies seem suggest that online users have an adequate e-health literacy and can effectively filter the false information in emergent public health crises. Studies showed that users can critically evaluate the source of the health information received and are capable to discriminate between reliable and unreliable content, and they place higher trust in the medical professionals and scientists than the mass media and social media, and they also rated the authorities' social media channels as more trustworthy than the user-generated content (25, 26). Big data analysis also showed that information from questionable sources or false information posted on social media only represents a small fraction compared to the reliable or science-based ones (27). Researchers claimed that there is a higher potential of true information to capture more user engagement (28). True information was also found to circulate more, reach a higher level of diffusion, spread more quickly, and have a longer lifetime than false information (29, 30). These suggest that false information dissemination may not be the only cause of infodemic and public anxiety.

Infodemic vs. Pandemic Factors of Public Anxiety

Previous studies have shown that people generally have varying degrees of anxiety in the context of public health emergencies. Uncertainty situations make people more vulnerable to mental and psychological distress. In the early stage of the COVID-19 outbreak, especially in China, people were exposed to unknown threats, and highly uncertain about the infectivity, susceptibility, and treatment methods of the COVID-19. A study showed that more than half of the Chinese participants suffered from psychological distress, anxiety, depression, and stress at moderate to severe levels (31). Perceived risk of infection is one of the most direct factors that cause individual anxiety and fear. Studies have revealed that perceived risk of COVID-19 significantly associated with anxiety-related feelings such as sleeping disorder, stress, worry, and disruption of daily life (3234).

Besides the pandemic factors, public anxiety levels are also largely influenced by the infodemic factors, especially in the highly developed information technology era. Researchers found that the excessive social media use leads to increased levels of stress, anxiety, and vicarious trauma (35). An online survey indicated a positive link between information exposure during the COVID-19 pandemic and the occurrence of anxiety and insomnia symptoms, and the strength of the association increases with the duration of the media exposure (12, 36). Major information sources of the COVID-19 pandemic not only involve social media, but also traditional media (12). Experts and scholars criticized that social media as well as traditional mass media were disseminating inaccurate information during the COVID-19 pandemic, and even most extreme pictures found elsewhere sending the wrong message were being used in many mainstream newspapers and TV reports (9).

Information Overload

Information overload represents a state in which an individual's efficiency in using information in their work is hampered by the amount of relevant, and potentially useful, information available to them. The feeling of overload is usually associated with a loss of control over the situation, and sometimes with feelings of being overwhelmed (37). Information is a double-edged sword in the COVID-19 pandemic. On the one hand, effective communication of facts helps people to obtain adequate risk perceptions and make adaptive health behavior, while on the other hand, overloaded information can also impose strains on crisis management (26, 38, 39). Studies showed that as people are intensively exposed to negative information about a crisis, their levels of anxiety and other unpleasant emotions could be triggered and elevated for an extended period (4043), especially when their personal experience with the disease is limited (25, 44, 45).

Media Vicarious Traumatization

Vicarious trauma describes the trauma experiences people have after being exposed to others' trauma stories and having witnessed the pain, fear, and terror that traumatized survivors have endured (46, 47). Media could be another source of vicarious trauma (48), when audiences indirectly experience the traumatic events via the vividly presented videos, pictures, and texts exposed by the media. Studies showed that obtaining more informational support via social media increased users' vicarious trauma levels (35). When the information and media content are perceived as threatening, aversive emotions can be elicited (4952), and when information is contradictory or uncertain, the distress may be even more elevated (53, 54). Extensive research indicated that consuming media coverage to the natural or humanmade disasters typically associates with increased incidences of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), anxiety, and depression (5558).

Goal of This Study

Most researchers and related organizations engaged in dealing with the control of false information dissemination online (such as misinformation, fake news, rumors, conspiracy theories) as well as their negative influence on public mental health and health related behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic. However, a key question remained was that “is the false information spreading the only cause of public anxiety?” A critical part was almost or completely ignored in the existing studies and countermeasures, which is, the infodemic caused by the over-abundance of the mixed inaccurate and accurate information disseminated by the social media, mass media, and even government official media.

This study will focus on the influence of infodemic across different media sources on people's anxiety in the early stage of the COVID-19 outbreak in China. It aims to answer the following three research questions:

(a) will the infodemic factors vs. pandemic factors significantly associated with people's anxiety?

(b) what are the underlying mechanisms of the impact of infodemic on people's anxiety; that is, how information overload and media vicarious traumatization mediate the impact?

(c) what are the roles played by the three main information sources in the Chinese media context (i.e., government official media, commercial media, and social media) in the infodemic?

Methods

Recruitment

The data were collected online by a sample service provider (i.e., Changsha Ranxing IT Ltd.), who owns one of the biggest online sample with more than 2.6 million members all over China. The survey was conducted by randomly inviting 5,203 members from the 30 provinces of mainland China via webpages and WeChat in April, 2020. A total of 1,342 members responded to the invitation. Among them, 225 invalid responses were systematically or manually eliminated by the sample service provider, and the final valid responses received were 1,117 with a response rate of 21.5%. Cities mostly influenced by the COVID-19 in the early stage of the outbreak were all covered, and the regional distribution of the samples were as follows: Wuhan (9%) and other cities (14%) of Hubei Province; Guagnzhou (6%) and Shenzhen (6%) of Guangdong Province; Wenzhou (7%) of Zhejiang Province; Beijing (7%); Shanghai (6%); Chongqing (7%); and other cities of the other 24 provinces (38%). Participation of the survey was anonymous and voluntary.

Ethical Consideration

The protocol of this study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Shanghai Jiao Tong University (No: H2020038I). The data were treated with confidentiality and the results did not identify the participants personally.

Participants

Less than half of the participants are male (45.9%) and 54.1% are female. A majority age between 18 and 40 years old (85.5%). Almost all of them are in good health condition (98%). During the pandemic, 23.0% of the participants stayed in Hubei Province, and 77% stayed in other provinces; most of the participants stayed with family members or friends (97.7%), and only 2.3% stayed alone. Detailed participant characteristics are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the participants.

Measurements

Psychological Anxiety

The psychological anxiety questionnaire was adapted from Zung's Self-Rating Anxiety Scale (SAS) (59). Participants were asked to rate their level of anxiety in the early stage of the COVID-19 outbreak on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = completely disagree, 5 = completely agree) with three items, including “I feel nervous and anxious due to the coronavirus pandemic,” “I have sleeping problems during the coronavirus pandemic,” and “I feel panicky and cannot sit still easily during the coronavirus pandemic.” Higher scores indicate higher levels of psychological anxiety. Cronbach's alpha of this questionnaire is 0.83. Correlations between items 1 through 3 with the total score are 0.85 (p < 0.01), 0.88 (p < 0.01), and 0.87 (p < 0.01), respectively.

Pandemic Factors

Participants were asked to rate their perceptions to the COVID-19 pandemic in the early stage of the outbreak. Four indicators were adopted from the widely used measurements for the Health Belief Model variables (60, 61): (a) perceived risk of oneself getting infected by coronavirus (from 0 to 100%), (b) perceived risk of people around getting inflected by coronavirus (from 0 to 100%), (c) worry about oneself getting infected by coronavirus (1 = not at all, 5 = very much), and (d) worry about people around getting infected by coronavirus (1 = not at all, 5 = very much).

Infodemic Factors

Participants were asked to rate their information consumption in the early stage of the COVID-19 outbreak, which includes five indicators: attention to coronavirus information and attention to information irrelevant to the coronavirus on 5-point Likert scales (1 = hardly ever, 2 = less than an hour, 3 = 1–3 h, 4 = 3–5 h, 5 = more than 5 h); exposure to different media sources including the government official media (e.g., CCTV, People's Daily, Hubei Daily), commercial media (e.g., The Paper, Sanlian Life Week, Caixin), and social media (e.g., WeChat, Weibo, TikTok) on 5-point Likert scales (1 = never, 5 = often).

Information Overload

Information overload was measured by the questions adapted from Zhang and colleagues' Information Overload Questionnaire on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = completely disagree, 5 = completely agree) including five items, for example, “I find that only a small part of the coronavirus information is relevant to my needs,” “I find that I am overwhelmed by the amount of coronavirus information I have to process on a daily basis,” and “There is too much information so I find it a burden to handle” (62). Cronbach's alpha of this questionnaire is 0.76.

Media Vicarious Traumatization

Media vicarious traumatization was measured by the questions adapted from the Vrklevski's Vicarious Traumatization Scale (VTS) on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = completely disagree, 5 = completely agree) with seven items, including “I was exposed to distressing news and experiences about coronavirus via media,” “It is hard to stay positive and optimistic given some of the coronavirus information I get from the media,” and “I find myself thinking about distressing coronavirus news on media” (63). Cronbach's alpha of this questionnaire is 0.78.

Statistical Analysis

All statistical analysis were performed using SPSS statistics (v25, IBM, USA). Descriptive analysis concerning the minimums, maximums, means, and standard deviations of the main variables were reported. To examine the influences of the pandemic and infodemic factors on anxiety, a hierarchical regression analysis was conducted including the control variables (i.e., gender, age, health condition, accommodation, and place of residence), the pandemic factors (i.e., risk of oneself, risk of people around, worry about oneself, worry about people around), and the infodemic factors (i.e., attention to coronavirus information, attention to coronavirus-irrelevant information, government official media exposure, commercial media exposure, and social media exposure) in three blocks, respectively. Improvements in model fit was indicated by the R2 change in each block. To analyze the underlying mechanisms of the influence of the infodemic factors, mediational analyses using Process Macro model 4 (64) were conducted. Direct and indirect effects were reported with their 95% confidence intervals.

Results

Descriptives

Descriptives of the statistics are shown in Table 2. Psychological anxiety of the participants in the early stage of the pandemic is relatively low with the mean score of 2.83 out of 5. Among the pandemic factors, the average perceived risk of oneself getting infected is 41.67%, and the average perceived risk of people around getting infected is 44.25%. The mean score of worry about oneself getting infected is 3.49, and the mean score of worry about people around getting infected is 3.38. Among the infodemic factors, the mean score of attention to coronavirus information is 3.02, and the mean score of attention to coronavirus-irrelevant information is 2.87. Media sources exposed to the participants from the most frequent to the least frequent are social media (mean score is 4.24), government official media (mean score is 4.01), and commercial media (mean score is 2.73). The mean score of information overload is 2.97. The mean score of media vicarious traumatization is 3.27.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptives of the main variables.

Regression Analysis for the Pandemic and Infodemic Factors of Anxiety

A hierarchical regression was conducted to analyze the pandemic and infodemic factors on psychological anxiety, with the control variables entered to the first block, the pandemic factors entered to the second block, and the infodemic factors entered to the third block. Results (see Table 3) showed that among the control variables, age (β = 0.095, p < 0.01), health condition (β = −0.148, p < 0.001), and accommodation (β = 0.075, p < 0.05) are significantly correlated to anxiety. In specific, participants who are older, in poorer health condition, or staying alone are more anxious than their counterparts. Gender and place of residence are not correlated to anxiety. Variance explained by the control variables (ΔR2) is 0.043 (F = 9.870, p < 0.001). Among the pandemic factors, perceived risk of oneself getting infected (β = 0.094, p < 0.01) and worry about oneself getting infected (β = 0.180, p < 0.001) are positively correlated to anxiety after controlling for the effects of the control variables, while perceived risk of people around and worry about people around getting infected are not significantly correlated to anxiety. Variance uniquely explained by the pandemic factors (ΔR2) is 0.062 (F = 19.073, p < 0.001). Among the infodemic factors, attention to the coronavirus information (β =0.154, p < 0.001) and commercial media exposure (β = 0.147, p < 0.001) are positively related to anxiety after controlling for the effects of the control variables and the pandemic factors, while attention to coronavirus-irrelevant information, government official media exposure, and social media exposure are not significantly related to anxiety.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Herarchical regression for the pandemic and infodemic factors of anxiety.

The hierarchical regression analysis showed that variance uniquely explained by the infodemic factors (ΔR2) is 0.054 (F = 14.199, p < 0.001). It also indicated a unique contribution of the infodemic factors on anxiety over and above that of the pandemic factors. In other words, statistics supports that the influence of the infodemic factors are beyond that of the pandemic factors in increasing psychological anxiety.

Mediational Analysis of Information Overload and Media Vicarious Traumatization

Since the above analysis indicates that attention to coronavirus information and commercial media exposure are the two key infodemic factors, the underlying mechanisms of these two factors was further explored with mediational analyses.

A mediational analysis was firstly conducted for the effect of attention to coronavirus information on anxiety (see Figure 1). Results showed that when information overload is treated as the mediator, the mediation effect is not significant (B = 0.025, Boot SE = 0.018, and 95% Boot CI [−0.011, 0.059]), and there is only a direct effect of attention of coronavirus information on anxiety (B = 0.232, SE = 0.030, and 95% CI [0.174, 0.290]). When media vicarious traumatization is treated as the mediator, the mediation effect is significant (B = 0.155, Boot SE = 0.022, and 95% Boot CI [0.112, 0.198]), and the direct effect of attention of coronavirus information on anxiety is also significant (B = 0.102, SE = 0.028, and 95% CI [0.047, 0.157]). That is, the effect of attention to coronavirus information on anxiety is mediated by media vicarious traumatization.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mediational analysis for the effect of attention to coronavirus information on anxiety. B, unstandardized regression coefficient; ***p < 0.001; ns, nonsignificant.

A mediational analysis was secondly conducted for the effect of commercial media exposure on anxiety (see Figure 2). Results showed that when information overload is treated as the mediator, the mediation effect is significant (B = 0.035, Boot SE = 0.014, and 95% Boot CI [0.009, 0.062]), and the direct effect of commercial media exposure on anxiety is also significant (B = 0.114, SE = 0.022, and 95% CI [0.071, 0.157]). When media vicarious traumatization is treated as the mediator, the mediation effect is significant (B = 0.106, Boot SE = 0.017, and 95% Boot CI [0.072, 0.140]), and the direct effect of commercial media exposure on anxiety is also significant (B = 0.042, SE = 0.020, and 95% CI [0.003, 0.082]). That is, the effect of commercial media exposure on anxiety is mediated by both information overload and media vicarious traumatization.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mediational analysis for the effect of commercial media exposure on anxiety. B, unstandardized regression coefficient; ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, and *p < 0.05; ns, nonsignificant.

Discussion

This study focuses on the infodemic vs. pandemic influence on people's anxiety across different media sources in the early stage of the COVID-19 outbreak among the Chinese participants. In specific, it (a) explored the influence of infodemic vs. pandemic on people's anxiety, (b) explored the mediation effect of information overload and media vicarious traumatization, and (c) compared the differences in the roles of government official media, commercial media, and social media. Findings showed that pandemic factors of perceived risk of oneself getting infected and worry about oneself getting infected are positively related to the level of anxiety; infodemic factors of attention to coronavirus information and commercial media exposure are positively related to the level of anxiety; government official media exposure, social media exposure, and attention to coronavirus-irrelevant information were found to be the insignificant infodemic factors. More importantly, statistics also indicated that influence of the infodemic factors is beyond that of the pandemic factors. Mediation analysis testing the underlying mechanisms of the infodemic influence showed that vicarious traumatization mediates the effect of attention to coronavirus information on anxiety; both information overload and media vicarious traumatization mediate the effect of commercial media exposure on anxiety.

The findings first suggest that the infodemic influence on people's anxiety with mixed accurate and inaccurate information does exist, which could possibly be more profound than that of the pandemic itself. During an emergent public health crisis, people are more inclined to acquire information in order to alleviate the sense of uncertainty (65), however, findings suggest that paying too much attention to the crisis information and being intensively exposed to certain types of media content about the crisis may exacerbate the anxious and stressful feelings. Findings of this study also indicated that even distractions from the coronavirus-irrelevant information, including entertainment, games, and daily news, do not effectively alleviate the anxiety. Thus, this further supports experts' opinions that infodemic is not only caused by the spreading of false information or rumors, accurate information routinely spread by different media outlets could also become potential sources of infodemic. On the one hand, it is important to increase the speed and width of spreading of information and scientific evidence from trustworthy sources, such as the public health officials, medical professionals, scientists, verified social media accounts, official reports, etc. The most crucial and official information should be communicated by these credible groups, in order to effectively lower the emotional taxing of the crisis (25). On the other hand, active citizenship against the spread of false information should also be advocated, knowing that users have the potential to be trained to debunk false information through scientific literacy cultivation (28).

Information overload and media vicarious traumatization were found to be the important underlying mechanisms explaining why and how infodemic may be associated with anxiety. When individuals are intensively exposed to the crisis information, it is inevitable to vicariously experience the traumatic contents, which will in turn, increase their level of anxiety. This problem is more salient in the case of commercial media exposure compared to the government official media exposure and social media exposure. Commercial media may not only trigger the distressed perception by the overloaded amounts of reports, but also bring about vicarious traumatization. During the pandemic in China, commercial media circulated vast amount of coronavirus information intensively, and such information were further pushed to their users continuously with the utilization of artificial intelligence based on algorithms and historical data. Thus, consumers of commercial media may passively receive overloaded coronavirus information that probably carries traumatic contents. While researchers have proposed the empathic style of communication and personal experience sharing as the infodemic countermeasure (66), our study suggested that such style could be inappropriate considering the vicarious traumatization effect of the media coverage. On the contrary, media should convey information to the public without sensationalizing the situation or providing disturbing images and videos so as to prevent bringing emotional trauma to the public.

Comparisons across information sources showed that commercial media could be a major source of infodemic in the Chinese media context. Commercial media coverages could directly and indirectly cause public anxiety by overloaded information output and vicarious traumatization. Such impact exists among the commercial media more obviously than other types of media, and this could be explained by the market-oriented nature and the report genres of the commercial media. Commercial media in China intend to focus on those vivid cases from the microscopic perspective, and their story-telling feature could more easily trigger the traumatic feelings of the audience. In the early stage of the COVID-19 outbreak, the commercial media were inclined to cover the contents such as the situation of the first-line treatment, the patients' and families' stressful experience, and how the Wuhan citizens were expelled in other cities or countries, meanwhile, they tended to focus on the detailed and negative incisions from the patients, family members, and medical staff perspectives. Some typical examples include the article “Mother died in Wuhan isolation ward” released by Phoenix News on 28 January, 2020, the article “Wuhan Community under the pressure of epidemic: After the elderly died of high fever at home” released by Caixin on 29 January, 2020, and the article “When the hotel reception heard that I was from Wuhan, they immediately called the police” released by ThePaper on 28, January, 2020, and these articles went viral in only a few hours. In contrast, the government official media coverages in China are more neutral, macroscopic, and science-based, which mostly covered the authentic data, progress of the pandemic, and the government responses. Social media were usually found to be a major source of false information and rumors during crises in many studies, however, it is important to note that social media not only spread the crisis information, but also play a role in the health information support as well as social and emotional support from family members, friends, and significant others (67, 68). This could be the reason why social media was not found to be a source of infodemic in the current study. Thus, commercial media together with other media outlets should actively mitigate infodemic and public anxiety during public health crises by avoiding overloaded crisis information reports and detailed trauma-related content. Governments could also direct public health policies to address the impact of media portals in their routine spreading of information in times of pandemics rather than merely dealing with false information (66).

Conclusion

This study gives insights for the in-depth understanding of the infodemic impact by analyzing the essential attributes of the infodemic from aspects of definition, information sources, communication mechanisms, and social psychological impact. The research findings provide valuable implications and suggestions for infodemic governance from the perspective of media practitioners, policy makers, and media consumers.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Shanghai Jiao Tong University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Social Science Fund of China (Grant Number 20CXW031).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. United Nations. UN Tackles Infodemic of Misinformation and Cybercrime in COVID-19 Crisis. (2020). Available online at: https://www.un.org/en/un-coronavirus-communications-team/un-tackling-%E2%80%98infodemic%E2%80%99-misinformation-and-cybercrime-covid-19 (accessed May 15, 2020).

2. World Health Organisation. Novel Coronavirus (2019-nCoV) Situation Report 13. (2020). Available online at: https://www.who.int/docs/default-source/coronaviruse/situation-reports/20200202-sitrep-13-ncov-v3.pdf (accessed May 15, 2020).

Google Scholar

3. Hao K, Basu T. The Coronavirus is the First True Social-Media Infodemic. (2020). Available online at: https://www.technologyreview.com/2020/02/12/844851/the-coronavirus-is-the-first-true-social-media-infodemic/ (accessed May 15, 2020).

4. Sharma M, Yadav K, Yadav N, Ferdinand KC. Zika virus pandemic-analysis of Facebook as a social media health information platform. Am J Infect Control. (2017) 45:301–2. doi: 10.1016/j.ajic.2016.08.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Ahmad AR, Murad HR. The impact of social media on panic during the COVID-19 pandemic in Iraqi Kurdistan: online questionnaire study. J Med Internet Res. (2020) 22:e19556. doi: 10.2196/19556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Lau S. How China's Industrial Internet is Fighting COVID-19. World Economic Forum Internet (2020). Available online at: https://www.weforum.org/agenda/2020/04/china-covid-19-digital-response (accessed May 15, 2020).

7. Hua J, Shaw R. Corona virus (Covid-19)“infodemic” and emerging issues through a data lens: the case of china. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:2309. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17072309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Abd-Alrazaq A, Alhuwail D, Househ M, Hamdi M, Shah Z. Top concerns of tweeters during the COVID-19 pandemic: infoveillance study. J Med Internet Res. (2020) 22:e19016. doi: 10.2196/19016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Zarocostas J. How to fight an infodemic. Lancet. (2020) 395:676. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30461-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Shimizu K. 2019-nCoV, fake news, and racism. Lancet. (2020) 395:685–6. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(20)30357-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Chung RY, Li MM. Anti-Chinese sentiment during the 2019-nCoV outbreak. Lancet. (2020) 395:686–687. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(20)30358-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Mongkhon P, Ruengorn C, Awiphan R, Thavorn K, Hutton B, Wongpakaran N, et al. Exposure to COVID-19-related information and its association with mental health problems in Thailand: nationwide, cross-sectional survey study. J Med Internet Res. (2021) 23:e25363. doi: 10.2196/preprints.25363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Jones JH, Salathe M. Early assessment of anxiety and behavioral response to novel swine-origin influenza A (H1N1). PLoS One. (2009) 4:e8032. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Bults M, Beaujean DJMA, de Zwart O, Kok G, van Empelen P, van Steenbergen JE, et al. Perceived risk, anxiety, and behavioural responses of the general public during the early phase of the Influenza A (H1N1) pandemic in the Netherlands: results of three consecutive online surveys. BMC Public Health. (2011) 11:2. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

15. Cheng CC, Lam TH, Cheng KK. Mass masking in the COVID-19 epidemic: people need guidance. Lancet. (2020) 395:945. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30547-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Holmes EA, Connor RCO, Perry VH, Tracey I, Wessly S, Arseneault L, et al. Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic: a call for action for mental health science. Lancet Psychiatry. (2020) 7:547–60. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30168-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Li HO, Bailey A, Huynh D, Chan J. YouTube as a source of information on COVID-19: a pandemic of misinformation? BMJ Glob Health. (2020) 5:e002604. doi: 10.1136/bmjgh-2020-002604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. World Health Organization. Novel Coronavirus (2019-nCoV) Advice for the Public: Myth Busters. (2020). Available online at: https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019/advicefor-public/myth-busters (accessed May 15, 2020).

19. United Nations. UN Tackles ‘Infodemic’ of Misinformation and Cybercrime in COVID-19 Crisis. (2020). Available online at: https://www.un.org/en/un-coronavirus-communications-team/un-tackling-%E2%80%98infodemic%E2%80%99-misinformation-and-cybercrime-covid-19 (accessed May 15, 2020).

20. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Stop the Spread of Rumors. (2020). Available online at: https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/daily-life-coping/share-facts.html (accessed May 15, 2020).

21. Scott M. Facebook's Private Groups are Abuzz With Coronavirus Fake News. (2020). Available online at: https://www.politico.eu/article/facebook-misinformation-fake-news-coronavirus-covid19/ (accessed May 15, 2020).

22. Valinsky J. Amazon Deleted 1 Million Items for Price Gouging or False Advertising About Coronavirus. (2020). Available online at: https://www.cnn.com/2020/03/02/tech/amazon-coronavirus-products/index.html (accessed May 15, 2020).

23. Porter T. Twitter Deleted a Tweet by Rudy Giuliani for Spreading Coronavirus Misinformation. (2020). Available online at: https://www.businessinsider.com/coronavirus-twitter-deletes-giuliani-tweet-forspreading-misinformation-2020-3 (accessed May 15, 2020).

24. Warzel C. What We Pretend to Know About the Coronavirus Could Kill Us. (2020). Available online at: https://www.nytimes.com/2020/04/03/opinion/sunday/coronavirus-fake-news.html (accessed May 15, 2020).

25. Lep Ž, Babnik K, Hacin Beyazoglu K. Emotional responses and self-protective behavior within days of the COVID-19 outbreak: the promoting role of information credibility. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:1846. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Siebenhaar KU, Köther AK, Alpers GW. Dealing with the COVID-19 infodemic: distress by information, information avoidance, and compliance with preventive measures. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:2981. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.567905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Cinelli M, Quattrociocchi W, Galeazzi A, Valensise CM, Brugnoli E, Schmidt AL, et al. The covid-19 social media infodemic. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-73510-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Pulido CM, Villarejo-Carballido B, Redondo-Sama G, Gómez A. COVID-19 infodemic: more retweets for science-based information on coronavirus than for false information. Int Sociol. (2020) 35:377–92. doi: 10.1177/0268580920914755

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Del Vicario M, Bessi A, Zollo F, Petroni F, Scala A, Caldarelli G, et al. The spreading of misinformation online. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2016) 113:554–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1517441113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Fung IC, Fu KW, Chan CH, Chan BS, Cheung CN, Abraham T, et al. Social media's initial reaction to information and misinformation on Ebola, August 2014: facts and rumors. Public Health Rep. (2016) 131:461–73. doi: 10.1177/003335491613100312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Wang C, Pan R, Wan X, Tan Y, Xu L, Ho CS, et al. Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:1729. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17051729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Casagrande M, Favieri F, Tambelli R, Forte G. The enemy who sealed the world: effects quarantine due to the COVID-19 on sleep quality, anxiety, and psychological distress in the Italian population. Sleep Med. (2020) 75:12–20.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

33. Zhang SX, Wang Y, Rauch A, Wei F. Unprecedented disruption of lives and work: Health, distress, and life satisfaction of working adults in China one month into the COVID-19 outbreak. Psychiatry Res. (2020) 288:112958. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Kwok KO, Li KK, Chan HH, Yi YY, Tang A, Wei WI, et al. Community responses during the early phase of the COVID-19 epidemic in Hong Kong. Emerg Infect Dis. (2020) 26:1575–579. doi: 10.1101/2020.02.26.20028217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Zhong B, Jiang Z, Xie W, Qin X. Association of social media use with mental health conditions of nonpatients during the COVID-19 outbreak: insights from a national survey study. J Med Internet Res. (2020) 22:e23696. doi: 10.2196/preprints.23696

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Liu C, Liu Y. Media exposure and anxiety during COVID-19: the mediation effect of media vicarious traumatization. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:4720. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17134720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Bawden D, Robinson L. The dark side of information: overload, anxiety and other paradoxes and pathologies. J Inf Sci. (2009) 35:180–91. doi: 10.1177/0165551508095781

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Garfin DR, Silver RC, Holman EA. The novel coronavirus (COVID-2019) outbreak: amplification of public health consequences by media exposure. Health Psychol. (2020) 39:355–7. doi: 10.1037/hea0000875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Kim L, Fast SM, Markuzon N. Incorporating media data into a model of infectious disease transmission. PLoS One. (2019) 142:e0197646. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Sweeny K, Melnyk D, Miller W, Shepperd JA. Information avoidance: who, what, when, and why. Rev Gen Psychol. (2010) 14:340–53. doi: 10.1037/a0021288

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Xie W, Newhagen JE. The effects of communication interface proximity on user anxiety for crime alerts received on desktop, laptop, and hand-held devices. Communic Res. (2012) 41:375–403. doi: 10.1177/0093650212448670

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Swar B, Hameed T, Reychav I. Information overload, psychological ill-being, and behavioral intention to continue online healthcare information search. Comput Hum Behav. (2017) 70:416–25. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.12.068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Van den Bulck J, Custers K. Television exposure is related to fear of avian flu, an ecological study across 23 member states of the European Union. Eur J Public Health. (2009) 19:370–4. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckp061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Brug J, Aro AR, Oenema A, de Zwart O, Richardus JH, Bishop GD. SARS risk perception, knowledge, precautions, and information sources, the Netherlands. Emerg Infect Dis. (2004) 10:1486–9. doi: 10.3201/eid1008.040283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Lau J, Griffiths S, Au D, Choi K. Changes in knowledge, perceptions, preventive behaviours and psychological responses in the precommunity outbreak phase of the H1N1 epidemic. Epidemiol Infect. (2011) 139:80–90. doi: 10.1017/S0950268810001925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Pearlman L, Saakvitne K. Trauma and the Therapist: Countertransference and Vicarious Traumatization in Psychotherapy With Incest Survivors. New York, NY: WW Norton (1995).

Google Scholar

47. McCann L, Pearlman LA. Vicarious traumatization: a framework for understanding the psychological effects of working with victims. J Trauma Stress. (1990) 3:131–49. doi: 10.1007/BF00975140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Thompson RR, Jones NM, Holman EA, Silver RC. Media exposure to mass violence events can fuel a cycle of distress. Sci Adv. (2019) 5:eaav3502. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aav3502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Rubin GJ, Amlot R, Page L, Wessely S. Public perceptions, anxiety, and behaviour change in relation to the swine flu outbreak: cross sectional telephone survey. BMJ. (2009) 339:b2651. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b2651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Wheaton MG, Abramowitz JS, Berman NC, Fabricant LE, Olatunji BO. Psychological predictors of anxiety in response to the H1N1 (swine flu) pandemic. Cogn Ther Res. (2012) 36:210–18. doi: 10.1007/s10608-011-9353-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Klemm C, Das E, Hartmann T. Swine flu and hype: a systematic review of media dramatization of the H1N1 influenza pandemic. J Risk Res. (2016) 19:1–20. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2014.923029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Thompson RR, Garfin DR, Holman EA, Silver RC. Distress, worry, and functioning following a global health crisis: a national study of americans' responses to ebola. Clin Psychol Sci. (2017) 5:513–21. doi: 10.1177/2167702617692030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Taha SA, Matheson K, Anisman H. H1N1 was not all that scary: uncertainty and stressor appraisals predict anxiety related to a coming viral threat. Stress Health. (2014) 30:149–57. doi: 10.1002/smi.2505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Fischhoff B, Wong-Parodi G, Garfin DR, Holman EA, Silver RC. Public understanding of Ebola risks: mastering an unfamiliar threat. Risk Anal. (2018) 38:71–83. doi: 10.1111/risa.12794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Pfefferbaum B, Newman E, Nelson SD, Nitiéma P, Pfefferbaum RL, Rahman A. Disaster media coverage and psychological outcomes: descriptive findings in the extant research. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2014) 16:464. doi: 10.1007/s11920-014-0464-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Bernstein KT, Ahern J, Tracy M, Boscarino JA, Vlahov D, Galea S. Television watching and the risk of incident probable posttraumatic stress disorder: a prospective evaluation. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2007) 195:41–7. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000244784.36745.a5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Lachlan KA, Spence PR, Seeger M. Terrorist attacks and uncertainty reduction: media use after September 11. Behav Sci Terror Polit Aggress. (2009) 1:101–10. doi: 10.1080/19434470902771683

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Garfin DR, Holman EA, Silver RC. Cumulative exposure to prior collective trauma and acute stress responses to the Boston marathon bombings. Psychol Sci. (2015) 26:675–83. doi: 10.1177/0956797614561043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Zung WW. A rating instrument for anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics. (1971) 12:371–9. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(71)71479-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Janz NK, Becker MH. The health belief model: a decade later. Health Educ Behav. (1984) 11:1–47.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

61. Shim E, Chapman GB, Townsend JP, Galvani AP. The influence of altruism on influenza vaccination decisions. J R Soc Interface. (2012) 9:2234–43. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2012.0115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Zhang S, Zhao L, Lu Y, Yang J. Do you get tired of socializing? An empirical explanation of discontinuous usage behaviour in social network services. Inf Manag. (2016) 53:904–14. doi: 10.1016/j.im.2016.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Vrklevski LP, Franklin J. Vicarious trauma: the impact on solicitors of exposure to traumatic material. Traumatology. (2008) 14:106–18. doi: 10.1177/1534765607309961

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2013).

Google Scholar

65. Brashers DE, Neidig JL, Haas SM, Dobbs LK, Cardillo LW, Russell JA. Communication in the management of uncertainty: the case of persons living with HIV or AIDS. Commun Monogr. (2000) 67:63–84. doi: 10.1080/03637750009376495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Mheidly N, Fares J. Leveraging media and health communication strategies to overcome the COVID-19 infodemic. J Public Health Policy. (2020) 41:410–20. doi: 10.1057/s41271-020-00247-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Zhong B, Huang Y, Liu Q. Mental health toll from the coronavirus: social media usage reveals Wuhan residents' depression and secondary trauma in the COVID-19 outbreak. Comput Human Behav. (2021) 114:106524. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2020.106524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Shim M, Jo HS. What quality factors matter in enhancing the perceived benefits of online health information sites? Application of the updated DeLone and McLean information systems success model. Int J Med Inform. (2020) 137:104093. doi: 10.1016/j.ijmedinf.2020.104093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: infodemic, COVID-19, anxiety, information overload, vicarious trauma, commercial media, urban governance

Citation: Xu J and Liu C (2021) Infodemic vs. Pandemic Factors Associated to Public Anxiety in the Early Stage of the COVID-19 Outbreak: A Cross-Sectional Study in China. Front. Public Health 9:723648. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.723648

Received: 11 June 2021; Accepted: 09 August 2021;
Published: 30 August 2021.

Edited by:

Elena V. Libin, Independent Researcher, Chevy Chase, United States

Reviewed by:

Tanay Maiti, All India Institute of Medical Sciences, Bhubaneswar, India
Si-Tong Chen, Victoria University, Australia

Copyright © 2021 Xu and Liu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Cong Liu, lcong26@sjtu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.