- 1Key Laboratory of Learning and Cognition, School of Psychology, Capital Normal University, Beijing, China
- 2Alliance for Human Development, Lunenfeld-Tanenbaum Research Institute, Toronto, ON, Canada
- 3Department of Psychology, Western University, London, ON, Canada
- 4Institute for Human Development, Aga Khan University, Nairobi, Kenya
- 5Centre for Child Development, Mental Health, and Policy, University of Toronto Mississauga, Mississauga, ON, Canada
- 6Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
Background: Over 250 million children in low- and middle-income countries are at risk of not achieving their fullest developmental potential due to co-occurring risks such as poor nutrition and inadequate learning opportunities. Early intervention programs integrating the aspects of nurturing care, that is, good health, adequate nutrition, safety and security, responsive caregiving, and learning opportunities, may ameliorate against the negative impact of these adverse conditions.
Methods: This meta-analytic review updates the evidence base of parenting interventions comprising stimulation and responsive caregiving components on developmental outcomes for children under age 2 years in low- and middle-income countries. It also describes and assesses the moderation effects of population characteristics and implementation features on the intervention effectiveness. Studies were identified based on previous systematic reviews and an updated literature search in eight databases and the gray literature up to December 2020. A random-effect model was used to explore the pooled effect sizes accounted for by the intervention for developmental outcome of cognition, language, motor, and social-emotional capacities. Exploratory moderation analyses were also conducted.
Results: Twenty-one randomized controlled trials representing over 10,400 children from 12 low- and middle-income countries and regions across three continents (Africa, Latin America, and Asia) were identified. The interventions showed overall small-to-moderate effects on children's cognitive development (ES = 0.44; 95% CI = [0.30, 0.57]); language development (ES = 0.33; 95% CI = [0.18, 0.49]); and motor skills (ES = 0.21; 95% CI = [0.10, 0.32]). The overall effect on social-emotional development was non-significant (ES = 0.17; 95% CI = [−0.01, 0.34]). Effect sizes (ES) varied significantly across the studies. Parenting programs that targeted vulnerable groups, including rural communities and caregivers with lower education levels, had more significant effects on children's development. Group sessions (vs. individual visits) and high program dose (≥12 sessions) were also associated with stronger effects on child development. Further research is needed to determine the effectiveness of the workforce and training on programmatic outcomes.
Conclusion: The findings indicate that parenting interventions that encourage nurturing care are effective in improving the early development of children, especially among vulnerable populations. We discuss opportunities to strengthen the implementation of research-based parenting interventions in such contexts.
Introduction
In low- and middle-income countries (LMICs), over 250 million children under age five are not achieving their developmental potential due to adverse living environments, such as chronic poverty, poor nutrition and sanitation, violence, and inadequate learning opportunities (1). As emphasized in multiple United Nations Sustainable Development Goals, child development has been recognized as playing a key role in the long-term development and well-being of the society at large (2–4).
The first few years of life are sensitive periods for brain and neural development, laying the foundation for long-term mental development (5–7). Specifically, there has been a push for the improved focus and need for investment in the first 1,000 days of life (which equates to the period from pregnancy to 2 years) (8). During this period, children are highly susceptible to environmental influences such as responsive parenting and cognitive stimulation. On the one hand, exposure to risk factors during this time can pose long-term and sometimes difficult-to-reverse detrimental impacts on children's developmental, educational, and health outcomes and productivity, ultimately incurring costs to society (9). Alternatively, this can also be a window for children to benefit from a nurturing caregiving home environment and thrive despite the adverse larger social environment (10).
Caregivers provide the primary environment in the early years of a child's life and are the entry point of many interventions supporting child development (11, 12). Such interventions include parenting programs, which are defined as interventions or services aimed at improving parenting interactions, behaviors, knowledge, attitudes, practices and beliefs (10). While earlier parenting interventions had a heavy emphasis on child health and nutrition, more and more research has recognized the importance and potential of psychosocial stimulation, especially given the shift from surviving and promoting physical growth to thriving and nurturing mental development (1). According to the World Health Organization, psychosocial stimulation consists of physical and emotional stimulation which are both aimed at facilitating children's cognitive, emotional, social and language development (13). Emotional stimulation comprises the expression of affection and warmth to a child in ways that are consistent with cultural norms, responding to the needs of the child in a timely manner, encouraging verbal and non-verbal communication between caregiver and the child, and praising or showing appreciation when the child manages to do something (13). Physical stimulation involves providing the child with opportunities for adequate sensory experiences through playing together with the child and providing them with age-appropriate play materials as well as, providing meaning to the child's physical world for example, by helping the child to name, count, and compare objects (13). The concept of psychosocial stimulation is also captured in the Nurturing Care Framework under its components of “responsive caregiving” and “opportunities for early learning” (14). Responsive caregiving entails propensity on the part of caregivers to notice, understand and respond to their child's cues in an appropriate and timely manner. Responsive care also creates opportunities for early learning, which refers to any opportunity for a baby or child to interact with objects, people, and place in their environment (14). Responsive feeding is also embedded in the concept of responsive caregiving and involves practices such as minimizing distractions during meals, feeding slowly and patiently, talking to a child with eye-to-eye contact during feeding, and introducing different food combinations (textures, tastes) at an appropriate age (15, 16). Overall, psychosocial stimulation interventions/programs train caregivers on how to support their children's development through responsive and sensitive caregiver-child interactions (e.g., age-appropriate play, telling stories, responsive feeding, exploring picture books, praising, cuddling among others). Noteworthy, psychosocial stimulation interventions are increasingly being integrated into other maternal and child health programs like nutrition, sanitation, and cash transfer programs (17, 18).
Along this line, several psychosocial stimulation or responsive care interventions have been implemented for children younger than 2 to 3 years of age in LMICs (10, 17, 19–22). A recent review of 75 parenting programs, which included 14 studies (12 from LMICs) on psychosocial stimulation or responsive care, found that in LMICs, these interventions had significant effects on cognitive development [ES = 0.49, CI (0.27–0.72)], language skills [ES = 0.43, CI (0.11–0.76)], and on motor skills development [ES = 0.39, CI (0.13–0.65)] in children below 5 years (17). Previous reviews have also corroborated the findings of moderate positive effects of parenting interventions with psychosocial stimulation aspects on language outcomes (21, 22), cognitive skills (10, 21, 22), motor development (10) and psycho-social skills (10) of young children. Another most recent review consistently reports the positive benefits of these interventions on cognitive development but notes that these benefits are only sustained for a short-term (1–3 years) duration (20). These results suggest that caring and stimulating environments with opportunities to play and communicate do play a significant role in child development outcomes.
Despite the promising effect of parenting interventions that include psychosocial stimulation or responsive care on developmental outcomes of children in low-resource settings, the key factors contributing to and barriers preventing the success of the interventions are still not well-understood. It has been proposed that the intervention and implementation processes, such as the mode of delivery (i.e., home visits, group sessions, or clinic appointments) (1, 4–6); the sensitivity of the measures utilized to assess child developmental outcomes (3, 4); the frequency of sessions and overall duration of the intervention (2, 5); the curriculum characteristics, for example, the form of behavior change techniques used (1, 2, 4, 6); the characteristics of the delivery agents (e.g., level and quality of training, being a part of existing service delivery systems) (2, 4); and the fidelity and quality control measures in place (2, 5) are likely important factors besides the content that account for the level of effectiveness and sustainability of the intervention. The characteristics of the target population, including caregivers' education status, household socioeconomic status, characteristics of children (e.g., age at enrolment), and caregivers' study setting (e.g., rural or urban) are also outlined among potential sources of variation in intervention effects (17, 19–21). Thus, researchers have increasingly expressed a need for an improved understanding of the common mechanisms that drive sustained parenting intervention treatment gains (20).
Although there has been consensus that these underlying factors require careful inspection, only the meta-analysis by Aboud and Yousafzai (2015) included some form of moderation analysis to inspect variation attributable to some of these factors. They found that the weighted mean of 10 effects sizes was improved (from d = 0.324 to 0.592) upon stratification based on seven interventions which utilized group sessions with some home visits, indicating the potential advantage of using a mixed approach (home visits with group sessions) over interventions with only home visits or clinic visits separately (21). They also noted that the implementation of parenting interventions requires a structured curriculum with sufficient dosage but not overloading messages, a format of delivery, well-trained and supervised personnel. However, they found that only 5 of the 21 studies in their review reported on fidelity to the intended program (21). Generally, most of the existing reviews descriptively summarize a few intervention/implementation factors with mixed depths of discussion (mainly briefly) about their potential implications for the findings and future work on parenting interventions and policy/decision-making. Thus, how intervention effects vary across other implementation delivery methods and sample characteristics are still not well-understood.
The Present Study
The current systematic meta-analytic review builds upon previous reviews (10, 17, 19–22) and aims to extend the knowledge of parenting interventions and their developmental outcomes, specifically interventions with the components of psychosocial stimulation or responsive care for children under the age of 2 years in low- and middle-income countries. The specific objectives are: (i) to update the existing literature of the effectiveness of parenting interventions with components of psychosocial stimulation or responsive caregiving on cognitive, language, motor, and socio-emotional skills development of children under the age of 2 years in LMICs; and (ii) to describe and assess the moderation effects of implementation and sample characteristics on the outcomes of interventions with the components of psychosocial stimulation or responsive care. We, therefore, conducted exploratory moderation analyses, whenever possible, on intervention effect sizes across select study characteristics that were identified to be key in successful interventions (21) and also described a variety of other implementation aspects.
Methods
Literature Search
Parenting interventions that include psychosocial stimulation or responsive care of children under age two in low- and middle-income countries were extracted from previous systematic reviews on this topic. At the inception of this work, the available literature dated till 04/2015 (10, 21, 22). We conducted an updated literature search from 05/2015 to 10/2020. Our systematic review followed the guidelines for preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses (PRISMA) (23). The updated literature search was conducted in eight electronic databases of PubMed, EMBASE, CINAHL, Scopus, PsycInfo, ERIC, ProQuest Dissertation & These Global, and EconLit. Additionally, gray literature was searched in the OAIster and OpenGrey databases and institutional electronic resources (e.g., websites of UNICEF, WHO, The World Bank). Reference tracking and hand-searching were also conducted to identify any relevant materials that could have been missed during the indexing process, as well as any additional relevant articles published between 11/2020 and 12/2020. The search strategy comprised a combination of free terms or keywords on participants (e.g., child, toddler, infant), types of intervention (e.g., attachment, stimulation, psychosocial), and types of outcomes (e.g., development, cognitive, language, social, motor) combined with Boolean operators of “OR” and “AND” (see Supplementary Table 1).
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
Articles were screened by title, abstract and full text for eligibility by trained undergraduate psychology research assistants, the first author and a co-author author (DS). Studies were included if they met the following criteria: (1) the intervention focused on improving caregiving quality through individual or group-based training on responsive, stimulating, and sensitive caregiver-child interactions (e.g., age-appropriate play, telling stories, responsive feeding, exploring picture books, praising, cuddling among others); (2) the study enrolled children with an average age below 24 months; (3) the study reported quantitative findings on at least one of the childhood developmental outcomes of cognitive, motor, social-emotional capacities, and language skills; (4) the intervention was implemented in low and middle-income countries; and (5) the study used randomized controlled design. Studies were excluded if they only enrolled children or caregivers with selected pre-existing health risks such as preterm infants or infants of mothers with HIV and if they did not report quantitative findings on any of the childhood developmental outcomes.
Data Extraction and Quality Assessment
The information extracted from the eligible studies included: study characteristics (e.g., country, study design, period of study implementation, enrolment criteria, caregiver age), intervention and implementation features (e.g., curriculum used, delivery mode, dosage, intervention facilitators, characteristics of delivery agents, capacity building, formative research and quality assurance aspects), effect sizes on child developmental outcomes and assessment instruments. Quality assessment of the studies in this review was conducted following the Cochrane Collaboration Risk of Bias guidelines (24). Specifically, the studies were rated on the potential of bias (high, low, unclear) in the following domains: random sequence generation, allocation concealment, blinding of participants and personnel, blinding of outcome assessors, completeness of outcome data, and selectivity in reporting. Ratings of risks of bias were not used as an exclusion criterion (see Supplementary Table 2). Both the extraction and risk of bias assessment were conducted independently by the first author, a co-author (DS) and a trained graduate student using structured coding spreadsheets and discrepancies were resolved through discussion and consensus.
Outcomes
This review focused on the outcomes in the four childhood developmental domains of cognitive, language, motor and social-emotional capacities, assessed at the first post-intervention period. The cognitive outcomes included broad cognitive processing such as sensory-perceptual skills, problems solving, memory (25), which are often captured collectively as scores on scales like the Bayley II Mental Developmental Index (26), the Bayley-III cognitive scale (27), and Performance subscale of the Griffiths Scales of Child Development (28). Language outcomes included communication skills such as receptive and expressive language (29). Motor outcomes included gross locomotor and fine motor skills such as hand and eye coordination (30). Social-emotional outcomes included a broad range of social and emotional capacities. Specifically, these are adaptive behaviors either collectively reported such as personal-social development skills on the Ages and Stages Questionnaire, or separately reported such as responsiveness to examiner, emotional tone, cooperation, and emotional vocalization using Wolke scale (31) and modified behavior rating scales (32, 33).
Data Analysis
Data were synthesized both quantitatively and narratively. The effect sizes (ESs) for each of the four domains (cognitive, language, motor, and social-emotional capacities) were calculated as the standardized mean difference between the intervention and control groups at post-test using pooled standard deviations and weighted by the inverse variance method (Hedge's g) (34, 35). When the data reported in a study was not sufficient to compute the Hedge's g, we instead narratively summarized the effects without including the reported measure of effect in the quantitative pooled effects analysis. For studies with multiple outcomes within one category (e.g., expressive and receptive language), the effect sizes were aggregated under the assumption that the outcomes were moderately correlated (r = 0.50) when actual correlations were not available (36). For studies with multiple intervention or control groups, effect sizes were combined across the groups when possible following the procedures described in chapters 23–25 of the handbook by Borenstein et al. (36). Specifically, for intervention groups, we prioritized effect sizes from arms of a factorial randomized controlled trial which comprised the intervention package specifically focused on the responsive, stimulating, and sensitive caregiver-child interactions components. For example, if a study had four arms of “psychosocial stimulation alone,” “nutrition supplementation alone,” “psychosocial stimulation and nutrition supplementation,” and “control (routine care)” we focused on effect sizes from the “psychosocial stimulation alone” and the “control (routine care)” arms. Effect sizes were analyzed with a random-effect model to explore the estimates accounted for by the intervention on each of the four developmental outcomes. The random-effect model was fitted using the meta-analysis command “metan” in STATA 15 software package (37). The variation in effect size attributable to heterogeneity was evaluated with the Q statistic and the I2 statistic of the DerSimonian and Laird method (38).
Exploratory moderation analyses were conducted to examine whether the effect sizes varied significantly according to study characteristics. Sample characteristics included the context of the sample (0 = rural, 1 = urban or peri-urban), the mean age of children at enrollment (0 = less than 12 months, 1 = equal to or greater than 12 months), average levels of maternal education (0 = lower than 6 years or over 50% did not complete primary education, 1 = greater than 6 years or over 50% completed primary and higher education). Implementation characteristics included mode of delivery (0 = individual visits, 1 = primarily group sessions or group sessions combined with individual visits), average dosage of delivery (0 = 2 times or less per month, 1 = more than 2 times per month), total number of sessions (0 = less than 12 sessions, 1 = 12 or more sessions), duration of intervention (0 = less than 12 months, 1 = 12 months or longer), characteristics of delivery agents (0 = trained paraprofessionals with previous experience in the healthcare system, 1 = trained paraprofessionals without previous experience in the healthcare system, 2 = mixed paraprofessionals and/or health professionals) and duration of training of delivery agents (0 = 2 weeks or more, 1 = less than 2 weeks, 2 = details not reported).
Narrative synthesis involved a summary (both in text and table format) of risk of bias assessment, study characteristics (e.g., country, year of implementation), curriculum utilized, tools utilized to assess developmental outcomes, capacity building of intervention staff, quality control and fidelity aspects and formative research components of the interventions.
Results
Study and Sample Characteristics
Twenty-one studies were identified from a total of 81,210 records from the database search and previous reviews based on the eligibility criteria (see Figure 1 for the flow chart). Of the 21 eligible studies, 16 of them (39–54) were previously captured in four previous reviews (17, 20–22) assessing the effects of parenting interventions with the components of psychosocial stimulation or responsive care on early child neurodevelopmental outcomes. This present review includes 5 additional studies (55–59). Common reasons for exclusion include the use of a non-RCT design, study conducted outside a low-and-middle-income country context, a single focus on nutrition supplementation or education, the enrollment of predominantly children over 24 months, and the absence of child developmental outcomes (e.g., a focus on child physical growth or parent-child interactions).
The study and sample characteristics are summarized in Table 1. Of the 21 eligible studies, seven were conducted in the African region (42, 46, 49, 55–58), and their implementation period was between 2013 and 2019. There were four eligible studies from the Caribbean region (41, 50–52), implemented between 1999 and 2012; however, two of these studies did not report the implementation dates (50, 51). Both studies from the Latin American region were from Colombia, conducted between 2010 and 2016 (40, 59). Eight studies were conducted in Southern and South Eastern Asia (39, 43–45, 47, 48, 53, 54). The intervention period of the Asian studies was between 2000 and 2012; however, 3 of these (44, 45, 47) did not report the implementation periods. Over 10,400 children and families were enrolled in these 21 studies, with sample sizes ranging from 41 to 1,381 per study. Among these studies, 13 were conducted in rural areas and seven were in urban or peri-urban areas (1 unidentified). Children's age at enrollment ranged from pre-birth (i.e., third trimester of pregnancy) to 36 months with a mean below 12 months among 10 studies (39, 41, 42, 47, 49, 52–54, 58, 59) and between 12 to 24 months for the other 10 studies. Enrollment age was missing in one of the studies (55). Maternal education of the study participants was below primary level (mean <6 years or > 50% did not complete primary education) in 13 interventions (of note, almost all in rural contexts) and above the primary level in 7 interventions (40, 41, 51, 52, 56, 58, 59); which were mostly from the urban context. One of the studies did not report the maternal education level (50).
Assessment of Child Outcomes
All the seven studies in Africa reported cognitive outcomes, while five reported language skills (42, 46, 55–57) and 4 reported on social-emotional capacities and motor skills (42, 55–57). All four studies from the Caribbean reported cognitive, motor and language outcomes, but none reported socio-emotional data. Both studies in Colombia reported cognitive, motor and language outcomes. Dimensions of social-emotional development were only reported in one study in Colombia (59). Four studies in Asia reported on language (39, 48, 53, 54) and social-emotional outcomes (43, 45, 47, 53), while 5 reported on motor skills (43–45, 47, 53). Only one study did not report cognitive outcomes (48).
Assessment measures of child outcomes are summarized in Table 2. Twelve of the studies that assessed cognitive development used the Mental Development Index or the Cognition subscale of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (Bayley II or Bayley III) (26, 27) and 4 used the Performance subscale of the Griffiths Scales of Child Development (28). The most frequently utilized tools for assessment of language development were the Receptive and Expressive Language subscales of the Bayley Scales (reported in 8 studies) and the Hearing and Speech subscale of the Griffiths Scales (reported in 4 studies). Similarly, the majority of studies on motor development utilized the Psychomotor or fine motor subscales of the Bayley Scales and the Hand and Eye Coordination subscale of the Griffiths Scales. The parents' report on Ages and Stages (60) (used in 4 studies) and the Behavior rating scales (in 2 studies) were the most commonly utilized tools in assessing social-emotional development.
Effect Sizes on Child Outcomes
Pooled Effect Sizes and Heterogeneity
Effect sizes and forest plots of the individual studies are presented in Figures 2–5. The pooled average effect sizes are 0.44 on cognitive development (n = 18; 95% CI = [0.30, 0.57]), 0.33 on language development (n = 13; 95% CI = [0.18, 0.49]), 0.21 on motor development (n = 14; 95% CI = [0.10, 0.32]), and 0.17 on social-emotional development (n = 7; 95% CI = [−0.01, 0.34]). The effect sizes varied significantly across studies on cognitive development (Q = 141.99, df = 17, p < 0.001, I2 = 88.0), language development (Q = 102.39, df = 12, p < 0.001, I2 = 88.3), motor development (Q = 49.87, df = 13, p < 0.001, I2 = 73.9), and social-emotional development (Q = 45.33, df = 6, p < 0.001, I2 =86.8).
We did not include two studies in the meta-analysis as the data reported in both were not sufficient to compute the Hedge's g (57, 59).
Intervention Characteristics
The intervention and implementation characteristics are narratively summarized in Table 3 and in the Supplementary Table 2.
Mode and Dose of Intervention Delivery
Interventions were delivered through home visits in nine studies and group sessions (with or without home visits) in 12 studies. Of note, 71% of the studies in the rural context used group sessions compared to 29% of the urban or peri-urban studies.
The interventions were delivered in varying levels of frequency, ranging from twice weekly to two times over the course of 6 months. The average frequency was two times or less per month in nine studies and over twice per month in 12 studies. The total number of sessions ranged from 2 to 124, with the majority of the interventions (15 out of 21) delivered in 12 or more sessions. The overall duration of the interventions ranged from 2 to 24 months, with the majority (14 out of 21) under 12 months.
Delivery Agents
Ten studies (39, 40, 43–47, 49, 58, 59) clearly reported that the intervention delivery agents were women; however, gender was not clearly stated in some studies (41, 50–53, 55). Only eight of the 21 interventions relied on trained delivery agents who did not formally work with the local health care system (e.g., women who were selected to serve as play leaders, volunteers, or home visitors based on being mothers, having a reputation in the community, or having some basic education and good communication skills) (40, 43, 45–47, 55, 57, 58).
All studies reported that intervention delivery agents were trained, with the majority indicating that the delivery agents received one-off in-person training sessions ranging in length from 3 days of training (41, 55) to 3 weeks of training (40, 45, 58). Overall, in 10 intervention studies the delivery agents' training duration was 2 weeks or more (40, 43, 45, 46, 51, 52, 56–59); however, the training duration was not clearly reported in 5 studies (42, 44, 47, 50, 53). Refresher training of delivery agents was reported among four intervention studies (46, 54, 56, 61). In some studies, training sessions were spread out within the implementation period (40, 56, 58). Depending on the specific focus of the intervention, general training content included theoretical and practical skills on counseling and effective communication (e.g., facilitating support groups, problem identification and solving) and parenting practices (including responsive feeding, child stimulation, play). Trainers included trained graduates (mainly in health sciences like nutrition health and psychology) (40, 42, 59), trained paraprofessionals (49), professionals in ECD (61), researchers together with staff from implementing/local organizations working in child and maternal health (46, 54, 56). However, the majority of the studies did not report in detail on training aspects.
Intervention Curricula
There were various reported sources of intervention curricula, with a common tendency for studies to report multiple sources. Some of the commonly cited sources included UNICEF and WHO's “Care for Child Development” program (39, 49, 57), the Jamaica Home Visiting program (40, 59), WHO/PAHO guidelines on complementary feeding (42, 47, 54), WHO's social Baby and Improving psychosocial development (58), among others. However, in some of the studies, the sources of the curricula were not clearly reported (55, 56). Routine care, mostly referring to routine services provided mainly by community healthcare systems, was the most typical reported component in the control groups.
Fidelity and Quality Control
Various modalities were utilized to ensure fidelity and quality of the implementation process (See Supplementary Table 2). Some commonly reported ones included: routine on-the-job observation of agents by supervisors (mostly on a monthly basis), utilization of designed monitoring tools such as visit forms, fidelity checklists and home visit forms to track progress and compliance to curricula (41, 49, 54); formal and informal sessions involving supervisors and delivery agents were utilized to provide feedback and to discuss challenges and solutions to keep activities on track (41, 44, 52, 56). Although the majority of studies reported that delivery agents were provided with support supervision, most did not elaborate on what such supervision entailed. A few indicated that it entailed assistance of the agents to prepare their sessions, provision of feedback on performance through a structured process (e.g., monitoring forms), reviewing the intervention topics with them and providing them guidance in discussions and practice prior to their activities (41, 46). Other quality improvement approaches reported feedback on the intervention experience directly from the beneficiaries (47), providing mentorship and on-the-job coaching for delivery agents (40, 59), and sending frequent reminders reinforcing key messages to delivery agents (40).
Moderation Effects
Effect sizes at each level of the moderators are summarized in Table 4. These effect sizes are for individual moderators from un-adjusted moderation analyses and thus should be interpreted cautiously. Moderations were not reported for social-emotional outcomes due to the small number of studies reporting on this domain. Generally, 5 or more studies are needed to reasonably and consistently achieve sufficient power from random-effects meta-analyses (62).
Among the sample and context characteristics [i.e., study setting (rural and urban/peri-urban), child age at enrolment and maternal education], we found statistically significant group differences in the pooled effect sizes across maternal education (for language skills) and rural settings (for motor skills). Specifically, the effect sizes for language were larger for children of caregivers with lower educational level (ES = 0.45 vs. 0.17, Q = 4.25, p = 0.04). The context where the intervention was delivered (urban vs. rural), as well as the mode of delivery, also explained a significant proportion of variance on the effect sizes on motor skills outcomes. Effect sizes were greater for children in rural areas compared to urban settings (ES = 0.29 vs. 0.11, Q = 5.2, p = 0.02). Thus, consistent with previous research, we find that program effects were larger for vulnerable children, including lower levels of family education and rural residence.
The moderation effects of the intervention characteristics of mode of delivery, duration of the intervention, number of sessions delivered, dose of delivery, the type of delivery agents as well as the duration of their training are also summarized in Table 4. Statistically significant group differences in the pooled effect sizes were only found across the mode of delivery (for cognitive and motor skills) and the number of sessions (for language skills). The effect sizes for language skills were larger for children of caregivers who received interventions with 12 or more sessions (ES = 0.44 vs. 0.12, Q = 6.24, p = 0.01). Effect sizes for motor skills were also greater for interventions delivered through group sessions compared to individual sessions (ES = 0.31 vs. 0.08, Q = 7.59, p = 0.006). Also, for the cognitive development domain, the effect sizes were greater for interventions delivered through group sessions compared to individual sessions (ES = 0.53 vs. 0.28, Q = 4.99, p = 0.03).
Discussion
This systematic literature review identified 21 parenting interventions published from 2004 to 2020 with the components of responsive, stimulating, and sensitive caregiver-child interactions for children under age two in low- and middle-income countries evaluated in randomized controlled trials. We also summarized the various intervention features and implementation aspects, for example, relating to context and participant characteristics, quality and fidelity, capacity building, delivery agents, and intervention curricula. We investigated how some of these features moderated the effects of these interventions on childhood developmental outcomes.
A key finding from our meta-analyses is that there are overall positive, albeit small-to-medium, effects of the parenting interventions on childhood developmental outcomes, including cognitive, language, and motor skills. Our findings are consistent with findings from a previous systematic review published in 2015, where stimulation interventions among children aged 0–2 years were moderately effective for cognitive outcomes (d = 0.42) and language development (d = 0.47) (21). Similarly, our results are supported by findings from a more recent systematic review that reported significant effect sizes of 0.49 in cognitive skills, 0.43 in language skills, and 0.39 in motor skills among interventions with components on responsive care and learning opportunities LMICs (17). In our present review, we presume that the lack of statistical significance for the socio-emotional domain may possibly arise from the few intervention studies (n = 7) that were currently published and eligible. Taken together, our updated review captures literature up to the end of 2020 and shows that results on intervention effects in the cognitive, language and motor skills domains are, more or less, consistent with previous systematic reviews in this area.
Although cognitive-linguistic development in the early years is important in relation to later educational achievement, the review supports our call for increased attention to social-emotional outcomes for three main reasons. First, early social-emotional development plays a key role in subsequent achievement, health, and well-being over and above the contribution of early cognitive development, especially in adverse contexts (63–66). Second, social-emotional development is sensitive to influences of early adverse experiences such as exposure to violence and stress (7, 67–70). Third, social-emotional development can be crucially shaped through parenting techniques such as developmentally appropriate support and warmth, engaging in sensitive parent-child interactions, and fostering secure parent-child bonds (71–74).
Another important finding is the significant variation in the overall effect of the interventions on the domains of cognitive, language and motor skills that were attributable to some intervention and implementation features. First, the use of group sessions or group sessions combined with home visits in our review was found to be more beneficial than individual sessions alone (specifically for cognitive and motor skills development). This approach has been endorsed by other researchers who suggest that its effectiveness arises from its potential to modify group norms on child-rearing, being less labor intensive compared to home visits and because it encourages peer support (21). However, it is unclear why the variation in effects attributable to delivery modality was only registered in cognitive and motor development and not in the language domain.
Still, in connection to the delivery approach, we found that interventions that delivered a greater number of sessions (12 or more) across the intervention period were more beneficial for language development compared to those with fewer sessions. Studies on the dose-effect of interventions on language development are scanty; however, one systematic review found that interventions aimed at improving child language through enhancing maternal responsivity were generally effective when offered for ~10–12 weekly sessions programs (75). We suggest that the findings indicating better effects of the interventions among children of rural (for motor skills) and lower educated (for language skills) caregivers are symbolic of catch-up neurodevelopmental growth in children from less privileged settings where social inequalities and a myriad of negative social determinants of health hinder optimal neuro-development in the early years (76). Another explanation may be that the differences result from the smaller number of studies conducted in urban settings and higher educated caregivers.
The findings on the use of formally trained paraprofessionals (e.g., community health volunteers) as intervention delivery agents accentuate their central role in promoting community health and early child development (77), but more importantly, the feasibility and promising sustainability of utilizing this delivery approach for mainstreaming nurturing care in community health within LMIC setting. Another important finding was that some interventions were still feasible and effective even when trained community resource persons who were not necessarily trained formal paraprofessionals and without prior work experience in the healthcare system were utilized as delivery agents. This is important because formally trained health paraprofessionals may sometimes face large workloads from their formal roles and various ongoing community health program activities (78, 79) that might negatively impact their performance when asked to offer extra duties on parenting interventions. However, owing to their formal recognition in the communities and by the governmental health structures, their extensive experience and accrued training in the cross-cutting aspects of child and maternal health (77, 80), formally trained health paraprofessionals like community health volunteers are likely to present a more sustainable choice of delivery agents of parenting interventions.
Our findings on capacity building and fidelity/quality control measures utilized among the interventions have important implications. First, current studies on parenting interventions inconsistently report about core attributes (content, methods, duration, trainers) of training programs for intervention delivery agents. Although there is likely to be extensive variation in training content and duration, partly due to previous formal training possessed by the paraprofessionals, it is important to have some basic/foundational skills and knowledge areas (modules) for improved scalability and quality assurance of parenting interventions across settings. We also argue that the brevity of the training (most without refresher training) of delivery agents in many of the interventions was because most of these were brief interventions lasting less than 12 months. Second, a number of useful and transferable quality assurance techniques can be borrowed for future interventions. Notable among these is the process monitoring using designed monitoring tools like fidelity checklists, support supervision and on-the-job mentoring, maintaining ongoing open feedback and process learning, and involving beneficiaries in appraising the implementation process. Third, the current evidence indicates the need for more implementation research and better data to determine which features (i.e., training, skills, supervision, remuneration) predict program effectiveness and efficiency. Besides, clearer documentation of the formative and implementation aspects, including the sources of intervention components and the curriculum/intervention adaptation procedures, are crucial for improving scalability and adaptation to new settings and the comparability of study findings across different settings.
The major strengths of this systematic review include the expansion of the existing body of evidence on stimulation and responsive caregiving interventions in LMICs by including the most recent studies published in this area, expansion of the domains to include domains of social-emotional development and the thorough synthesis of the intervention and implementation features, including quantitative analysis of the moderation effects of various intervention features on the intervention effect sizes on childhood development outcomes. Our findings should be interpreted cautiously due to several limitations that should be noted. First, we only included a small body of randomized controlled trials and thus excluded interventions that were evaluated in other less rigorous designs (e.g., quasi-experimental) and those that did not assess child developmental outcomes. Nonetheless, the excluded interventions may still contribute valuable information to the field (22, 81). Second, the overlap in the classification of the sub-categories of the moderators presents challenges in delineating the specific impact of the moderator factor on the effectiveness of the intervention. For example, although the number of sessions (<12 vs. ≥12), age of the child at enrolment (<12 months vs. ≥12 months), study context (Rural vs. Urban/Peri-Urban), dosage (≤ 2 times/month vs. > 2 times/month), duration of training for delivery agents, among other factors, are classified into simplified binary or categorical variables, there are variations within these sub-categorizations which can have varying moderation on the overall effectiveness of the interventions. Nonetheless, such broader categorizations, as in the case of our analysis, are feasible given the limited number of studies currently available within this field. Although we conducted meta-analysis only for neurodevelopmental outcomes with 5 or more studies, we found that for the moderation analysis, some of the sub-categories of the moderator variables had fewer than 5 studies. This was specific for the moderators on delivery agents in both motor and language sub-domains, for duration of the intervention (in the language domain) and maternal education (in the motor domain). This may potentially attribute to the statistical non-significance of these moderators. Besides, there are additional factors that are theoretically important in explaining the heterogenous effect sizes but could not be evaluated with the limited evidence base (82). Examples include the timing (e.g., from birth vs. 12 months), the level of adaptation of intervention curriculums and assessment tools to the local context, and the quality of implementation (e.g., fidelity, training, supervision). Nonetheless, we have narratively summarized in more detail some of these implementation features and draw lessons for future research. Third, we did not conduct meta-regression to assess the unique effect of each moderator on intervention effect sizes while controlling for other moderators, owing to a small number of studies. Instead, we computed effect sizes separately for each moderator and therefore, the estimates should be interpreted with caution because overlap among the moderators was not accounted for. It is plausible that in an adjusted moderation analysis (i.e., when more than one moderator is included in the same model), statistical significance may shift and become attributable to other moderating factors than those currently identified in the un-adjusted analyses. Therefore, in the future where more studies are available in this field, adjusted meta-regression will be more informative in elucidating the implication of intervention characteristics on the effectiveness of parenting interventions with components of psychosocial stimulation or responsive caregiving on cognitive, language, motor and socio-emotional skills development of children.
Conclusion
Notwithstanding the limitations noted above, this review provided a valuable synthesis of the evidence on parenting interventions for children under age two in low- and middle-income countries. In spite of the promising effects, this is still a small area with significant potential to grow. We noted several directions that warrant further investigation. First, inadequate nurturing care often co-occurs with other risks in the environment, such as maternal mental health problems, lack of social support, and exposure to violence within the family and community. It may be beneficial to explore integrative models that combine nurturing care with other interventions that address environmental risk factors. One example of such models comes from rural Uganda, where researchers incorporated modules promoting paternal involvement and maternal mental health into a nurturing care intervention (46). Second, in spite of the converging evidence for short-term benefits on early child development, there is still a dearth of follow-up studies examining the extent to which demonstrated effects sustain later in development. Evidence from a small-scale intervention of stunted 9- to 24-month children in Jamaica suggested promising long-term benefits on educational, social-emotional, and economic outcomes at 22 years of age (22, 83, 84). In contrast, follow-up studies from two more recent RCT trials of children from rural Pakistan (39, 85) and urban Colombia (40, 86) produced mixed findings on whether the intervention effects are sustained 2 years after the intervention. Clearly, more research is needed to better understand the long-term benefits of early interventions and whether booster interventions at a later stage are needed. Third, despite the overall positive effect for children who received early interventions, the mechanisms (i.e., why the intervention works) and specificities (i.e., in what contexts and for whom the intervention works more or less) the effectiveness are still not well-understood. Only a small number of studies examined the mechanisms (e.g., maternal sensitivity, home stimulation) through which the intervention had an impact on child outcomes (46, 87, 88). Researchers have just begun to explore child and family characteristics (e.g., biological sensitivity, socioeconomic status) that may account for the differential intervention effects (58, 89). In light of this, we call for more investigations in this arena using existing data and future research.
Finally, nurturing care interventions in LMICs have been mainly implemented in low-income populations and populations at risk for undernutrition and rarely in other adverse contexts, such as those affected by armed conflicts and forced migration. In recent years, researchers have brought attention to the value of nurturing care interventions in fragile and conflict contexts and their potential to reduce violence and promote peace (4, 90). The Early Childhood Peace Consortium is one such innovative approach aimed at peacebuilding through investment in early child development (91). Although this work is still in the infancy stage, preliminary evidence has shown a promising effect on violence reduction and child development (9, 92). With an eye toward the future, more research with more detailed documentation of the intervention and implementation features is needed to understand what works, what contexts, and why in early parenting interventions.
Data Availability Statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.
Author Contributions
The idea of this review was envisaged by KM, M-CM, TM, and SL. LZ and DS developed and ran the search strategies, screened the titles and abstracts, as well as the full texts, and assessed the methodological quality. LZ, TM, KP, M-CM, AA, and GM developed the protocol. LZ, DS, and JM extracted data. KP, M-CM, AA, TM, GM, KM, and SL provided technical advice. LZ and DS drafted the manuscript with input from KP, M-CM, AA, KM, and TM. All authors have contributed significantly by taking part in reading, writing, and reviewing this manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by funding from the International Development Research Centre (IDRC) and Aga Khan Foundation Canada (AKFC) through the project on Improving Early Childhood Development and Well-Being in Refugee and Other Marginalized Communities (Centre File: 108506-002). The funding bodies did not play any role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing this manuscript.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2021.671988/full#supplementary-material
References
1. Black MM, Walker SP, Fernald LC, Andersen CT, DiGirolamo AM, Lu C, et al. Early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course. Lancet. (2017) 389:77–90. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31389-7
2. Daelmans B, Darmstadt GL, Lombardi J, Black MM, Britto PR, Lye S, et al. Early childhood development: the foundation of sustainable development. Lancet. (2017) 389:9–11. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31659-2
3. Heckman JJ. Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science. (2006) 312:1900–2. doi: 10.1126/science.1128898
4. Leckman JF, Britto PR. Steps toward peace and violence prevention across generations: the potential of early child development in the context of the 2030 sustainable development goals. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. (2018) 2018:5–12. doi: 10.1002/cad.20227
5. Cusick SE, Georgieff MK. The role of nutrition in brain development: the golden opportunity of the “first 1000 days”. J Pediatr. (2016) 175:16–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.05.013
6. Shonkoff JP, Garner AS, Siegel BS, Dobbins MI, Earls MF, McGuinn L, et al. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics. (2012) 129:e232–46. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2663
7. Wachs TD, Georgieff M, Cusick S, McEwen BS. Issues in the timing of integrated early interventions: contributions from nutrition, neuroscience, and psychological research. Ann NY Acad Sci. (2014) 1:89–106. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12314
8. Black RE, Alderman H, Bhutta ZA, Gillespie S, Haddad L, Horton S, et al. Maternal and child nutrition: building momentum for impact. Lancet. (2013) 382:372–5. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)60988-5
9. Richter LM, Lye SJ, Proulx K. Nurturing care for young children under conditions of fragility and conflict. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. (2018) 2018:13–26. doi: 10.1002/cad.20232
10. Britto PR, Lye SJ, Proulx K, Yousafzai AK, Matthews SG, Vaivada T, et al. Nurturing care: promoting early childhood development. Lancet. (2017) 389:91–102. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31390-3
11. Engle PL, Fernald LC, Alderman H, Behrman J, O'Gara C, Yousafzai A, et al. Strategies for reducing inequalities and improving developmental outcomes for young children in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet. (2011) 378:1339–53. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60889-1
12. Eshel N, Daelmans B, Mello MCd, Martines J. Responsive parenting: interventions and outcomes. Bull World Health Organ. (2006) 84:991–8. doi: 10.2471/BLT.06.030163
13. WHO. Mental Health and Psychosocial Well–Being Among Children in Severe Food Shortage Situations. Geneva (2006).
14. WHO, UNICEF, World Bank Group. Nurturing Care for Early Childhood Development: A Framework for Helping Children Survive and Thrive to Transform Health and Human Potential. Geneva (2018).
15. Pan American Health Organization. Guiding Principles for Complementary Feeding of the Breastfed Child. Washington, DC (2003).
16. Black MM, Aboud FE. Responsive feeding is embedded in a theoretical framework of responsive parenting. J Nutr. (2011)141:490–4. doi: 10.3945/jn.110.129973
17. Prado EL, Larson LM, Cox K, Bettencourt K, Kubes JN, Shankar AH. Do effects of early life interventions on linear growth correspond to effects on neurobehavioural development? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Glob Health. (2019) 7:e1398–413. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(19)30361-4
18. Bentley ME, Johnson SL, Wasser H, Creed-Kanashiro H, Shroff M, Fernandez-Rao S, Cunningham M. Formative research methods for designing culturally appropriate, integrated child nutrition and development interventions: an overview. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2014) 1308:54. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12290
19. Jeong J, Pitchik HO, Yousafzai AK. Stimulation interventions and parenting in low-and middle-income countries: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. (2018) 141:e20173510. doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-3510
20. Jeong J, Pitchik HO, Fink G. Short-term, medium-term and long-term effects of early parenting interventions in low-and middle-income countries: a systematic review. BMJ Glob Health. (2021) 6:e004067. doi: 10.1136/bmjgh-2020-004067
21. Aboud FE, Yousafzai AK. Global health and development in early childhood. Annu Rev Psychol. (2015) 66:433–57. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015128
22. Grantham-McGregor SM, Fernald L, Kagawa R, Walker S. Effects of integrated child development and nutrition interventions on child development and nutritional status. Ann NY Acad Sci. (2014)1308:11–32. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12284
23. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, The PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. (2009) 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097
24. Higgins JP, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Jüni P, Moher D, Oxman AD, et al. The Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. (2011) 343:d5928. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d5928
25. Harvey PD. Domains of cognition and their assessment. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2019) 21:227. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2019.21.3/pharvey
26. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development (Bsid-II). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. (1993).
27. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development® 3rd edition (Bayley-III®). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (2006). doi: 10.1037/t14978-000
28. Griffiths R. The Griffiths Mental Development Scales From Birth to 2 Years. Manual. The 1996 revision Huntley: Association for research in infant and child development (1996). doi: 10.1037/t03301-000
29. Harris M. Language Experience and Early Language Development: From Input to Uptake. London: Psychology Press (2013). doi: 10.4324/9780203765265
30. Adolph KE, Franchak JM. The development of motor behavior. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. (2017) 8:e1430. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1430
31. Wolke D, Skuse D, Mathisen B. Behavioral style in failure-to-thrive infants: a preliminary communication. J Pediatr Psychol. (1990) 15:237–54. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/15.2.237
32. Godoy L, Chavez AE, Mack RA, Carter AS. Rating Scales for Social-Emotional Behavior and Development. Clinical Guide to Psychiatric Assessment of Infants and Young Children. Cham: Springer (2019) p. 217–51. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-10635-5_7
33. Darling-Churchill KE, Lippman L. Early childhood social and emotional development: advancing the field of measurement. J Appl Dev Psychol. (2016) 45:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2016.02.002
34. Rosnow RL, Rosenthal R. Effect sizes for experimenting psychologists. Can J Exp Psychol. (2003) 57:221. doi: 10.1037/h0087427
35. Hedges LV. Distribution theory for Glass's estimator of effect size and related estimators. J Educ Stat. (1981) 6:107–28. doi: 10.3102/10769986006002107
36. Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JP, Rothstein HR. Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons (2011).
37. Harris RJ, Deeks JJ, Altman DG, Bradburn MJ, Harbord RM, Sterne JA. Metan: fixed-and random-effects meta-analysis. Stata J. (2008) 8:3–28. doi: 10.1177/1536867X0800800102
38. DerSimonian R, Kacker R. Random-effects model for meta-analysis of clinical trials: an update. Contemp Clin Trials. (2007) 28:105–14. doi: 10.1016/j.cct.2006.04.004
39. Yousafzai AK, Rasheed MA, Rizvi A, Armstrong R, Bhutta ZA. Effect of integrated responsive stimulation and nutrition interventions in the Lady Health Worker programme in Pakistan on child development, growth, and health outcomes: a cluster-randomised factorial effectiveness trial. Lancet. (2014) 384:1282–93. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60455-4
40. Attanasio OP, Fernández C, Fitzsimons EO, Grantham-McGregor SM, Meghir C, Rubio-Codina M. Using the infrastructure of a conditional cash transfer program to deliver a scalable integrated early child development program in Colombia: cluster randomized controlled trial. BMJ. (2014) 349:g5785. doi: 10.1136/bmj.g5785
41. Chang SM, Grantham-McGregor SM, Powell CA, Vera-Hernández M, Lopez-Boo F, Baker-Henningham H, Walker SP. Integrating a parenting intervention with routine primary health care: a cluster randomized trial. Pediatrics. (2015) 136:272–80. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-0119
42. Muhoozi GK, Atukunda P, Diep LM, Mwadime R, Kaaya AN, Skaare AB, et al. Nutrition, hygiene, and stimulation education to improve growth, cognitive, language, and motor development among infants in Uganda: a cluster-randomized trial. Matern Child Nutr. (2018) 14:e12527. doi: 10.1111/mcn.12527
43. Hamadani JD, Huda SN, Khatun F, Grantham-McGregor SM. Psychosocial stimulation improves the development of undernourished children in rural Bangladesh. J Nutr. (2006) 136:2645–52. doi: 10.1093/jn/136.10.2645
44. Nahar B, Hossain MI, Hamadani JD, Ahmed T, Huda SN, Grantham-McGregor SM, Persson LA. Effects of a community-based approach of food and psychosocial stimulation on growth and development of severely malnourished children in Bangladesh: a randomised trial. Eur J Clin Nutr. (2012) 66:701–9. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2012.13
45. Tofail F, Hamadani JD, Mehrin F, Ridout DA, Huda SN, Grantham-McGregor SM. Psychosocial stimulation benefits development in nonanemic children but not in anemic, iron-deficient children. J Nutr. (2013) 143:885–93. doi: 10.3945/jn.112.160473
46. Singla D, Kumbakumba E, Aboud F. A randomised cluster evaluation of a parenting programme to address child development and maternal wellbeing in Uganda. Lancet Glob Health. (2014) 2:S44. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(15)70066-5
47. Vazir S, Engle P, Balakrishna N, Griffiths PL, Johnson SL, Creed-Kanashiro H, et al. Cluster-randomized trial on complementary and responsive feeding education to caregivers found improved dietary intake, growth and development among rural Indian toddlers. Matern Child Nutr. (2013) 9:99–117. doi: 10.1111/j.1740-8709.2012.00413.x
48. Aboud FE, Akhter S. A cluster-randomized evaluation of a responsive stimulation and feeding intervention in Bangladesh. Pediatrics. (2011) 127:e1191–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-2160
49. Rockers PC, Fink G, Zanolini A, Banda B, Biemba G, Sullivan C, et al. Impact of a community-based package of interventions on child development in Zambia: a cluster-randomised controlled trial. BMJ Glob. Health. (2016) 1:3. doi: 10.1136/bmjgh-2016-000104
50. Gardner JMM, Powell CA, Baker-Henningham H, Walker SP, Cole TJ, Grantham-McGregor SM. Zinc supplementation and psychosocial stimulation: effects on the development of undernourished Jamaican children–. Am J Clin Nutr. (2005) 82:399–405. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/82.2.399
51. Powell C, Baker-Henningham H, Walker S, Gernay J, Grantham-McGregor S. Feasibility of integrating early stimulation into primary care for undernourished Jamaican children: cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ. (2004) 329:89. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38132.503472.7C
52. Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA, Grantham-McGregor SM. Psychosocial intervention improves the development of term low-birth-weight infants. J Nutr. (2004) 134:1417–23. doi: 10.1093/jn/134.6.1417
53. Jin X, Sun Y, Jiang F, Ma J, Morgan C, Shen X. “Care for Development” intervention in rural China: a prospective follow-up study. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2007) 28:213–8. doi: 10.1097/dbp.0b013e31802d410b
54. Aboud FE, Singla DR, Nahil MI, Borisova I. Effectiveness of a parenting program in Bangladesh to address early childhood health, growth and development. Soc Sci Med. (2013) 97:250–8. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.06.020
55. Abimpaye M, Dusabe C, Nzabonimpa JP, Ashford R, Pisani L. Improving parenting practices and development for young children in Rwanda: results from a randomized control trial. Int J Behav Dev. (2020) 44:205–15. doi: 10.1177/0165025419861173
56. Luoto JE, Garcia IL, Aboud FE, Singla DR, Fernald LC, Pitchik HO, et al. Group-based parenting interventions to promote child development in rural Kenya: a multi-arm, cluster-randomised community effectiveness trial. Lancet Glob Health. (2020) 9:e309–19. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(20)30469-1
57. Barnhart DA, Farrar J, Murray SM, Brennan RT, Antonaccio CM, Sezibera V, et al. Lay-worker delivered home visiting promotes early childhood development and reduces violence in Rwanda: a randomized pilot. J Child Fam Stud. (2020) 29:1804–17. doi: 10.1007/s10826-020-01709-1
58. Murray L, Cooper P, Arteche A, Stein A, Tomlinson M. Randomized controlled trial of a home-visiting intervention on infant cognitive development in peri-urban South Africa. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2016) 58:270–6. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12873
59. Attanasio O, Baker-Henningham H, Bernal R, Meghir C, Pineda D, Rubio-Codina M. Early Stimulation and Nutrition: The Impacts of a Scalable Intervention. National Bureau of Economic Research. Report No.: 0898-2937 (2018). doi: 10.3386/w25059
60. Bricker D, Squires J, Mounts L, Potter L, Nickel R, Twombly E, Farrell J. Ages and Stages Questionnaire. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. (1999).
61. Yousafzai AK, Aboud F. Review of implementation processes for integrated nutrition and psychosocial stimulation interventions. Ann NY Acad Sci. (2014) 1308:33–45. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12313
62. Jackson D, Turner R. Power analysis for random-effects meta-analysis. Res Synth Methods. (2017) 8:290–302. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1240
63. Becker BE, Luthar SS. Social-emotional factors affecting achievement outcomes among disadvantaged students: closing the achievement gap. Educ Psychol. (2002) 37:197–214. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3704_1
64. Ellis BJ, Bianchi J, Griskevicius V, Frankenhuis WE. Beyond risk and protective factors: an adaptation-based approach to resilience. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2017)12:561–87. doi: 10.1177/1745691617693054
65. Jones DE, Greenberg M, Crowley M. Early social-emotional functioning and public health: the relationship between kindergarten social competence and future wellness. Am J Public Health. (2015) 105:2283–90. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2015.302630
66. Malti T, Song J-H. Social-Emotional Development and Aggression. In: Rubin TMKH, editor. Handbook of Child and Adolescent Aggression. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2018). p. 127–44.
67. Blair C, Raver CC. Child development in the context of adversity: experiential canalization of brain and behavior. Am Psychol. (2012) 67:309. doi: 10.1037/a0027493
68. Malti T. Children and Violence. Social Policy Report, Society for Research in Child Development (in press).
69. Feldman R. Sensitive periods in human social development: new insights from research on oxytocin, synchrony, and high-risk parenting. Dev Psychopathol. (2015) 27:369–95. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000048
70. Feldman R, Vengrober A, Eidelman-Rothman M, Zagoory-Sharon O. Stress reactivity in war-exposed young children with and without posttraumatic stress disorder: relations to maternal stress hormones, parenting, and child emotionality and regulation. Dev Psychopathol. (2013) 25:943–55. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000291
71. Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR. Responsive parenting: establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Dev Psychopathol. (2006) 42:627. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.627
72. Malti T, Eisenberg N, Kim H, Buchmann M. Developmental trajectories of sympathy, moral emotion attributions, and moral reasoning: the role of parental support. Soc Dev. (2013) 22:773–93. doi: 10.1111/sode.12031
73. Page M, Wilhelm MS, Gamble WC, Card NA. A comparison of maternal sensitivity and verbal stimulation as unique predictors of infant social–emotional and cognitive development. Infant Behav Dev. (2010) 33:101–10. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.12.001
74. Weisleder A, Cates CB, Dreyer BP, Johnson SB, Huberman HS, Seery AM, et al. Promotion of positive parenting and prevention of socioemotional disparities. Pediatrics. (2016) 137:2. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-3239
75. Brady N, Warren SF, Sterling A. Interventions aimed at improving child language by improving maternal responsivity. Int Rev Res Dev Disabil. (2009) 37:333–57. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7750(09)37010-X
76. Emerson E, Brigham P. Exposure of children with developmental delay to social determinants of poor health: cross-sectional case record review study. Child. (2015) 41:249–57. doi: 10.1111/cch.12144
77. Lewin S, Munabi-Babigumira S, Glenton C, Daniels K, Bosch-Capblanch X, Van Wyk BE, et al. Lay health workers in primary and community health care for maternal and child health and the management of infectious diseases. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2010) 3: CD004015. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004015.pub3
78. Rahman SM, Ali NA, Jennings L, Seraji MHR, Mannan I, Shah R, et al. Factors affecting recruitment and retention of community health workers in a newborn care intervention in Bangladesh. Hum Resour Health. (2010) 8:1–14. doi: 10.1186/1478-4491-8-12
79. Jaskiewicz W, Tulenko K. Increasing community health worker productivity and effectiveness: a review of the influence of the work environment. Hum Resour Health. (2012) 10:38. doi: 10.1186/1478-4491-10-38
80. Kok MC, Ormel H, Broerse JE, Kane S, Namakhoma I, Otiso L, et al. Optimising the benefits of community health workers' unique position between communities and the health sector: a comparative analysis of factors shaping relationships in four countries. Glob Public Health. (2017) 12:1404–32. doi: 10.1080/17441692.2016.1174722
81. Rempel LA, Rempel JK, Khuc TN, Vui LT. Influence of father–infant relationship on infant development: a father-involvement intervention in Vietnam. Dev Psychol. (2017) 53:1844. doi: 10.1037/dev0000390
82. Aboud FE, Yousafzai AK, Nores M. State of the science on implementation research in early child development and future directions. Ann NY Acad Sci. (2018) 1419:264–71. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13722
83. Gertler P, Heckman J, Pinto R, Zanolini A, Vermeersch C, Walker S, et al. Labor market returns to an early childhood stimulation intervention in Jamaica. Science. (2014) 344:998–1001. doi: 10.1126/science.1251178
84. Walker SP, Chang SM, Vera-Hernández M, Grantham-McGregor S. Early childhood stimulation benefits adult competence and reduces violent behavior. Pediatrics. (2011) 127:849–57. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-2231
85. Yousafzai AK, Obradović J, Rasheed MA, Rizvi A, Portilla XA, Tirado-Strayer N, et al. Effects of responsive stimulation and nutrition interventions on children's development and growth at age 4 years in a disadvantaged population in Pakistan: a longitudinal follow-up of a cluster-randomised factorial effectiveness trial. Lancet Glob Health. (2016) 4:e548–58. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(16)30100-0
86. Andrew A, Attanasio O, Fitzsimons E, Grantham-McGregor S, Meghir C, Rubio-Codina M. Impacts 2 years after a scalable early childhood development intervention to increase psychosocial stimulation in the home: a follow-up of a cluster randomised controlled trial in Colombia. PLoS Med. (2018) 15:e1002556. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002556
87. Brown N, Finch J, Obradović J, Yousafzai A. Maternal care mediates the effects of nutrition and responsive stimulation interventions on young children's growth. Child. (2017) 43:577–87. doi: 10.1111/cch.12466
88. Obradović J, Yousafzai AK, Finch JE, Rasheed MA. Maternal scaffolding and home stimulation: key mediators of early intervention effects on children's cognitive development. Dev Psychol. (2016) 52:1409. doi: 10.1037/dev0000182
89. Morgan B, Kumsta R, Fearon P, Moser D, Skeen S, Cooper P, et al. Serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) polymorphism and susceptibility to a home-visiting maternal-infant attachment intervention delivered by community health workers in South Africa: reanalysis of a randomized controlled trial. PLoS Med. (2017) 14:e1002237. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002237
90. Tracy M, Salo M, Appleton AA. The mitigating effects of maternal social support and paternal involvement on the intergenerational transmission of violence. Child Abuse Negl. (2018) 78:46–59. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.09.023
91. Salah R. Global citizenship and the role of the United Nations: the promise of the Early Childhood Peace Consortium. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. (2018) 2018:99–105. doi: 10.1002/cad.20231
Keywords: parenting intervention, stimulation, early childhood development, low- and middle-income countries, systematic review
Citation: Zhang L, Ssewanyana D, Martin M-C, Lye S, Moran G, Abubakar A, Marfo K, Marangu J, Proulx K and Malti T (2021) Supporting Child Development Through Parenting Interventions in Low- to Middle-Income Countries: An Updated Systematic Review. Front. Public Health 9:671988. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.671988
Received: 24 February 2021; Accepted: 15 June 2021;
Published: 16 July 2021.
Edited by:
Jane Fisher, Monash University, AustraliaReviewed by:
Dana McCoy, Harvard Graduate School of Education, United StatesSatinder Aneja, Sharda University, India
Copyright © 2021 Zhang, Ssewanyana, Martin, Lye, Moran, Abubakar, Marfo, Marangu, Proulx and Malti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Derrick Ssewanyana, c3Nld2FueWFuYSYjeDAwMDQwO2x1bmVuZmVsZC5jYQ==
†These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship