- Department of Psychology, Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience, Paris-Lodron-University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria
Introduction: Coronavirus 2019 (COVID-19) quickly evolved into a global pandemic in early 2020, and most countries enforced social confinements to reduce transmission. This seems to dovetail with increasing, potentially problematic, screen use habits, such as gaming and “binge-watching.” Yet, the subjective experience of the common confinements may vary not only between individuals depending on age, sex, and living conditions (i.e., living alone) but also within individuals from day to day: confinements might interfere with habitual activity schedules more strongly on some days than on others. Such dynamic confinement experience has not been studied in relation to screen use yet but might guide targeted intervention.
Method: In total, 102 participants (n = 83 female, n = 80 university students) completed 14 days of ecological momentary assessment during a COVID-19-related lockdown in Germany and Austria. Each evening, they indicated the extent to which they felt restricted by confinements in their social and work lives and whether they engaged in unusually high and intense levels of television watching, social media use, news consumption, internet surfing, and gaming. They also reported on how much they experienced their day to be structured.
Results: Experienced work confinements were positively associated with social media usage. Further, work confinements were positively associated with gaming in males and with news consumption, especially in individuals living alone. Social confinements were positively associated with watching television especially in younger participants and with social media consumption in younger participants. Higher experienced day structure was related to less television watching, gaming, and internet surfing but more news consumption.
Discussion: Screen use behaviors increased with higher confinements within person, dependent on sex, age, and living situation. Such knowledge allows tailoring on the person level (who should be addressed?) and the time level (when should interventions be scheduled?) as the negative consequences of excessive screen use behaviors on mental and physical health are well-documented. One potential low-threshold intervention might be day-structuring.
Introduction
Throughout 2020, the coronavirus 2019 (COVID-19) evolved into a global pandemic with a negative impact on physical and psychological health [e.g., increased anxiety and depression; (1)]. To slow down the spreading of the coronavirus and to stabilize overstrained healthcare systems, most countries enforced partial lockdowns and confinements on social interaction. However, these lockdowns were associated with higher levels of stress, anxiety, and depression especially in younger individuals (2).
Besides having to deal with the uncertainty about the possible consequences of COVID-19 infections, such as medical complications, citizens were faced with novel situations as they experienced a loss of their usual routine and had to adapt to social and work confinements (e.g., reduced social contact, home-office, and -schooling). This might cause boredom, frustration, and feelings of isolation (3). Additionally, the reduction of recreational outdoor activities and social contact limits the available sources for habitually used positive reinforcement and thus protective factors of psychological health (4–6). To sum, these deprivations in concert with the breakdown of daily routines experienced during COVID-19-related confinements have the potential to increase reinforcing (indoor) behaviors that are still accessible and easily available. To illustrate, it has previously been shown that potentially problematic behaviors, such as increased consumption of food (7, 8), alcohol (9), or cannabis (10), become more likely during COVID-19-related confinements.
Another potential source of easily accessible and highly reinforcing activities, especially in highly technologized societies, may be intense screen use behaviors, such as watching television, gaming, internet surfing, or social media usage (11, 12). To exemplify, studies showed that the overall screen time increased during lockdown in children and adolescents (13, 14), as well as in office workers and students (15–19). More specifically, recent studies reported increased screen use habits, such as gaming (20), watching television, or even binge-watching (7, 21, 22), as well as social media use [e.g., (21, 23, 24)], during COVID-19-related confinements (8).
Such excessive screen use behaviors can be associated with negative effects on psychological well-being during COVID-19-related confinements: students were negatively affected in their sleep quality, sleep duration, physical well-being, and mental health by excessive screen time (15); increased social media use was associated with a greater tendency to be diagnosed with depression or anxiety (24); finally, more time consuming news led to higher levels of anxiety and stress (3, 25). Moreover, individuals during adolescence and young adulthood may be especially vulnerable to develop excessive, impulsive–compulsive screen use behaviors corresponding to the concept of “behavioral addiction” (11, 26). Hence, examining screen use behaviors during COVID-19 confinements seems important to prevent the negative health outcomes mentioned above.
The COVID-19 situation and related confinements have been very dynamic with new regulations introduced almost on a daily basis. Moreover, within individuals, the subjective experience of these objective confinements might have varied significantly from day to day: individuals may have experienced confinements as more impacting on days where they used to engage in activities that are now restricted (e.g., outdoor recreation, social gatherings on weekends, or work meetings on work days). Similarly, the confinements might have affected different life areas (e.g., individuals may experience more work-related confinements during the week but suffer more from social confinements on the weekend, depending on their usual routines). This creates much variability within individuals (i.e., day-to-day variability in experienced confinements). As some new findings showed, such situational factors [i.e., varying degrees of experienced confinements; (18)] may contribute to increased screen use. Hence, it may be most appropriate to assess various perceived COVID-19-related confinements and screen use on a daily basis using ecological momentary assessment (EMA) that accounts for this within-person variance.
Additionally, between individuals, confinements may have different effects depending on different professions or living situations (e.g., some individuals stopped working or lost their job, whereas others had to work in home-office). Thus, also between-person variables need to be acknowledged: demographic and environmental factors, i.e., age, sex, or living alone, have been linked to excessive screen use [i.e., online gamers typically are male, young, university graduates, and live alone; (27)].
It has been shown that the subjective experience of social isolation is as likely to predict negative effects on well-being, compared with objective social isolation (28). Thus, we focused on the subjective experienced degree of social and work confinements from day to day and their association with increases of screen use behaviors, but additionally assessed whether the participants lived alone or together with others, as individuals who lived alone might have been objectively more isolated during the lockdown period.
On that background, the present naturalistic study examined the relationship of daily varying experiences of COVID-19-related confinements with screen use behaviors, as well as the moderating roles of demographic, environmental, and situational factors, in this relationship across 14 days of day level EMA. Based on the literature reviewed above, it was hypothesized that increased subjective work and social confinements would be associated with an increased probability for screen use behaviors (television watching, social media usage, internet surfing, gaming, and news consumption) within person. Additionally, we hypothesized that age, sex, and living situation (alone or with others) may moderate the increase of different screen use behaviors with regard to the experience of increased confinements. As it was recommended that a more structured daily routine should be followed to avoid excessive engagement in screen use behaviors (12), we hypothesized that days marked by a more structured daily routine would be negatively associated with the probability for increased engagement in screen use behaviors on that day.
Materials and Methods
Participants
In total, 102 participants (n = 83 female) were included. The participants were recruited via social media postings and mailing lists in Germany and Austria. All participants completed the study during a COVID-19-related lockdown (from March to May 2020) in both countries. Participants were recruited for an EMA study on the influence of COVID-19-related confinements on eating behavior. The sample mainly consisted of university students (78.4%), followed by employees (15.7%) and few self-employed, homemakers, pupils, and retirees (5.9%). Thus, mainly young adults participated in the study (age: M = 25.5, SD = 9.20, range 18–71 years; 25th percentile 20.0, 75th percentile 26.3 years). Most participants reported to live with others (n = 86), whereas the rest lived alone (n = 16). The ethics committee of the University of Salzburg approved the study, and all participants signed an informed consent form approved by the ethics committee of the University of Salzburg.
Measures
Sociodemographic Measures
Participants reported sociodemographic data via an online questionnaire (i.e., sex, age, whether they live alone or together with others, and other unrelated measures).
EMA Measures
Participants completed five EMA signals a day, repeatedly asking about emotions, stress, eating behavior, and other variables that are not of interest for the present study. At the last signal of each day, participants indicated the extent to which they felt restricted by confinements in their (a) social and (b) work lives via visual analog scales in the form of continuous rating sliders (“How much did you feel confined in your social life today?” and “How much did you feel confined in your work life today?”: from not at all [0] to very much [100]). Further, they reported whether they engaged, more than they usually do, in one or several of five screen use behaviors (“Did you engage in one or more of the following activities in your leisure time today? In comparison with usual intensified and increased.” television watching, social media use, news consumption, internet surfing, and gaming: yes or no for each screen use behavior). Finally, the participants reported how much they experienced their day to be structured (“How structured was your day?”: from not at all [0] to very much [100]).
Procedure
At the start of the study, all participants signed the informed consent form and completed an online questionnaire asking about the abovementioned sociodemographic factors. Via phone, participants were instructed on how to install and use the customized EMA application PsyDiary. The duration of the EMA protocol lasted for 14 days, with five signal-contingent prompts per day1. At the last prompt of each day (at 09:00 pm), participants answered the questions regarding confinements in social and work lives, engagement in unusually high and intense levels of screen use behaviors, and the experienced day structure. In general, participants could delay the signal response for up to 1 h while later responses were treated as missing. All participants were compensated for their participation with 3–5 course credits (depending on their EMA compliance) and a personalized feedback of their data.
Statistical Analyses
A multilevel modeling (MLM) approach was used to account for nesting of within-person (prompts, level 1, e.g., day-to-day variation in experienced confinement) and between-person (individuals, level 2, e.g., sociodemographic data) variance. Thus, social and work confinements, as well as screen use behaviors, were modeled as level 1 variables, whereas sex, age, and living situation (i.e., living alone or together with others) were modeled as level 2 variables. To account for the binomial distribution of the outcome measures (i.e., reports of increased television watching, social media use, news consumption, gaming, and internet surfing), the Bernoulli–MLM models were used. Level 1 variables (social and work confinements and day structure) were person-mean centered (centered within person), and continuous level 2 variables (age) were grand-mean centered (centered around the group mean).
In a first step, MLM Null-models (including only a random intercept for participants) were tested for significance. Significance of these tests indicated a nested data structure. Thus, MLMs with random effect structure were preferable to general linear regression models. Therefore, random intercepts and random slopes for each participant were added to the fixed factors (work and social confinements), to model variance between and within individuals.
To account for the expected moderating roles of sex, age, and living situation on certain screen use behaviors, additional interaction models were conducted. To exemplify, increased gaming was modeled as a dependent variable with COVID-19-related confinements, and sex and their interaction as independent variables. For all outcomes, additional models with multilevel interactions separately including sex, age and living situation were calculated. All MLMs were setup with nested random effect structure (30) and analyzed in HLM7 (31).
Additionally, we used Rstudio (32) and the packages lme4 (33) and nlme (34) to recalculate our models and test whether all assumptions of MLMs were met for our data. Linearity of the data was checked upon visual inspection by plotting the residuals of each model vs. the observed outcome values. Homogeneity of residual variance was checked by a variation of the Levene's test: the residual variance from each participant was extracted, and an ANOVA of the between subject residuals was calculated (for each model). Normal distribution of residuals was checked upon visual inspection of Q–Q plots of the random effects of each model.
Data from n = 3 participants were excluded from the analyses due to insufficient data quantity, as they answered <50% of all EMA prompts.
Results
Descriptive Data
In total, 1,257 EMA evening prompts were answered and used for analyses. On average, 12.2 out of 14 EMA prompts per participant were answered, equaling to a good compliance of 87.4% (SD = 13.1, range 50–100%). On average, participants reported mild to moderate confinements in their social (M = 33.2, SD = 28.1, range 0–100) and work lives (M = 24.4, SD = 30.0, range 0–100) and moderately structured days (M = 49.7, SD = 27.5, range 0–100). Increased screen use behaviors compared with usual were reported, as can be seen in Table 1.
EMA Measures
Effects of Work Confinements2
Higher experienced work confinements were positively associated with a higher probability of social media usage within participants (β01 = 0.006, SE = 0.002, p = 0.004). An association of work confinements and reports of increased gaming (β01 = −0.003, SE = 0.001, p = 0.024) was moderated by sex, yielding a positive association of work confinements and increased gaming only in males (β11 = 0.006, SE = 0.002, p = 0.006), as can be seen in Figure 1A. Further, work confinements were positively associated with reports of news consumption, especially in participants who lived alone (β11 = −0.015, SE = 0.006, p = 0.009), as can be seen in Figure 1B. No further associations of work confinements and increases in other screen use behaviors were found, and no further interactions of work confinements and age, sex, or living situation were found regarding the different dependent variables of increased screen use behaviors.
Figure 1. From left to right: (A) simple slopes of the probability for increased gaming in relation to experience of work confinements moderated by sex, (B) simple slopes of the probability for increased news consumption in relation to experience of work confinements moderated by living situation. Scaling of the x-axis is based on the entire range of person-mean centered scores of work confinements.
Effects of Social Confinements2
Higher social confinements were positively associated with higher probability of reports of increased television watching within participants (β01 = 0.007, SE = 0.003, p = 0.019). This association was moderated by age so that it was stronger in younger participants (β11 = −0.001, SE = 0.0002, p = 0.004), as can be seen in Figure 2A. Social confinements were also significantly associated with higher probability of reports of increased social media consumption in younger than in older participants (β11 = −0.001, SE = 0.0002, p = 0.001), as can be seen in Figure 2B. No further associations of social confinements and increases in other screen use behaviors were found, and no further interactions of social confinements and age, sex, or living situation were found regarding the different dependent variables of increased screen use behaviors.
Figure 2. From left to right: (A) simple slopes of the probability for increased television watching in relation to experience of social confinements moderated by age, (B) simple slopes of the probability for increased social media usage in relation to experience of social confinements moderated by age. For visualization only, the 25th percentile (20.0 years) and 75th percentile (26.3 years) of the participants are displayed to highlight the interaction of age and social confinements. Scaling of the x-axis is based on the entire range of person-mean centered scores of social confinements.
All reported significant main effects of social and work confinements on increased screen use behaviors remained significant after combining social and work confinements into one model to control for shared variance. For detailed tables of all models and results, see Supplementary Materials.
Effects of Day Structure2
A higher experienced day structure was related to a lower probability of reports of increased television watching (β01 = −0.012, SE = 0.003, p < 0.001), gaming (β01 = −0.005, SE = 0.002, p = 0.014), and internet surfing (β01 = −0.006, SE = 0.002, p = 0.008) within participants, but to a higher probability news consumption within participants (β01 = 0.005, SE = 0.002, p = 0.049).
Discussion
The present study examined the impact of subjectively perceived work and social COVID-19-related confinements on the increase of different screen use behaviors in daily life using day level EMA across 14 days. Due to the highly dynamic COVID-19 situation and the fact that confinements might interfere with habitual activity schedules more strongly on some days than on others, we were explicitly interested in within-person relationships of these variables, but also examined the role of potential moderators of these associations, such as age, sex, and living alone vs. with others.
Results showed that participants reported increased screen use during leisure time, mostly social media and television watching, followed by news consumption, other internet usage, and gaming. In line with previous research, screen use increased during COVID-19-related confinements compared with usual every day conditions [e.g., (13–17, 19)]. Potentially, the low prevalence of increased gaming in our study is due to our mostly female sample, as previous research showed excessive gaming being prevalent mostly in male individuals (27). Indeed, increased gaming was reported by most male individuals in our sample but only few female participants, so that future studies might profit from a sample with a higher percentage of male individuals.
Increased screen use during COVID-19 may have positive and negative side effects. On the one hand, increased screen use may aid individuals in coping with the COVID-19 crisis. On the other hand, it may worsen psychological well-being. To illustrate, increased social media use might enable individuals during the COVID-19-related confinement to stay in contact with others and overcome social distancing (23), and increased news consumption may help individuals to stay informed and cope with COVID-19-related uncertainty (35, 36). Still, excessive screen use is related to decrease physiological and psychological well-being, and increased news consumption may even be related to greater fear about infection.
Moreover, our results showed that the effects of COVID-19-related confinements differ within individuals on a day-to-day basis as the subjectively experienced degree and the life domain of confinements vary: work confinements were positively associated with the probability of increased social media usage, whereas social confinements were positively associated with the probability of increased television watching within participants. This suggests that COVID-19-related confinements may not be seen as temporally stable or as an “all or nothing” factor, but significant day-to-day variations exist and those go along with variations in screen use. To exemplify, within-person confinements might interfere with personal recreational habits on one-day (i.e., leading to increased television watching) and with important job tasks on another day (i.e., leading to increased social media usage), yet on another day, the confinements may not interfere with any activities or duties at all (e.g., weekend day at home with family). Hence, instead of focusing on confinements as a dichotomous state (confined, non-confined), a more fine-grained assessment may be more appropriate for explaining screen use behavior and for intervening on it in case of problematic levels.
Regarding interventions, one potential protective factor might be a well-structured day, which has already been recommended by previous research (12): our data showed that the degree to which participants experienced their day to be structured was negatively associated with increased screen use behaviors (television watching, gaming, and internet surfing) within participants. Only news consumption was positively associated with the degree of day structure, but news consumption might inherently structure the day. Thus, the present study calls for targeted prevention and intervention and sheds some light on a potential low-threshold intervention in the form of day-structuring and planning. Such interventions might aid individuals in managing excessive screen use behaviors by preplanning different duties and recreational activities beforehand to minimize the degree of confinements actually experienced later on, due to a lack of preparation.
Further, our results provided new insights by showing who is more affected in their screen use behaviors by COVID-19-related confinements than others are: increased gaming was reported by males more on days with more work confinements. Increased news consumption was seen especially in individuals who live alone on days with more work confinements. Additionally, increased television watching and social media usage were reported by younger participants on days with more social confinements. Thus, to some degree, our results underpin previous studies [e.g., (27), (37)] showing that young, male, and individuals who live alone, may be most vulnerable for certain excessive screen use behaviors and thus represent an important target population for prevention and intervention strategies.
Increased screen use behaviors may further be problematic, as subgroups of especially vulnerable individuals may be at risk of developing chronic and excessive usage patterns. Previous research showed that such behaviors relate to poorer psychological and physical well-being (3, 15, 24, 25). Additionally, these behaviors may become addictive over time, so that several researchers argued that addiction-related disorders need special attention during the COVID-19 pandemic (38). Such mostly sedentary behaviors additionally seem to constitute a risk factor for weight gain during the COVID-19 pandemic (39). These points should be considered in prevention and intervention approaches in order to help individuals adapt their health behaviors. Several guidelines have been developed recently, providing advices on how to manage excessive behaviors (i.e., screen use) during the COVID-19 pandemic [e.g., (12, 40)] and to prevent and treat addictive behavior-related disorders (38). Still, the current study calls for targeted preventions and interventions toward particularly vulnerable individuals (i.e., between-person relationships regarding sex, age, and living alone).
Limitations and Future Research
The study mainly sampled university students at younger age who are at risk for developing chronically excessive behaviors, such as internet addiction (37), and experiencing a decline in psychological well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic, even more so in female university students (41). Nevertheless, this limits the generalizability of the present findings and calls for a replication in a sample with more diverse socio-economic characteristics. Additionally, the current study only covered 14 days of EMA assessment, mainly to limit participant burden and enhance compliance. However, long-term trajectories may be interesting, particularly with regard to the potential chronicity of addictive-like behaviors.
Clinical Impact and Future Directions of Research
In case of prolonged COVID-19-related confinements (i.e., potential upcoming lockdowns), a direct application of our findings would be to tailor a day-structuring intervention to counteract excessive screen use behaviors in vulnerable individuals. The possible intervention techniques would be planning of recreational activities, for example, through implementation intentions (42), situational specific action plans with an if-then structure. According to our data, especially young adults might profit from such interventions to limit increased television watching, whereas males may limit increased gaming.
Especially children and adolescents showed excessive screen use behaviors during COVID-19-related confinements [i.e., (13, 43–45)]; future research could examine if day-structuring might also be preventative in these subgroups. Simultaneously, a day-structuring intervention might even be useful to reduce other potentially problematic behaviors during COVID-19-related confinements, but this remains to be examined in future research. Moreover, day-structuring might also aid in deliberate integration and realization of healthy recreational activities, which might add to the overall psychological and physiological well-being during COVID-19-related confinements.
Apart from day-structuring, cognitive interventions might also be useful: results of the current study suggest that the subjective experience of COVID-19-related confinements seems especially important when it comes to increased screen use behaviors. Previous research makes it seem likely that the subjective experience of quarantine as either enforced vs. voluntary resembles an important differentiation with regard to health outcomes (3). Hence, applying framing of confinement measures as appeals to each and everyone's responsibility for the community (e.g., to avoid transmission, to protect the beloved ones), and emphasizing some positive aspects (e.g., time for family, time to recover from work stress) might also aid in avoiding increased screen use behaviors. As a result, future research should build on these findings and develop targeted and temporally precise interventions to tackle the negative psychological outcomes of COVID-19-related confinements.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics committee of the University of Salzburg. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
A-KA, JB, BP, and JR were involved in designing the present study. A-KA, BP, and JR contributed to the data acquisition and preparation. A-KA conducted the data analyses and wrote the first draft of the present manuscript. JB and JR revised the present manuscript. All authors approved the submitted version.
Funding
This work was supported by the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (ERC-StG-2014 639445 NewEat).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank Beatrice Ammer, Teresa Angleitner, Selina Bergmann, Nadine König, Verena Lebitsch, Natalia Logides, and Lisa Rachbauer for their assistance in collecting data for this study. Further, we thank all participants for their participation in this study.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2020.623205/full#supplementary-material
Footnotes
1. ^The assessments of the other prompts were focused on eating behavior. The data are reported elsewhere (29).
2. ^For all reported models, the assumptions of linearity (upon visual inspection of plots), homogeneity of residual variance (all p's > 0.500), and normal distribution of residuals (upon visual inspection of plots) were met.
References
1. Rajkumar RP. COVID-19 and mental health: a review of the existing literature. Asian J Psychiatr. (2020) 52:102066. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102066
2. Ozamiz-Etxebarria N, Idoiaga Mondragon N, Dosil Santamaría M, Picaza Gorrotxategi M. Psychological symptoms during the two stages of lockdown in response to the COVID-19 outbreak: an investigation in a sample of citizens in Northern Spain. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:2116. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02116
3. Brooks SK, Webster RK, Smith LE, Woodland L, Wessely S, Greenberg N, et al. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet. (2020) 395:912–20. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8
4. Hall-Lande JA, Eisenberg ME, Christenson SL, Neumark-Sztainer D. Social isolation, psychological health, and protecive factors in adolescence. Adolescence. (2007) 42:265–86. Available online at: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=asn&AN=26159861&site=ehost-live
5. Korpela K, Borodulin K, Neuvonen M, Paronen O, Tyrväinen L. Analyzing the mediators between nature-based outdoor recreation and emotional well-being. J Environ Psychol. (2014) 37:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.11.003
6. Mazzucchelli TG, Kane RT, Rees CS. Behavioral activation interventions for well-being: a meta-analysis. J Posit Psychol. (2010) 5:105–21. doi: 10.1080/17439760903569154
7. Allabadi H, Dabis J, Aghabekian V, Khader A, Khammash U. Impact of COVID-19 lockdown on dietary and lifestyle behaviours among adolescents in Palestine. Dynam Human Health. (2020) 7:2170. Available online at: https://journalofhealth.co.nz/?page_id=2170
8. Rolland B, Haesebaert F, Zante E, Benyamina A, Haesebaert J, Franck N. Global changes and factors of increase in caloric food, screen and substance use during the early COVID-19 containment phase in France: a general population online survey. JMIR Public Health Surveill. (2020) 6:e19630. doi: 10.2196/preprints.19630
9. Koopmann A, Georgiadou E, Kiefer F, Hillemacher T. Did the general population in Germany drink more alcohol during the COVID-19 pandemic lockdown? Alcohol Alcoholism. (2020) 55:698–9. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agaa058
10. Capuzzi E, Di Brita C, Caldiroli A, Colmegna F, Nava R, Buoli M, et al. Psychiatric emergency care during coronavirus 2019 (COVID 19) pandemic lockdown: results from a department of mental health and addiction of northern Italy. Psychiatry Res. (2020) 293:113463. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113463
11. Grant JE, Potenza MN, Weinstein A, Gorelick DA. Introduction to behavioral addictions. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. (2010) 36:233–41. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2010.491884
12. Király O, Potenza MN, Stein DJ, King DL, Hodgins DC, Saunders JB, et al. Preventing problematic internet use during the COVID-19 pandemic: consensus guidance. Compr Psychiatry. (2020) 100:152180. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2020.152180
13. López-Bueno R, López-Sánchez GF, Casajús JA, Calatayud J, Gil-Salmerón A, Grabovac I, et al. Health-related behaviors among school-aged children and adolescents during the Spanish Covid-19 confinement. Front Pediatrics. (2020) 8:573. doi: 10.3389/fped.2020.00573
14. Pietrobelli A, Pecoraro L, Ferruzzi A, Heo M, Faith M, Zoller T, et al. Effects of COVID-19 lockdown on lifestyle behaviors in children with obesity living in Verona, Italy: a longitudinal study. Obesity. (2020) 28:1382–5. doi: 10.1002/oby.22861
15. Akulwar-Tajane I, Parmar KK, Naik PH, Shah AV. Rethinking screen time during COVID-19: impact on psychological well-being in physiotherapy students. Int J Clin Exp Med Res. (2020) 4:201–16. doi: 10.26855/ijcemr.2020.10.014
16. Cellini N, Canale N, Mioni G, Costa S. Changes in sleep pattern, sense of time and digital media use during COVID-19 lockdown in Italy. J Sleep Res. (2020) 29:e13074. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13074
17. Kumari A, Ranjan P, Vikram NK, Kaur D, Sahu A, Dwivedi SN, et al. A short questionnaire to assess changes in lifestyle-related behaviour during COVID 19 pandemic. Diab Metab Synd Clin Res Rev. (2020) 14:1697–701. doi: 10.1016/j.dsx.2020.08.020
18. Majumdar P, Biswas A, Sahu S. COVID-19 pandemic and lockdown: cause of sleep disruption, depression, somatic pain, and increased screen exposure of office workers and students of India. Chronobiol Int. (2020) 37:1–10. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2020.1786107
19. Sañudo B, Fennell C, Sánchez-Oliver AJ. Objectively-assessed physical activity, sedentary behavior, smartphone use, and sleep patterns pre- and during-COVID-19 quarantine in young adults from Spain. Sustainability. (2020) 12:5890. doi: 10.3390/su12155890
20. Balhara Y, Kattula D, Singh S, Chukkali S, Bhargava R. Impact of lockdown following COVID-19 on the gaming behavior of college students. Indian J Public Health. (2020) 64:172–6. doi: 10.4103/ijph.IJPH_465_20
21. Alomari MA Khabour OF Alzoubi KH. Changes in physical activity and sedentary behavior amid confinement: the BKSQ-COVID-19 project. Risk Manag. Healthc Policy. (2020) 13:1757–64. doi: 10.2147/RMHP.S268320
22. Dixit A, Marthoenis M, Arafat SMY, Sharma P, Kar SK. Binge watching behavior during COVID 19 pandemic: a cross-sectional, cross-national online survey. Psychiatry Res. (2020) 289:113089. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113089
23. Ammar A, Brach M, Trabelsi K, Chtourou H, Boukhris O, Masmoudi L, et al. Effects of COVID-19 home confinement on eating behaviour and physical activity: results of the ECLB-COVID19 international online survey. Nutrients. (2020) 12:1583. doi: 10.3390/nu12061583
24. Burhamah W, AlKhayyat A, Oroszlányov á M, AlKenane A, Almansouri A, Behbehani M, et al. The psychological burden of the COVID-19 pandemic and associated lockdown measures: experience from 4000 participants. J Affect Disord. (2020) 277:977–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.09.014
25. Canet-Juric L, Andrés ML, del Valle M, López-Morales H, Poó F, Galli JI, et al. A Longitudinal study on the emotional impact cause by the COVID-19 pandemic quarantine on general population. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:565688. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.565688
26. Martin K. Electronic Overload: The Impact of Excessive Screen Use on Child and Adolescent Health and Wellbeing. Perth: Department of Sport and Recreation (2011).
27. Achab S, Nicolier M, Mauny F, Monnin J, Trojak B, Vandel P, et al. Massively multiplayer online role-playing games: comparing characteristics of addict vsnon-addict online recruited gamers in a French adult population. BMC Psychiatry. (2011) 11:144. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-11-144
28. Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2015) 10:227–37. doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352
29. Reichenberger J, Pannicke B, Arend AK, Schnepper R, Blechert B. Too Close for Comfort (Food) - Anxiety, Anxiety Eating and Weight Changes During COVID-19. Paris; Lodron-University of Salzburg, Department of Psychology (2020).
30. Bliese PD. Multilevel Modeling in R (2.6) – A Brief Introduction to R, The Multilevel Package and the nlme Package (2013).
31. Raudenbusch SW, Bryk AS, Congdon R. HLM7 for Windows [Computer software]. Skokie, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc. (2011).
33. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. (2015) 67:1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01
34. Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D, Heisterkamp S, Van Willigen B, et al. Package ‘nlme'. Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models, Version 3. Granada: University of Granada (2017).
35. Kapasia N, Paul P, Roy A, Saha J, Zaveri A, Mallick R, et al. Impact of lockdown on learning status of undergraduate and postgraduate students during COVID-19 pandemic in West Bengal, India. Child Youth Serv Rev. (2020) 116:105194. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105194
36. Qi M, Li P, Moyle W, Weeks B, Jones C. Physical activity, health-related quality of life, and stress among the chinese adult population during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:6494. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17186494
37. Mafé RC, Blas SS. Explaining Internet dependency: an exploratory study of future purchase intention of Spanish Internet users. Internet Res. (2006) 16:380–97. doi: 10.1108/10662240610690016
38. Du J, Fan N, Zhao M, Hao W, Liu T, Lu L, et al. Expert consensus on the prevention and treatment of substance use and addictive behaviour-related disorders during the COVID-19 pandemic. General Psychiatry. (2020) 33:e100252. doi: 10.1136/gpsych-2020-100252
39. Parekh N, Deierlein AL. Health behaviours during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic: implications for obesity. Public Health Nutrition. (2020) 23:3121–5. doi: 10.1017/S1368980020003031
40. Amin KP, Griffiths MD, Dsouza DD. Online gaming during the COVID-19 pandemic in India: strategies for work-life balance. Int J Ment Health Addict. (2020) 1–7. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00358-1. [Epub ahead of print].
41. Elmer T, Mepham K, Stadtfeld C. Students under lockdown: comparisons of students' social networks and mental health before and during the COVID-19 crisis in Switzerland. PLoS ONE. (2020) 15:e0236337. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0236337
42. Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P. Implementation intentions and goal achievement: a meta-analysis of effects and processes. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. (2006) 38:69–119. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38002-1
43. Moore SA, Faulkner G, Rhodes RE, Brussoni M, Chulak-Bozzer T, Ferguson LJ, et al. Impact of the COVID-19 virus outbreak on movement and play behaviours of Canadian children and youth: a national survey. Int J Behav Nutrition Phys Activ. (2020) 17:85. doi: 10.1186/s12966-020-00987-8
44. Xiang M, Zhang Z, Kuwahara K. Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on children and adolescents' lifestyle behavior larger than expected. Prog Cardiovasc Dis. (2020) 63:531–2. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2020.04.013
Keywords: COVID-19, confinements, screen use, day structure, ecological momentary assessment (EMA)
Citation: Arend A-K, Blechert J, Pannicke B and Reichenberger J (2021) Increased Screen Use on Days With Increased Perceived COVID-19-Related Confinements—A Day Level Ecological Momentary Assessment Study. Front. Public Health 8:623205. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.623205
Received: 29 October 2020; Accepted: 21 December 2020;
Published: 02 February 2021.
Edited by:
Ornella Corazza, University of Hertfordshire, United KingdomReviewed by:
Siddharth Sarkar, All India Institute of Medical Sciences, IndiaKonstantinos E. Siomos, University of Thessaly, Greece
Copyright © 2021 Arend, Blechert, Pannicke and Reichenberger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Ann-Kathrin Arend, ann-kathrin.arend@stud.sbg.ac.at