Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry , 20 March 2025

Sec. Autism

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1549092

Exploring the clinical features of minimally verbal autistic children

Silvia Guerrera*&#x;Silvia Guerrera1*†Elisa Fuc&#x;Elisa Fucà1†Emanuela PetroloEmanuela Petrolo1Andrea De Stefano,Andrea De Stefano2,3Laura CasulaLaura Casula1Maria Grazia LogriecoMaria Grazia Logrieco4Giovanni ValeriGiovanni Valeri1Stefano Vicari,Stefano Vicari1,5
  • 1Child Adolescent Neuropsychiatry Unit, Bambino Gesù Children’s Hospital IRCCS, Rome, Italy
  • 2Child Neurology and Psychiatry Unit, Fondazione Policlinico Universitario Agostino Gemelli IRCCS, Rome, Italy
  • 3Department of Neuroscience, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Rome, Italy
  • 4Department of Humanities, University of Foggia, Foggia, Italy
  • 5Life Sciences and Public Health Department, Catholic University, Rome, Italy

Introduction: It is recognized that around 25-30% of autistic children do not develop functional speech and remain minimally verbal beyond the age of 5. However, little is known about the clinical characteristics of this group.

Methods: We retrospectively examined a sample of 189 autistic children and adolescents classified as minimally verbal (mean age: 7.37 ± 1.51; 152 males, 37 females) and compared them with a group of 184 verbal autistic children and adolescents (mean age: 7.71 ± 2.52; 160 males, 24 females). We considered intellectual functioning, severity of autism, emotional and behavioural problems, and parenting stress.

Results: Children in the minimally verbal group exhibited significantly lower nonverbal Intelligent Quotient and an increase in restricted repetitive behaviours compared to the verbal group. Exploring potential differences in emotional and behavioural problems, the verbally group showed higher levels of anxiety symptoms. In addition, minimally verbal group showed high score of parenting stress.

Discussion: This study highlights the importance of accurately characterizing minimally verbal autistic children and adolescents to facilitate the identification of specific and individualized interventions based on individual functioning profiles.

1 Introduction

According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition, Text Revision (DSM-5-TR), Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is defined as a neurodevelopmental condition. It is characterized by deficits in social communication and interaction, as well as repetitive and restricted patterns of behavior and interests (e.g., repetitive body movements such as hand flapping, sensory sensitivities, and circumscribed interests) (1). The male sex is widely recognized as one of the most established etiological factors for ASD, with an estimated prevalence of 3.8 times higher among boys than girls (2), leading to the concept of a “female protective effect,” where females may require a greater etiological burden to exhibit the same level of impact as males. The current global prevalence of ASD is estimated to be approximately 1% (3) and has significantly increased over the last 20 years (4).

About 25-30% of autistic children do not develop or fail to develop functional spoken language and remain minimally verbal past age 5 (5, 6), with implications for social and adaptive functioning in adulthood (7). The recent literature has attempted to delve into the variables that may influence the different levels of language development in autistic children (8). As previously noted by Chenausky (9), language development is closely linked to speech development, and a challenge remains in understanding how these two aspects may intertwine in children with a neurodevelopmental disorder such as ASD. The term “minimally verbal” (MV) usually refers to children who exhibit a limited vocabulary consisting of a small number of spoken words and fixed phrases (7).

Using a broader definition, Chakrabarti and colleagues, in 2017, defined MV children whose communicative abilities are severely limited due to a language deficit, a language disorder, intellectual disability, deficits in social and cognitive skills, or a combination of these factors. According to Kasari et al. (10), MV autistic children are characterized by “a highly restricted repertoire of spoken words or fixed phrases used for communication”; additionally, echolalic or stereotyped language may be present. The specific number of words used to define MV autistic children and the criteria for determining this may vary, ranging from 1 to 10 words (11), 25 or fewer words (12). More recently, Chenausky and collaborators have referred to a range of fewer than 20 intelligible words (13). Similarly, methods for clinical evaluation may be based on the number of different words in a natural language sample or on clinical judgment. In 2015, Norrelgen and colleagues (6) defined MV autistic children as those who, according to the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (14), had a language age of less than 24 months and used between three words and some combinations of two words. In 2016, a study conducted by Plesa-Skwerer and colleagues defined MV autistic children as those who, according to parental reports, did not use phrase speech spontaneously and effectively and/or produced fewer than 30 words (15). The use of standardized tests commonly employed in language therapy to evaluate the language skills of MV autistic children has been questioned due to the complexity of instructions and the need for active participation from the child (16). Therefore, a common definition of MV autistic children is associated with the use of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2) (17), where “minimally verbal” refers to individuals assessed with Module 1 of the ADOS-2, designated for children aged over 30 months who have no speech or use very few words in simple combinations (1721).

Investigations into intellectual functioning in this population have yielded highly heterogeneous results (18). General intellectual disability has been reported in MV autistic individuals (2224). Additionally, it has been reported that, in MV autistic preschoolers, nonverbal intelligence quotient is one of the major predictors of subsequent language gains for those children who acquire some language before the age of 5 (20, 2527). On the other hand, Bal and colleagues (18) reported that 16% of children in their sample had non-verbal cognitive abilities within average limits, contrasting with the hypothesis that minimal verbalization is synonymous with cognitive impairment. Additionally, Slusna and collaborators, studying a group of MV autistic youth and adults across the lifespan, found that 10.2% of the youth had a non-verbal intelligent quotient ≥ 70 (24).These results suggest that the mechanisms underlying the lack of language development are not solely attributable to cognitive level, but rather to a heterogeneous set of predictive factors, some of which are precursors to language that vary across the autism spectrum (26, 28).

Regarding the severity of ASD, previous research has suggested that MV autistic children do not exhibit significantly higher scores than verbal individuals (29). In a longitudinal study examining a group of MV preschoolers, none of the ADOS Calibrated Severity Scores (CSS) were correlated with the subsequent development of phrase speech (20). It remains to be determined whether there is a possible association between ASD core symptom severity and language abilities, given that most observational measures of ASD symptom severity are heavily influenced by language level (5, 16).

Previous research has described a higher prevalence of psychiatric comorbidities and emotional and behavioral problems in autistic children and adolescents (3035). However, few studies have explored the relationship between emotional and behavioral problems in children with different language abilities (19). Indeed, it has been suggested that communication impairments can hinder development across various domains, with 25% of MV autistic children experiencing increased social withdrawal during adolescence (36). Moreover, MV autistic children are more likely to have associated oral-motor difficulties (37), and due to limited social interaction, adaptive behavioral skills, academic achievement, vocational success, and social relationships would also be affected (27, 38). Finally, it is necessary to consider that the inability to develop expressive verbal communication is a primary concern frequently reported by parents of autistic children (39). Parents of autistic children typically encounter higher levels of parenting stress compared to parents of children with other disabilities (40). Recently, it has been highlighted that children’s linguistic and communication difficulties appear to be a common source of parental stress and valid predictors of it (41). However, these difficulties have often been associated with the presence of greater emotional and behavioral problems (42), leaving the directly involved causes unexplored.

Considering that the current research has primarily focused on autistic individuals characterized as “high functioning” or having “lower symptom presentation”, the subgroup of ASD children who are MV is significantly underrepresented in both descriptive and intervention studies (13). In 2017, the strategic plan of the Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee underscored the imperative need to study autistic children exhibiting extremely limited verbal abilities, with the aim that 90% of autistic children acquire functional speech by age 5. Of note, most studies on MV autistic children and adolescents have explored small sample sizes (43). Therefore, the present study aimed to compare the clinical characteristics of a large group of MV autistic children and adolescents with a group of age- and sex-matched verbal autistic children and adolescents, specifically exploring possible differences in terms of intellectual functioning, autistic symptomatology and associated emotional and behavioral problems. Finally, maternal stress levels in the two populations were investigated.

A better understanding of the functioning characteristics of this group of autistic children can facilitate the development of personalized diagnostic strategies and identify specific interventions developed based on a child’s interests and strengths in other areas.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Procedure and participants

Data were retrospectively collected from an in-depth review of the files of patients who referred to the Child and Adolescent Neuropsychiatry Unit of a third level Children’s Hospital between 2017 and 2023 for a neuropsychiatric evaluation following pediatrician’s clinical suspicion of ASD or for clinical follow-ups after receiving ASD diagnosis. Routine assessment procedure always included neuropsychiatric examination, cognitive and adaptive functioning evaluation, assessment of ASD symptoms and an emotional and behavioral evaluation. Grouping was based on the ADOS–2 module completed with the child (minimally verbal = Module 1, based on the use of module 1 designed for children with an absence of language or with production of a few single words; verbal = Module 2 or 3). Inclusion criteria were as follows: ascertained diagnosis of ASD supported by gold-standard instruments; age comprised between 5 and 18 years. Exclusion criteria were as follows: presence of neurological conditions (e.g., epilepsy); presence of genetic syndromes. The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the local Ethics Committee (protocol code: 2423_OPBG_2021, approved on 27 October 2021).

The sample included 189 autistic children and adolescents classified as MV (MV group) and 184 age- and sex-matched verbally autistic children and adolescents (VB group) aged 5-18 years. Tables 1, 2 summarize the demographic characteristics of the sample and the ADOS-2 data (modules and scores), respectively.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the sample * p value <0.05.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2) data for each group.

2.2 Measures

2.2.1 Autistic symptoms assessment

The diagnosis of ASD was established in accordance with the DSM-5 and was confirmed by the administration of the “gold-standard” instruments for the assessment of ASD symptoms, namely the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd Edition (ADOS-2) (17) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) (44). The ADOS-2 is a semi-structured direct assessment of communication, social interaction, and play or imaginative use of materials for individuals with a suspected diagnosis of ASD. The ADOS-2 consists of five modules designed for children and adults with different levels of language, ranging from nonverbal to verbal; it was administered and scored by licensed clinicians. Total score combines symptoms from the Social Affect (SA) and Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors (RRB) domains. In the analyses, the CSS were considered for the ADOS-2. The ADI-R is a standardized, semi‐structured interview during which caregivers report information about an individual suspected of having an ASD. The instrument generates algorithm scores for each of the three subdomains of autistic symptoms: qualitative impairments in reciprocal social behavior; qualitative abnormalities in communication and restricted range of interests and/or stereotypic behaviors.

2.2.2 Cognitive assessment

Cognitive development was assessed by the Leiter International Performance Scale – 3rd Edition - Leiter-3 (45) – which provides a nonverbal measure of intelligence and assesses the ability to reason by analogy, by matching and perceptual reasoning in general, irrespective of language and formal schooling. The Global Non-Verbal Intelligent Quotient obtained through this test is based on four subtests: Figure Ground, Form Completion, Classification and Analogies, and Sequential Order. We used also the Colored Progressive Matrices, a 60-item test to assess mental ability associated with abstract reasoning, and considered a nonverbal estimate of fluid intelligence. The test consists of increasingly difficult pattern matching tasks and has little dependency on language abilities. All participants (100%) in the MV group were assessed using the Leiter-3, while 11% of the VB group were assessed using the Colored Progressive Matrices.

2.2.3 Behavioral and psychological screening

Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL). Behavioral and psychological screening was performed by means of the parent-report questionnaire CBCL (46). For preschoolers, we used the CBCL for ages 1.5 to 5, which consists of 100 problem items. The instrument generates seven syndrome scales and five DSM-oriented scale profiles, consistent with the diagnostic categories of DSM-IV-TR and DSM-5. For participants aged 6-18 years, we used the CBCL 6-18, which generates eight syndrome and embraces six DSM-Oriented scales. Both versions include three general domains, namely internalizing, externalizing and total problems. In the current study, we considered the three general domains and the DSM-Oriented scales overlapping in the two versions of the instrument (i.e., versions for ages 1.5-5 and 6-18 years), namely Affective problems, Anxiety problems, Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity problems, and Oppositional defiant problems. Clinical Cutoffs Description: T-Scores: 65-69 (Borderline) and 70+ (Clinical) are generated for narrow band scales. Moreover, for each participant we calculated the Dysregulation Profile (DP) of CBCL as described elsewhere (47, 48). Briefly, CBCL-DP is characterized by simultaneous high values in three syndrome scales, namely anxious/depressed, attention problems, and aggressive behavior, using the criteria based on a sum of t-scores, deficient emotional self-regulation as an aggregate cut-off score of >180 but <210 (elevation of 1 Standard Deviation - SD) on the abovementioned scales and Severe Dysregulation as an aggregate cut-off score of ≥210 (elevation of 2 SD or more). Of note, data from CBCL were available for 142 out of 189 participants in the MV group.

2.2.4 Maternal stress assessment

To investigate maternal stress levels, the Parenting Stress Index-Short Form (PSI) (49) was used. PSI is an easy-to administer tool to measure maternal stress. It consists of 36 questions and each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale from (1) strongly disagree to (5) strongly agree. The PSI captures three domains—parental distress (PD), parent–child dysfunctional interaction (P-CDI), and difficult child (DC). The sum of all questions results in the Total Stress score. PSI has been translated into several languages and has been frequently used in ASD research (5053). Data from PSI were available for a subgroup of 262 mothers (N= 148 for VB group and N=114 for MV group).

2.3 Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were used to analyze demographic and clinical characteristics of the whole sample. Chi-squared test was used to investigate group differences on sex distribution. Group differences were examined by t test and analysis of co-variance (ANCOVA), adjusting for IQ. Post hoc analyses were performed using Tukey HSD test. Partial eta squared (ηp2) was used to measure effect size. A p-value less than or equal to 0.05 was considered as statistically significant.

3 Results

3.1 Differences on cognitive level and autistic symptoms

Children belonging to the MV group exhibited significantly lower IQ than children in the VB group (59.59 ± 16.55 and 87.6 ± 16.07, respectively). The 26.5% of participants in the MV group exhibited a non-verbal intelligent quotient ≥ 70 compared to 90% of participants in the VB group. Regarding the investigation of potential group differences on ASD symptoms, measured by ADOS-2 CSS, ANCOVA (IQ as covariate) failed to detect significant differences on the SA domain F (1,357) =.462, p = 0.497, ηp2 = 0.01; 6.46 ± 1.62 and 5.98 ± 1.55 for MV and VB, respectively). On the other hand, participants in the MV groups exhibited significantly higher scores on the Restricted and RRB domain F(1,357) = 4.372, p = 0.037, ηp2 = 0.012; 7.43 ± 1.43 and 6.47 ± 1.95, respectively).

3.2 Differences on CBCL scores

Multivariate ANCOVA showed that participants in the MV exhibited significantly lower scores than participants in the VB group in the anxiety DSM-Oriented scale of the CBCL. Table 3 summarizes the results. No group differences emerged in the CBCL DP scores (180.59 ± 19.99 and 183.4 ± 21.86 for MV and VB groups, respectively; p = 0.228).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Group differences on Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) scores.

3.3 Differences on maternal parenting stress levels

ANCOVA (IQ as covariate) failed to detect significant differences on the PD scale of the PSI F(1,259) =1.167, p = 0.28, ηp2 = 0.004; 29.52 ± 10.11 and 26.88 ± 10.42 for MV and VB, respectively), as well as in the DC scale F(1,259) =.010, p = 0.919, ηp2 < 0.000; 31.07 ± 10 and 31.8 ± 15.18 for MV and VB, respectively). On the other hand, mothers of participants in the MV groups exhibited significantly higher scores on the P-CDI scale F(1,259) = 5.213, p = 0.023, ηp2 = 0.019; 26.68 ± 7.14 and 24.13 ± 7.3, respectively).

4 Discussion

The main goal of this study was to provide a description of the clinical characteristics of a large group of autistic children, defined as MV based on the use of module 1 of the ADOS-2 and compared to a sex- and age-matched group of verbal autistic children. Participants in the MV group showed below-average intellectual functioning compared to the VB group, with a relatively small subgroup showing a non-verbal intelligent quotient falling in the normal range. Several studies have highlighted the significant variability in skills across various domains of cognitive, social, and linguistic functioning among autistic individuals. The prevalence of intellectual disability in autistic children with an average age of 8 years, calculated in the United States, has been estimated to be one-third (54). Although it is a common assumption that MV autistic individuals have a greater impairment in cognitive functioning (7, 9), recent studies have shown that some MV autistic children appear to report a typical or borderline non-verbal intelligent quotient (18, 24, 55), suggesting a fundamental divergence between verbal and nonverbal functioning in this population. This refutes the notion that reduced language abilities necessarily correspond to compromised intellectual abilities and, instead, suggests a wide range of profiles wherein language may be impaired or even absent in ASD children with otherwise intact intellectual abilities. Chenausky et al. (55) reported that the average non-verbal intelligent quotient of a sample of 54 MV autistic children was 68, with half of the sample having non-verbal intelligent quotient scores below 70. Slusna et al. (24) exploring 49 autistic individuals above 6 years of age minimally or nonverbal, using Leiter International Performance Test-Revised (Leiter-R), found a non-verbal intelligent quotient > 70, only in 5 participant. Finally, Bal et al. in 2016 (18), in a group of 257 MV autistic children and adolescents, 4–17 years old found a typical non-verbal intelligent quotient in 16% of individuals, with differences dependent on the individual assessment instruments used. In general, the rates of intellectual disability in the autistic population MV seem to vary depending on the IQ assessment used, the characteristic of sample explored (eg. age range) as well as the definition of MV identified. The limited number of studies including verbal autistic children with intellectual disability (56) and MV autistic children has not yet allowed for a clear understanding of the possible “double dissociation” between linguistic and intellectual functioning, as previously stated by Smith N & Tsimpli IM (57), who stated the independence of language from other forms of cognition. Further longitudinal studies are needed on this matter.

The second finding of the study showed that, regarding autistic symptomatology, the MV group exhibited a slight but significant increase in CSS scores in the RRB domain compared to the VB group, after accounting for IQ differences. Literature on the relationship between RRBs and language development remains contradictory. Bal and colleagues (18), as well as Zheng and collaborators (58), reported slightly higher RRB scores in children with “Few to No Words” compared to those with “Some Words”, based on ADOS Module 1, suggesting the existence of different levels of ASD symptom severity based on spoken language levels. Harrop and collaborators (59) exploring a group of MV autistic children (defined as children with fewer than 20 spontaneous functional spoken words) ages 5 to 8 years, reported high rates of verbal RRBs, assessed through observational coding of caregiver-child interactions, videotaped. On the other hand, our result differs from few studies that have investigated the association between autistic symptoms and verbal language development, where participants in the MV group do not exhibit significantly higher ADOS scores than verbal individuals (15, 20). In particular, Thurm and collaborators conducted a longitudinal study investigating language outcomes in a group of MV autistic children assessed during the preschool years. The authors showed that the severity of ASD symptoms in core domains, as indicated by the CSS score, did not predict the emergence of spoken language by the age of 5 (20). Furthermore, the variations in SA-CSS scores were no longer significant when nonverbal cognitive abilities were considered in the model. According to the authors, this result suggests the presence of collinearity between nonverbal cognitive abilities and the SA-CSS, which undermines the ability to accurately assess predictive models of language acquisition in ASD. This implication may indicate that, in the current study, the slight significance of the RRB scores in MV autistic children could be attributed to the higher prevalence of intellectual disability, compared to the VB group, which in some studies has been linked to a greater number of motor stereotypies included in RRB, particularly among older children (60). Beyond the current difficulty in providing unequivocal conclusions regarding this correlation (61), the possible presence of more severe autistic symptoms implies the need to provide alternative communication interventions. Our results, by providing a more appropriate understanding of certain clinical characteristics of MV autistic individuals, could support the development of specific interventions by leveraging certain peculiarities. As suggested by Harrop and colleagues (59), more severe autistic symptomatology in terms of RRBs, particularly those involving objects manipulation, could be leveraged within social communication intervention programs to promote greater child engagement. The focus would be on expanding or redirecting these behaviours, ultimately aiming for increased adherence to the communicative intervention.

An additional objective of the current study was to compare the MV group with the VB group on a measure for behavioural and psychological screening. In fact, few studies have explored how emotional and behavioural problems varies in children with different language abilities (19). Our results showed greater parent-reported anxiety issues in VB group compared to MV autistic children. The CBCL is one of the most widely investigated instruments to detect emotional and behavioural problems in children and adolescents with neurodevelopmental disorder (30), such as ASD. Many studies support its reliability and validity across different clinical groups (62). Data on the distribution of anxiety disorders in autistic children remain unclear, with some studies exploring the possible association with intellectual functioning and others with language functioning. Indeed, it is possible that certain psychiatric comorbidities (e.g., anxiety, depression) manifest differently in individuals with varying intellectual and language abilities (63). Relatively to cognitive functioning, some studies reported that low IQ may be associated with more anxious symptoms in ASD children (6466). More recently, other studies, including a meta-analysis conducted on 49 papers on the topic, suggested that anxiety symptoms are more frequently seen among autistic children with borderline intellectual functioning, whereas those with intellectual disability generally show considerably lower anxiety symptom scores (67, 68). The difficulty in assessing anxious symptoms in autistic individuals due to their communication difficulties (69), high methodological variability in assessment (70), and the absence of good specific measures for this group of children (71) would explain the high variability in results, especially in the presence of a prevalence of intellectual disability (67). Additionally, it is necessary to consider that the lower levels of anxiety reported by parents of MV autistic children could be explained by the parents’ difficulty in recognizing anxious symptoms in their children, mainly due to reduced use of verbal language and anxiety-related behaviours overlapping with other behaviours (e.g., ASD characteristics), as suggested by Tarver and colleagues in 2021 (72). Indeed, most studies on anxiety in autistic children rely on parent reports (71). Depending on parental reporting can be challenging, as it often requires the child to effectively communicate their emotions to the caregiver. Consequently, psychiatric conditions such as anxiety may be underdiagnosed in autistic individuals with MV and intellectual disability. The limited communication skills in MV children, often associated with impaired intellectual functioning, may lead to significant difficulties in verbally expressing their concerns or identifying complex internal states such as anxiety or other internalizing symptoms. In this regard, Plesa-Skwerer and colleagues (73), examining a group of MV autistic children and adolescents in terms of psychiatric comorbidity and emotional dysregulation through the exclusive use of parent reports, reported low rates of emotional dysregulation despite a high percentage of psychiatric comorbidity. In this case as well, the authors attribute this result to the objective difficulty that parents have in understanding the internal states of minimally verbal children, as well as the persistent phenomenon of diagnostic overshadowing (34). Furthermore, consistently with the results of our study, no difference was found on the externalizing symptoms scales. Therefore, it would be useful for future studies to focus on examining the characteristics of the MV population, exploring their intellectual functioning in greater depth and how this may influence other aspects, including emotional and behavioral problems.

Finally, when exploring parenting stress in mothers of MV autistic children compared to parents of verbally autistic children, parents of MV group showed greater problems on the parent-child interaction subscale. It is acknowledged that parents of autistic children report higher levels of stress compared to parents of non-autistic children and children with other developmental disabilities (7477). Although stress levels may vary across the different studies considered, depending on the composition of the sample and the tools used to measure stress, our findings appear to be in line with the literature on families of children with developmental disabilities, which has showed that the severity of child’s impairments seems to be an important factor related to parenting stress (78). Specifically, in families with autistic children, the elements of stress reported by the families have been found to be correlated with the degree of child’s impairment, including the severity of cognitive functioning (53, 74) and language functioning (79). In particular, PSI in parents of autistic children has often been linked to the child’s level of intellectual ability, as the latter is correlated with their autonomy, learning potential, communication skills, and the manifestation of problematic behaviors. In this regard, Scibelli and colleagues (53) found an association between cognitive impairment and PSI levels while exploring samples of autistic adolescents. From this, we could infer that our results on PSI levels in school-age children and MV adolescents might be associated not only with the communication difficulties of this population but also with the effect of intellectual impairment. In autistic children, additional factors that seem to affect parental stress are emotional and behavioural issues (80). Therefore, the increased stress in parents of MV autistic children could be related to the influence of all these factors.

Our study, which focused on a large sample of MV autistic children and adolescents, revealed a distinct profile for this population, characterized by lower cognitive functioning and slightly greater repetitive behaviours/restricted interests compared to the verbal group. From a psychopathological perspective, differences in anxiety symptoms between the two groups indicated a worse profile for verbal children and adolescents. Additionally, the MV group showed higher levels of parental stress compared to the ASD group with language abilities. Based on these findings, we emphasize the need for further research, particularly longitudinal studies, to address these challenges and develop tailored rehabilitative interventions for this diverse clinical population as early as possible.

This study has some limitations that must be taken into account: first and foremost, the retrospective nature of the study. Specifically, this determined the possibility of retrieving language assessment data for only 32% of autistic participants classified as MV. A linguistic assessment of both language production and comprehension in these children would have expanded the understanding of this sample. Second, our sample was defined as MV based on one of the definitions considered in the literature, in terms of linguistic function and age, but since there is still no established operational definition for MV in autistic individuals, the results of the current study are not fully comparable to the results of previous studies. A similar consideration must be made regarding the assessment tools used to define the sample, although we used Module 1 of the ADOS, which falls among the instruments considered in the literature as valid for one of the definitions. Third, a notable limitation of this study is the age range included in both the verbally fluent and minimally verbal groups, which spans from 5 to 18 years. Although the age range was chosen to ensure a broad representation of individuals, it is important to acknowledge that the linguistic capabilities of children at the upper end of this spectrum, especially in the verbally fluent group, may differ significantly from those at the lower end, or from those in the minimally verbal group. Thus, caution is necessary when drawing conclusions about language abilities across the age span, as the developmental trajectory of language acquisition may not be fully captured within these ranges. Future studies with more closely matched age groups or a more narrow focus on specific developmental stages may help to further refine our understanding of these differences In addition, although the CBCL is one of the most commonly used measures for behavioural and psychological screening in autistic children, it includes many items directly dependent on verbal language abilities, which may not be appropriate for ASD (81) and MV individuals.

Despite these limitations, given the limited representation of MV autistic children in both descriptive and intervention research (81), the current study explored a large sample of scholar MV autistic children and adolescents, contributing to the existing literature. The observed variability in cognitive and linguistic abilities among MV autistic children supports the idea that no single underlying mechanism explains why these children do not acquire speech. On the other hand, previous research has highlighted that a deeper understanding of the characteristics of non-verbal or minimally verbal autistic children—particularly in terms of intellectual and linguistic functioning, as well as multiple behavioral domains across various social contexts—could be achieved through their involvement in large-scale intervention studies. This, in turn, may enhance our comprehension of how these children respond to treatments, ultimately paving the way for the development of personalized interventions to optimize outcomes (7). Koegel and colleagues (82), in considering the factors that may facilitate the implementation of specific interventions, emphasize not only the importance of establishing a clear definition of MV based on word count and age, along with the development of precise and specific assessment tools, but also the need for studies estimating verbal and/or non-verbal cognitive abilities. By identifying the child’s overall functioning, such studies could be valuable for post hoc analyses of intervention effects in both nonverbal and MV individuals. In this regard, the findings from our sample may contribute to a deeper understanding of MV autistic individuals. Finally, regardless of the underlying causes of the heightened stress levels observed in the parents of this group of children and adolescents, this finding underscores the need for targeted interventions to provide them with support, particularly during the school years. While parent-mediated therapies have been designed for preschool children with ASD (83, 84), there are still limited programs addressing the needs of families of autistic adolescents (53), specially MV. As emphasized by Scibelli and colleagues, it is essential to develop intervention strategies that actively involve parents without adding to their caregiving responsibilities, such as home-based support or tools like video feedback to enhance their perception of interactions with their child. Future studies, including homogeneous age groupings (43), or including specific age groups as such as adolescence, may encourage stakeholders and decision-makers to improve their efforts to provide targeted interventions and support services. Furthermore, given the severe lack of research on language and communication interventions for MV autistic children aged 5 and above, future studies exploring communication development in MV autistic children are essential.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by local Ethics Committee Bambino Gesù Children Hospital protocol code: 2423_OPBG_2021, approved on 27 October 2021. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

SG: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. EF: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. EP: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. AD: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. LC: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. ML: Data curation, Writing – review & editing. GV: Writing – review & editing. SV: Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Italian Ministry of Health with “Current Research” funds.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (2024). DSM Library (Accessed June 25, 2024).

Google Scholar

2. Maenner MJ, Warren Z, Williams AR, Amoakohene E, Bakian AV, Bilder DA, et al. Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2020. MMWR Surveill Summ. (2023) 72:1–14. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss7202a1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Zeidan J, Fombonne E, Scorah J, Ibrahim A, Durkin MS, Saxena S, et al. Global prevalence of autism: A systematic review update. Autism Res. (2022) 15:778–90. doi: 10.1002/aur.2696

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Baio J, Wiggins L, Christensen DL, Maenner MJ, Daniels J, Warren Z, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2014. MMWR Surveill Summ. (2018) 67:1–23. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6706a1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Anderson DK, Lord C, Risi S, DiLavore PS, Shulman C, Thurm A, et al. Patterns of growth in verbal abilities among children with autism spectrum disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2007) 75:594–604. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.4.594

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Norrelgen F, Fernell E, Eriksson M, Hedvall Å, Persson C, Sjölin M, et al. Children with autism spectrum disorders who do not develop phrase speech in the preschool years. Autism. (2015) 19:934–43. doi: 10.1177/1362361314556782

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Tager-Flusberg H, Kasari C. Minimally verbal school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder: the neglected end of the spectrum. Autism Res. (2013) 6:468–78. doi: 10.1002/aur.1329

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Vogindroukas I, Stankova M, Chelas E-N, Proedrou A. Language and speech characteristics in autism. NDT. (2022) 18:2367–77. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S331987

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Chenausky KV, Maffei M, Tager-Flusberg H, Green JR. Review of methods for conducting speech research with minimally verbal individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Augment Altern Commun. (2023) 39:33–44. doi: 10.1080/07434618.2022.2120071

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Kasari C, Brady N, Lord C, Tager-Flusberg H. Assessing the minimally verbal school-aged child with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2013) 6:479–93. doi: 10.1002/aur.1334

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Schreibman L, Stahmer AC. A randomized trial comparison of the effects of verbal and pictorial naturalistic communication strategies on spoken language for young children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2014) 44:1244–51. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1972-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Yoder PJ, Layton TL. Speech following sign language training in autistic children with minimal verbal language. J Autism Dev Disord. (1988) 18:217–29. doi: 10.1007/BF02211948

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Chenausky K, Norton A, Tager-Flusberg H, Schlaug G. Auditory-motor mapping training: comparing the effects of a novel speech treatment to a control treatment for minimally verbal children with autism. PloS One. (2016) 11:e0164930. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0164930

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Sparrow SS, Cicchetti DV, Balla DA. Vineland adaptive behavior scales:(Vineland II), survey interview form/caregiver rating form. Livonia, MN: Pearson Assessments (2005). p. 10.

Google Scholar

15. Plesa Skwerer D, Jordan SE, Brukilacchio BH, Tager-Flusberg H. Comparing methods for assessing receptive language skills in minimally verbal children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism. (2016) 20:591–604. doi: 10.1177/1362361315600146

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Schaeffer J, Abd-El-Raziq M, Castroviejo E, Durrleman S, Ferré S, Grama I, et al. Language in autism: domains, profiles and co-occurring conditions. J Neural Transm (Vienna). (2023) 130:433–57. doi: 10.1007/s00702-023-02592-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism diagnostic observation schedule–2nd edition (ADOS-2). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Corporation (2012). p. 284.

Google Scholar

18. Bal VH, Katz T, Bishop SL, Krasileva K. Understanding definitions of minimally verbal across instruments: evidence for subgroups within minimally verbal children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2016) 57:1424–33. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12609

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Fok M, Bal VH. Differences in profiles of emotional behavioral problems across instruments in verbal versus minimally verbal children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2019) 12:1367–75. doi: 10.1002/aur.2126

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Thurm A, Manwaring SS, Swineford L, Farmer C. Longitudinal study of symptom severity and language in minimally verbal children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2015) 56:97–104. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12285

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Williams DL, Siegel M, Mazefsky CA, Autism and Developmental Disorders Inpatient Research Collaborative (ADDIRC). Problem behaviors in autism spectrum disorder: association with verbal ability and adapting/coping skills. J Autism Dev Disord. (2018) 48:3668–77. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3179-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hewitt AS, Stancliffe RJ, Sirek AJ, Hall-Lande J, Taub S, Engler J, et al. Characteristics of adults with autism spectrum disorder who use adult developmental disability services: Results from 25 US states. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2012) 6:741–51. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.10.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Luyster RJ, Kadlec MB, Carter A, Tager-Flusberg H. Language assessment and development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2008) 38:1426–38. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0510-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Slušná D, Rodríguez A, Salvadó B, Vicente A, Hinzen W. Relations between language, non-verbal cognition, and conceptualization in non- or minimally verbal individuals with ASD across the lifespan. Autism Dev Lang Impair. (2021) 6:23969415211053264. doi: 10.1177/23969415211053264

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Ellis Weismer S, Kover ST. Preschool language variation, growth, and predictors in children on the autism spectrum. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2015) 56:1327–37. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12406

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Thurm A, Lord C, Lee L-C, Newschaffer C. Predictors of language acquisition in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2007) 37:1721–34. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0300-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Wodka EL, Mathy P, Kalb L. Predictors of phrase and fluent speech in children with autism and severe language delay. Pediatrics. (2013) 131:e1128–1134. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-2221

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Pecukonis M, Plesa Skwerer D, Eggleston B, Meyer S, Tager-Flusberg H. Concurrent social communication predictors of expressive language in minimally verbal children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2019) 49:3767–85. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04089-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Knaus TA, Kamps J, Foundas AL, Tager-Flusberg H. Atypical PT anatomy in children with autism spectrum disorder with expressive language deficits. Brain Imaging Behav. (2018) 12:1419–30. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9795-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Guerrera S, Menghini D, Napoli E, Di Vara S, Valeri G, Vicari S. Assessment of psychopathological comorbidities in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder using the child behavior checklist. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:535. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00535

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hossain MM, Khan N, Sultana A, Ma P, McKyer ELJ, Ahmed HU, et al. Prevalence of comorbid psychiatric disorders among people with autism spectrum disorder: An umbrella review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Psychiatry Res. (2020) 287:112922. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112922

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Lai M-C, Kassee C, Besney R, Bonato S, Hull L, Mandy W, et al. Prevalence of co-occurring mental health diagnoses in the autism population: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. (2019) 6:819–29. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30289-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Lecavalier L. Behavioral and emotional problems in young people with pervasive developmental disorders: relative prevalence, effects of subject characteristics, and empirical classification. J Autism Dev Disord. (2006) 36:1101–14. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0147-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Simonoff E, Pickles A, Charman T, Chandler S, Loucas T, Baird G. Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2008) 47:921–9. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Fucà E, Guerrera S, Valeri G, Casula L, Novello RL, Menghini D, et al. Psychiatric comorbidities in children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder: A study on prevalence, distribution and clinical features in an italian sample. J Clin Med. (2023) 12:677. doi: 10.3390/jcm12020677

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Lord CE. Autism: from research to practice. Am Psychol. (2010) 65:815–26. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.65.8.815

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Gernsbacher MA, Sauer EA, Geye HM, Schweigert EK, Hill Goldsmith H. Infant and toddler oral- and manual-motor skills predict later speech fluency in autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2008) 49:43–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01820.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Binger C, Kent-Walsh J, Ewing C, Taylor S. Teaching educational assistants to facilitate the multisymbol message productions of young students who require augmentative and alternative communication. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. (2010) 19:108–20. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2009/09-0015)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Franchini M, Duku E, Armstrong V, Brian J, Bryson SE, Garon N, et al. Variability in verbal and nonverbal communication in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder: predictors and outcomes. J Autism Dev Disord. (2018) 48:3417–31. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3607-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Hayes SA, Watson SL. The impact of parenting stress: a meta-analysis of studies comparing the experience of parenting stress in parents of children with and without autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2013) 43:629–42. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1604-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Ibrahimagić A, Patković N, Herwig R. Autism spectrum disorders child language and communication skills and its impact on parental emotions and stress. Psychiatr Danub. (2022) 34:530.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

42. Palmer M, Tarver J, Carter Leno V, Paris Perez J, Frayne M, Slonims V, et al. Parent, teacher and observational reports of emotional and behavioral problems in young autistic children. J Autism Dev Disord. (2023) 53:296–309. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05421-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Koegel LK, Glugatch LB, Koegel RL, Castellon FA. Targeting IEP social goals for children with autism in an inclusive summer camp. J Autism Dev Disord. (2019) 49:2426–36. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-03992-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Rutter M, Le Couteur A, Lord C. Autism diagnostic interview-revised Vol. 29. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services (2003). p. 30.

Google Scholar

45. Roid GH, Koch C. Leiter-3: Nonverbal cognitive and neuropsychological assessment, in: McCallum RS (Ed.), Handbook of Nonverbal Assessment. Cham: Springer International Publishing (2017). p. 127–50. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-50604-3_8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Achenbach TM, Dumenci L, Rescorla LA. Ratings of relations between DSM-IV diagnostic categories and items of the CBCL/6-18, TRF, and YSR. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont (2001) p. 1–9.

Google Scholar

47. Keefer A, Singh V, Kalb LG, Mazefsky CA, Vasa RA. Investigating the factor structure of the child behavior checklist dysregulation profile in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2020) 13:436–43. doi: 10.1002/aur.2233

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Masi G, Pisano S, Milone A, Muratori P. Child behavior checklist dysregulation profile in children with disruptive behavior disorders: A longitudinal study. J Affect Disord. (2015) 186:249–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.05.069

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Abidin RR. Manual for the parenting stress index: Short form. Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology Press (1990).

Google Scholar

50. Huang C-Y, Yen H-C, Tseng M-H, Tung L-C, Chen Y-D, Chen K-L. Impacts of autistic behaviors, emotional and behavioral problems on parenting stress in caregivers of children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2014) 44:1383–90. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-2000-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Leonardi E, Cerasa A, Servidio R, Costabile A, Famà FI, Carrozza C, et al. The route of stress in parents of young children with and without autism: A path-analysis study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:10887. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182010887

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Papanikolaou K, Ntre V, Gertsou I-M, Tagkouli E, Tzavara C, Pehlivanidis A, et al. Parenting children with autism spectrum disorder during crises: differential responses between the financial and the COVID-19 pandemic crisis. J Clin Med. (2022) 11:1264. doi: 10.3390/jcm11051264

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Scibelli F, Fucà E, Guerrera S, Lupi E, Alfieri P, Valeri G, et al. Clinical and individual features associated with maternal stress in young adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2021) 14:1935–47. doi: 10.1002/aur.2539

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Maenner MJ, Shaw KA, Baio J, Washington A, Patrick M, DiRienzo M, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2016. MMWR Surveill Summ. (2020) 69:1–12. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6904a1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Chenausky K, Brignell A, Morgan A, Tager-Flusberg H. Motor speech impairment predicts expressive language in minimally verbal, but not low verbal, individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Dev Lang Impairments. (2019) 4:2396941519856333. doi: 10.1177/2396941519856333

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Silleresi S. Structural language and nonverbal ability profiles in monolingual and bilingual children with ASD. Tours. (2018).

Google Scholar

57. Smith NV, Tsimpli I-M. The mind of a savant: Language learning and modularity. Malden: Blackwell Publishers (1995).

Google Scholar

58. Zheng S, Kaat A, Farmer C, Thurm A, Burrows C, Kanne S, et al. Bias in measurement of autism symptoms by spoken language level and non-verbal mental age in minimally verbal children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:1051464. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1051464

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Harrop C, Sterrett K, Shih W, Landa R, Kaiser A, Kasari C. Short-term trajectories of restricted and repetitive behaviors in minimally verbal children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2021) 14:1789–99. doi: 10.1002/aur.2528

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Bishop SL, Richler J, Lord C. Association between restricted and repetitive behaviors and nonverbal IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychol. (2006) 12:247–67. doi: 10.1080/09297040600630288

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Pickett E, Pullara O, O’Grady J, Gordon B. Speech acquisition in older nonverbal individuals with autism: a review of features, methods, and prognosis. Cognit Behav Neurol. (2009) 22:1–21. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e318190d185

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Bérubé RL, Achenbach TM. Bibliography of published studies using the ASEBA. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Cneter for Childeren, Youth, and Families (2007).

Google Scholar

63. Einfeld SL, Aman M. Issues in the taxonomy of psychopathology in mental retardation. J Autism Dev Disord. (1995) 25:143–67. doi: 10.1007/BF02178501

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Rosenberg RE, Kaufmann WE, Law JK, Law PA. Parent report of community psychiatric comorbid diagnoses in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res Treat. (2011) 2011:405849. doi: 10.1155/2011/405849

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Salazar F, Baird G, Chandler S, Tseng E, O’sullivan T, Howlin P, et al. Co-occurring psychiatric disorders in preschool and elementary school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2015) 45:2283–94. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2361-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Van Steensel FJ, Bögels SM, Perrin S. Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis. Clin Child Family Psychol Rev. (2011) 14:302–17. doi: 10.1007/s10567-011-0097-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Lerner MD, Mazefsky CA, Weber RJ, Transue E, Siegel M, Gadow KD, et al. Verbal ability and psychiatric symptoms in clinically referred inpatient and outpatient youth with ASD. J Autism Dev Disord. (2018) 48:3689–701. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3344-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Mingins JE, Tarver J, Waite J, Jones C, Surtees AD. Anxiety and intellectual functioning in autistic children: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Autism. (2021) 25:18–32. doi: 10.1177/1362361320953253

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. White SW, Oswald D, Ollendick T, Scahill L. Anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Clin Psychol Rev. (2009) 29:216–29. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.01.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Wood JJ, Gadow KD. Exploring the nature and function of anxiety in youth with autism spectrum disorders. Clin Psychology: Sci Pract. (2010) 17:281. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2010.01220.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Baker-Ericzén MJ, Brookman-Frazee L, Stahmer A. Stress levels and adaptability in parents of toddlers with and without autism spectrum disorders. Res Pract Persons Severe Disabil. (2005) 30:194–204. doi: 10.2511/rpsd.30.4.194

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Tarver J, Pearson E, Edwards G, Shirazi A, Potter L, Malhi P, et al. Anxiety in autistic individuals who speak few or no words: A qualitative study of parental experience and anxiety management. Autism. (2021) 25:429–39. doi: 10.1177/1362361320962366

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Plesa Skwerer D, Joseph RM, Eggleston B, Meyer SR, Tager-Flusberg H. Prevalence and correlates of psychiatric symptoms in minimally verbal children and adolescents with ASD. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:43. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00043

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Estes A, Olson E, Sullivan K, Greenson J, Winter J, Dawson G, et al. Parenting-related stress and psychological distress in mothers of toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Dev. (2013) 35:133–8. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2012.10.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Mugno D, Ruta L, D’Arrigo VG, Mazzone L. Impairment of quality of life in parents of children and adolescents with pervasive developmental disorder. Health Qual Life Outcomes. (2007) 5:22. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-5-22

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Olsson MB, Hwang CP. Depression in mothers and fathers of children with intellectual disability. J Intellect Disabil Res. (2001) 45:535–43. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2001.00372.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Tobing LE, Glenwick DS. Relation of the childhood autism rating scale-parent version to diagnosis, stress, and age. Res Dev Disabil. (2002) 23:211–23. doi: 10.1016/s0891-4222(02)00099-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Bebko JM, Konstantareas MM, Springer J. Parent and professional evaluations of family stress associated with characteristics of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (1987) 17:565–76. doi: 10.1007/BF01486971

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Davis NO, Carter AS. Parenting stress in mothers and fathers of toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: associations with child characteristics. J Autism Dev Disord. (2008) 38:1278–91. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0512-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Medeiros K, Mazurek M, Kanne S. Investigating the factor structure of the Child Behavior Checklist in a large sample of children with autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2017) 40:24–40. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2017.06.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

81. DiStefano C, Shih W, Kaiser A, Landa R, Kasari C. Communication growth in minimally verbal children with ASD: The importance of interaction. Autism Res. (2016) 9:1093–102. doi: 10.1002/aur.1594

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Koegel LK, Bryan KM, Su PL, Vaidya M, Camarata S. Definitions of nonverbal and minimally verbal in research for autism: A systematic review of the literature. J Autism Dev Disord. (2020) 50:2957–72. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04402-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Green J, Charman T, Mc Conachie H, Aldred C, Slonims V, Howlin P, et al. Parent-mediated communication-focused treatment in children with autism (PACT): A randomised controlled trial. Lancet (London England). (2010) 375:2152–60. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)60587-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Valeri G, Casula L, Menghini D, Amendola FA, Napoli E, Pasqualetti P, et al. Cooperative parent-mediated therapy for Italian preschool children with autism spectrum disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2020) 29:935–46. doi: 10.1007/s00787-019-01395-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: verbal language, psychiatric co-occurrences, behavioral symptoms, intellectual functioning, parenting stress, communication difficulties

Citation: Guerrera S, Fucà E, Petrolo E, De Stefano A, Casula L, Logrieco MG, Valeri G and Vicari S (2025) Exploring the clinical features of minimally verbal autistic children. Front. Psychiatry 16:1549092. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1549092

Received: 20 December 2024; Accepted: 03 March 2025;
Published: 20 March 2025.

Edited by:

Antonio Narzisi, Stella Maris Foundation (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Arianna Bentenuto, University of Trento, Italy
Simone Aparecida Lopes-Herrera, University of São Paulo, Brazil

Copyright © 2025 Guerrera, Fucà, Petrolo, De Stefano, Casula, Logrieco, Valeri and Vicari. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Silvia Guerrera, c2lsdmlhLmd1ZXJyZXJhQG9wYmcubmV0

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset

95% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more