
95% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
CONCEPTUAL ANALYSIS article
Front. Psychiatry , 18 March 2025
Sec. ADHD
Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1536207
This article is part of the Research Topic ADHD and Anxiety: Causality Sequences Through a Biopsychosocial Model View all 9 articles
Recent findings on stress and anxiety in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) suggest that specific processes related to brain developmental disorganization could create a vulnerable background that increases sensitivity to stress stimuli from the psychosocial environment. These basic neurodevelopmental processes are closely related to the developmental mechanisms of primitive functions and their integration or disintegration. In this context, the psychopathological processes that manifest in ADHD are linked to the mechanisms of disturbed inhibitory functions that may cause incongruent neural interactions (“neural interference”) between the more primitive functions and the higher levels of attentional and cognitive neural processes. These disturbed developmental processes may also determine increased sensitivity to stressful experiences that, in ADHD cases, could lead to the manifestations of various psychopathological symptoms such as disturbed attentional and motor functions, anxiety, and depression, among other cognitive and affective disturbances. These findings, based on previous research, suggest novel framework and hypothesis on how this neurodevelopment-based increased sensitivity to stress stimuli could manifest in the etiopathogenesis of ADHD in its relationship with cognitive, affective, and motor deficits.
Recent findings related to research on the retained primitive reflexes in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) indicate that the basic processes in ADHD etiopathogenesis may be related to dysfunctions in hierarchical organization during central nervous system (CNS) development when the different brain developmental stages are interconnected on various hierarchical and functional levels (1, 2). Uncovering these processes in more detail is extremely important for future treatment strategies and for the understanding of the complex etiopathogenesis of the disease. Current findings suggest that, in cases of dysfunctional neural development, these hierarchical and functional levels may manifest incongruent interactions (the so-called neural interference) between the early and the later developed brain functions during ontogenesis (1, 3). This neural interference may manifest in the case when the emergence of a new function that should have inserted the older one is not related to the diminishing or sufficient inhibition of this older function, which could lead to “neural disintegration” caused by incongruent neural processes. In this context, recent findings focused on the etiopathogenesis of ADHD suggest that these processes related to brain developmental disorganization could create a vulnerable background that increases sensitivity to stress stimuli from the psychosocial environment (2, 4). This increased sensitivity to stress stimuli that might occur in the etiopathogenesis of ADHD could then be related to various forms of cognitive, affective, and motor deficits that often manifest in individuals with ADHD (1, 3, 4). In this biopsychosocial context, this conceptual analysis focused on recent findings on the retained primitive reflexes in ADHD (2, 3) and provides novel perspectives into understanding the multiple etiopathogenetic factors of this condition. The main focus and objectives of the analysis are the interactions of the neurobiological developmental mechanisms with stress influences in the etiopathogenesis of ADHD, with main implications for developmental disorganization in its relationship with cognitive and affective disintegration and the related psychopathological symptoms that could manifest in ADHD. The currently available models and theories on the pathogenesis of ADHD are mainly focused on the neurobiological developmental mechanisms of this condition, but do not explain how these neural processes during development might interact with stress influences from the psychosocial environment. With this aim to link the neurobiological findings on the pathogenesis of ADHD, we briefly summarize the diagnostic definitions of ADHD to show how basic conceptualizations of “organic” minimal brain dysfunctions change during the time for a more complex understanding of this condition. From this historical perspective, this requirement for a more detailed understanding was mainly influenced by findings that the psychosocial environment and mainly stress stimuli strongly interact with the developmental abnormalities specifically related to the etiopathogenesis of ADHD.
ADHD represents a historically heterogeneous concept that started with the introduction of “minimal brain dysfunction” by Still in 1902 (5), who provided detailed descriptions of the hyperactivity and hyperkinetic symptoms. Much later, in the 1970s, attentional dysfunctions were described by Douglas (6). The historical term “minimal brain dysfunction” was replaced in 1968 by the conceptualization and definition of hyperactivity, but even then was still understood mainly as a result of some biological origin more than of environmental causes (7–9). This concept was later incorporated into the official diagnostic nomenclature described in the second edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-II) (7, 10) as a disorder characterized by overactivity, restlessness, distractibility, and short attention span, particularly in young children, that usually diminishes in adolescence (7–9, 11).
The diagnostic definitions of ADHD in the last decades have reflected an increased knowledge of this condition and its developmental etiopathogenesis. Its recent definitions included in basic diagnostic classification systems, such as the International Classification of Diseases, 10th revision (ICD-10), DSM-IV, and DSM-V, describe the behavioral characteristics of ADHD as related to deficits in “executive functions” that negatively influence the control and regulation of cognitive processes and “self-control” (12). In the case of ADHD as a typical developmental disorder, these neurocognitive characteristics often manifest in the different ontogenetic stages from early childhood to adulthood, which are mainly related to specific deficits in attentional and executive functions (13, 14).
Most typical symptoms according to DSM-IV-TR (DSM-IV, text revision) include excessive motor activity, inattention, and impulsiveness that manifest in childhood (11), while according to the DSM-V definition (15), ADHD is characterized by a pattern of behavior that can be divided into two categories: inattention, and hyperactivity and impulsivity. Children must have at least six symptoms from either (or both) the inattention group of criteria and the hyperactivity and impulsivity criteria, while older adolescents and adults (over 17 years old) must show at least five symptoms. According to DSM-V, symptoms of ADHD must be present prior to age 12 years, compared to 7 years as the age of onset in DSM-IV. In addition, DSM-V does not include the exclusion criteria for people with autism spectrum disorder as the symptoms of both disorders may co-occur.
According to epidemiological data, symptoms of ADHD developed later in adolescence are very similar to ADHD in children with typically increased attentional deficits that are more frequent than hyperactivity and impulsivity, and the treatment procedures in adolescence are also very similar (2, 16–18). Reported evidence also shows that adult symptoms of ADHD, due to disturbances of the executive functions, are significantly related to a higher prevalence of antisocial personality and behavioral disorders. These data show that antisocial personality disorder manifests in 12%–28% of adults with ADHD (in healthy controls, it is 2%–8%), while behavioral disorders manifest in 22%–62% of adults with ADHD (only 4%–8% in healthy controls) (2, 14, 17–25). These highly prevalent antisocial personality and behavioral disorders in adults with ADHD are also related to increased manifestations of criminal behavior, mood disorders, anxiety disorders, and addictive behavior in comparison to healthy controls (19, 25–27). In addition, reported data have shown that antisocial behavior in children is often related to the same difficulties in adulthood in approximately 20%–45% of adults with an ADHD diagnosis (2, 7, 14, 18, 20). For example, according to reported data, approximately 10% or more of individuals in prison have a diagnosis of ADHD (28, 29).
Important prediction factors of later difficulties and bad prognosis are also early manifestations of symptoms of ADHD, and reported data have shown that later manifestations of these ADHD symptoms indicate better prognosis (30–33). Other very important negative factors for future prognosis represent the early occurrence of oppositional defiant disorder, mood disorder and anxiety, and a lower level of intelligence (2, 17, 19, 25, 34, 35). Major negative factors also represent the occurrence of psychopathology in parents, ADHD in other family members, the social and economic status of the family, and frequent conflicting situations and psychosocial stress (2, 24, 25, 34, 36).
The major results in recent ADHD research show that the processes of executive control are typically affected in ADHD and mainly include dysfunctions in inhibitory control (2, 37–39). In addition, recent empirical findings and theoretical conceptualizations indicate that, together with inhibitory dysfunctions, increased emotional excitation may play a role in ADHD deficits. For example, the “cool” cognitive deficits in executive functions are closely linked to attentional dysfunctions; on the other hand, “hot” deficits are related to the dysfunctional ability to process emotional information that produces hyperactivity and impulsivity (2, 39–42). These recent findings indicate that ADHD cannot be explained solely as a consequence of frontal lobe executive dysfunctions. An important role can also be attributed to emotional dysregulation associated with increased excitability in the limbic system, which may cause ADHD disturbances even when frontal executive dysfunctions are not a primary factor in the etiopathology of ADHD (39–42).
Altogether, these findings suggest the so-called dual-pathway concept of the two basic developmental trajectories that could lead to ADHD (43). The first is represented by frontal executive dysfunctions (2, 39, 42–44), while the second is mainly linked to dysfunctions in brain systems related to emotions and motivation (45).
Recent findings suggest that attentional and executive dysfunctions may be related to impulsivity, often observed in ADHD, which can also contribute to social dysfunctions and increased vulnerability to stress-related influence (39). For example, recent findings show that children with ADHD often manifest antisocial behavior, most likely due to deficits in executive functions, impulsivity, and aggressive behavior related to stressful situations (4, 39, 42). Impulsivity is often associated with antisocial behavior, which occurs in 20%–45% of adults with ADHD and contributes to interpersonal problems (14, 18, 20). According to some data, approximately 10% or more of individuals from populations who display various forms of criminal behavior have ADHD (2, 14, 28, 29).
According to epidemiological data, ADHD is related to significantly increased levels of mental stress (4, 39, 46, 47). For a more detailed understanding of how stress could influence individuals with ADHD, it seems extremely important to note that the various functional changes in ADHD and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are frequently similar, and it is possible to expect that the different processes described in stress-related disorders are extremely important in the etiopathogenesis of ADHD (4, 36, 39, 48, 49).
In this context, recent evidence indicates that experiencing traumatic events or repeated stressors in childhood often may cause severe mental problems that could have delayed effects and lead to various neurobiological changes that influence attentional dysfunctions, disturbed cognitive control, and emotional dysregulation (4, 39, 50–52). The development of ADHD is also linked to deficits of neural mechanisms that might underlie specific changes in attentional functions and decreased cognitive control, often associated with impaired inhibitory functions (2, 39–44).
According to neurodevelopmental findings, later developed functions during the ontogenesis of the CNS tend to replace the older ones when higher stages of CNS development have been successfully achieved (1, 53, 54). The development of neural functions based on ontogenetically successive complex neuronal levels enables the performance of more adaptive functions; on the other hand, disinhibition or the release of developmentally older functions from inhibitory control manifests in various neurological and psychiatric disorders (1, 2, 54).
As recent findings show, the highest risk of neural disintegration is during the sensitive developmental stages of brain functions that are also particularly vulnerable to various insults, such as brain damage, toxic influences, or psychological stress (4, 55–57). The particularly important postnatal developmental deficits of higher motor and cognitive functions that likely also have various etiological backgrounds are persisting “primitive reflexes” (3, 58–60), such as symmetric tonic neck reflex (STNR) and asymmetric tonic neck reflex (ATNR), among others (59, 61). These primitive (or primary) reflexes (3, 62, 63) present specific forms of innate “behavioral movement patterns” (64) that are replaced by higher motor and cognitive functions (58–60), and when they occur in the later stages of development, they may present a form of “soft neurological signs” (65).
In this context, recent clinical evidence indicates that manifestations of primitive reflexes in later age than is ontogenetically typical are likely linked to frontal lobe dysfunctions and cortical disinhibition and may occur in various neuropsychiatric syndromes such as ADHD, schizophrenia, depressive and anxiety disorders (3, 66), dementia and Parkinsonism (67), and delirium (68), among other neuropsychiatric disorders (59, 60, 69). These data suggest that persistent (or retained) primary reflexes in general represent evolutionary lower levels of neurophysiological processes that may interfere with processing on higher levels and cause neural disintegration, which may be linked to different neuropsychiatric conditions including ADHD, anxiety, mood disorders, and other mental disorders (1, 54).
According to current evidence, ADHD shares a high comorbidity with anxiety disorders. These findings show that symptoms of anxiety may increase the symptoms of ADHD; on the other hand, deficits of executive functions related to ADHD may increase anxiety (25, 70). Nevertheless, the causal relations where the anxiety would predict ADHD, or the ADHD would predict anxiety, were not found, indicating that both diagnoses could coexist as comorbidities. However, there is no evidence that ADHD could create anxiety as its symptom or that anxiety would implicate ADHD (25, 71). These findings are in agreement with the “dual-pathway” trajectory in the etiopathogenesis of ADHD based on the two interacting systems, where the first is represented by frontal executive dysfunctions and the second is mainly linked to dysfunctions in brain systems related to emotions and motivation (39, 42). This mutual comorbidity and “interplay” between ADHD symptoms and anxiety indicates that inhibitory deficits specifically interact with emotional excitation related to stress stimuli, and the dysfunctional inhibitory systems could cause higher vulnerability with respect to stress stimuli from the social environment (2, 4). On the other hand, in cases of ADHD where the executive dysfunctions are not the main etiopathogenetic factors, increased emotional excitation caused by stress stimuli may also play a role in ADHD deficits and symptoms (2, 4). This interplay between executive dysfunctions and emotional dysregulation due to stressful experiences may then also implicate the observed comorbidities and relationships between the attentional symptoms related to ADHD and the anxiety-related emotional dysregulation in patients with ADHD (2, 4). This interplay between the attentional symptoms of ADHD and the symptoms of anxiety also reflects the dual pathway between the “cool” cognitive deficits mainly related to executive dysfunctions and the “hot” deficits related to the dysfunctional ability to process emotional information that often may produce anxiety, hyperactivity, and impulsivity (39–42). These recent findings on the relationship between ADHD symptoms and anxiety also confirm that ADHD and its symptoms cannot be explained only as a consequence of frontal lobe executive dysfunctions and that important influences on the etiopathogenesis of ADHD are also related to emotional dysregulation that is closely linked to increased excitability in the limbic system (4, 39, 42).
Recent findings suggest that the etiopathogenesis of ADHD could represent a process related to the “incongruent interactions” of the more primitive neural mechanisms, such as the primitive reflexes with higher levels of brain functions, due to an insufficiently developed cognitive and motor integration. This developmental disintegration is also related to the disturbed balance in ADHD (2, 3). In some cases of ADHD, these retained reflexes and incoordination are related to the disturbed balance and attentional dysregulation linked to incongruent interactions (or conflict) between the higher and lower levels of cognitive and motor functions during brain processing (46, 47).
Recent findings also show that a high proportion of individuals with ADHD manifests altered balance and motor abnormalities (72–74). According to brain imaging studies, these balance deficits are likely linked to prefrontal cortex deficits that influence the attention and executive functions (75–77). These dysfunctions could also have a cerebellar origin: individuals with ADHD, in many cases, exhibit atrophy in the cerebellar regions associated with balance and gait control, and these balance and motor dysfunctions are linked to inhibitory deficits due to cerebellar abnormalities (72, 78–80).
In future research, this relationship between the dysregulation of emotional systems and executive dysfunctions could also help in understanding the unresolved relationship between internalizing the symptoms of ADHD, which are mainly related to anxiety and depression, and externalizing the symptoms related to behavioral dysfunctions, which mainly manifest as conduct problems, aggressive behavior, and oppositional defiant disorder (2).
PB: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MP: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by Charles University Project Cooperatio SVV.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
1. Franz EA, Gillett G. John Hughlings Jackson’s evolutionary neurology: a unifying framework for cognitive neuroscience. Brain. (2011) 134:3114–20. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr218
2. Bob P, Konicarova J. ADHD, stress, and development. New York: Springer International Publishing (2018).
3. Bob P, Konicarova J, Raboch J. Disinhibition of primitive reflexes in attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder: insight into specific mechanisms in girls and boys. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.430685
4. Lee SH, Jung EM. Adverse effects of early-life stress: focus on the rodent neuroendocrine system. Neural Regeneration Res. (2024) 19:336–41. doi: 10.4103/1673-5374.377587
5. Still G. The Goulstonian lectures on some abnormal psychical conditions in children. Lecture 1. Lancet. (1902) 1:1008–12.
6. Douglas VI. Stop, look and listen: the problem of sustained attention and impulse control in hyperactive and normal children. Can J Behav Sci. (1972) 4:259–82. doi: 10.1037/h0082313
7. Barkley RA. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2006).
8. Burd L, Kerbeshian J. Historical roots of ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (1988) 27:262. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198803000-00029
9. Lange KW, Reichl S, Lange KM, Tucha L, Tucha O. The history of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disord. (2010) 2:241–55. doi: 10.1007/s12402-010-0045-8
10. Volkmar FR. Changing perspectives on ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. (2003) 160:1025–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1025
11. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-II). 4th edn. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association (2000). Text revision.
12. Hallowell EM, Ratey JJ. Delivered from distraction: Getting the most out of life with attention deficit disorder. New York: Ballantine Books (2005).
13. Seidman LJ. Neuropsychological functioning in people with ADHD across the lifespan. Clin Psychol Rev. (2006) 26:466–85. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.004
14. Cherkasova M, Sulla EM, Dalena KL, Pondé MP, Hechtman L. Developmental course of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and its predictors. J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2013) 22:47–54.
15. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-V). 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing (2014).
16. Biederman J, Mick E, Faraone SV. Age-dependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Impact of remission definition and symptom type. Am J Psychiatry. (2000) 157:816–8. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.816
17. Molina BSG, Hinshaw SP, Swanson JM, Arnold LE, Vitiello B, Jensen PS, et al. The MTA at 8 Years: Prospective follow-up of children treated for combined-type ADHD in a multisite study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2009) 48:484–500. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819c23d0
18. Caye A, Spadini AV, Karam RG, Grevet EH, Rovaris DL, Bau CH, et al. Predictors of persistence of ADHD into adulthood: a systematic review of the literature and meta-analysis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2016) 25:1151–9. doi: 10.1007/s00787-016-0831-8
19. Barkley RA, Murphy KR, Firscher M. ADHD in adults: What the Science Says. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2008).
20. Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Mick E, Spencer T, Wilens TE, Silva JM, et al. Young adult outcome of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A controlled 10-year follow-up study. psychol Med. (2006) 36:167–79. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006410
21. Klein RG, Mannuzza S, Olazagasti MA, Roizen E, Hutchison JA, Lashua EC, et al. Clinical and functional outcome of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder 33 years later. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2012) 69:1295–303. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.271
22. Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bessler A, Malloy P, Hynes ME. Educational and occupational outcome of hyperactive boys grown up. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (1997) 36:1222–7. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199709000-00014
23. Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bessler A, Malloy P, LaPadula M. Adult psychiatric status of hyperactive boys grown up. Am J Psychiatry. (1998) 155:493–8. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.4.493
24. Weiss G, Hechtman L. Hyperactive children grown up: ADHD in children, adolescents, and adults. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press (1993).
25. Gair SL, Brown HR, Kang S, Grabell AS, Harvey EA. Early development of comorbidity between symptoms of ADHD and anxiety. Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol. (2021) 49:311–23. doi: 10.1007/s10802-020-00724-6
26. Kessler RC, Adler L, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners CK, Demler O, et al. The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: Results from the national comorbidity survey replication. Am J Psychiatry. (2006) 163:716–23. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.4.716
27. Sobanski E. Psychiatric comorbidity in adults with attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Vol. 256. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience (2006) p. i26–31.
28. Black DW, Arndt S, Hale N, Rogerson R. Use of the mini international neuropsychiatric interview (MINI) as a screening tool in prisons: Results of a preliminary study. J Am Acad Psychiatry Law. (2004) 32:158–62.
29. Gunter TD, Arndt S, Wenman G, Allen J, Loveless P, Sieleni B, et al. Frequency of mental and addictive disorders among 320 men and women entering the iowa prison system: Use of the MINI-plus. J Am Acad Psychiatry Law. (2008) 36:27–34.
30. Brocki KC, Nyberg L, Thorell LB, Bohlin G. Early concurrent and longitudinal symptoms of ADHD and ODD: Relations to different types of inhibitory control and working memory. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2007) 48:1033–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01811.x
31. LeBlanc N, Boivin M, Dionne G, Brendgen M, Vitaro F, Tremblay R, et al. The development of hyperactive–impulsive behaviors during the preschool years: The predictive validity of parental assessments. J Abnormal Child Psychol. (2008) 36:977–87. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9227-7
32. Wahlstedt C, Thorell LB, Bohlin G. ADHD symptoms and executive function impairment: Early predictors of later behavioral problems. Dev Neuropsychol. (2008) 33:160–78. doi: 10.1080/87565640701884253
33. Chronis AM, Lahey BB, Pelham WE Jr., Williams SH, Baumann BL, Kipp H, et al. Maternal depression and early positive parenting predict future conduct problems in young children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dev Psychol. (2007) 43:70–82. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.70
34. Biederman J, Petty CR, Clarke A, Lomedico A, Faraone SV. Predictors of persistent ADHD: An 11-year follow-up study. J Psychiatr Res. (2011) 45:150–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.06.009
35. Swanson JM, Hinshaw SP, Arnold LE, Gibbons RD, Marcus SUE, Hur K, et al. Secondary evaluations of MTA 36-month outcomes: Propensity score and growth mixture model analyses. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2007) 46:1003–14. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3180686d63
36. Punski-Hoogervorst JL, Engel-Yeger B, Avital A. Attention deficits as a key player in the symptomatology of posttraumatic stress disorder: A review. J Neurosci Res. (2023) 101:1068–85. doi: 10.1002/jnr.v101.7
37. Nigg JT, Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Sonuga-Barke EJ. Causal heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: do we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 57:1224–30. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.025
38. Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley AD. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn Sci. (2012) 16:174–80. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006
39. Martinez L, Prada E, Satler C, Tavares MC, Tomaz C. Executive dysfunctions: the role in attention deficit hyperactivity and post-traumatic stress neuropsychiatric disorders. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:1230. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01230
40. Castellanos FX, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Milham MP, Tannock R. Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction. Trends Cogn Sci. (2006) 10:117–23. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011
41. Toplak ME, Jain U, Tannock R. Executive and motivational processes in adolescents with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Behav Brain Functions. (2005) 1:8. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-1-8
42. Antonini TN, Becker SP, Tamm L, Epstein JN. Hot and cool executive functions in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and comorbid oppositional defiant disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2015) 21:584–95. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000752
43. Sonuga-Barke EJ. The dual pathway model of AD/HD: an elaboration of neuro-developmental characteristics. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2003) 27:593–604. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.08.005
44. Solanto MV, Abikoff H, Sonuga-Barke E, Schachar R, Logan GD, Wigal T, et al. The ecological validity of delay aversion and response inhibition as measures of impulsivity in AD/HD a supplement to the NIMH multi-modal treatment study of AD/HD. J Abnormal Child Psychol. (2001) 29:215–28. doi: 10.1023/A:1010329714819
46. Johnson KA, Robertson IH, Kelly SP, Silk TJ, Barry E, Daibhis A, et al. Dissociation in performance of children with ADHD and high-functioning autism on a task of sustained attention. Neuropsychologia. (2007) 45:2234–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.019
47. Sugar J, Ford JD. Peritraumatic reactions and posttraumatic stress disorder in psychiatrically impaired youth. J Traumatic Stress. (2012) 25:41–9. doi: 10.1002/jts.21668
48. Adler LA, Kunz M, Chua HC, Rotrosen J, Resnick SG. Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adult patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): Is ADHD a vulnerability factor? J Attentional Disord. (2004) 8:11–6. doi: 10.1177/108705470400800102
49. Daud A, Rydelius PA. Comorbidity/overlapping between ADHD and PTSD in relation to IQ among children of traumatized/non-traumatized parents. J Attentional Disord. (2009) 13:188–96. doi: 10.1177/1087054708326271
50. Henry JP. Psychological and physiological responses to stress: the right hemisphere and the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis, an inquiry into problems of human bonding. Integr Physiol Behav Sci. (1993) 28:369–87. doi: 10.1007/BF02690935
51. Henry JP. Psychological and physiological responses to stress: the right hemisphere and the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis, an inquiry into problems of human bonding. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica. (1997) 161:10–25.
52. Hasler G, van der Veen JW, Grillon C, Drevets WC, Shen J. Effect of acute psychological stress on prefrontal GABA concentration determined by proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Am J Psychiatry. (2010) 167:1226–31. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09070994
53. Andermann AA. Hughlings Jackson’s deductive science of the nervous system: a product of his thought collective and formative years. Neurology. (1997) 48:471–81. doi: 10.1212/WNL.48.2.471
54. Jacyna LS. Process and progress: John Hughlings Jackson’s philosophy of science. Brain. (2011) 134:3121–6. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr236
55. Teicher MH, Tomoda A, Andersen SL. Neurobiological consequences of early stress and childhood maltreatment: are results from human and animal studies comparable? Ann New York Acad Sci. (2006) 1071:313–23.
56. Fagiolini M, Jensen CL, Champagne FA. Epigenetic influences on brain development and plasticity. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2009) 19:207–12. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.05.009
57. Kolb B, Gibb R. Brain plasticity and behaviour in the developing brain. J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2011) 20:265–76.
58. Allen MC, Capute AJ. The evolution of primitive reflexes in extremely premature infants. Pediatr Res. (1986) 20:1284–9. doi: 10.1203/00006450-198612000-00018
59. Zafeiriou DI. Primitive reflexes and postural reactions in the neurodevelopmental examination. Pediatr Neurol. (2004) 31:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2004.01.012
61. Ellis MD, Drogos J, Carmona C, Keller T, Dewald JP. Neck rotation modulates flexion synergy torques indicating an ipsilateral reticulospinal source for impairment in stroke. J Neurophysiol. (2012) 108:3096–104. doi: 10.1152/jn.01030.2011
62. Touwen BCL. Primitive reflexes-conceptional or semantic problem. In: Prechtl HFR, editor. Continuity of neural functions from prenatal to postnatal life. Spastics International Medical Publications, Oxford, Great Britain (1984).
63. Capute AJ, Accardo PJ. Developmental disabilities in infancy and childhood. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brooks (1991).
64. Niklasson M. The relation between postural movement and bilateral motor integration: Comment on Lin, et al., (2012). Perceptual Motor Skills. (2013) 117:647–50.
65. Polatajko HJ. Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD): alias, the clumsy child syndrome. In: Whitmore K, Hart H, Willems G, editors. A neurodevelopmental approach to specific learning disorders. Mac Keith Press, London (1999). p. 119–33.
66. Youssef HA, Waddington JL. Primitive (developmental) reflexes and diffuse cerebral dysfunction in schizophrenia and bipolar affective disorder: overrepresentation in patients with tardive dyskinesia. Biol Psychiatry. (1988) 23:791–6. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(88)90067-4
67. Links KA, Merims D, Binns MA, Freedman M, Chow TW. Prevalence of primitive reflexes and Parkinsonian signs in dementia. Can J Neurological Sci. (2010) 37:601–7. doi: 10.1017/S0317167100010763
68. Nicolson SE, Chabon B, Larsen KA, Kelly SE, Potter AW, Stern TA. Primitive reflexes associated with delirium: a prospective trial. Psychosomatics. (2011) 52:507–12. doi: 10.1016/j.psym.2011.06.008
69. Keshavan MS, Yeragani VK. Primitive reflexes in psychiatry. Lancet. (1987) 1:1264. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(87)92714-0
70. Bubier JL, Drabick DA. Co-occurring anxiety and disruptive behavior disorders: The roles of anxious symptoms, reactive aggression, and shared risk processes. Clin Psychol Rev. (2009) 29:658–69. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.08.005
71. Overgaard KR, Aase H, Torgersen S, Zeiner P. Co-occurrence of ADHD and anxiety in preschool children. J Attention Disord. (2016) 20:573–80. doi: 10.1177/1087054712463063
72. Buderath P, Gärtner K, Frings M, Christiansen H, Schoch B, Konczak J, et al. Postural and gait performance in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Gait Posture. (2009) 29:249–54. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.08.016
73. D’Agati E, Casarelli L, Pitzianti MB, Pasini A. Overflow movements and white matter abnormalities in ADHD. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 34:441–5. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.01.013
74. Ghanizadeh A. Predictors of postural stability in children with ADHD. J Attention Disord. (2011) 15:604–10. doi: 10.1177/1087054710370936
75. Arnsten AF. Toward a new understanding of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder pathophysiology: an important role for prefrontal cortex dysfunction. CNS Drugs. (2009) 23:33–41. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200923000-00005
76. Shaw P, Rabin C. New insights into attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using structural neuroimaging. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2009) 11:393–8. doi: 10.1007/s11920-009-0059-0
77. Makris N, Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Seidman LJ. Towards conceptualizing a neural systems-based anatomy of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dev Neurosci. (2009) 31:36–49. doi: 10.1159/000207492
78. Berquin PC, Giedd JN, Jacobsen LK, Hamburger SD, Krain AL, Rapoport JL, et al. Cerebellum in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a morphometric MRI study. Neurology. (1998) 50:1087–93. doi: 10.1212/WNL.50.4.1087
79. Baillieux H, De Smet HJ, Paquier PF, De Deyn PP, Marien P. Cerebellar neurocognition: insights into the bottom of the brain. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. (2008) 110:763–73. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2008.05.013
Keywords: ADHD, anxiety, depression, stress, developmental disintegration, primitive reflexes, neural interference
Citation: Bob P and Privara M (2025) ADHD, stress, and anxiety. Front. Psychiatry 16:1536207. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1536207
Received: 28 November 2024; Accepted: 21 February 2025;
Published: 18 March 2025.
Edited by:
Hongbao Cao, George Mason University, United StatesReviewed by:
Xiaorong Guo, Feinstein Institute for Medical Research, United StatesCopyright © 2025 Bob and Privara. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Petr Bob, cGV0cmJvYkBuZXRzY2FwZS5uZXQ=
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.