Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 30 January 2025
Sec. Adolescent and Young Adult Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic Life with Tic Disorders: From Childhood to Adulthood View all articles

Premonitory urge in tic disorders – a scoping review

  • 1Department of Neurology, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York City, NY, United States
  • 2Department of Neurology, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, United States
  • 3Annette and Irwin Eskind Family Biomedical Library and Learning Center, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, United States

Introduction: Premonitory urges are uncomfortable bodily sensations preceding tics. They are highly prevalent, frequently bothersome, and increasingly recognized as a central phenotypic feature in tic disorder populations. This scoping review aimed to systematically consolidate published knowledge and identify knowledge gaps regarding premonitory urges in primary tic disorders.

Methods: Search strategies were deployed in five databases and five topic-relevant journals. Two independent reviewers screened all candidate abstracts against predefined inclusion criteria. One hundred and fifty-five articles were included in the scoping review. The same two reviewers independently extracted and consolidated pertinent data from included articles.

Results: Multiple methods for assessing premonitory urge were identified, each with strengths and weaknesses. The subjective quality of premonitory urges varies between individuals, with increased prevalence of a “not just right” urge quality in individuals with comorbid obsessive-compulsive disorder. Awareness of premonitory urge appears to arise several years after tic-onset, yet many individuals perceive their tics as voluntary responses to premonitory urges. Premonitory urges and tics are temporally coupled in real time, but premonitory urge severity and tic severity, as assessed by clinical scales, are not consistently associated. The mechanistic and developmental relationship between premonitory urges and tics remains unclear. Data are limited on premonitory urge response to treatment, but several promising interventions were identified. The insula and supplementary motor area are the neuroanatomical structures most strongly implicated in emergence of the premonitory urge.

Discussion: Knowledge of the clinical characteristics, measurement, and neural mechanisms of premonitory urge has advanced considerably in recent years, but important knowledge gaps remain in each of these domains. Addressing these knowledge gaps will be key to developing effective interventions for premonitory urge.

Systematic Review Registration: Open Science Framework (OSF) https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/WT43Z.

1 Introduction

Primary tic disorders (1), including Tourette syndrome (TS), chronic motor tic disorder, chronic vocal tic disorder, and transient tic disorder, are estimated to affect 0.77%, 1.65%, 0.69%, and 2.99% of the global population, respectively (2). Tics are the defining feature of these disorders, but the vast majority of individuals in these populations also experience premonitory urges. Premonitory urges are uncomfortable bodily sensations preceding tics, often building in the moments before a tic and resolving once the tic is performed. In Cohen and Leckman’s seminal 1992 study of sensory phenomena in TS, nearly 80% of participants endorsed premonitory urges, and nearly 60% reported their premonitory urges were more distressing than tics themselves (3). Subsequent research in other tic disorder samples has replicated these original findings, demonstrating that premonitory urges are common, particularly in adolescence and adulthood, and bothersome.

Since the initial publications describing premonitory urges more than thirty years ago, numerous studies have sought to characterize them, clarify their relationship with tics, identify their clinical and neurobiological correlates, and, to a lesser extent, treat them. As a result of these research endeavors, knowledge of premonitory urges has significantly advanced over the past three decades. However, given the complexity of the phenomenon, as well as the diverse methods employed and populations recruited to study it, much ambiguity remains concerning premonitory urges. Critically, the developmental and pathophysiologic interrelationship between premonitory urges and tics is unclear, and an effective treatment for premonitory urges has yet to be established. A comprehensive review of published research is needed to consolidate the breadth of current knowledge about premonitory urges and, equally important, to identify limitations and gaps in this knowledge. Reviews of the premonitory urge literature have previously been conducted but have focused on specific aspects of the phenomenon (4, 5) (e.g., neurobiological correlates) or adopted a non-systematic approach (6, 7). To address these limitations, we conducted a scoping review of premonitory urge in primary tic disorders.

2 Methods

This scoping review was conducted in accordance with the Joanna Briggs Institute (JBI) methodology for scoping reviews (8). The study protocol was registered on Open Science Framework (OSF) (https://osf.io/b6tnf/files/osfstorage/65b18bb94aa63c067bdf2550). To be eligible for inclusion, studies had to be original research, published in English, and contain information about premonitory urge in primary tic disorders. There was no restriction based on study date. Studies involving participants with functional tic-like behaviors (9) were not included unless participants with primary tic disorders were also enrolled; for such studies, only data on individuals with primary tic disorders were reviewed and extracted. This scoping review considered experimental and quasi-experimental studies (i.e., randomized controlled trials, non-randomized controlled trials, before and after studies, and interrupted time-series studies), analytical observational studies (i.e., prospective and retrospective cohort studies, case-control studies, and analytical cross-sectional studies), descriptive observational studies (i.e., case series, individual case reports, and descriptive cross-sectional studies), and qualitative studies. Conference abstracts, grey literature, unpublished works, literature reviews of any type, and opinion papers were excluded.

PubMed (National Library of Medicine), Embase (Elsevier), PsycINFO (ProQuest), Web of Science (Clarivate), and Scopus (Elsevier) were searched for relevant articles. The journals Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, CNS Spectrums, Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, Behavioral Neurology, and Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology were also independently searched as they are not fully indexed in the included databases. A biomedical librarian (K.D.G.) developed the search strategy consisting of a combination of relevant keywords and database-specific subject terms. The search was conducted on January 9, 2023, and a revised search was conducted on January 24, 2024 after the authors identified studies not captured by the original search. The finalized search strategies are available in the Supplementary Material. Following the search, all identified citations were imported into Covidence (10), a systematic review management software. Covidence was also used to deduplicate results from the revised searched. Titles and abstracts of all imported publications were independently evaluated for inclusion or exclusion by two reviewers (D.A.I. and J.B.W.). Subsequently, the same two reviewers (D.A.I. and J.B.W.) independently assessed the full text of the included publications using the predefined inclusion criteria. Inter-reviewer disagreements at each stage of the selection process were resolved by the independent decision of a third reviewer (K.H.W.). Results of the search and study inclusion process are presented in a Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses extension for scoping review (PRISMA-ScR) flow diagram (Figure 1) (11).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Premonitory urge in tic disorders.

Data were extracted by two independent reviewers (D.A.I. and J.B.W.) using a data extraction tool (presented in the Supplementary Material) developed by the reviewers. The following data, if relevant and available, were extracted from eligible articles: 1) clinical characteristics of the study sample; 2) method of premonitory urge assessment; 3) qualitative and/or quantitative characteristics of premonitory urge; 4) associations between premonitory urge characteristics and other clinical characteristics; 5) premonitory urge response to treatment and/or tic suppression; and 6) neural correlates of premonitory urge and method for investigating such neural correlates. Within each of the above categories, additional specific data elements were extracted (as shown in the Supplementary Material). Following data extraction for all studies, the two reviewers consolidated their findings and arrived at a consensus as to the relevant data to be included from each article. All relevant findings were grouped into one of the above six categories. The overarching term “tic disorder” is used in this review to refer to all primary tic disorders including TS, chronic (persistent) motor tic disorder, chronic (persistent) vocal tic disorder, and transient tic disorder, but not functional tic disorder. The term “CTD” is used to refer collectively to TS, chronic motor tic disorder, and chronic vocal tic disorder.

3 Results

3.1 Methods of premonitory urge ascertainment

Of the 155 studies meeting inclusion criteria, 25 examined methods to measure premonitory urge (Table 1 contains descriptions of assessment methods). The majority of these studies employed questionnaire-based methods, but a small minority employed urge monitors.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Methods of premonitory urge measurement.

3.1.1 Premonitory urge for tics scale

The most commonly used questionnaire was the Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale (PUTS), a 10-item, self-report scale developed by Woods et al. (12). The questionnaire contains 9 statements about premonitory urges and 1 statement about tic suppressibility. Respondents indicate their degree of agreement with each statement on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“Not at all”) to 4 (“Very much”). Regarding content validity [i.e., the extent to which the phenomenon of interest is thoroughly represented by the questionnaire items (13)], a description of the two-stage method for PUTS item selection is provided in Woods et al’s original publication. In the initial validation study, PUTS item 10 (“I am able to stop my tics, even if only for a short period of time”) correlated poorly with the rest of the scale (12), a finding subsequently replicated in multiple other studies (1419). Consequently, the tenth item is often excluded from the PUTS total score. Supplementary Table S1 contains a full summary of publications examining psychometric properties of the PUTS.

The original PUTS, developed in English, has been translated into multiple languages: German, Greek, Hebrew, Italian, Japanese, Korean, Mandarin/Cantonese, and Spanish. The translation process is described in some (2026) but not all (14, 15, 17, 27) studies. No studies involving non-English versions of the PUTS reported re-assessing content validity of the translated scale, though this scoping review did not encompass non-English publications.

Twenty studies (12, 1432) examined internal consistency of the PUTS, with Cronbach’s alpha values for the 9-item PUTS ranging from 0.75 (23, 27, 31) to 0.85 (24) in sample populations ranging in size from 22 (15) to 656 (17). Seven of these studies assessed internal consistency across children of different age groups within their sample, typically stratifying participants into those 10-years-old and younger and those older than 10 years (12, 17, 18, 2023). Four of these studies found lower Cronbach’s alpha values (ranging from 0.57 to 0.70) in the younger age group (12, 18, 20, 21), but the other three studies did not observe this pattern (17, 22, 23). Three studies observed similar Cronbach’s alpha values between youth (< 18- years-old) and adults (14, 15, 24). Overall, the 9-item PUTS has shown acceptable internal consistency, with conflicting findings on the scale’s internal consistency in tic disorder populations younger than 10 years of age.

PUTS scores have been found to be temporally stable over a range of less than two weeks to four months (12, 19, 22, 23, 26), with between-timepoint correlations ranging from 0.49 (22) to 0.89 (23). In the largest of these studies, Li et al. observed a between-timepoint correlation of 0.89 at one month in 147 children with tic disorder (23), and Reese et al. observed a between-timepoint correlation of 0.79 at two weeks in 122 adolescents and adults with CTD (19). A single study examined differences in temporal stability across age groups, administering the Spanish version of the 9-item PUTS at baseline and 4-month follow-up (22). The correlation between scores was significant and positive for the subgroup older than 10 years of age and non-significant for the subgroup 10-years-old and younger (sample sizes of these subgroups were not reported). Notably, to assess temporal reliability of the PUTS, the above studies either employed Pearson’s correlations or did not report the specific correlation method used (12, 19, 23); no study reported intraclass correlations.

A number of studies conducted factor analyses to assess the dimensions of premonitory urge captured by the PUTS, with mixed results (14, 15, 17, 18, 2224, 26). Study sample sizes varied widely, ranging from 38 (26) to 656 (17) participants, with many studies involving fewer than 100 participants (15, 18, 22, 24, 26). Multiple studies found that items 4 and 5 loaded onto a factor interpreted as “OCD-related items” or “just right phenomena” (17, 22, 24, 26). Other commonly proposed factors included “quality” (14, 15, 18, 22, 24, 26) and “intensity” (14, 15, 23, 24), although the items composing these factors were inconsistent across studies.

A single study, in a sample of 102 adults with TS, examined floor and ceiling effects of the PUTS (16). Both floor (3.9%) and ceiling (2.0%) effects were found to be small.

3.1.2 Individualized premonitory urge for tics scale

Whereas the PUTS is a self-report questionnaire, the Individualized Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale (I-PUTS) is a clinician-administered scale. The clinician assesses the presence, bodily location, frequency, and intensity of urges for individual tics experienced over the past week, employing a checklist analogous to the tic checklist in the Yale Global Tic Severity Scale (YGTSS). Each urge is rated on a 4-point Likert scale for both frequency and intensity. The scale generates three scores: urge number, urge frequency, and urge intensity (33). The authors note that the I-PUTS, in contrast to the PUTS, permits assessment of individual premonitory urges, quantifies premonitory urge in three separate dimensions, and specifies the timeframe in question.

In the initial validation study, McGuire et al. administered the I-PUTS to 75 children with tic disorders and found excellent inter-rater reliability (33). The I-PUTS scores did not significantly correlate with scores from scales assessing severity of common comorbid psychiatric symptoms. In contrast, the PUTS score, in the same study, did significantly correlate with some psychiatric symptom severity scores, suggesting that the I-PUTS possesses better divergent validity than the PUTS. The three I-PUTS scores showed small correlations (I-PUTS number: r = 0.28, p < 0.02, I-PUTS frequency: r = 0.23, p < 0.05, I-PUTS intensity: r = 0.19, p = 0.10) with the 9-item PUTS score. The study did not examine the test-retest reliability of the I-PUTS. Che et al. (34) translated the I-PUTS into Chinese, describing their methods for doing so, and then administered the resultant Chinese I-PUTS to 123 children with tic disorder. They found good internal consistency, both for children 8-10-years-old and children 11-14-years-old; good test-retest reliability at one month; and good inter-rater reliability for all three I-PUTS scores (34). The three Chinese I-PUTS scores exhibited significant, medium-sized positive correlations with the Chinese PUTS score (I-PUTS number: r = 0.433, p < 0.01, I-PUTS frequency: r = 0.486, p < 0.01, I-PUTS intensity: r = 0.489, p < 0.01).

3.1.3 Urge monitors

In contrast to the above studies employing questionnaires or structured interviews, several studies have used a “live urge monitor” (15, 3537) or an urge thermometer (3842) to precisely gauge premonitory urge intensity over short time periods. In a factor analysis including real-time urge intensity (average live urge monitor score over five minutes) and the 10 PUTS items, real-time urge intensity loaded on the second of the factors, which the authors interpreted as “intensity” of premonitory urges (15).

3.2 Characteristics of premonitory urge

3.2.1 Prevalence of premonitory urges

Prevalence estimates of premonitory urges in individuals with tic disorders range from 37% (43) to 93% (3, 19, 34, 4351). Prevalence rates vary by age of the sample population. In a sample of 656 children with CTDs, prevalence of premonitory urges, as assessed by the PUTS, was 81% for children 7-years-old and younger, 95.5% for children 8-10-years-old, and 97.5% for children 10-years-old and older (17). The following two studies, which asked individuals categorical questions about the presence of premonitory urges (“yes/no” or “yes/no/don’t know” response options) rather than administering a scale, observed lower prevalence estimates. In Sambrani et al’s study of 1,032 individuals with tic disorders, premonitory urges were reported by 46.7% of those less than 10-years-old, 61.3% of those 10-12-years-old, and 79.7% of those 12-years-old and older (50). In Banaschewski et al’s study of 251 children and adolescents with TS, premonitory urges were reported by 24% of 8-10-year-olds, 34% of 11-14-year-olds, and 57% of 15-19-year-olds (43).

3.2.2 The nature and subjective quality of premonitory urges

In a sample of 122 adolescents and adults with CTDs, the most frequently endorsed premonitory urge qualities from the PUTS were “an energy in my body that needs to get out” (>80%) and “an inner feeling of being wound up or tense” (>80%) (19). In another study of 656 youth with CTDs, “an energy in my body that needs to get out” was again the most frequently endorsed urge quality from the PUTS (17). Schunke et al. (52) administered a questionnaire to 14 adults with TS, allowing participants to select the most accurate descriptors for their premonitory urges from a list of 14 different qualities. The three most selected qualities were inner urge (100%), increased tension (71%), and urge to move (64%) (52). In a sample of 291 adults with CTDs, the following premonitory urge qualities were endorsed, in order of decreasing frequency (based on the publication’s Supplementary Table S1): “feelings of tension” (61%), “pressure” (45%), “feeling discomfort” (43%), “a feeling of energy that needs to be released” (40%), “a not-just-right feeling” (33%), “the feeling that something was building up” (31%), “an itch” (29%), and “incompleteness” (21%) (53). Participants also rated urge intensity on a 1-to-11-point scale in this study; urge intensity did not significantly differ between the above urge qualities. In contrast to the more quantitative approaches discussed above, two case reports and series provide narratives of individuals with tics describing their experience of premonitory urges (3, 54).

The quality of premonitory urges appears to differ in tic disorder subgroups with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and/or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) compared to those without these comorbidities. In particular, multiple studies have observed increased prevalence (47, 53) and/or intensity (19) of the “not-just-right” quality of premonitory urges in individuals with tic disorder and comorbid OCD or obsessive-compulsive symptoms. In a study of 74 individuals with CTDs, Edwards et al. found that the subgroup of participants with comorbid ADHD and/or OCD reported greater “tense” or “wound up” feelings (PUTS item 3) than those without comorbid ADHD or OCD, though PUTS total scores did not significantly differ between groups (32).

Based on only two small studies, premonitory urges are distressing for some individuals with tics. In Cohen and Leckman’s study, 57% of 28 participants reported their urges to be “more bothersome than the tics themselves” (3). In more recent interviews of 42 youth with TS, some found their urges to be “uncomfortable or painful” and perceived reducing their urges as “an important outcome of treatment” (55).

3.2.3 Number and frequency of premonitory urges

Among individuals who experience premonitory urges, significant variability exists as to the number and frequency of the urges. In one study with 135 adults and children with tic disorders, participants endorsed an average of 8.7 distinct urges (48), while in a different study of 75 children with tic disorders, participants endorsed an average of 2.9 distinct urges (33). Among a sample of 50 children and adults with TS, 92% reported premonitory urges, but only 35% endorsed premonitory urges with all of their tics (47). In this same study, only 3% of participants reported urges with fewer than 25% of their tics. In a sample of 75 children with tic disorders, McGuire et al. found that for a given tic with an associated premonitory urge, participants “experienced urges over 50% of the time they had the tic” (33).

3.2.4 Premonitory urge location

In their seminal study, Cohen and Leckman found that, of 28 children and adults with TS, 45% felt premonitory urges “in their mind,” while the other 55% felt the urge “in another body area” (3). In a subsequent study of children and adults with tic disorders, Leckman et al. found that, of 123 participants with premonitory urges, 89% reported their urges were “either partly or wholly a physical experience” as opposed to a mental one (48). O’Connor et al. posed a similar question to 60 adults with CTDs and found that, of the participants that experienced premonitory urges (>80%), only two reported the sensation was mental as opposed to physical (49). In their aforementioned study, Leckman et al. found that 40% of participants felt the urge exclusively in muscle, 24% felt it in both muscle and joints, and 8% felt it exclusively in joints (48). Similarly, Kwak et al. found that urges were more frequently perceived to occur in muscles than in joints or skin (47). Among 291 adults with CTDs, premonitory urges for motor tics were most frequently felt “exactly at the location or in direct proximity to the corresponding tic” (44). However, 52.6% of participants were unable to report a location for at least one premonitory urge (“diffuse, non-circumscribed premonitory urge”), and 3.9% of participants endorsed an urge located “outside the body” for at least one of their tics. In this study, a diffuse premonitory urge location was more common for complex tics than simple tics and more common for vocal tics than motor tics (44). In a large study of 700 children and adults with tic disorders who experienced premonitory urges, 64% reported their urges were localized, 16% reported their urges were diffuse, and 13.7% were unable to identify the precise nature of their urges (50). These findings generally align with those of a smaller study of 19 children and adults with TS: 53% reported their urges as focal sensations, 11% as generalized sensations, and 37% as both generalized and focal sensations (56).

The face and shoulders have tended to predominate as the most common bodily locations for premonitory urges (33, 34, 44, 47, 48, 52). Two studies utilized the I-PUTS to determine the relative intensity of premonitory urges across bodily locations. In these studies, urges in the “whole body/other region” (33) and the “neck/throat region” (34), respectively, were rated as most intense.

3.2.5 Development of premonitory urges over time

As aforementioned, prevalence rates of premonitory urge increase over the course of development (17, 43, 50). The trajectory of premonitory urges, however, is unclear due to a paucity of longitudinal investigations. In their cross-sectional study of 135 children and adults with tic disorders, Leckman et al. found that individuals who experienced premonitory urges (n = 123) recalled first becoming aware of their urges at 10.0-years-old (SD = 6.2 years) (48). Awareness of premonitory urges emerged an average of 3.1 years (SD = 5.7 years) after tic-onset. Gulisano et al. administered the PUTS to 95 children with TS at baseline (mean age = 7.3 years, SD = 1.5 years) and at long-term follow-up (mean age = 13.1 years, SD = 3.7 years), observing that mean PUTS score was significantly higher at follow-up (24.1) than at baseline (13.5) (21).

Multiple cross-sectional studies have compared measures of premonitory urge severity between different age groups. Kyriazi et al. found that children with tic disorder who were 12-15-years-old (n = 13) had significantly higher PUTS scores than those who were 6-11-years-old (n = 39) (25). Yet, other studies have failed to identify significant differences in PUTS scores between older and younger youth (12, 18, 20, 23) or between adults and youth (24, 26). Langelage et al. (37) collected data using a live urge monitor over five minutes in 25 youth (8-18-years-old) with TS and compared their results to those from Schubert et al. (36), a similar study in 21 adults with TS (mean age = 30.5 years, SD = 10.6 years). They found that average urge monitor intensity ratings during the task did not significantly differ between youth and adult samples.

Other cross-sectional studies have examined the correlation between measures of premonitory urge severity and age. In some studies of children and adolescents, age positively correlated with premonitory urge severity, as indexed by the PUTS (29, 33), but other studies failed to identify a significant correlation using the PUTS (34, 57, 58) or the I-PUTS (33, 34). Most studies involving adults have not observed a significant correlation between age and PUTS score (16, 19, 59, 60), though one small study of 18 adults and children with TS did observe a significant positive association (61). In contrast, Brandt et al., assessing urge intensity on an 11-point Likert scale in 291 adults with CTDs, found that younger age was associated with more intense urges; however, older age was associated with a greater likelihood of experiencing urges (dichotomous variable), after accounting for gender and psychiatric comorbidity (53).

3.2.6 Premonitory urge relationship to tics

Likelihood of premonitory urges appears to vary by tic location, complexity, and severity. Tics of the head, neck, and shoulders were most commonly associated with a premonitory urge in a sample of 135 children and adults with tic disorders (48). In another study of 291 adults with CTD, premonitory urges were reported for 80% of complex motor tics compared to only 67% of simple motor tics (44). Among 240 individuals with CTDs, tics with an associated premonitory urge were on average rated as more severe compared to tics without an associated urge (62).

Evidence indicates that tics generally alleviate premonitory urges. Reese et al. found that 85% of 122 older adolescents and adults with CTD endorsed PUTS item 9 (“After I do a tic, [the sensation] goes away at least for a little while”) (19), and Essing et al. (44) found that 97% of 291 adults with CTD reported relief from their urges following at least one of their tics. In two studies involving both children and adults with TS (with sample sizes of 28 and 50), over three-fourths of participants (75% and 83%, respectively) reported relief from premonitory urge following a tic (3, 47). Moreover, 71% (3) and 67% (47) of participants believed their tics were voluntary responses to their premonitory urges. In the latter of these two studies, 88% of participants endorsed an increase in urge intensity if prevented from executing their motor tics (47).

A few studies have utilized a live urge monitor to examine the relationship between urges and tics in real time. Brandt et al. assessed real-time urge intensity in 16 adults with TS using a live urge monitor while two independent raters recorded tic occurrence (35). In both free ticcing and tic suppression conditions, urge intensity tended to increase prior to a tic and decrease after a tic. In a separate study of 22 adults with TS, number of tics during a five-minute live urge monitor task positively correlated with average real-time urge intensity (15). Two similar studies (36, 37) found that greater real-time urge intensity was associated with increased likelihood of real-time tic occurrence and tic intensity. In a study of 11 adolescents and adults with CTDs, Wellen et al. used a live urge monitor and video-based tic rating to assess urges and tics (63). They observed that real-time urge intensity was positively associated with tic occurrence in the subsequent second for nine (82%) of the participants. Additionally, a relationship between tic occurrence in the previous second and urge intensity in the subsequent second was significant for eight (73%) of the participants, but for seven of these eight participants the relationship was such that tic occurrence predicted an increase in urge intensity in the subsequent second. Six of these seven participants showed an eventual decrease of urge intensity following tics but at varying timepoints (four to nine seconds post-tic).

3.2.7 Associations between premonitory urge severity and tic severity

Results are mixed concerning the cross-sectional association between premonitory urge severity and tic severity. Correlations of PUTS score with Yale Global Tic Severity Scale-Total Tic Score (YGTSS-TTS) and YGTSS Impairment Score range widely, from 0.08 (20) to 0.63 (64) and from 0.02 (58) to 0.60 (65), respectively. In studies with sample sizes of more than 100 participants, YGTSS-TTS consistently correlated with PUTS score, with correlation values of 0.13-0.27 in youth samples (23, 29, 46) and 0.20-0.32 in adult samples (19, 66). Supplementary Table S2 contains detailed results from studies examining the relationship between PUTS score and measures of tic severity. The relationship between I-PUTS scores and tic severity has been less extensively studied, but the existing data demonstrate a positive relationship between I-PUTS and YGTSS scores [(33, 34, 67, 68); Supplementary Table S3].

In the sole study to examine the relationship between premonitory urge severity and long-term tic severity, 80 CTD participants aged 16-30 years completed the PUTS and YGTSS at baseline and at a long-term follow-up (mean = 11.2 years later) (69). Baseline PUTS score did not significantly predict follow-up YGTSS-TTS or Impairment Score after controlling for selected covariates, nor did baseline PUTS score predict change in tic impairment over time. However, baseline PUTS score did significantly predict change in YGTSS-TTS between baseline and follow-up. That is, those with more severe premonitory urges at baseline experienced less reduction in tic severity over time.

3.2.8 Associations between premonitory urge severity and tic suppression ability

Several studies have evaluated the relationship between self-reported tic suppression ability and premonitory urge severity. Cohen and Leckman (3) found that 55% of their 28 participants felt their urges enabled them to better suppress their tics, but in Leckman et al.’s subsequent study (n = 132, age range 8-71-years-old, with 123 participants endorsing premonitory urge), only 20% of participants reported their urges enhanced their tic suppression ability (48). Among 1,032 children and adults with any tic disorder, presence of premonitory urge was positively associated with reported ability to suppress tics (50). Conversely, in a sample of 62 children and adults with TS, Matsuda et al. found no significant correlation between PUTS score and the tic suppression ability sub-score of the Tic Suppression Scale (24). However, in the subgroup of participants without comorbid OCD or ADHD (n = 37), these two scores showed a significant negative correlation, such that those with greater premonitory urge severity reported less ability to suppress tics. Within the 8-10-year-old subgroup (n = 81) of a sample of 254 youth with TS, those who endorsed premonitory urges were 2.75 times more likely to report tic suppressibility compared to 8-10-year-olds who did not endorse urges (43). In the other age groups, those who endorsed premonitory urges were not significantly more likely to report the ability to suppress tics.

Three studies have evaluated the relationship between objectively measured tic suppression ability and premonitory urge severity. In two studies of adults with TS, PUTS score did not significantly correlate with tic inhibition potency, as assessed by percent difference in the Modified Rush Video-Based Tic Rating Scale (MRVS) score during free-to-tic and tic inhibition conditions (70, 71). In their study of children with CTDs, Capriotti et al. included a condition in which participants were instructed to suppress tics but had the option to initiate a 10-second break during which they were free to tic (40). After removing one outlier, PUTS score positively correlated with average number of breaks taken.

3.2.9 Psychosocial impact of premonitory urge

Two cross-sectional studies have examined the association between premonitory urge severity and the impact or consequences of ticcing. Zinner et al. administered a neuropsychiatric battery, including the PUTS, and a questionnaire assessing peer victimization and bullying behavior to 211 parent-child dyads, in which all children had a CTD (28). Based on questionnaire results, children were classified as “victims,” “bullies,” “bully-victims,” or “non-victims.” Severity of premonitory urge, as well as of tics, explosive outbursts, and internalizing symptoms, was greater in “victims” compared to “non-victims.” The authors suggested that peer victimization perceived to be tic-related may increase the aversive experience of tics, which in turn may intensify premonitory urges. Consistent with the above study results, Capriotti et al. observed in their questionnaire-based study of 118 youth with TS that tic-related impact score predicted PUTS score after adjusting for tic severity and comorbid psychiatric diagnoses (72).

3.3 Associations between premonitory urges and other clinical characteristics

3.3.1 Interoception

Three studies have examined the potential association between premonitory urge severity and interoceptive sensibility, defined as “self-reported sensitivity to bodily sensations” (73), with differing results. In a sample of 18 adolescents and adults with TS, PUTS score and Private Body Consciousness scale score did not correlate (74). In contrast, in 21 adults with TS, score on the “awareness” section of the Body Perception Questionnaire significantly positively correlated with PUTS score (73), and in 48 adults with CTDs, a Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, Version 2 (MAIA-2) composite score was significantly associated with PUTS score after adjusting for tic and OCD symptom severity (75), indicating that participants with greater interoceptive sensibility reported more severe premonitory urges.

Several studies have examined the potential association between premonitory urge severity and interoceptive accuracy and/or interoceptive awareness. Interoceptive accuracy is a construct that reflects “objective interoceptive performance” (73) on tasks such as the heartbeat counting task, whereas interoceptive awareness is a construct defined as “insight into interoceptive ability, reflecting correspondence between subjective and objective measures” (73). In two studies assessing interoceptive accuracy in youth with CTDs (respective tic disorder sample sizes of 28 and 29), accuracy on a heartbeat perception task did not correlate with PUTS score (27, 30). One of these studies also assessed interoceptive accuracy (IAcc) by examining the correlation between self-rated muscle tension in the corrugator and masseter muscles and electromyography (EMG) activity, finding that PUTS score significantly positively correlated with Masseter IAcc but not Corrugator IAcc (27). In a study of 21 adults with TS, PUTS score did not correlate with participants’ accuracy in a heartbeat perception task or a heartbeat discrimination task nor did PUTS score correlate with metacognitive insight into accuracy on those two tasks (interoceptive awareness) (73). In contrast, in 19 adults with TS, better performance on a heartbeat counting task was significantly associated with higher PUTS score after adjusting for tic and OCD symptom severity (76).

A single study has examined the relationship between premonitory urge and embodiment. Rae et al. recruited 23 adults with TS to complete the rubber hand illusion task (77). PUTS score was not significantly associated with “body ownership plasticity” as indexed by the proprioceptive drift between synchronous and asynchronous conditions. However, PUTS score was correlated with embodiment prediction error, a metric generated from the difference between proprioceptive drift (objective measure of embodiment) and self-ratings of rubber hand embodiment during synchronous stimulation (subjective measure of embodiment). In this study, PUTS score was also associated with change in the “experience of rubber hand ownership” between synchronous and asynchronous conditions.

3.3.2 Exteroception

Several studies have examined associations between premonitory urges and self-reported perception of the external environment, again with mixed results. In a study of 18 children and adults with TS, PUTS score did not significantly correlate with scores from the Sensory Gating Inventory or the Structured Interview for Assessing Perceptual Anomalies (61). In contrast, in a sample of 34 adults with CTDs, PUTS score significantly correlated with both Sensory Gating Inventory and Sensory Perception Quotient scores, indicating that participants with greater premonitory urge severity reported more sensory hypersensitivity, defined as “heightened awareness of and reactivity to external stimuli” (31). In a sample of 140 children and adults with TS, presence of premonitory urges was not significantly associated with presence of stimulus sensitization, defined as “heightened sensitivity to tactile, auditory, and visual stimuli that [result] in uncomfortable sensation, tension, or non-tic movement” (78). Whereas the above studies relied on self-reported sensory perception, Schunke et al. used the “Quantitative Sensory Testing” (QST) battery to objectively assess 13 sensory parameters, including thermal, mechanical, and pain thresholds, in 14 adults with TS (52). PUTS score did not significantly correlate with any of the sensory parameters (52).

3.3.3 Cognitive traits and functions

Several studies have examined the associations between premonitory urges and various aspects of cognition and metacognition. In a study of 122 adolescents and adults with CTD, PUTS score showed a small but significant positive correlation with intelligence quotient (IQ) as measured by the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (19). Two other studies, with samples of 42 (12) and 29 (57) children and adolescents, found no such association.

Sections 3.2.8 and 3.4.1 review investigations of premonitory urge and tic suppression ability, but two studies have examined the relationship of premonitory urges with facets of inhibition outside the context of tic suppression. In a sample of 18 adolescents and adults with TS administered the standard and emotional Stroop tasks, PUTS score negatively correlated with standard Stroop time difference but not with standard Stroop error difference or emotional Stroop time difference or error difference (74). The authors suggested that the negative correlation between standard Stroop time difference and PUTS score may indicate reduced inhibitory interference in those with more severe premonitory urges. In a study of 40 adults with TS, average number of escape blinks during a blink suppression task, which included emotional and neutral facial expression conditions, was not associated with PUTS score (79).

Ganos et al. assessed the experience of intention during voluntary action in 27 adolescents with TS (80). Shorter time interval between the experience of intention and action performance was associated with higher PUTS score. The authors suggested that severe premonitory urges may interfere with self-perceived volition, as urges create perceptual noise from which signals of volitional action must be discriminated.

Arbuzova et al. conducted an assessment of metacognitive ability in 21 adults with TS, using a forced discrimination task of tactile stimuli (first-order) and confidence ratings regarding the accuracy of participants’ judgements in this task (second-order) to calculate the M-ratio, reflecting “the sensitivity of subjective second-order reports relative to objective first-order task performance” (81). They found no significant correlation between PUTS score and M-ratio. In a sample of 23 adults with TS, PUTS score significantly correlated with score from the Thinking About Tics questionnaire (“tic-related cognitions”) but not from the Meta-Cognitions Questionnaire (41).

3.3.4 Comorbid psychiatric symptoms/diagnoses, quality of life, and global functioning

Many studies, involving children, adolescents, and/or adults with tic disorders, have observed significant associations between severity of premonitory urges and OCD symptoms [(12, 15, 17, 1921, 23, 24, 26, 29, 32, 50, 53, 59, 61, 65, 66, 75, 76, 82, 83); Supplementary Table S5]. Results are more ambiguous from studies that have examined associations between severity of premonitory urges and severity of ADHD symptoms, depression, and/or anxiety [(12, 15, 17, 1921, 26, 29, 33, 37, 53, 59, 75, 8285); Supplementary Tables S6, S7]. A smaller number of studies have examined the association between premonitory urges and various behavioral, conduct, and emotional problems, including distress tolerance and externalizing behavior [(12, 17, 21, 33, 51, 58, 60, 86); Supplementary Table S8]. Of the studies that have assessed the relationship between premonitory urges and quality of life or global functioning, most indicate that more severe premonitory urges are associated with greater disability, poorer global functioning, and poorer health-related quality of life [(37, 53, 58, 59, 65, 66, 71, 72, 75, 82, 83, 87, 88); Supplementary Table S9].

3.3.5 Other characteristics

Of the eight studies that have examined differences in PUTS or I-PUTS scores between males and females, no between-sex differences were evident (16, 19, 29, 32, 34, 5961). However, in a sample of 1,032 children and adults with any tic disorder (mean age = 20.9 years, SD = 12.9 years; male-female proportion of sample not reported), males were more likely to report presence of premonitory urges than females (males: 72.3%, females: 65.1%) (50). Conversely, in a sample of 291 adults with CTDs (mean age not reported but 33.3% of sample older than 35 years of age; 24% female), females were more likely to report premonitory urges than males, after adjusting for age and comorbid diagnoses (53). In their study of 74 youth and young adults with CTD, Edwards et al. found that the relationship between PUTS score and self-reported diagnoses of ADHD and OCD did not differ by sex (32).

In a study of 60 individuals (mean age = 32.2 years, SD = 14.1 years) with TS, Eddy and Cavanna administered the PUTS and assessed coprolalia and mental coprolalia (urge to swear as a tic) using a non-obscene socially inappropriate symptoms (NOSIS) questionnaire. PUTS score did not significantly correlate with the presence of coprolalia or mental coprolalia (89). In a separate study of 60 individuals (mean age 33 years, SD 14 years) with TS, Eddy and Cavanna found that those who reported NOSIS (n = 40) had significantly higher PUTS scores than those who did not (n = 20) (90).

A study of 1,032 children and adults with any tic disorder observed a significant association between the presence of premonitory urges and “not just right experiences” (50). In 111 adults with TS, scores from the Non-Just-Right-Experiences Questionnaire (NJRE-QR) and the Feelings of Incompleteness Questionnaire significantly correlated with PUTS score (66). Notably, PUTS item 4 (“Right before I do a tic I feel like something is not ‘just right’ “) score only moderately correlated with NJRE-QR score, suggesting that PUTS item 4 and the NJRE-QR may not index the exact same construct (66).

3.4 Premonitory urge response to intervention

3.4.1 Response to tic suppression

Mixed results have emerged from studies examining the effect of tic suppression on premonitory urge. Several have identified increased urge intensity during tic suppression. In a study of 13 children with CTDs, participants engaged in a tic suppression condition during which they had the option to initiate a 10-second break to tic freely (40). Participants rated their urge intensity on an “urge thermometer” at the initiation and termination of each break. Across participants, urge intensity significantly decreased between break initiation and termination. In this same study, the researchers also observed that average urge ratings were significantly higher in a reinforced tic suppression condition compared to the free-to-tic baseline condition. In a study of 45 children and adolescents with TS, participants completed a free-to-tic condition, a tic suppression condition, and an urge-acceptance condition, in which they were instructed to monitor “urges while practicing willingness to experience the urge and giving up any fight” (91). During each of these two-minute conditions, participants rated the intensity of each urge that occurred on a 1-9-point scale, allowing for assessment of both urge intensity and frequency. Urge intensity, but not frequency, was significantly higher during the tic suppression condition compared to the free-to-tic condition. Interestingly, both urge intensity and frequency were significantly lower in the urge acceptance condition compared to the free-to-tic condition. Brabson et al. conducted a study in which 12 children with CTD completed alternating baseline and differential reinforcement of zero rate behavior (DRO; i.e., incentivized tic suppression) conditions (42). Two participants showed an “M” pattern, with urge intensity lower in baseline conditions and higher in DRO conditions; three participants showed a “W” pattern, with urge intensity higher in baseline conditions and lower in DRO conditions; and the remaining participants showed other, less clear patterns of urge intensity across conditions. In a study of 16 adults with TS, participants rated their premonitory urge intensity on a live urge monitor during tic suppression and free-to-tic conditions (35). The timing of peak urge intensity differed between conditions: in the free-to-tic condition urge intensity peaked just after tic occurrence, while in the tic suppression condition urge intensity peaked just before tic occurrence. Notably, urge intensity tended to increase over the course of the suppression condition.

In contrast to the above, several studies have observed no change in premonitory urge intensity during tic suppression. In a study of 12 children and adolescents with CTD, participants completed three 10-minute free-to-tic conditions and two 40-minute DRO (incentivized tic suppression) conditions (39); urge intensity was rated every 10 seconds on an “urge thermometer.” Aggregate urge intensity did not significantly differ between baseline and DRO conditions, but tic frequency was significantly lower in DRO conditions. In a study of 11 individuals 17-60-years-old with CTD, participants engaged in multiple behavioral conditions, including free-to-tic (10 minutes each, 3 separate occurrences, though only first occurrence was used in analyses), tic suppression (30 minutes, 1 occurrence), and habit reversal tic suppression (tic suppression with a competing response; 30 minutes, 1 occurrence), while rating their urge intensity with a live urge monitor (63). Most participants showed similar urge levels across conditions, even though tic frequency was lower in the tic suppression and habit reversal tic suppression conditions compared to the free-to-tic condition. In a study of 22 adults with TS, participants completed the PUTS at baseline, suppressed their tics for 10 minutes, and then completed the PUTS again just before the end of the 10-minute task (70). The PUTS was also administered at 10 minutes, 20 minutes and 30 minutes post-suppression task. Mean PUTS score did not significantly differ between any of the timepoints. In study of 23 adults with TS, Hermann et al. assessed urge intensity in five conditions during which participants were instructed to focus their attention on different objects or phenomena: a picture of an empty room (baseline), a live video of themselves, a video of themselves ticcing, various objects within a room (distraction condition), and uncomfortable aspects of their tics (41). Participants completed each condition twice: once in a free-to-tic state and once in a tic suppression state. Attention condition, but not tic state, was significantly associated with urge intensity ratings. Relative to baseline, urge intensity was higher during the live feedback condition and the thoughts-about-uncomfortable-aspects-of-tics condition.

3.4.2 Response to behavioral interventions

Nine studies have assessed whether various behavioral interventions, such as Comprehensive Behavioral Intervention for Tics (CBIT), exposure and response prevention (ERP), and habit reversal training (HRT), reduce premonitory urge severity (56, 9299). Supplementary Table S10 provides detailed results of these studies. Most studies employed the PUTS and observed no effect of behavioral interventions on PUTS scores. In a randomized controlled trial comparing 10 sessions of CBIT versus psychoeducation and supportive therapy (PST) in 122 adults with CTD, PUTS scores did not significantly differ between treatment groups, though PUTS scores did significantly decrease over time across both groups (93). Interestingly, in the companion pediatric study of CBIT versus PST (n = 126), premonitory urge severity did not significantly change over time for either treatment group (93). Notably, one study, investigating the effectiveness of ERP in 18 children and adults with TS, indexed premonitory urge severity with subjective units of distress rather than the PUTS: subjective distress from premonitory urges decreased both within and across 10 ERP sessions (56).

3.4.3 Response to pharmacotherapy

Nineteen studies have assessed responsiveness of premonitory urges to medication (47, 84, 100116). Supplementary Table S11 contains detailed results of these studies. In a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of topiramate for children and adults with TS (20 individuals completed the study), premonitory urge severity (as measured by clinical global impression), as well as tic severity, improved in the topiramate arm (101). In contrast, premonitory urge severity did not improve during open-label treatment with aripiprazole (84) or ecopipam (100). Three studies examined the effect of cannabis on premonitory urges and observed some benefit (111113), though each study had notable limitations: one study solely interviewed individuals about their experience using marijuana, another study enrolled a small sample (n = 18) of adults with TS for open-label cannabis treatment using varying doses and administration methods, and the only randomized controlled trial (cross-over design) enrolled a small sample (n = 12) of adults with TS for a single dose. Botulinum toxin injections improved premonitory urge severity in a number of case series (47, 103, 104, 106, 107) and in one randomized controlled trial (n=18) (105), though it is notable that none of these studies assessed premonitory urge with a validated, symptom-specific scale. In a combined pharmacotherapy-behavioral intervention, McGuire et al. randomized 20 youth with TS or chronic motor tic disorder to one session of HRT with either D-cycloserine 50 mg or placebo (taken one hour pre-HRT session). The D-cycloserine group exhibited significantly more improvement in I-PUTS frequency score, but not I-PUTS intensity score, than the placebo group (102).

3.4.4 Response to neural stimulation

Most studies assessing response of premonitory urges to deep brain stimulation (DBS) have involved small case series and open-label studies, with the majority demonstrating some degree of improvement. Among 18 TS participants (age range: 17-47-years-old) with bilateral DBS in the centromedian–parafascicular (CM–Pfc) and ventralis oralis (Voa) nuclei of the thalamus, nine underwent blinded rating in “on” and “off” DBS conditions nine months post-implantation (117). In the “off” condition, a subset of participants (precise number not reported in the publication) experienced return of their premonitory urges. In two adults with refractory TS who underwent bilateral DBS in the CM-Pfc-ventral oral nuclei of the thalamus, PUTS scores improved after implantation, with sustained improvement two years post-implantation (118). In a case report of a 17-year-old female with TS who received bilateral globus pallidus internus (GPi) DBS, PUTS score reduced from 36 to 15 two months after surgery, with further reduction to 9 two years post-surgery (119). In contrast to the above studies, a nine-month, randomized, double-blind crossover trial comparing DBS of the centromedian-ventro-oral internus of the thalamus (CM-Voi), the posteroventral lateral globus pallidus internus (pvl GPi), and sham stimulation in nine adults with TS found no significant differences in PUTS or premonitory urge visual analogue scale scores across stimulation targets (120).

In the single pilot study to examine the impact of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on premonitory urge, Eapen et al. administered tDCS to the anterior supplementary motor area (SMA) in two adult males with TS over the course of six weeks (121). At three months post-treatment, PUTS scores had improved for both participants.

Median nerve stimulation (MNS) is a non-invasive intervention currently under study to reduce tic severity. Among 16 adolescents and adults with TS six, one-minute rounds of 10 Hz-MNS decreased live urge monitor ratings by 33% (not significant) during stimulation periods (122). In the same study, results of a stepwise multiple regression model found that higher PUTS score corresponded to greater reduction in tic intensity during MNS. The study also found that, during MNS, reduction in live urge monitor ratings was associated with reduction in tic frequency. Subsequently, Morera Maiquez et al. conducted a double blind, sham-controlled trial of home-administered MNS, obtaining complete data from 117 participants (123). Participants were randomized into one of three groups: active stimulation, sham stimulation, or treatment as usual. The treatment consisted of four weeks of a daily, 14-minute stimulation protocol. Reductions in premonitory urge severity, as assessed by the Premonitory Urges for Tic Disorders Scale-Revised (PUTS-R), did not significantly differ between groups.

Fu et al. conducted a sham-controlled trial of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) (0.5 Hz, intensity 90% of resting motor threshold) to the bilateral parietal cortices in 30 adolescents and adults with TS (124). Participants received three rounds of 400 pulses per side (10 minutes between rounds) for 10 consecutive days. Symptom rating scales were administered at baseline, at the end of treatment, one week after treatment, and one month after treatment. PUTS scores decreased significantly more in the rTMS group, with persistent improvements one-month post-treatment. A similar pattern emerged for YGTSS global scores (i.e., sum of Total Tic Score and Impairment Score), YGTSS motor tic scores, YGTSS vocal tic scores, and MRVS.

3.5 Neural correlates of premonitory urges

3.5.1 Functional imaging

Numerous functional imaging studies have examined the neural activity associated with premonitory urges, but cross-study comparisons are challenging due to between-study differences in task, imaging, and statistical methodology. That said, findings generally suggest a relationship of premonitory urge with functional activity and/or connectivity in sensorimotor cortical and subcortical structures, particularly the insula and SMA. Supplementary Table S12 provides detailed results, including effect sizes and p-values, from all studies investigating neural correlates of premonitory urge.

In a resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study of 13 adults with TS, PUTS score was associated with functional connectivity of the right dorsal anterior insula (dAI) with the right SMA2 and left SMA1 (125) (Table 1 in the cited publication reports MNI coordinates for “SMA1” and “SMA2”). In a separate resting state fMRI study of 40 adults with TS, PUTS score was negatively associated with connectivity between the insula and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), between the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and pre- and post-central gyri, and between the putamen and IFG (126). In contrast to the above resting state fMRI studies, Tinaz et al. found no significant associations between PUTS score and functional connectivity of sensorimotor cortex (SMC) with any brain regions in 16 adults with TS (127).

Two studies have examined the relationship between premonitory urge and fMRI changes during suppression tasks. In the study by Ganos et al., 14 adults with TS completed free-to-tic and tic suppression conditions during fMRI (128). Regional homogeneity (ReHo), a fMRI measure of local connectivity, in the left IFG was greater during the tic suppression condition than the free-to-tic condition. However, ReHo contrast value in the left IFG showed no correlation with PUTS score. In the study by Bhikram et al., 40 adults with TS underwent fMRI while completing a blink suppression task that included free-to-blink and blink suppression conditions, during which participants were shown either angry or neutral facial expressions (79). Contrasts in brain activity during free-to-blink and blink suppression conditions were not significantly associated with PUTS score. However, when contrasting angry facial expression conditions with neutral facial expression conditions, greater activity in the thalamus, hippocampus, mid-cingulate, caudate, and middle temporal gyrus was associated with greater urge severity as quantified by the PUTS.

Other studies have investigated the relationship of premonitory urge severity to fMRI activation under a variety of task conditions that did not explicitly involve tic suppression. In 25 adults with TS completing a theory of mind task, PUTS score was associated with fMRI activity in the right temporal-parietal junction, left amygdala, and right amygdala (129). In a follow-up study using the same sample population, participants completed a reading-the-eyes task in which they made judgments about either mental state or age based on an image of eyes (130). In both task conditions, greater activity in the bilateral temporal-parietal junctions was associated with higher PUTS score. In a study of eight adults with TS, participants completed a visuospatial priming task during fMRI scan, before and after 10 weeks of CBIT (94). Prior to CBIT, task-related activation in the superior temporal gyrus was negatively correlated with PUTS score. The change in task-related activation from pre- to post- CBIT did not correlate with baseline PUTS score or change in PUTS score. In a study of 23 adults with TS, participants completed a modified “Go/NoGo” fMRI task (131). Greater representational similarity in the caudate nucleus between “go” and “no go” conditions was positively correlated with PUTS score, but the significance of this association did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. With the same sample population and Go/NoGo/Choose task, Rae et al. found no correlations of PUTS score with task effects for Choose-Go (“volitional action”) or Choose-NoGo (“intentional inhibition”) conditions (132). However, during trials in which participants “chose-go,” pre-SMA functional connectivity with the following regions was positively associated with PUTS score: caudate nucleus, globus pallidus, and thalamus. In a study of 21 adults with TS, participants completed a face perception task during fMRI in which they viewed angry or neutral faces and were instructed to report whether the face was male or female (133). Contrasts in both the neutral face and angry face conditions (relative to baseline) were not significantly associated with PUTS score. However, during the face perception task, functional connectivity between the insula and the following regions was positively associated with PUTS score: SMA, posterior cingulate, precuneus, and fusiform gyrus/cerebellum.

Three studies have examined associations between premonitory urges and neurotransmitter or neurotransmitter metabolite concentrations in different brain regions. Tinaz et al. used GABA 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) to measure GABA concentration in the sensorimotor cortex (SMC) in 16 adults with TS: GABA+/Creatine ratio in the SMC did not significantly correlate with PUTS score (127). In a larger study of 68 children with TS, He et al. used proton-edited MRS to assess concentrations of GABA and glutamate + glutamine (Glx) in the right insula, right SMC, and right SMA (134). After correcting for multiple comparisons, SMA GABA+ concentration significantly negatively correlated with I-PUTS frequency, intensity, and number scores such that lower level of SMA GABA+ was associated with greater premonitory urge severity. There were no significant associations of I-PUTS scores with GABA+ concentration in the SMC or with Glx concentration in any of the three brain regions of interest. In those without co-occurring ADHD, GABA+ level in the insula was negatively associated with all three I-PUTS scores, but in those with co-occurring ADHD these associations were either nonexistent or positive. A separate MRS study in 37 adults with TS found a significant negative association between thalamic glutamate concentration and PUTS score (135).

Some studies have examined the neural correlates of premonitory urge without a formal assessment of urge, rather inferring the neural correlates of premonitory urges by examining brain activity prior to tics or instructing participants to imagine the experiences of urges and tics. Hampson et al. conducted a study of 16 adults with CTD and 16 matched controls without any tic disorder (136). During fMRI scan, CTD participants were free to tic, and controls were instructed to imitate the tics of the CTD participant to which they were matched. Based on data from 14 CTD participants with premonitory urges and their matched controls, SMA activity was greater before and after tics in CTD participants compared to before and after intentional movements in controls. In a study of 10 adults with TS undergoing fMRI during a free ticcing condition, the following regions had stronger fMRI activation two seconds prior to tic-onset than at tic-onset: bilateral insula, bilateral posterior putamen, bilateral anterior cingulate cortex, bilateral SMA, and bilateral parietal operculum (137). In a study with data from 23 individuals (mean age = 26.3 years, SD = 6.4 years) with TS, participants underwent fMRI during multiple conditions, including one in which they imagined a situation that promoted tic occurrence (“urge phase”) and a condition in which they imagined performing a tic (138). Following these conditions, participants rated their distress or relief on a 5-point scale. Distress rating during the urge phase was positively associated with activation in multiple cortical and subcortical areas (Supplementary Table S12 provides further details).

3.5.2 Structural imaging

Several studies have sought to identify structural correlates of premonitory urges, generally identifying associations between premonitory urge severity and structural alterations in the insula and sensorimotor cortical regions. In a combined sample of 28 children and adolescents (mean age = 14.6 years, SD = 3.4 years) with TS, PUTS score positively correlated with grey matter volume in clusters within the anterior-dorsal right insula (139) and the left hemisphere posterior mid-cingulate cortex (140). In contrast, Draper et al. found no positive correlations between PUTS score and cortical grey matter thickness in any brain region in 29 children and adolescents (mean age = 14 years, SD = 3.1 years) with TS (57). However, PUTS score was negatively correlated with grey matter thickness in clusters within the right Rolandic operculum, left inferior occipital gyrus, left insula, and left pre-central gyrus. In a study of 28 adolescents with TS, focused on regions of interest within the cerebellum, PUTS score was positively correlated with grey matter volume in a cluster within the right cerebellar lobule VI (141). In 15 adults (mean age = 30.9 years, SD = 9.9 years) with TS, Tinaz et al. found no significant correlations between PUTS score and sensorimotor cortical volume (127). In contrast, in a larger study of 34 adults (mean age = 32.4 years, SD = 11 years) with TS, PUTS score positively correlated with grey matter volume in the left somatosensory cortex, dorsal left premotor cortex, and right prefrontal cortex (PFC) and with cortical thickness in the left somatosensory cortex and right PFC (142). Also, PUTS score was negatively associated with fractional anisotropy of white matter for portions of the bilateral superior longitudinal fascicles. Sigurdsson et al. used diffusion tensor imaging to assess white matter microstructure in 35 adolescents with TS, finding no association between diffusion indices and PUTS score (143). However, in a regression analysis to predict graph theory metrics in 24 regions of interest from clinical scales, adjusting for age, IQ, and total intracranial volume, the authors identified significant positive associations between PUTS score and local efficiency (“efficacy of information transfer”) in the right posterior insula and left anterior insula, as well as a significant negative association between PUTS score and local efficiency in the left caudate.

3.5.3 Neurophysiological approaches

Several studies have used neurophysiological methods, including electroencephalography (EEG), magnetoencephalography (MEG), and TMS, to study premonitory urge. Collectively, findings are mixed across these studies, which enrolled small sample populations and employed diverse methodologies.

Loo et al. recorded EEG activity during cued eyes blinks in 23 children with CTDs and examined the correlation of premonitory urges with spectral power in four cortical areas (144). Urge strength, as measured by a subset of PUTS items, positively correlated with alpha power in the left parietal cortex and with gamma power in the dorsolateral PFC and SMA. Urge strength negatively correlated with alpha power in the posterior cingulate cortex. Urge presence, as measured by a different subset of PUTS items, also positively correlated with gamma power in the SMA. In a sample of 11 adults with TS (PUTS data for n = 8), Niccolai et al. used MEG to assess beta power change in the motor and premotor cortices during time periods in which participants were free to tic (145). Two pre-tic time windows were defined: 1-0.5s pre-tic and 0.5-0s pre-tic. Correlations between PUTS score and beta power slope in each time window were not significant. However, beta power slope in the 1-0.5s pre-tic time window significantly correlated with PUTS item 6 (“I feel like there is energy in my body that needs to get out”) score, which the authors interpreted as reflective of motor urges. In 16 adults with TS, Tinaz et al. used MEG to measure (baseline) beta power in the sensorimotor cortex and found no significant correlation between this MEG measure and PUTS score (127).

Sigurdsson et al. used neuro-navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation (nTMS) in 16 children and adults with TS to map the representation of different muscles in the sensorimotor cortex (146). PUTS score positively correlated with the area of first dorsal interosseous (FDI) cortical representation. Additionally, PUTS score negatively correlated with the Euclidean distance between the FDI representation site and the representation sites for the orbicularis oris, masseter, and orbicularis oculi. Brandt et al. conducted a study in which 12 adults with TS completed a paired associative stimulus (PAS) procedure (64). During this procedure, electrical stimulation is repeatedly administered at the wrist (with a peripheral nerve stimulator) and over primary motor cortex (M1) (with a TMS coil) such that both stimuli simultaneously arrive in the cortex, which, theoretically, should transiently strengthen involved synapses (64). Motor evoked potentials (MEPs) were measured using EMG at the right abductor pollicis brevis muscle. The change in MEP from pre- to post-PAS procedure strongly correlated with PUTS score, suggesting greater sensorimotor cortex plasticity was associated with greater premonitory urge severity. Interestingly, although greater long-term potentiation-like response was associated with greater premonitory urge severity, MEP change was lower in those with TS compared to controls. In a sample of 17 children with TS, Larsh et al. found that higher I-PUTS intensity score (and lower YGTSS-TTS) was associated with lower TMS-evoked cortical excitability and lower long-interval cortical inhibition in the left and right M1 (67). Batschelett et al. used TMS and EMG to assess measures of M1 physiology, including short-interval cortical inhibition and intracortical facilitation, in a sample of 30 children with TS (68). They found no significant associations between M1 physiology measures and I-PUTS scores.

4 Discussion

In this scoping review, we identified 155 publications of studies examining the assessment, characteristics, clinical correlates, neural correlates, and treatment of premonitory urges. The extant literature reveals important knowledge and knowledge gaps concerning our current understanding of premonitory urges.

Several measures exist to quantify premonitory urge, but the PUTS is by far the most frequently utilized and has been translated into multiple languages. The PUTS has exhibited acceptable (13) internal consistency in adolescent and adult tic disorder populations (14, 15, 19, 24), with more varied internal consistency in tic disorder populations younger than 10-years-old (12, 18, 2022). Factor analyses of the PUTS have yielded inconsistent results in terms of items that load onto particular factors, although many of these analyses proposed similar names for factors (“quality,” “intensity”), and across multiple studies items 4 (“not just-right”) and 5 (“something isn’t complete”) loaded onto a factor interpreted as “OCD-related items” or “just right phenomena” (17, 22, 24, 26). The mixed results of PUTS factor analyses may be partially explained by differences in items included in the analyses (i.e., 9-item vs 10-item PUTS), version language (e.g., English, Spanish), and sample population characteristics (e.g., age, prevalence and severity of psychiatric comorbidities, sample size). Further research is needed to clarify the dimensionality, test-retest reliability [as intraclass correlation may be more suitable than Pearson’s correlation in this context (13)], and floor and ceiling effects of the PUTS. The PUTS is a brief, easily administered and scored method for assessing premonitory urge, but the scale has notable limitations: the PUTS produces a single score, reducing premonitory urge to a unidimensional construct, with no clear differentiation between quality, frequency, and intensity of urges; the PUTS score significantly correlates with various internalizing symptoms, including anxiety and obsessive-compulsive symptoms, potentially indicating the scale’s discriminant validity may be suboptimal; and the PUTS provides no explicit timeframe for respondents to consider when answering questions about urges. The clinician-administered I-PUTS, while more time-intensive, addresses many of these limitations, generating details about individual premonitory urges, quantifying premonitory urge in three separate dimensions, and specifying the timeframe in question. While assessed in only two studies thus far, the I-PUTS has demonstrated good internal consistency and inter-rater reliability, as well as potentially superior divergent validity to the PUTS (33, 34). Further research is needed to replicate these findings and to clarify the scale’s floor and ceiling effects. Notably, the I-PUTS, unlike the PUTS, does not assess the quality of premonitory urge. English versions of the PUTS and I-PUTS exhibited small correlations, while Chinese versions of the two scales exhibited moderate correlations. More granular assessment of urge intensity is possible with live urge monitor and urge thermometer approaches, though as typically implemented, these temporally refined approaches do not provide information about different types of urges experienced and their associations with particular tics. Each of the existing methods for assessing premonitory urge possesses advantages and disadvantages, and context will determine which tool is most appropriate. One important direction for future research is to develop methods that capture the valence and quality of premonitory urge. Existing measures do not directly address how uncomfortable or painful urges are; rather, they assess the degree or intensity of urges. Additionally, the aforementioned scales either make no mention of urge quality or assess urge quality by providing a relatively short list of qualities to endorse that may not adequately correspond to an individual’s urge experience. Limited research exists on the subjective qualities of premonitory urges (3, 54). Ultimately, accurate characterization and quantification of premonitory urge dimensions are critical to identifying clinical and neurobiological correlates, developing targeted interventions, and gauging evolution and response of the phenomenon over time.

Premonitory urges are prevalent in tic disorder populations. Though estimates vary widely, many studies observed prevalence rates greater than 60% (17, 50), with some observing rates greater than 90% (48). Variability in prevalence estimates likely arises in part due to differences in study methods for premonitory urge ascertainment [e.g., multi-part questionnaire about urge (17) versus binary question about urge presence/absence (50)] and due to differences in sample population characteristics (e.g., child, adolescent, and/or adult participants). Multiple cross-sectional studies involving large sample populations have observed that prevalence of urges increases from childhood to adolescence (17, 43, 50), with urges typically emerging a few years after tic onset (48). However, evidence for a relationship between severity of urges (based on PUTS scores) and age is mixed. Longitudinal studies, of which there are few to date (21, 69, 88), will be crucial to enhance knowledge of the trajectory of premonitory urges over time.

Most studies exploring the subjective quality of premonitory urges have employed questionnaires, as opposed to qualitative methods (17, 19, 52, 53). From these largely quantitative approaches, overarching characteristics of premonitory urges emerge. Bodily tension and restlessness appear to be the most frequently endorsed quality of urges (17, 19, 52, 53). A “not-just-right” urge quality is more common among individuals with comorbid OCD (19, 47, 53). The majority of adults and children with tics experience more than one distinct premonitory urge (33, 48), though some do not experience urges with every tic and/or every time a given tic occurs (47). Premonitory urges most commonly manifest as localized bodily sensations in the muscles near the site of the associated tic (44, 47, 48, 50, 56), with urge intensity tending to increase immediately prior to the tic (3537, 63). Most individuals experience relief from their urges following tic execution (19, 44, 47, 48). The distress due to urges has been examined in only two studies, both observing that a subset of individuals experience urges as bothersome in and of themselves (3, 55). More severe premonitory urges are associated with greater disability, poorer global functioning, and poorer health-related quality of life (37, 53, 58, 59, 65, 66, 82, 87, 88), but the degree to which premonitory urges causally contribute to these negative outcomes remains uncertain.

The mechanistic and developmental relationship between urges and tics is not clear, but logically one would expect a significant association between real-time urge intensity and tic occurrence, and this, indeed, is the case (15, 3537). However, correlations between the PUTS and YGTSS show mixed results, with many studies showing weak or non-significant correlations, possibly in part due to limitations of these scales. Two studies have found a significant relationship between premonitory urges and negative consequences related to tics (e.g., bullying) (28, 72), leading the authors to speculate that negative consequences of tics cause pre-tic sensations to become more aversive. In the only study to assess the longitudinal relationship between premonitory urge and tic severity, baseline PUTS score did not predict tic severity or impairment at an average follow-up time of 11 years; however, participants with more severe premonitory urges at baseline demonstrated less reduction in tic severity at follow-up (69). Importantly, tic and premonitory urge severity are differentially affected by some interventions [e.g., aripiprazole (84), ecopipam (100), MNS (123), CBIT (95), and ERP (97)], possibly indicating that urge and tic severity can be decoupled, though differences in the properties of the scales [e.g., responsiveness (13)] used to quantify these phenomena are also an important consideration. Additionally, some (43, 50), but not all (24, 70, 71), studies suggest that the presence of premonitory urge enhances tic suppression ability. Deeper insights into the relationship between premonitory urges and tics may facilitate development of novel interventions for both symptoms.

Of the comorbid psychiatric features of tic disorders, premonitory urge severity is most commonly associated with OCD symptom severity. Studies have found greater intensity (19) and prevalence (47, 53) of the “not just right” quality of urge in those with comorbid OCD or obsessive-compulsive symptoms. PUTS items 4 (feeling that “something is not ‘just right’”) and 5 (feeling that “something isn’t complete”) often load onto a factor interpreted as “OCD- related items,” and these two items may drive the association between premonitory urge severity and OCD symptom severity. In one study of 656 youth, correlations between PUTS score and OCD measures were no longer significant after removing PUTS items 4 and 5 (17). Given its relatively consistent association with OCD symptoms, investigations of premonitory urge should account for the presence and/or severity of these symptoms in their study design and/or analyses.

Premonitory urges are a sensory phenomenon, and thus one would assume that urges are associated with alterations in interoceptive and/or exteroceptive processing. To date, however, investigations into the relationships between perceptual processes and premonitory urges have generated mixed results. Considering self-reported perceptual experience, PUTS score correlated with interoceptive sensibility [“self-reported sensitivity to bodily sensations” (73)] in two studies of adults (73, 75) but not a third (74) and correlated with sensory hypersensitivity [“heightened awareness of and reactivity to external stimuli” (31)] in one study of adults (31) but not two others involving both children and adults (61, 78). Conflicting results may have arisen due to differences in questionnaires, sample population ages and clinical characteristics, and/or sample sizes. Considering objectively quantified sensory processing, PUTS score correlated with embodiment prediction error (“mismatch of subjective and objective embodiment”) in one study of adults with TS (77) and correlated with interoceptive accuracy, as determined by heartbeat counting tasks, in one study of adults (76) but not in another study of adults (73) nor in two studies of youth (27, 30). PUTS score did not significantly correlate with any parameters in a quantitative sensory testing battery administered to a small sample of adults (52). The lack of identified associations may be due to true absence of a relationship of objective interoception and exteroception measures with premonitory urge, but further investigations are warranted given biological plausibility of such relationships. Anatomical structures involved in interoception [e.g., insular cortex (147)] and exteroception [e.g., somatosensory cortex (148)] are abnormal in individuals with tic disorders, and moreover, several of these same structures are implicated in the pathophysiology of premonitory urge, as will be discussed subsequently.

A number of studies, many with small sample sizes, identified interventions that improved premonitory urge severity, including “urge acceptance” (91), botulinum toxin injections (103107), topiramate (101), DBS (117119), and repetitive TMS (124). However, of the botulinum toxin studies, only one was a randomized, double blind, placebo-controlled trial (105), and none of the botulinum toxin studies nor the topiramate study used a validated scale to assess premonitory urges. Of the DBS studies (117120), the sole randomized, double blind trial comparing thalamic, GPi, and sham stimulation showed no effect of DBS on urges (120). In a sham-controlled, randomized controlled trial of 30 adolescents and adults, repetitive TMS to the bilateral parietal cortices significantly improved premonitory urges and tics, with effects lasting through the end of the trial (one month) (124), but further research is needed to replicate these findings and determine duration of improvement. Unfortunately, common behavioral therapies (CBIT, ERP, HRT) generally did not impact premonitory urge severity [Supplementary Table S10 (92, 94, 95, 97, 98)]. Interestingly, one session of HRT combined with D-cycloserine improved premonitory urge frequency, but not intensity, more than one session of HRT alone (102). MNS did not significantly improve premonitory urges in a double blind, sham-controlled trial (123). Although several promising interventions were identified in this review, key limitations existed for many studies, including small sample sizes, lack of blinding, lack of a control arm, and/or unvalidated methods for measuring premonitory urges. Furthermore, given that age and psychiatric comorbidity (particularly OCD) are associated with premonitory urge severity, clinical trials should account for these variables in their study design and/or analyses.

Studies that sought to identify anatomical and/or functional correlates of premonitory urge varied in their imaging modalities, experimental tasks, participant characteristics, and statistical approaches, but despite these methodological differences, published findings do implicate specific brain structures, particularly the insula and SMA, in emergence of the premonitory urge. Premonitory urge was associated with differences in insula gray matter volume (57, 139), insula white matter microstructure (143), and functional connectivity of the insula with various other regions in resting state and during a face perception task (125, 126, 133). Two of the three above studies found premonitory urges were associated with functional connectivity between the insula and the SMA. Aspects of the SMA, including its GABA concentration (134) and functional connectivity with basal ganglia structures during voluntary action (132), have been shown to be associated with premonitory urges. The SMA also appears to display heightened activity prior to tics (136, 137), the time period in which urges are present. Physiological differences in other brain regions, including the plasticity of the sensorimotor cortex (64) and the excitably of the motor cortex (67), also seem to be associated with premonitory urges.

The means by which dysfunction in these structures generates the experience of premonitory urge is unclear, but Rae et al. have proposed a theoretical model, based on aberrant predictive processing (149). In this model, GABAergic interneuron dysfunction in the somatosensory region of the putamen leads to transmission of abnormally precise sensory predictions to the primary somatosensory cortex, posterior insula, and anterior insula. The anterior insula must integrate these overly precise sensory predictions with sensory input reflecting current bodily state, resulting in a prediction error that is posited to culminate in a conscious bodily discomfort. Per this same model, the anterior insula also transmits signals to the SMA and other midline motor structures to prompt a corrective movement, i.e., tic, thereby resolving the sensory prediction error. Further research is needed to empirically test this compelling model.

A fully developed mechanistic theory of premonitory urges must also account for several additional facts: premonitory urges become increasingly prevalent with age, not all individuals with tics experience urges, and not all tics within a given individual are associated with urges. Longitudinal studies will be critical to quantify the relationship between premonitory urge and implicated pathophysiologic mechanisms over time. In particular, longitudinal studies following children with tic disorders through early adolescence may yield insights into how urges arise during development and clarify if the alterations in brain structure and function associated with premonitory urges are present and/or change in children who do not endorse that symptom.

Many uncertainties also remain regarding the real-time neural correlates of premonitory urges, as studies to date have either assessed the relationship of a clinical scale (quantifying urge severity over an indefinite period or the past week, i.e., PUTS or I-PUTS, respectively) with a neuroimaging metric or recorded brain activity prior to tics and inferred the presence of urges. No study, to our knowledge, has paired a high temporal resolution neuroimaging technique with a live urge monitor to assess the neural correlates of urge in real time.

5 Conclusion

Premonitory urges are prevalent, bothersome features of tic disorders, and, to this point, have received substantially less attention than tics in studies of CTD populations. More research is needed to properly characterize and measure premonitory urge and to clarify its neurobiological basis, which will inform strategies to effectively treat this core feature of tic disorders. Important areas of focus for future research include qualitative and longitudinal studies of premonitory urge, ongoing optimization of premonitory urge measures, further determination of premonitory urge’s responsiveness to established interventions for tics, development of interventions targeting premonitory urge, and elucidation of premonitory urge’s neurobiological underpinnings.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

JBW: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Visualization. KHW: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. KDG: Formal analysis, Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing. DAI: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. DAI receives funding from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (K23NS131592).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1504442/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Müller-Vahl KR, Sambrani T, Jakubovski E. Tic disorders revisited: introduction of the term “tic spectrum disorders. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2019) 28:1129–35. doi: 10.1007/s00787-018-01272-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Knight T, Steeves T, Day L, Lowerison M, Jette N, Pringsheim T. Prevalence of tic disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatr Neurol. (2012) 47:77–90. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2012.05.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Cohen AJ, Leckman JF. Sensory phenomena associated with Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. J Clin Psychiatry. (1992) 53:319–23. Available online at: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/1517194/.

Google Scholar

4. Cavanna AE, Black KJ, Hallett M, Voon V. Neurobiology of the premonitory urge in Tourette’s syndrome: pathophysiology and treatment implications. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2017) 29:95–104. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16070141

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Rajagopal S, Seri And S, Cavanna AE. Premonitory urges and sensorimotor processing in Tourette syndrome. Behav Neurol. (2013) 27:65–73. doi: 10.3233/BEN-120308

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Conceição VA, Dias Â, Farinha AC, Maia TV. Premonitory urges and tics in Tourette syndrome: computational mechanisms and neural correlates. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2017) 46:187–99. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2017.08.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Cox JH, Seri S, Cavanna AE. Sensory aspects of Tourette syndrome. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 88:170–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.03.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Aromataris E, Lockwood C, Porritt K, Pilla B, Jordan Z, editors. JBI manual for evidence synthesis. JBI (2024). doi: 10.46658/JBIMES-24-01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Pringsheim T, Ganos C, Nilles C, Cavanna AE, Gilbert DL, Greenberg E, et al. European Society for the Study of Tourette Syndrome 2022 criteria for clinical diagnosis of functional tic-like behaviors: International consensus from experts in tic disorders. Eur J Neurol. (2023) 30:902–10. doi: 10.1111/ene.15672

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Covidence systematic review software. Veritas Health Innovation, Melbourne, Australia. Available online at: www.covidence.org.

Google Scholar

11. Tricco AC, Lillie E, Zarin W, O’Brien KK, Colquhoun H, Levac D, et al. PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): Checklist and explanation. Ann Intern Med. (2018) 169:467–73. doi: 10.7326/M18-0850

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Woods DW, Piacentini J, Himle MB, Chang S. Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale (PUTS): initial psychometric results and examination of the premonitory urge phenomenon in youths with Tic disorders. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2005) 26:397–403. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200512000-00001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Terwee CB, Bot SDM, de Boer MR, van der Windt DAWM, Knol DL, Dekker J, et al. Quality criteria were proposed for measurement properties of health status questionnaires. J Clin Epidemiol. (2007) 60:34–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2006.03.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Baumung L, Müller-Vahl K, Dyke K, Jackson G, Jackson S, Golm D, et al. Developing the premonitory urges for tic disorders scale-revised (PUTS-R). J Neuropsychol. (2021) 15:129–42. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12216

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Brandt VC, Beck C, Sajin V, Anders S, Münchau A. Convergent validity of the PUTS. Front Psychiatry. (2016) 7:51. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00051

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Crossley E, Seri S, Stern JS, Robertson MM, Cavanna AE. Premonitory urges for tics in adult patients with Tourette syndrome. Brain Dev. (2014) 36:45–50. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2012.12.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Openneer TJC, Tárnok Z, Bognar E, Benaroya-Milshtein N, Garcia-Delgar B, Morer A, et al. The Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale in a large sample of children and adolescents: psychometric properties in a developmental context. An EMTICS study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2020) 29:1411–24. doi: 10.1007/s00787-019-01450-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Raines JM, Edwards KR, Sherman MF, Higginson CI, Winnick JB, Navin K, et al. Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale (PUTS): replication and extension of psychometric properties in youth with chronic tic disorders (CTDs). J Neural Transm (Vienna). (2018) 125:727–34. doi: 10.1007/s00702-017-1818-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Reese HE, Scahill L, Peterson AL, Crowe K, Woods DW, Piacentini J, et al. The premonitory urge to tic: measurement, characteristics, and correlates in older adolescents and adults. Behav Ther. (2014) 45:177–86. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2013.09.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Steinberg T, Shmuel Baruch S, Harush A, Dar R, Woods D, Piacentini J, et al. Tic disorders and the premonitory urge. J Neural Transm (Vienna). (2010) 117:277–84. doi: 10.1007/s00702-009-0353-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Gulisano M, Calì P, Palermo F, Robertson M, Rizzo R. Premonitory Urges in Patients with Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome: An Italian Translation and a 7-Year Follow-up. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. (2015) 25:810–6. doi: 10.1089/cap.2014.0154

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Forcadell E, Garcia-Delgar B, Nicolau R, Pérez-Vigil A, Cordovilla C, Lázaro L, et al. Tic disorders and premonitory urges: validation of the Spanish-language version of the Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale in children and adolescents. Neurologia. (2023) 38:319–25. doi: 10.1016/j.nrleng.2020.09.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Li Y, Woods DW, Gu Y, Yu L, Yan J, Wen F, et al. Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the premonitory urge for tics scale: A preliminary report. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:573803. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.573803

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Matsuda N, Nonaka M, Kono T, Fujio M, Nobuyoshi M, Kano Y. Premonitory awareness facilitates tic suppression: subscales of the premonitory urge for tics scale and a new self-report questionnaire for tic-associated sensations. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:592. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00592

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Kyriazi M, Kalyva E, Vargiami E, Krikonis K, Zafeiriou D. Premonitory urges and their link with tic severity in children and adolescents with tic disorders. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:569. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00569

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Kim M, Chung SK, Yang JC, Park JI, Nam SH, Park TW. Development of the Korean form of the premonitory urge for tics scale: A reliability and validity study. Soa Chongsonyon Chongsin Uihak. (2020) 31:146–53. doi: 10.5765/jkacap.200013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Schütteler C, Woitecki K, Döpfner M, Gerlach AL. Interoception and premonitory urges in children and adolescents with tic disorders. Clin Psychol Eur. (2023) 5:e8185–. doi: 10.32872/cpe.8185

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Zinner SH, Conelea CA, Glew GM, Woods DW, Budman CL. Peer victimization in youth with Tourette syndrome and other chronic tic disorders. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. (2012) 43:124–36. doi: 10.1007/s10578-011-0249-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Rozenman M, Johnson OE, Chang SW, Woods DW, Walkup JT, Wilhelm S, et al. Relationships between premonitory urge and anxiety in youth with chronic tic disorders. Child Health Care. (2015) 44:235–48. doi: 10.1080/02739615.2014.986328

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Pile V, Lau JYF, Topor M, Hedderly T, Robinson S. Interoceptive accuracy in youth with tic disorders: exploring links with premonitory urge, anxiety and quality of life. J Autism Dev Disord. (2018) 48:3474–82. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3608-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Isaacs D, Key AP, Cascio CJ, Conley AC, Walker HC, Wallace MT, et al. Sensory hypersensitivity severity and association with obsessive-compulsive symptoms in adults with tic disorder. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2020) 16:2591–601. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S274165

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Edwards KR, Raines JM, Winnick JB, Sherman MF, Higginson CI, Navin K, et al. Sex and psychiatric comorbidity correlates of the premonitory urge for tic scale in youth with persistent tic disorders. J Neural Transm (Vienna). (2020) 127:977–85. doi: 10.1007/s00702-020-02151-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. McGuire JF, McBride N, Piacentini J, Johnco C, Lewin AB, Murphy TK, et al. The premonitory urge revisited: An individualized premonitory urge for tics scale. J Psychiatr Res. (2016) 83:176–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.09.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Che G, Ren W, McGuire JF, Li P, Zhao Z, Tian J, et al. Clinical evaluation of premonitory urges in children and adolescents using the Chinese version of Individualized Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale. Front Psychiatry. (2023) 14:1224825. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1224825

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Brandt VC, Beck C, Sajin V, Baaske MK, Bäumer T, Beste C, et al. Temporal relationship between premonitory urges and tics in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Cortex. (2016) 77:24–37. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.01.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Schubert L, Verrel J, Behm A, Bäumer T, Beste C, Münchau A. Inter-individual differences in urge-tic associations in Tourette syndrome. Cortex. (2021) 143:80–91. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.06.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Langelage J, Verrel J, Friedrich J, Siekmann A, Schappert R, Bluschke A, et al. Urge-tic associations in children and adolescents with Tourette syndrome. Sci Rep. (2022) 12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-19685-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Himle MB, Woods DW, Conelea CA, Bauer CC, Rice KA. Investigating the effects of tic suppression on premonitory urge ratings in children and adolescents with Tourette’s syndrome. Behav Res Ther. (2007) 45:2964–76. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.08.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Specht MW, Woods DW, Nicotra CM, Kelly LM, Ricketts EJ, Conelea CA, et al. Effects of tic suppression: ability to suppress, rebound, negative reinforcement, and habituation to the premonitory urge. Behav Res Ther. (2013) 51:24–30. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.09.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Capriotti MR, Brandt BC, Turkel JE, Lee HJ, Woods DW. Negative reinforcement and premonitory urges in youth with Tourette syndrome: an experimental evaluation. Behav Modif. (2014) 38:276–96. doi: 10.1177/0145445514531015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Herrmann K, Sprenger A, Baumung L, Alvarez-Fischer D, Munchau A, Brandt V. Help or hurt? How attention modulates tics under different conditions. Cortex. (2019) 120:471–82. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.06.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Brabson LA, Brown JL, Capriotti MR, Ramanujam K, Himle MB, Nicotra CM, et al. Patterned changes in urge ratings with tic suppression in youth with chronic tic disorders. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2016) 50:162–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.07.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Banaschewski T, Woerner W, Rothenberger A. Premonitory sensory phenomena and suppressibility of tics in Tourette syndrome: developmental aspects in children and adolescents. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2003) 45:700–3. doi: 10.1017/s0012162203001294

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Essing J, Jakubovski E, Psathakis N, Cevirme SN, Leckman JF, Müller-Vahl KR. Premonitory urges reconsidered: urge location corresponds to tic location in patients with primary tic disorders. J Mov Disord. (2022) 15:43–52. doi: 10.14802/jmd.21045

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Fremer C, Szejko N, Pisarenko A, Haas M, Laudenbach L, Wegener C, et al. What distinguishes patients with mass social media-induced illness presenting with Tourette-like behavior from those with Tourette syndrome? Results of a prospective cohort study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2024) 274:227–33. doi: 10.1007/s00406-023-01603-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Gu Y, Li Y, Cui Y. Correlation between premonitory urges and tic symptoms in a Chinese population with tic disorders. Pediatr Investig. (2020) 4:86–90. doi: 10.1002/ped4.12189

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Kwak C, Dat Vuong K, Jankovic J. Premonitory sensory phenomenon in Tourette’s syndrome. Mov Disord. (2003) 18:1530–3. doi: 10.1002/mds.10618

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Leckman JF, Walker DE, Cohen DJ. Premonitory urges in Tourette’s syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. (1993) 150:98–102. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.1.98

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. O’Connor K, St-Pierre-Delorme MÈ, Leclerc J, Lavoie M, Blais MT. Meta-cognitions in Tourette syndrome, tic disorders, and body-focused repetitive disorder. Can J Psychiatry. (2014) 59:417–25. doi: 10.1177/070674371405900804

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Sambrani T, Jakubovski E, Muller-Vahl KR. New insights into clinical characteristics of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome: Findings in 1032 patients from a single German center. Front Neurosci. (2016) 10:org/10.3389/fnins.2016.00415. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00415

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Silvestri PR, Chiarotti F, Giustini S, Cardona F. Alexithymia and tic disorders: a study on a sample of children and their mothers. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2019) 28:461–70. doi: 10.1007/s00787-018-1209-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Schunke O, Grashorn W, Kahl U, Schöttle D, Haggard P, Münchau A, et al. Quantitative Sensory Testing in adults with Tourette syndrome. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2016) 24:132–6. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.01.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Brandt V, Essing J, Jakubovski E, Müller-Vahl K. Premonitory urge and tic severity, comorbidities, and quality of life in chronic tic disorders. Mov Disord Clin Pract. (2023) 10:922–32. doi: 10.1002/mdc3.13742

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Karp BI, Hallett M. Extracorporeal “phantom” tics in Tourette’s syndrome. Neurology. (1996) 46:38–40. doi: 10.1212/wnl.46.1.38

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Cuenca J, Glazebrook C, Kendall T, Hedderly T, Heyman I, Jackson G, et al. Perceptions of treatment for tics among young people with Tourette syndrome and their parents: A mixed methods study. BMC Psychiatry. (2015) 15:46. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-0430-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Verdellen CW, Hoogduin CA, Kato BS, Keijsers GP, Cath DC, Hoijtink HB. Habituation of premonitory sensations during exposure and response prevention treatment in Tourette’s syndrome. Behav Modif. (2008) 32:215–27. doi: 10.1177/0145445507309020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Draper A, Jackson GM, Morgan PS, Jackson SR. Premonitory urges are associated with decreased grey matter thickness within the insula and sensorimotor cortex in young people with Tourette syndrome. J Neuropsychol. (2016) 10:143–53. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12089

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Ramsey KA, Essoe JK, Storch EA, Lewin AB, Murphy TK, McGuire JF. Urge intolerance and impairment among youth with Tourette’s and chronic tic disorders. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. (2021) 52:761–71. doi: 10.1007/s10578-020-01085-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Crossley E, Cavanna AE. Sensory phenomena: clinical correlates and impact on quality of life in adult patients with Tourette syndrome. Psychiatry Res. (2013) 209:705–10. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.04.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Ramsey KA, De Nadai AS, Espil FM, Ricketts E, Stiede JT, Schild J, et al. Urge intolerance predicts tic severity and impairment among adults with Tourette syndrome and chronic tic disorders. Front Psychiatry. (2022) 13:org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.929413. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.929413

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Sutherland Owens AN, Miguel EC, Swerdlow NR. Sensory gating scales and premonitory urges in Tourette syndrome. ScientificWorldJournal. (2011) 11:736–41. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2011.57

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. McGuire JF, Piacentini J, Scahill L, Woods DW, Villarreal R, Wilhelm S, et al. Bothersome tics in patients with chronic tic disorders: Characteristics and individualized treatment response to behavior therapy. Behav Res Ther. (2015) 70:56–63. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2015.05.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Wellen BCM, Ramanujam K, Lavelle M, Capriotti MR, Butner J, Euler MJ, et al. A test of the behavioral model of tic disorders using a dynamical systems framework. Behav Ther. (2024) 55:513–27. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2023.08.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Brandt VC, Niessen E, Ganos C, Kahl U, Bäumer T, Münchau A. Altered synaptic plasticity in Tourette’s syndrome and its relationship to motor skill learning. PloS One. (2014) 9:e98417–. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098417

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Kano Y, Matsuda N, Nonaka M, Fujio M, Kuwabara H, Kono T. Sensory phenomena related to tics, obsessive-compulsive symptoms, and global functioning in Tourette syndrome. Compr Psychiatry. (2015) 62:141–6. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.07.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Brandt V, Otte JH, Fremer C, Jakubovski E, Müller-Vahl K. Non-just-right experiences are more closely related to OCD than tics in Tourette patients. Sci Rep. (2023) 13:19627. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-37658-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Larsh TR, Huddleston DA, Horn PS, Wu SW, Cecil KM, Jackson HS, et al. From urges to tics in children with Tourette syndrome: associations with supplementary motor area GABA and right motor cortex physiology. Cereb Cortex. (2022) 33:3922–33. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac316

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Batschelett MA, Huddleston DA, Crocetti D, Horn PS, Mostofsky SH, Gilbert DL. Biomarkers of tic severity in children with Tourette syndrome: Motor cortex inhibition measured with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2023) 65:1321–31. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.15578

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Ricketts EJ, Woods DW, Espil FM, McGuire JF, Stiede JT, Schild J, et al. Childhood predictors of long-term tic severity and tic impairment in Tourette’s disorder. Behav Ther. (2022) 53:1250–64. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2022.07.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Muller-Vahl KR, Riemann L, Bokemeyer S. Tourette patients’ misbelief of a tic rebound is due to overall difficulties in reliable tic rating. J Psychosom Res. (2014) 76:472–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.03.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Ganos C, Kahl U, Schunke O, Kühn S, Haggard P, Gerloff C, et al. Are premonitory urges a prerequisite of tic inhibition in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2012) 83:975–8. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2012-303033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Capriotti MR, Espil FM, Conelea CA, Woods DW. Environmental factors as potential determinants of premonitory urge severity in youth with Tourette syndrome. J Obsessive Compuls Relat Disord. (2013) 2:37–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2012.10.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Rae CL, Larsson DEO, Garfinkel SN, Critchley HD. Dimensions of interoception predict premonitory urges and tic severity in Tourette syndrome. Psychiatry Res. (2019) 271:469–75. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.036

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Eddy CM, Rickards HE, Cavanna AE. Physiological awareness is negatively related to inhibitory functioning in Tourette syndrome. Behav Modif. (2014) 38:319–35. doi: 10.1177/0145445513504431

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Narapareddy A, Eckland MR, Riordan HR, Cascio CJ, Isaacs DA. Altered interoceptive sensibility in adults with chronic tic disorder. Front Psychiatry. (2022) 13:914897. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.914897

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Ganos C, Garrido A, Navalpotro-Gómez I, Ricciardi L, Martino D, Edwards MJ, et al. Premonitory urge to tic in Tourette’s is associated with interoceptive awareness. Mov Disord. (2015) 30:1198–202. doi: 10.1002/mds.26228

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Rae CL, Larsson DEO, Eccles JA, Ward J, Critchley HD. Subjective embodiment during the rubber hand illusion predicts severity of premonitory sensations and tics in Tourette Syndrome. Conscious Cognit. (2018) 65:368–77. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.09.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Janik P, Milanowski L, Szejko N. Phenomenology and Clinical Correlates of Stimulus-Bound Tics in Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome. Front Neurol. (2018) 9:477. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00477

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Bhikram T, Crawley A, Arnold P, Abi-Jaoude E, Sandor P. Neuroimaging the emotional modulation of urge inhibition in Tourette Syndrome. Cortex. (2021) 135:341–51. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.10.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Ganos C, Asmuss L, Bongert J, Brandt V, Münchau A, Haggard P. Volitional action as perceptual detection: predictors of conscious intention in adolescents with tic disorders. Cortex. (2015) 64:47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.09.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Arbuzova P, Guo S, Koß C, Kurvits L, Faivre N, Kühn AA, et al. No evidence of impaired visual and tactile metacognition in adults with Tourette disorder. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2022) 97:29–33. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2022.02.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Eddy CM, Cavanna AE. Premonitory urges in adults with complicated and uncomplicated Tourette syndrome. Behav Modif. (2014) 38:264–75. doi: 10.1177/0145445513504432

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Isaacs DA, Riordan HR, Claassen DO. Clinical correlates of health-related quality of life in adults with chronic tic disorder. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.619854. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.619854

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Gerasch S, Kanaan AS, Jakubovski E, Muller-Vahl KR. Aripiprazole improves associated comorbid conditions in addition to tics in adult patients with Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome. Front Neurosci. (2016) 10:org/10.3389/fnins.2016.00416. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00416

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Steinberg T, Harush A, Barnea M, Dar R, Piacentini J, Woods D, et al. Tic-related cognition, sensory phenomena, and anxiety in children and adolescents with Tourette syndrome. Compr Psychiatry. (2013) 54:462–6. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.12.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Hagstrøm J, Spang KS, Vangkilde S, Maigaard K, Skov L, Pagsberg AK, et al. An observational study of emotion regulation in children with Tourette syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2021) 62:790–7. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13375

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Cavanna AE, David K, Orth M, Robertson MM. Predictors during childhood of future health-related quality of life in adults with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Eur J Pediatr Neurol. (2012) 16:605–12. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2012.02.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Kano Y, Fujio M, Kaji N, Matsuda N, Nonaka M, Kono T. Changes in sensory phenomena, tics, obsessive-compulsive symptoms, and global functioning of Tourette syndrome: A follow-up after four years. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:619. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00619

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Eddy CM, Cavanna AE. It’s a curse!”: Coprolalia in Tourette syndrome. Eur J Neurol. (2013) 20:1467–70. doi: 10.1111/ene.12207

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Eddy CM, Cavanna AE. On being your own worst enemy: An investigation of socially inappropriate symptoms in Tourette syndrome. J Psychiatr Res. (2013) 47:1259–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.05.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Gev E, Pilowsky-Peleg T, Fennig S, Benaroya-Milshtein N, Woods DW, Piacentini J, et al. Acceptance of premonitory urges and tics. J Obsessive Compuls Relat Disord. (2016) 10:78–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2016.06.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Kang NR, Kim HJ, Moon DS, Kwack YS. Effects of group comprehensive behavioral intervention for tics in children with Tourette’s disorder and chronic tic disorder. Soa Chongsonyon Chongsin Uihak. (2022) 33:91–8. doi: 10.5765/jkacap.220013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Houghton DC, Capriotti MR, Scahill LD, Wilhelm S, Peterson AL, Walkup JT, et al. Investigating habituation to premonitory urges in behavior therapy for tic disorders. Behav Ther. (2017) 48:834–46. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2017.08.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Deckersbach T, Chou T, Britton JC, Carlson LE, Reese HE, Siev J, et al. Neural correlates of behavior therapy for Tourette’s disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 224:269–74. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.09.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Nonaka M, Matsuda N, Kono T, Fujio M, Scahill L, Kano Y. Preliminary study of behavioral therapy for Tourette syndrome patients in Japan. Children’s Health Care. (2015) 44:293–306. doi: 10.1080/02739615.2014.979922

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

96. van de Griendt J, van den Berg NME, Verdellen CWJ, Cath DC, Verbraak MJPM. Working mechanisms of exposure and response prevention in the treatment of Tourette syndrome and tic disorders revisited: no evidence for within-session habituation to premonitory urges. J Clin Med. (2023) 12(22):7087. doi: 10.3390/jcm12227087

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Heijerman-Holtgrefe AP, Verdellen CWJ, van de Griendt JMTM, Beljaars LPL, Kan KJ, Cath D, et al. Tackle your Tics: pilot findings of a brief, intensive group-based exposure therapy program for children with tic disorders. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2021) 30:461–73. doi: 10.1007/s00787-020-01532-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Nissen JB, Parner ET, Thomsen PH. Predictors of therapeutic treatment outcome in adolescent chronic tic disorders. BJPsych Open. (2019) 5:e74–. doi: 10.1192/bjo.2019.56

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Sukhodolsky DG, Woods DW, Piacentini J, Wilhelm S, Peterson AL, Katsovich L, et al. Moderators and predictors of response to behavior therapy for tics in Tourette syndrome. Neurology. (2017) 88:1029–36. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003710

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Gilbert DL, Budman CL, Singer HS, Kurlan R, Chipkin RE. A D1 receptor antagonist, ecopipam, for treatment of tics in Tourette syndrome. Clin Neuropharmacol. (2014) 37:26–30. doi: 10.1097/WNF.0000000000000017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Jankovic J, Jimenez-Shahed J, Brown LW. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of topiramate in the treatment of Tourette syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2010) 81:70–3. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2009.185348

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

102. McGuire JF, Ginder N, Ramsey K, Essoe JKY, Ricketts EJ, McCracken JT, et al. Optimizing behavior therapy for youth with Tourette’s disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2020) 45:2114–9. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-0762-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Jankovic J. Botulinum toxin in the treatment of dystonic tics. Movement Disord. (1994) 9:347–9. doi: 10.1002/mds.870090315

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Kwak CH, Hanna PA, Jankovic J. Botulinum toxin in the treatment of tics. Arch Neurol. (2000) 57:1190–3. doi: 10.1001/archneur.57.8.1190

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Marras C, Andrews D, Sime E, Lang AE. Botulinum toxin for simple motor tics: A randomized, double-blind, controlled clinical trial. Neurology. (2001) 56:605–10. doi: 10.1212/WNL.56.5.605

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Rath JJG, Tavy DLJ, Wertenbroek AAACM, van Woerkom TCAM, de Bruijn SFTM. Botulinum toxin type A in simple motor tics: Short-term and long-term treatment-effects. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2010) 16:478–81. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2009.11.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Porta M, Maggioni G, Ottaviani F, Schindler A. Treatment of phonic tics in patients with Tourette’s syndrome using botulinum toxin type A. Neurol Sci. (2004) 24:420–3. doi: 10.1007/s10072-003-0201-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Di Lazzaro G, Magrinelli F, Ganos C, Bhatia KP. The need to tic. Mov Disord Clin Pract. (2020) 7:863–4. doi: 10.1002/mdc3.13023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Muller-Vahl KR, Fremer C, Beals C, Ivkovic J, Loft H, Schindler C. Monoacylglycerol lipase inhibition in Tourette syndrome: A 12-week, randomized, controlled study. Movement Disord. (2021) 36:2413–8. doi: 10.1002/mds.28681

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Muller-Vahl KR, Fremer C, Beals C, Ivkovic J, Loft H, Schindler C. Endocannabinoid modulation using monoacylglycerol lipase inhibition in Tourette syndrome: A phase 1 randomized, placebo-controlled study. Pharmacopsychiatry. (2022) 55:148–56. doi: 10.1055/a-1675-3494

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Müller-Vahl KR, Kolbe H, Schneider U, Emrich HM. Cannabinoids: possible role in patho-physiology and therapy of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (1998) 98:502–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1998.tb10127.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Anis S, Zalomek C, Korczyn AD, Rosenberg A, Giladi N, Gurevich T. Medical Cannabis for Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome: An Open-Label Prospective Study. Behav Neurol. (2022) 2022:5141773. doi: 10.1155/2022/5141773

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Abi-Jaoude E, Bhikram T, Parveen F, Levenbach J, Lafreniere-Roula M, Sandor P. A double-blind, randomized, controlled crossover trial of cannabis in adults with Tourette syndrome. Cannabis Cannabinoid Res. (2022) 8:835–45. doi: 10.1089/can.2022.0091

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Silver AA, Shytle RD, Sanberg PR. Clinical experience with transdermal nicotine patch in Tourette syndrome. CNS Spectr. (1999) 4:68–76. doi: 10.1017/S1092852900011408

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Szejko N, Fremer C, Sühs KW, Macul Ferreira de Barros P, Müller-Vahl KR. Intravenous Immunoglobulin Treatment Did Not Improve Tics in a Patient With Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome and Intrathecal Antibody Synthesis. Front Neurol. (2020) 11:110. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Szejko N, Jakubovski E, Fremer C, Muller-Vahl KR. Vaporized cannabis is effective and well-tolerated in an adolescent with Tourette syndrome. Med Cannabis Cannabinoids. (2019) 2:60–4. doi: 10.1159/000496355

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Servello D, Porta M, Sassi M, Brambilla A, Robertson MM. Deep brain stimulation in 18 patients with severe Gilles de la Tourette syndrome refractory to treatment: the surgery and stimulation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2008) 79:136–42. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.104067

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Kano Y, Matsuda N, Nonaka M, Fujio M, Kono T, Kaido T. Sensory phenomena and obsessive-compulsive symptoms in Tourette syndrome following deep brain stimulation: Two case reports. J Clin Neurosci. (2018) 56:199–201. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2018.06.046

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Wilken M, Cerquetti D, Rossi M, Obeso JA, Merello M. Low-frequency oscillations at the limbic globus pallidus internus seem to be associated with premonitory urges in Tourette’s syndrome. Movement Disord. (2021) 36:2966–7. doi: 10.1002/mds.28784

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Muller-Vahl KR, Szejko N, Saryyeva A, Schrader C, Krueger D, Horn A, et al. Randomized double-blind sham-controlled trial of thalamic versus GPi stimulation in patients with severe medically refractory Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Brain Stimul. (2021) 14:662–75. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2021.04.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Eapen V, Baker R, Walter A, Raghupathy V, Wehrman JJ, Sowman PF. The role of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in Tourette syndrome: A review and preliminary findings. Brain Sci. (2017) 7:161. doi: 10.3390/brainsci7120161

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Morera Maiquez B, Sigurdsson HP, Dyke K, Clarke E, McGrath P, Pasche M, et al. Entraining movement-related brain oscillations to suppress tics in Tourette syndrome. Curr Biol. (2020) 30:2334–2342.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.04.044

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Maiquez BM, Smith C, Dyke K, Chou CP, Kasbia B, McCready C, et al. A double-blind, sham-controlled, trial of home-administered rhythmic 10-Hz median nerve stimulation for the reduction of tics, and suppression of the urge-to-tic, in individuals with Tourette syndrome and chronic tic disorder. J Neuropsychol. (2023) 17:540–63. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12313

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Fu M, Wei H, Meng X, Chen H, Shang B, Chen F, et al. Effects of low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the bilateral parietal cortex in patients with Tourette syndrome. Front Neurol. (2021) 12:602830. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.602830

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Tinaz S, Malone P, Hallett M, Horovitz SG. Role of the right dorsal anterior insula in the urge to tic in Tourette syndrome. Mov Disord. (2015) 30:1190–7. doi: 10.1002/mds.26230

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Bhikram T, Arnold P, Crawley A, Abi-Jaoude E, Sandor P. The functional connectivity profile of tics and obsessive-compulsive symptoms in Tourette Syndrome. J Psychiatr Res. (2020) 123:128–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.01.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Tinaz S, Belluscio BA, Malone P, van der Veen JW, Hallett M, Horovitz SG. Role of the sensorimotor cortex in Tourette syndrome using multimodal imaging. Hum Brain Mapp. (2014) 35:5834–46. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22588

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Ganos C, Kahl U, Brandt V, Schunke O, Baumer T, Thomalla G, et al. The neural correlates of tic inhibition in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Movement Disord. (2014) 29:S409–. doi: 10.1002/mds.25914

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Eddy CM, Cavanna AE, Rickards HE, Hansen PC. Temporo-parietal dysfunction in Tourette syndrome: Insights from an fMRI study of Theory of Mind. J Psychiatr Res. (2016) 81:102–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.07.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Eddy CM, Cavanna AE, Hansen PC. Empathy and aversion: the neural signature of mentalizing in Tourette syndrome. Psychol Med. (2017) 47:507–17. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716002725

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Rae CL, Raykov P, Ambridge EM, Colling LJ, Gould van Praag CD, Bouyagoub S, et al. Elevated representational similarity of voluntary action and inhibition in Tourette syndrome. Brain Commun. (2023) 5:fcad224–. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcad224

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Rae CL, Parkinson J, Betka S, Gouldvan Praag CD, Bouyagoub S, Polyanska L, et al. Amplified engagement of prefrontal cortex during control of voluntary action in Tourette syndrome. Brain Commun. (2020) 2:18. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcaa199

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Rae CL, Polyanska L, Gould van Praag CD, Parkinson J, Bouyagoub S, Nagai Y, et al. Face perception enhances insula and motor network reactivity in Tourette syndrome. Brain. (2018) 141:3249–61. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy254

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

134. He JL, Mikkelsen M, Huddleston DA, Crocetti D, Cecil KM, Singer HS, et al. Frequency and intensity of premonitory urges-to-tic in tourette syndrome is associated with supplementary motor area GABA+ Levels. Movement Disord. (2022) 37:563–73. doi: 10.1002/mds.28868

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Kanaan AS, Gerasch S, García-García I, Lampe L, Pampel A, Anwander A, et al. Pathological glutamatergic neurotransmission in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Brain. (2017) 140:218–34. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww285

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Hampson M, Tokoglu F, King RA, Constable RT, Leckman JF. Brain areas coactivating with motor cortex during chronic motor tics and intentional movements. Biol Psychiatry. (2009) 65:594–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.11.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Bohlhalter S, Goldfine A, Matteson S, Garraux G, Hanakawa T, Kansaku K, et al. Neural correlates of tic generation in Tourette syndrome: an event-related functional MRI study. Brain. (2006) 129:2029–37. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Zapparoli L, Devoto F, Mariano M, Seghezzi S, Servello D, Porta M, et al. Mapping Gilles de la Tourette syndrome through the distress and relief associated with tic-related behaviors: an fMRI study. Transl Psychiatry. (2024) 14:7. doi: 10.1038/s41398-023-02711-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Jackson SR, Loayza J, Crighton M, Sigurdsson HP, Dyke K, Jackson GM. The role of the insula in the generation of motor tics and the experience of the premonitory urge-to-tic in Tourette syndrome. Cortex. (2020) 126:119–33. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.12.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Jackson SR, Sigurdsson HP, Dyke K, Condon M, Jackson GM. The role of the cingulate cortex in the generation of motor tics and the experience of the premonitory urge-to-tic in Tourette syndrome. J Neuropsychol. (2021) 15:340–62. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12242

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Sigurdsson HP, Jackson SR, Jolley L, Mitchell E, Jackson GM. Alterations in cerebellar grey matter structure and covariance networks in young people with Tourette syndrome. Cortex. (2020) 126:1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.12.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Draganski B, Martino D, Cavanna AE, Hutton C, Orth M, Robertson MM, et al. Multispectral brain morphometry in Tourette syndrome persisting into adulthood. Brain. (2010) 133:3661–75. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq300

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Sigurdsson HP, Pépés SE, Jackson GM, Draper A, Morgan PS, Jackson SR. Alterations in the microstructure of white matter in children and adolescents with Tourette syndrome measured using tract-based spatial statistics and probabilistic tractography. Cortex. (2018) 104:75–89. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.04.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Loo SK, Miyakoshi M, Tung K, Lloyd E, Salgari G, Dillon A, et al. Neural activation and connectivity during cued eye blinks in Chronic Tic Disorders. NeuroImage Clin. (2019) 24:101956. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101956

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Niccolai V, Korczok S, Finis J, Jonas M, Thomalla G, Siebner HR, et al. A peek into premonitory urges in Tourette syndrome: Temporal evolution of neurophysiological oscillatory signatures. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. (2019) 65:153–8. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2019.05.039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Sigurdsson HP, Jackson SR, Kim S, Dyke K, Jackson GM. A feasibility study for somatomotor cortical mapping in Tourette syndrome using neuronavigated transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cortex. (2020) 129:175–87. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.04.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Chen WG, Schloesser D, Arensdorf AM, Simmons JM, Cui C, Valentino R, et al. The emerging science of interoception: sensing, integrating, interpreting, and regulating signals within the self. Trends Neurosci. (2021) 44:3–16. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.10.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Wang LH, Ding WQ, Sun YG. Spinal ascending pathways for somatosensory information processing. Trends Neurosci. (2022) 45:594–607. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2022.05.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Rae CL, Critchley HD, Seth AK. A Bayesian account of the sensory-motor interactions underlying symptoms of Tourette syndrome. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:29. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Tourette syndrome, tic disorder, tic, premonitory urge, premonitory sensation, sensory

Citation: Wohlgemuth JB, Watson KH, Gill KD and Isaacs DA (2025) Premonitory urge in tic disorders – a scoping review. Front. Psychiatry 16:1504442. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1504442

Received: 30 September 2024; Accepted: 06 January 2025;
Published: 30 January 2025.

Edited by:

Daphna Ruhrman, Sheba Medical Center, Israel

Reviewed by:

Mariam Hull, Texas Children’s Hospital, United States
Jinhua Sun, Fudan University, China
Cyril Atkinson-Clement, University of Nottingham, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2025 Wohlgemuth, Watson, Gill and Isaacs. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: John B. Wohlgemuth, am9obi5iLndvaGxnZW11dGhAdmFuZGVyYmlsdC5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.