Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry , 05 March 2025

Sec. Adolescent and Young Adult Psychiatry

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1497007

Association between maternal emotion socialization and emotion regulation in early adolescents with elevated internalizing symptoms: insights from multi-informant discrepancies

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
  • 2Orygen, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
  • 3Centre for Youth Mental Health, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
  • 4Mindful, Centre for Training and Research in Developmental Health, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia

Introduction: Although the relationship between parental emotion socialization and emotional competence, including emotion regulation, in children and adolescents has been extensively explored, there is a lack of research investigating this association in adolescents at high risk for mental health problems. The present study examined the association between maternal emotion socialization and emotion regulation in adolescents with high levels of internalizing symptoms, using multi-informant measurements (mother-reported, adolescent-reported, observer-reported). The study also explored whether discrepancies in the report of parental emotion socialization by different informants were related to adolescent emotion regulation, in addition to factors that may contribute to informant discrepancies.

Methods: Participants were 70 female adolescents (mean age = 11.46 years, SD = 0.77) with high levels of internalizing symptoms and their mothers. Maternal emotion socialization (i.e., emotion dismissing and emotion coaching) was assessed using mother- and adolescent-reported questionnaires, and via observation during an emotion discussion task. Adolescent emotion regulation was reported by mothers and adolescents, while maternal emotion regulation was self-reported.

Results: Adolescent-reported maternal emotion coaching and dismissing were significantly related to adolescent-reported adolescent emotion regulation. Informant discrepancies were not related to adolescent emotion regulation. Mothers higher in emotion regulation difficulties reported that their emotion coaching was more congruent with adolescent- and observer-reported emotion coaching, although this effect did not reach statistical significance.

Discussion: Our findings highlight the value of adolescent-reported variables in parenting and adolescent emotion research. Additionally, mothers’ emotion regulation may influence their assessments of their emotion socialization behaviors.

1 Introduction

Emotion regulation (ER) is a key component of emotional competence (which comprises understanding of emotions, expression of emotions, and regulation of emotions). It is a complex construct, which refers to the ability to influence the generation, experience, and expression of emotion (1). The ability to monitor and evaluate emotions (e.g., be aware of and accept emotions) is also considered a part of ER (2). As an essential developmental skill, ER is closely linked to a young person’s well-being. Difficulties in ER are considered a broad risk factor for mental health problems during childhood and adolescence, including internalizing disorders (3). On the other hand, ER competence in young people is critical for attenuating the risk of developing internalizing disorders (4). Therefore, the development of ER is crucial for children and adolescents and investigating factors that may shape their ER is important for promoting healthy development and for identifying targets for prevention or early intervention.

The development of ER primarily occurs during childhood and adolescence (5). The family, as the primary environment for young people’s maturation, significantly influences the development of emotional competence (6). Among parenting factors, Eisenberg et al. (6) emphasized that emotion-related parenting has a significant impact on children’s development of emotional competence. They named emotion-related parenting philosophy and behavior ‘parental emotion socialization’, which refers to parents’ beliefs of emotions, how parents respond to children’s expression of emotion, discuss emotion with children, and express emotion themselves. Aligned with Eisenberg’s theory, Gottman et al. (7) proposed parents’ ‘meta-emotion philosophy’ (i.e., parents’ thoughts, beliefs and attitudes about their own and their children’s emotions) impacts their parenting in ways that can be described as either emotion coaching or emotion dismissing. Emotion coaching, a supportive form of parental emotion socialization, involves five key steps: (1) being aware of and understanding one’s and their children’s emotions, especially emotions of low intensity; (2) viewing children’s negative emotions as an opportunity to teach children how to manage such emotions; (3) being accepting of and empathic with children’s emotions; (4) helping children to describe their feelings; and (5) aiding them to solve problems and setting limits around behavior. In contrast, emotion dismissing, which represents an unsupportive form of parental emotion socialization, occurs when parents ignore, minimize or disapprove of children’s emotions. Both Gottman and Eisenberg hypothesized that parental emotion socialization directly affects children’s emotional competence, including ER.

Providing support for the aforementioned theoretical models, a number of studies have found associations between parent emotion socialization and children’s general emotional competence (e.g., 8, 9). There are fewer studies, however, that have examined links with ER, specifically. These studies have found that preschool children who receive more supportive maternal responses to their negative emotions use better ER strategies, such as context-appropriate ways to manage negative emotions (10). Unsupportive maternal emotion socialization has been found to be associated with children’s ER difficulties, including a lack of emotional awareness and inappropriate emotional expression (11). Unsupportive paternal emotion socialization has also been related to poorer children ER ability (12). While these studies explore the relationship between parental emotion socialization and ER, they have not investigated these associations in at-risk samples. Understanding these associations in those with greater mental health risks or difficulties can provide a comprehensive picture of emotion socialization in families and inform parenting interventions for children experiencing ER difficulties.

Additionally, while prior work has largely focused on younger children; there is little research examining how parental emotion socialization behavior relate to adolescents’ ER. Adolescence is a transition period between childhood and adulthood, when the brain’s cognitive control system, self-schema, and self-regulation capacity undergo significant changes (13). These emotional and behavioral changes make parent-adolescent relationships particularly challenging during this period, resulting in an increase in family conflicts and the establishment of more equal power dynamics between parents and children (14). Furthermore, female adolescents are more likely to experience emotional instability and internalizing symptoms compared to male adolescents (15), and maternal behavior has been found to more strongly correlated with ER capacity in female than male adolescents (16). Among the stages of adolescence, early adolescence is often marked by emotional instability due to heightened emotional reactivity and the rapid development of the brain (17). Consequently, early adolescence is a critical period to shape the development of ER. While some research has found supportive maternal emotion socialization to predict an improvement in the regulation of anger and sadness and lower levels of daily negative emotions in female adolescents (18, 19), further research is needed on the relationship between maternal emotion socialization behavior and early female adolescents’ general emotion regulation ability, rather than focusing solely on the regulation of specific emotions.

A further consideration in this research area concerns the measurement of parent emotion socialization and the correspondence of reports from different informants. More broadly, research findings in parenting studies often show inconsistencies when the same measures is administered to different informants (20, 21). A meta-analysis examining informant discrepancies in parenting found that there was a significant difference between parents’ and adolescents’ reports (22). Parents were more likely to perceive their parenting behavior as more positive compared to adolescents, who were more critical. Other work has found observer-reported parenting behavior to show weak associations with parent-reported parenting behavior (23). While coding observed behavior (e.g., during a parent-child interaction task) has been suggested as a more objective assessment of parental responses to their children’s emotions (24), it can be argued that observational measurements simply capture different aspects of parenting compared to self-report questionnaires, rather than one being more accurate than the other (25). Moreover, the type of task can influence the interaction between parents and children (26), so observational measures are dependent on the specific task and setting. Further, previous research has demonstrated that parenting behavior, as reported by different informants, has varying associations with child outcomes (27, 28). Therefore, employing a comprehensive approach that combines questionnaires with observational coding may enhance the validity of evaluating parental emotion socialization.

On the other hand, the discrepancy among informants might provide unique information beyond independent perspectives. Previous research suggests that a greater degree of informant discrepancy, specifically between parents and children, is associated with child maladjustment (29, 30). Discrepant reports might indicate that parents have a lack of awareness of their children’s development (30), which may be related to a lack of communication/insight or a high level of parent-child conflict. The discrepancy might also arise from parents’ psychopathology. Research has shown, for example, that higher levels of mental health problems in parents are related to lower congruence between parent- and adolescent-reported parenting (22, 31). De Los Reyes et al. (32) emphasized the importance of measuring both parents and children’s characteristics (e.g., maternal psychopathological symptoms) to better capture the dynamic processes underlying discrepant reports. Given that difficulties in ER are suggested to be a key transdiagnostic factor in both internalizing and externalizing disorders (33), and also that ER difficulties affect perception of affective behavior (34), ER difficulties might be the mechanism through which maladjustment (e.g., mental health disorders) leads to discrepancies in reporting parenting behavior. Considering factors that may increase discrepant reporting will help to better interpret research findings, and it may also be clinically relevant, for example, in relation to deciding intervention eligibility.

Although previous research has identified associations between informant discrepancies and child maladjustment (29, 30), there is no literature addressing the relationship between discrepancies in maternal emotion socialization and adolescent ER. Maternal emotion socialization, being a dyadic process, involves the subjective experiences of both mothers and children during interactions. Therefore, having insight into informant discrepancies and their relationship to adolescent emotional outcomes may offer a new viewpoint for exploring the potential influence of maternal emotion socialization on adolescents’ ER. In a clinical context, understanding these discrepancies will enhance the assessment of parenting behavior or family relationships for clinical intervention.

In summary, few studies have focused on the relationship between maternal emotion socialization and adolescent ER in high-risk sample (35). Further, existing research has primarily focused on community samples, rather than specifically targeting adolescents at risk for mental health problems associated with the experience of intense negative emotions. Additionally, previous studies have not addressed the relationship between informant discrepancies in maternal emotion socialization and adolescent ER.

The current study aimed to examine the relationship between maternal emotion socialization and ER in female adolescents with high levels of internalizing symptoms, which is a risk factor for developing internalizing disorders (36), using multi-informant measurements. Given evidence for sex differences in the effect of parental emotion socialization on adolescents’ emotional competences (6), and the higher prevalence of internalizing disorders in female adolescents compared to male adolescents (37), only female adolescents and their mothers were recruited to reduce heterogeneity and maximize statistical power. We further investigated the discrepancies between adolescent-reported, mother-reported, and observed maternal emotion socialization and their relationship to adolescent ER. Moreover, as mentioned above, difficulties in emotion regulation might be the mechanism leading to informant discrepancies in parental emotion socialization. To explore this further, we investigated whether adolescent or maternal emotion regulation difficulties contribute to any identified discrepancies. We hypothesized that (1) emotion coaching would be positively correlated and emotion dismissing would be negatively correlated with adolescent ER, regardless of the informant of parent emotion socialization and adolescent ER; (2) informant discrepancies in maternal emotion socialization would be associated with lower adolescent ER; (3) both adolescent and maternal difficulties in ER would contribute to the discrepancies in ratings for maternal emotion socialization; greater difficulties in both mothers and adolescents would predict greater informant discrepancy in maternal emotion socialization. This final hypothesis focuses on the causes of informant discrepancies by examining whether difficulties in ER at the individual level explain these discrepancies.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

Seventy female adolescents aged 10-12 years (mean age = 11.46 years, SD = 0.77) and their mothers (mean age = 41.89 years, SD = 11.62) were recruited from primary schools, clinical services, social media platforms, local universities, and flyers in local community centers. Mothers were predominantly Caucasian (81.4%), with a minority of mothers being Asian (11.4%), Mixed Heritage (4.3%), and Aboriginal or Torres Strait Islander (2.9%). The household types reported by mothers were as follows: 74.3% original family (both biological parents present), 17.1% sole parent family, 5.7% other, and 2.9% stepfamily (two parents, one being a step-parent to the adolescent in the study). Regarding education, the majority of mothers had completed or partially completed a university degree (88.6%), including a bachelor’s degree (34.3%), Honors degree (12.9%), or postgraduate degree (41.4%); 8.6% of mothers had completed a TAFE (Technical And Further Education)/Vocational training course, one mother had experienced primary school education, and one mother had received upper second school education. The majority of mothers reported their family’s gross annual income as more than $100,000 (above average income in Australia ( (38), 72.9%), the other mothers reported $60,000-$99,000 (15.7%), $40,000-$59,999 (8.6%), $20,000-$39,000 (1.4%), and 0-$19,999 (1.4%). Adolescents were considered eligible if they showed moderate to high levels of internalizing symptoms (e.g., anxiety and depression), with scores above the 50th percentile on the self-reported Revised Children’s Anxiety and Depression Scale upon screening (39). Adolescents were excluded if they had a current diagnosis of a developmental or intellectual disorder, were currently using psychotropic medication, or had a history of head trauma.

2.2 Procedure

The current study utilized baseline data (pre-intervention) from a randomized controlled trial—Tuning in to Teens’ Brains (TINTB) study (ACTRN12621001304820). In the TINTB study, both mothers and adolescents were invited to a phone appointment where a study team member explained details of the study and conducted initial screening. Participants (both mothers and their adolescent daughter) provided verbal and written consent before participating in the study. Eligible participants were randomly assigned to either the intervention or the waitlist condition by a block-design computer randomizer. At the baseline assessment, mothers and their daughters completed questionnaires to assess maternal emotion socialization and adolescent ER. Mothers also reported on their own ER. Additionally, maternal emotion socialization was measured during an emotion discussion task performed in the families’ homes on Zoom with a researcher. Adolescents received compensation in the form of gift cards at a rate of $20 per hour. Mothers were not reimbursed for participation, but any reasonable travel and parking expense related to the study visit were covered in the form of parking/taxi vouchers.

2.3 Measures

2.3.1 Maternal emotion socialization (mother and adolescent report)

The Emotions As a Child scale [EAC; (40)] is a 45-item questionnaire assessing maternal emotion socialization behavior related to adolescents’ anger, fear, and sadness (15 items for each emotion), with each item using a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very strong). The questionnaire includes five subscales: Reward, Override, Neglect, Magnify, and Punish. Among these subscales, Reward (example item: “When I was sad, my mother asked me what made me sad”) is categorized as a kind of supportive maternal emotion socialization (emotion coaching), and the sum of Neglect and Punish (example items include “When I was sad, my mother did not pay attention to my sadness” and “When I was sad, my mother told me to stop being sad”) are considered as a kind of unsupportive maternal emotion socialization (emotion dismissing). Seven items were reversed, higher scores on the Reward scales indicated greater supportive maternal emotion socialization; higher scores on the sum of Neglect and Punish scales indicated greater unsupportive maternal emotion socialization. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.93 for adolescent-reported emotion coaching, 0.89 for adolescent-reported emotion dismissing, 0.91 for mother-reported emotion coaching, and 0.87 for mother-reported emotion dismissing.

These measures of supportive and unsupportive maternal emotion socialization have been operationalized in the same way in previous research (41). The reason for excluding Override and Magnify is that the roles of these strategies in children’s development remain ambiguous. Override refers to suppressing a child’s emotional expression by diverting them or changing their emotion (40). Some have interpreted this distraction of emotion as minimizing children’s emotions, potentially negatively impacting children’s understanding of emotion (42). In O’Neal and Magai’s model (43), however, Override is categorized as a supportive parenting strategy. Furthermore, Magnify refers to when a child expresses an emotion, parents respond by expressing the same emotion with a stronger intensity towards the child. In O’Neal and Magai’s model (43), Magnify is classified as unsupportive maternal emotion socialization. Conversely, magnifying fear and sadness has been found to have a positive correlation with emotional warmth and empathy (44).

2.3.2 Maternal emotion socialization (Observation)

The Emotion Discussion task [based on the work of Fivush et al. (45) and Suveg et al. (46)] is a 15-minute mother-adolescent interaction activity that asks the mother and adolescent to discuss occasions when the adolescent felt anxious, sad, and angry (5 minutes for each emotion). This task was performed at home with the mother-daughter dyad in the same physical room, and was recorded via Zoom (the one Zoom window captured both participants on video). A researcher, who was present throughout online, remained in the Zoom room but only provided cues to signal when to switch to the next emotion task. The observer kept their camera off and microphone muted during the task and unmuted only when it was time to move on to the next emotion task. The video recording was later coded for maternal emotion socialization.

For the coding of maternal emotion socialization behavior, a global rating system based on Baker et al.’s method (47) for coding emotion coaching and emotion dismissing was employed. Each subscale is rated on a scale from 1 (no signs of emotion coaching or emotion dismissing) to 5 (consistently showing emotion coaching or emotion dismissing during the task). Emotion coaching comprises five items: Structuring of emotion understanding, Sensitivity/acceptance, Validation and encouragement of emotional expression, Enthusiasm for discussion, and Intimacy/warmth/affection. For emotion dismissing, four items were coded: Derogation of child, Intrusiveness, Minimization of emotion/discouragement of expression, and Detachment/disinterest. Coders considered each subscale for emotion coaching and emotion dismissing to assign an overall 1 to 5 rating (“Overall Coaching”, “Overall Dismissing”). The overall scores for “Overall Coaching” and “Overall dismissing” were not calculated as a combination (e.g., sum or average) of the individual coding items. Instead, following Baker et al.’s method, coders viewed the recordings twice. During the first viewing, they generated an overall score to represent a global assessment of the parent’s behavior. During the second viewing, coders rated each item and compared these ratings to the overall coaching and dismissing scores. Structuring (for coaching) and Derogation of Child (for dismissing) were weighted most heavily when determining the overall scores. Coders considered any significant inconsistencies between item ratings and the overall score, making adjustments as necessary to ensure accuracy.

Two research students were first trained to deliver the Tuning into Teens intervention, a parenting intervention aimed at improving emotion coaching skills (48), and gain insight into the concept of parental emotion socialization. They were then trained to code the emotion discussion tasks by an experienced coder, utilizing data from the initial 5 recordings to familiarize themselves with the coding procedure, and then using 10 recordings to establish reliability. Inter-rater reliability was found to be acceptable, with a percent agreement of 92.86% (a <= one-point difference between raters was considered agreement; 49). During the inter-rater reliability check, coders aligned coding criteria and clarified any ambiguous description based on Baker et al.’s manual.

2.3.3 Adolescent emotion regulation (mother and adolescent report)

The Emotion Expression Scale for Children [EESC; (50)] is an adolescent-reported 16-item questionnaire designed to assess adolescents’ ER. It measures two aspects of ER: emotion awareness and motivation to express emotion regulation. Participants rate each item on a scale from 1 (not at all true) to 5 (extremely true), with higher scores indicating greater difficulties in regulating emotions. The total score is recommended in use of measuring difficulties in ER (51). The sum of all item ratings was used to obtain a total score for emotion regulation difficulties (Cronbach’s alpha = .83).

The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale – Parent Report [DERS-PR; (52)] was used to assess parent perceptions of adolescents’ ER. This 36-item mother-reported questionnaire encompasses six factors: Lack of emotional awareness, Lack of emotional clarity, Difficulty engaging in goal-directed behavior, Impulse control difficulties, Non-acceptance of emotional responses, and Limited access to emotion regulation strategies. Items are rated from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always), with higher scores indicating greater difficulties in emotion regulation. The sum of all items ratings was used (Cronbach’s alpha = .91).

2.3.4 Maternal emotion regulation (mother report)

The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale [DERS; (53)] is a 36-item questionnaire to assess ER through self-report. It has a similar structure to the DERS-PR, featuring the same six subscales. All item ratings were summed to obtain a total score, with higher scores indicating greater difficulties in ER (Cronbach’s alpha = .87).

2.3.5 Supplementary analysis variables

2.3.5.1 Demographic variables

Mothers’ race (1 = Aboriginal or Torres Strait Islander, 2 = Asian, 3 = Black people, 4 = Pasifika/Maori, 5 = White/Caucasian, 6 = Other/Mixed Heritage), family composition (1 = Original family (both biological parents present), 2 = Step family (two parents, one being a step parent to the child in the study), 3 = Sole parent family, 4 = Other), and family income were included as demographic variables.

2.3.5.2 Mother internalizing symptoms (mother report)

The Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale [DASS; (54)] is a 21-item self-reported questionnaire to assess distress along three scales of depression, anxiety, and stress. Each item is rated from 0 (Never) to 3 (Almost always). The total scores for all items were used to represent mother internalizing symptoms (Cronbach’s alpha = .92).

2.3.5.3 Adolescent internalizing symptom (adolescent report)

The Revised Child Anxiety and Depression Scale [RCADS; (39)] is a 47-item self-reported questionnaire designed to assess the level of internalizing symptoms in youth aged from 8 to 18 years. In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha = .91. Adolescent internalizing symptoms were used for screening and were not the primary focus of the present study. However, for descriptive purposes, correlations between internalizing symptoms and other variables are presented.

2.3.5.4 Sexual maturity status (mother report)

The Sexual Maturity Scale Parent-Report [SMS-PR; (55)] is a set of line drawings of girls bodies, based on five Tanner stages of pubertal development (56). Parents were presented with images of breast and female pubic hair development and asked to pick the image that most represents their adolescent’s development. If ratings of pubic hair and breast development were different, the higher value was used (57).

2.4 Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted in RStudio Version 2023.6.1 (58). One participant’s recording of the emotion discussion task was missing, and three participants did not complete the emotion discussion task due to personal reasons. Multiple imputation of these missing data was run with the mice package in RStudio (v3.16.0; 59).

The first hypothesis (i.e., the relationship between maternal emotion socialization and adolescent ER) was examined using bivariate correlations between maternal emotion socialization reported by multi-informants (mothers, adolescents, and observers), adolescents’ self-reported and mother-reported ER, self-reported maternal ER, and self-reported and mother-reported adolescent internalizing symptoms. To test the second hypothesis (i.e., investigate the relationship between informant discrepancy and adolescent ER), we used polynomial regression analyses to investigate informant discrepancy (60). The direct difference score correlation indicates (a) differences in correlations between reports from two different informants and the second variable, and (b) differences in the variances of two informant reports (60, 61). The use of difference scores has been extensively critiqued. Difference scores cannot be produced when two informants’ reports have equal variance and equal correlations with the outcome, even if the difference score is significantly correlated with the second variable. Polynomial regression analyses can be used as an alternative to difference scores to investigate informant discrepancy (60). Interaction terms added in the polynomial regression model directly assess whether high (or low) scores from one informant show a stronger or weaker association with the outcome when scores from the other informant are also high (or low). Polynomial regression analyses were thus employed to test the impact of interactions between informant reports on adolescent ER. In the current study, separate models were run for each pair of informants and for emotion coaching and emotion dismissing. If the interaction terms in polynomial regression models were associated with the outcome, we used post hoc tests to identify the pattern of associations (30). Linear regression models were used to examine the role of mothers’ and adolescents’ ER in informant discrepancies. First, models were tested for the moderating role of adolescent ER in the association between maternal, self-rated- and observer-reported emotion socialization. Separate models were tested for emotion coaching and emotion dismissing. Then, models were tested for the moderating role of maternal ER in the association between adolescent and observer-reported maternal emotion socialization; separate models were tested for emotion coaching and emotion dismissing. All variables were mean-centered before running the polynomial and linear regression models (62).

Finally, supplemental analyses repeated primary analyses with the inclusion of covariates. Demographic variables were included as covariates if they were significantly associated with study variables. Puberty status was also included as a covariate. In addition to emotion regulation difficulties, parents’ and adolescents’ characteristics (e.g., internalizing symptoms and demographic variables) may also explain discrepancies in reporting parenting (22). Therefore, internalizing symptoms and demographic variables were tested to determine whether they predicted informant discrepancies and contributed to these discrepancies in the supplemental analyses.

3 Results

3.1 Descriptive data

Means and standard deviations (SDs) for emotion coaching, emotion dismissing, and mothers’ and adolescents’ ER, and demographic variables as reported by multi-informants, are presented in Table 1. Bivariate correlations between variables are presented in Figure 1 (non-significant correlations are presented in Supplementary Table S1). The distribution of adolescent-reported maternal emotion dismissing was highly skewed (skewness = 1.04), and the distributions of other variables were normal or moderately skewed (range = -0.52 to 0.63), shown in Figure 2. Mothers rated their emotion coaching skills higher than adolescents, t(69) = -3.95, p <.001; mother reported emotion dismissing was significantly lower than that reported by adolescents, t(69) = 3.40, p <.005. The current study utilized different questionnaires for mother and adolescent-reported ER. Therefore, a t-test was not conducted to measure the informant discrepancy in adolescent ER. Similarly, as observer-reported maternal emotion socialization used a different scale compared to adolescent and mother report, we did not test for informant discrepancies.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Bivariate Correlations. Non-significant coefficients are left blank in this figure. AR, adolescent-reported; MR, mother-reported; OR, observer-reported; EC, emotion coaching; ED, emotion dismissing; ER, emotion regulation; Mother. ER, self-reported maternal emotion regulation; IS, internalizing symptoms.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Distribution plots for maternal emotion socialization, adolescent emotion regulation, mother emotion regulation, and internalizing symptoms. AR, adolescent-reported; MR, mother-reported; OR, observer-reported; EC, emotion coaching; ED, emotion dismissing; ER, emotion regulation; Mother.ER, self-reported maternal emotion regulation; IS, internalizing symptoms.

3.2 Associations between maternal emotion socialization, adolescent emotion regulation, and maternal emotion regulation from multi-informants

Adolescent-reported maternal emotion socialization and adolescent ER showed a correlation with a medium effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.53, p <.05 for emotion coaching, Cohen’s d = 0.8, p <.005 for emotion dismissing; see Figure 1). This correlation was positive for emotion coaching and negative for emotion dismissing. Neither maternal- nor observer-reported maternal emotion socialization was significantly correlated with adolescent ER. Additionally, adolescent-reported internalizing symptoms showed a medium effect size correlation with adolescent-reported emotion dismissing (Cohen’s d = 0.56, p <.05) and a large effect size correlation with adolescent-reported ER (Cohen’s d = 1.09, p <.001). Mother-reported internalizing symptoms were not correlated with maternal emotion socialization but were correlated with both adolescent- and mother-reported adolescent ER, with medium to large effect sizes (Cohen’s d = 0.65, p <.05 and Cohen’s d = 0.77, p <.005).

3.3 Informant discrepancies in maternal emotion socialization as predictors of adolescent emotion regulation

The second hypothesis tested informant discrepancies in maternal emotion socialization as predictors of adolescent ER. None of the interaction terms involving adolescent report (adolescent-reported X mother-reported emotion socialization, p = .136 for emotion coaching and p = .752 for emotion dismissing; adolescent-reported X observer-reported emotion socialization, p = .377 for emotion coaching and p = .609 for emotion dismissing) reached statistical significance, indicating that the discrepancies between adolescent and other reports of emotion socialization were not associated with adolescent-reported adolescent ER. Similarly, none of the interaction terms involving mother report (mother-reported X adolescent-reported emotion socialization, p = .803 for emotion coaching and p = .584 for emotion dismissing; mother-reported X observer-reported emotion socialization, p = .379 for emotion coaching and p = .175 for emotion dismissing) reached statistical significance, indicating that the discrepancies between mother and other reports of emotion socialization were not associated with mother-reported adolescent ER. Details of results are presented in the Supplementary Material (Supplementary Tables S2–S5). Therefore, no additional tests to probe interaction patterns were conducted.

3.4 Maternal and adolescent emotion regulation and informant discrepancies

Our third hypothesis was in relation to the contribution of both adolescent and maternal difficulties in ER to the informant discrepancy for emotion socialization. Linear regression models revealed that neither adolescent nor maternal difficulties in ER significantly moderated the associations between maternal emotion socialization reported by different informants (Table 2). However, the interaction between adolescent-reported emotion coaching and maternal difficulties in ER was approaching significance in predicting mother-reported emotion coaching (β = .01, SE = .003, p = .053).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Linear regression testing the contribution of adolescent or maternal emotion regulation to informant discrepancies.

The trend finding indicates that when mothers reported greater difficulties in ER, there was a positive association between mother-reported emotion coaching and adolescent-reported emotion coaching (see Figure 3). This finding thus shows when mothers reported greater difficulties in ER, there was greater concordance between mother-reported and adolescent-reported emotion coaching.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The association between mother- and adolescent-reported emotion coaching at high and low levels of maternal difficulties in emotion regulation.

3.5 Supplementary analysis

Results of the supplementary analysis including race of the mother, family composition, family income, pubertal maturity status, and maternal internalizing symptoms as covariates are available in the Supplementary Materials. When primary analyses were rerun including these covariates, results were unchanged. The supplementary analysis examining whether adolescent and maternal internalizing symptoms, family composition, and mother’s race predicted informant discrepancies is presented in the Supplementary Materials (S6–S17). The supplementary analysis showed informant discrepancies in maternal emotion socialization were not associated with supplementary variables (adolescent and maternal internalizing symptoms, family composition, and mother’s race); and all these supplementary variables did not significantly contribute to informant discrepancies in maternal emotion socialization.

4 Discussion

In studying the relationship between maternal emotion socialization and ER in early female adolescents using multi-informant measurements (mother-reported, adolescent-reported, and observational report), there were three key findings: (1) only adolescent-reported, but neither mother-reported nor observational reports, of maternal emotion coaching and emotion dismissing were associated with adolescent ER; (2) informant discrepancy was not related to adolescent ER; and (3) maternal ER was found to moderate the association between mother- and adolescent-reported emotion coaching at trend level only. Although the results are preliminary due to the sample size, and some only reached a trend level of significance, we will discuss them in light of the novel nature of the study. However, we acknowledge that these findings should be interpreted with caution, and they require further validation.

Correlational analyses suggested that only adolescent-reported (but not mother- or observer-rated) emotion coaching and emotion dismissing were negatively and positively (respectively) related to adolescent ER; and adolescent-reported internalizing symptoms was positively related to adolescent-reported emotion dismissing. This finding partially supports the theoretical model of parental emotion socialization that supportive maternal emotion socialization helps children understand and regulate emotions, while unsupportive maternal emotion socialization is associated with children’s low emotional competence (6, 7, 63). Regarding internalizing symptoms, our findings aligns with previous meta-analysis study, which showed that parental warmth has a small association with internalizing symptoms, whereas neglectful parenting is strongly associated with high levels of internalizing symptoms (64). This suggests that supportive and unsupportive parental emotion socialization may function differently in adolescents’ development.

Different informant reports yielded different results, consistent with prior research (21). The finding of adolescent-reported emotion socialization being associated with adolescent ER is consistent with some prior research on male and female adolescents (65). Research has shown, for example, that adolescents’ perceptions of parenting behavior have stronger associations with their ER than what is reported by parents (66, 67). One study with adolescent males and females showed that mothers’ reports of adolescents’ behaviors, mental health symptoms, and family environment do not predict adolescents’ emotional functioning (68).

However, that mother- and observer-reported emotion socialization were not associated with adolescent ER is inconsistent with other studies. Previous findings, in both female and mixed-sex samples, demonstrated that parent- and observer-reported supportive maternal emotion socialization was associated with better child emotional competence (810, 19). However, previous studies included community samples, whereas participants in the current study consisted of early female adolescents with high levels of internalizing symptoms. It is possible that because at-risk female adolescents are more likely to experience intense negative emotions (69), this might influence the impact of maternal emotion socialization (6). ER is largely an internal process; parents can only infer, but not directly observe, their children’s internal emotion states and thoughts (70). Therefore, adolescent-reported ER may offer more insight into adolescent subjective emotional experience compared to others’ reports.

Previous research focused on the effect of informant discrepancies on children’s behavior and mental health, and general parenting behavior (22, 29, 30). To our knowledge, the current study is the first to empirically investigate informant discrepancies in maternal emotion socialization. Consistent with previous research (22), mothers perceived their emotion-related parenting behavior to be more positive than their adolescent daughters’ ratings, such that they reported significantly higher scores for emotion coaching and significantly lower scores for emotion dismissing. The underlying reason for the discrepancy between mothers’ and adolescents’ reports could be due to several reasons. First, there could be a social desirability bias where parents want to present themselves more positively than the reality (71). Second, the informant discrepancy could be interpreted through the maladaptive hypothesis. This hypothesis suggests that the informant discrepancy is maladaptive for adolescent development because it may reflect problems in family relationships; there may be a lack of communication between parents and adolescents, resulting in adolescents perceiving parenting behavior more negatively (72). Conversely, the informant discrepancy could be interpreted as reflecting different agendas during the transition from childhood to adolescence (73). Parents prefer to provide a nurturing environment for adolescents, while early adolescents want to gain more autonomy and independence (74). Adolescents may interpret parenting behavior as hindering their independence, leading to a discrepancy in perceived parenting behavior. Additionally, the measurement of maternal emotion socialization may explain the informant discrepancy. Items like “When I was sad, my mother helped me deal with the issue that made me sad” in the EAC questionnaire are considered emotion coaching. However, if the parent gives advice immediately before validating their child’s emotions in response, the child might perceive the advice as criticism and emotion dismissing, while the parent would perceive it as supportive parental emotion socialization (75).

However, our results did not support the hypothesis that informant discrepancies would be associated with adolescent ER. This finding may support the hypothesis of different agendas, suggesting that the discrepancy arises from normal developmental changes during adolescence and is not reflective of family dysfunction which might negatively impact the adolescent. Although the informant discrepancies in maternal emotion socialization were not associated with adolescent ER, based on the maladaptive hypothesis, they may still have implications for other aspects of adolescent functioning, which could be investigate in further research.

Although our result showed that informant discrepancies in maternal emotion socialization were not related to adolescent ER, we further probed the potential reasons underlying the reported discrepancies. Results did not support De Los Reyes et al. (32)’s notion that parents’ and adolescents’ psychopathological symptoms contribute to the informant discrepancies. In our study, neither mother nor adolescent ER was significantly related to mothers’ and adolescents’ reports of maternal emotion socialization. However, there was a trend for a moderating role of maternal difficulties in ER in the association between mother- and adolescent-reported emotion coaching, such that greater difficulties were associated with greater concordance between informant reports of emotion coaching, which is inconsistent with our hypothesis. Prior research, however, has found that higher levels of parents’ own psychopathology (specifically, depressive) symptoms are related to greater correspondence between different informant ratings of parenting behavior (31). Given that difficulties in ER are known to be related to depression (76), these findings could support the depression realism hypothesis, suggesting that individuals experiencing depression may be more accurate in perceiving or judging their own behavior compared to non-depression individuals (77). It is possible that mothers with greater difficulties in ER may be more aware of their maternal emotion socialization and have more concordance with adolescent reports. Although the moderating role of maternal ER was not statistically significant in the current study, maternal emotion regulation may potentially contribute to the informant discrepancy in maternal emotion socialization.

4.1 Limitations and future directions

While this study has several strengths (including use of both self-report and observational data), there are also limitations. First, our sample size was relatively small, so we may not have had sufficient power to detect smaller effects if they existed. Thus, our null findings should be taken with caution and replicated further. Second, our emotion discussion tasks were conducted via video conferencing and the discussion was limited to three emotions with only five minutes per emotion. Further, we did not investigate positive emotions in the discussion task. The video-conferencing environment and time limit may make mothers and adolescents’ behavior less natural and not representative of their daily interactions. This limitation might explain why observed measurements of maternal emotion socialization were not correlated with adolescent ER. Third, we focused on emotion coaching and emotion dismissing broadly, possibly overlooking the nuanced and complex nature of maternal emotion socialization related to specific emotions. Emotion coaching and dismissing also do not represent the entire range of parental emotion socialization, which also includes parents’ beliefs of emotions and parents’ expression of emotion (6). Fourth, our study did not test the interaction hypothesis raised by Gottman et al. (7) that emotion coaching’s main function is to inhibit the negative effect of emotion dismissing. Whether emotion coaching has a moderating role in the association between emotion dismissing and emotional outcomes in adolescents with internalizing symptoms should be examined in the future to test the inhibiting effect of emotion coaching.

Fifth, gender of both parents and their children may influence the effect of parental emotion socialization on adolescent emotional competence (6). Therefore, our findings may not be generalized to fathers and male adolescents. Additionally, cultural factors have been found to influence parental emotion socialization and the majority of participants in the current study were Caucasian and had a high socioeconomic status (78). Thus, our results may not reflect patterns across all families. Furthermore, our sample did not include young people using psychotropic medications, which might limit the generalizability of findings to clinical samples. It will be important to expand this research to samples with different demographic features to gain a more comprehensive understanding of the effect of parental emotion socialization on the development of ER in early adolescents. Sixth, the current study investigated a limited number of factors that may influence informant discrepancies. Discrepant reports have challenged researchers and clinicians in drawing research conclusions and assessing adolescent psychopathology for treatment (20). The role of informant discrepancies in adolescent behavioral reports and their associated features is critical for a comprehensive understanding of adolescent psychopathology. This knowledge could lead to a more accurate measurement of family interaction. Therefore, further studies on the factors associated with informant discrepancy in maternal emotion socialization are needed to provide a deeper understanding of the reasons for the different perceptions of maternal parenting behavior between adolescents and their mothers.

4.2 Conclusion and implications

In the present study, results suggest that adolescent-reported maternal emotion socialization is associated with adolescent self-reported ER in female adolescents with elevated internalizing symptoms, and there was a trend suggesting that mothers’ ER might influence their reports of emotion coaching. Although this latter result was not statistically significant, it suggests that when clinicians or researchers measure the outcome of parenting interventions targeting adolescents, it is important to use a multi-informant approach and consider factors that may affect reporting. Further, our findings also suggest that adolescent-reported parenting might be more relevant to adolescents’ ER. It is important to consider how adolescents perceive maternal emotion socialization rather than other’s perspectives. However, further research on the extent of the effect of maternal emotion socialization on ER in adolescents with elevated internalizing symptoms is warranted.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because data cannot be made publicly available in order to protect participant privacy as per the approved ethics requirement. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to c3doaXR0bGVAdW5pbWVsYi5lZHUuYXU=.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Royal Children’s Hospital Melbourne. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participant and legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

JZ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. EP: Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. SL: Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing. CK: Writing – review & editing. SH: Writing – review & editing. SW: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by a grant from the National Health and Medical Research Council (1163499). SL was supported by a Melbourne Research Scholarship from the University of Melbourne.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge Ankita Sen, Olivia Durbin, Kate Bray, Luzy Zhang, Liz Zeng, Daniela Lin, Tanisha Suradkar, Daniel Liontos, Michelle Green, Ella Batty, Michaela Smith, Callista Shue, Christine Leonards, Aravindh Anura for their assistance with study recruitment, interventions, and data collection.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1497007/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol. (1998) 2 (3):271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Thompson RA. Emotional regulation and emotional development. Educ Psychol Rev. (1991) 3:269–307. doi: 10.1007/bf01319934

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Chronis-Tuscano A, Bui HNT, Lorenzo NE. Transdiagnostic implications of parental socialization of child and adolescent emotional development: commentary and future directions. Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol. (2022) 50:269–82. doi: 10.1007/s10802-021-00872-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hofmann SG. Interpersonal emotion regulation model of mood and anxiety disorders. Cogn Ther Res. (2014) 38:483–92. doi: 10.1007/s10608-014-9620-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Morris AS, Criss MM, Silk JS, Houltberg BJ. The impact of parenting on emotion regulation during childhood and adolescence. Child Dev Perspect. (2017) 11:233–8. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12238

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Eisenberg N, Cumberland A, Spinrad TL. Parental socialization of emotion. psychol Inq. (1998) 9:241–73. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Gottman JM, Katz LF, Hooven C. Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. J Family Psychol. (1996) 10:243–68. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.10.3.243

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Denham SA, MitchellCopeland J, Strandberg K, Auerbach S, Blair K. Parental contributions to preschoolers’ emotional competence: Direct and indirect effects. Motivation Emotion. (1997) 21:65–86. doi: 10.1023/A:1024426431247

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Fabes RA, Leonard SA, Kupanoff K, Martin CL. Parental coping with children’s negative emotions: relations with children’s emotional and social responding. Child Dev. (2001) 72:907–20. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00323

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Cole PM, Dennis TA, Smith-Simon KE, Cohen LH. Preschoolers’ Emotion regulation strategy understanding: relations with emotion socialization and child self-regulation. Soc Dev. (2009) 18:324–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00503.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Morelen D, Shaffer A, Suveg C. Maternal emotion regulation: Links to emotion parenting and child emotion regulation. J Family Issues. (2016) 37:1891–916. doi: 10.1177/0192513X14546720

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Shewark EA, Blandon AY. Mothers’ and fathers’ Emotion socialization and children’s emotion regulation: A within-family model. Soc Dev. (2015) 24:266–84. doi: 10.1111/sode.12095

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Steinberg L. A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Dev Rev. (2008) 28:78–106. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. De Goede IHA, Branje SJT, Meeus WHJ. Developmental changes in adolescents’ Perceptions of relationships with their parents. J Youth Adolescence. (2009) 38:75–88. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9286-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Rapee RM, Oar EL, Johnco CJ, Forbes MK, Fardouly J, Magson NR, et al. Adolescent development and risk for the onset of social-emotional disorders: A review and conceptual model. Behav Res Ther. (2019) 123:103501. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2019.103501

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Van Lissa CJ, Keizer R, Van Lier PAC, Meeus WHJ, Branje S. The role of fathers’ versus mothers’ parenting in emotion-regulation development from mid-late adolescence: Disentangling between-family differences from within-family effects. Dev Psychol. (2019) 55:377–89. doi: 10.1037/dev0000612

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Zeman J, Cassano M, Perry-Parrish C, Stegall S. Emotion regulation in children and adolescents. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2006) 27:155–68. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200604000-00014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Berona J, Sroka AW, Gelardi KL, Guyer AE, Hipwell AE, Keenan K. Maternal socialization of emotion and the development of emotion regulation in early adolescent girls. Emotion. (2023) 23:872–8. doi: 10.1037/emo0001110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Cui LX, Criss MM, Ratliff E, Wu ZZ, Houltberg BJ, Silk JS, et al. Longitudinal links between maternal and peer emotion socialization and adolescent girls’ Socioemotional adjustment. Dev Psychol. (2020) 56:595–607. doi: 10.1037/dev0000861

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. De Los Reyes A. Strategic objectives for improving understanding of informant discrepancies in developmental psychopathology research. Dev Psychopathol. (2013) 25:669–82. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000096

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Korelitz KE, Garber J. Congruence of parents’ and children’s perceptions of parenting: A meta-analysis. J Youth Adolescence. (2016) 45:1973–95. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0524-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hou Y, Benner AD, Kim SY, Chen ST, Spitz S, Shi Y, et al. Discordance in parents’ and adolescents’ Reports of parenting: A meta-analysis and qualitative review. Am Psychol. (2020) 75:329–48. doi: 10.1037/amp0000463

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Zahidi R, Rogers J, Guastaferro W, Whitaker D. Relationship between self- report and observed parenting among parents in treatment versus not in treatment populations. J Georgia Public Health Assoc. (2019) 7 (2):112–120. doi: 10.20429/jgpha.2019.070217

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Byrd AL, Vine V, Frigoletto OA, Vanwoerden S, Stepp SD. A multi-method investigation of parental responses to youth emotion: prospective effects on emotion dysregulation and reactive aggression in daily life. Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol. (2022) 50:117–31. doi: 10.1007/s10802-020-00754-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Gardner F. Methodological issues in the direct observation of parent-child interaction: do observational findings reflect the natural behavior of participants? Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. (2000) 3:185–98. doi: 10.1023/a:1009503409699

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ginsburg GS, Grover RL, Cord JJ, Ialongo N. Observational measures of parenting in anxious and nonanxious mothers: does type of task matter? J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2006) 35:323–8. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_16

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Gonzales NA, Cauce AM, Mason CA. Interobserver agreement in the assessment of parental behavior and parent-adolescent conflict: African American mothers, daughters, and independent observers. Child Dev. (1996) 67:1483–98. doi: 10.2307/1131713

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Sessa FM, Avenevoli S, Steinberg L, Morris AS. Correspondence among informants on parenting: Preschool children, mothers, and observers. J Family Psychol. (2001) 15:53–68. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.15.1.53

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Goodman KL, De Los Reyes A, Bradshaw CP. Understanding and using informants’ Reporting discrepancies of youth victimization: A conceptual model and recommendations for research. Clin Child Family Psychol Rev. (2010) 13:366–83. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0076-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Laird RD, De Los Reyes A. Testing informant discrepancies as predictors of early adolescent psychopathology: why difference scores cannot tell you what you want to know and how polynomial regression may. J Abnormal Child Psychol. (2013) 41:1–14. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9659-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Parent J, Forehand R, Dunbar JP, Watson KH, Reising MM, Seehuus M, et al. Parent and adolescent reports of parenting when a parent has a history of depression: associations with observations of parenting. J Abnormal Child Psychol. (2014) 42:173–83. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9777-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. De Los Reyes A, Goodman KL, Kliewer W, Reid-Quinones K. Whose depression relates to discrepancies? Testing relations between informant characteristics and informant discrepancies from both informants’ perspectives. psychol Assess. (2008) 20:139–49. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.20.2.139

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Aldao A, Gee DG, De Los Reyes A, Seager I. Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic factor in the development of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology: Current and future directions. Dev Psychopathol. (2016) 28:927–46. doi: 10.1017/S0954579416000638

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Blechert J, Sheppes G, Di Tella C, Williams H, Gross JJ. See what you think: reappraisal modulates behavioral and neural responses to social stimuli. Psychol Sci. (2012) 23:346–53. doi: 10.1177/0956797612438559

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Eisenberg N. Findings, issues, and new directions for research on emotion socialization. Dev Psychol. (2020) 56:664–70. doi: 10.1037/dev0000906

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Vinnakota A, Kaur R. A study of depression, externalizing, and internalizing behaviors among adolescents living in institutional homes. Int J Appl Basic Med Res. (2018) 8:89–95. doi: 10.4103/ijabmr.IJABMR_333_17

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kasturi S, Oguoma VM, Grant JB, Niyonsenga T, Mohanty I. Prevalence rates of depression and anxiety among young rural and urban Australians: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2023) 20 (1):800. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20010800

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. ABS. Household income and wealth. Australian bureau of statistics (2019). Available online at: https://www.abs.gov.au/statistics/economy/finance/household-income-and-wealth-Australia/2019-20 (Accessed October 1, 2024).

Google Scholar

39. Chorpita BF, Yim L, Moffitt C, Umemoto LA, Francis SE. Assessment of symptoms of DSM-IV anxiety and depression in children: a revised child anxiety and depression scale. Behav Res Ther. (2000) 38:835–55. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00130-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Magai C. Emotions as a child. Brooklyn: Long Island University (1996).

Google Scholar

41. Miller-Slough RL, Dunsmore JC. Emotion socialization by parents and friends: Links with adolescent emotional adjustment. J Appl Dev Psychol. (2020) 71:101197. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2020.101197

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Hastings PD, De I. Parasympathetic regulation and parental socialization of emotion: Biopsychosocial processes of adjustment in preschoolers. Soc Dev. (2008) 17:211–38. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00422.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. O’Neal CR, Magai C. Do parents respond in different ways when children feel different emotions? The emotional context of parenting. Dev Psychopathol. (2005) 17:467–87. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050224

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Costa Martins E, Ferreira-Santos F, Meira L. New version of the emotion socialization scale with the positive emotion of overjoy: initial validation evidence with Portuguese adolescents. Psicol Reflex Crit. (2018) 31:9. doi: 10.1186/s41155-018-0090-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Fivush R, Brotman MA, Buckner JP, Goodman SH. Gender differences in parent-child emotion narratives. Sex Roles. (2000) 42:233–53. doi: 10.1023/A:1007091207068

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Suveg C, Sood E, Barmish A, Tiwari S, Hudson JL, Kendall PC. I’d rather not talk about it”: emotion parenting in families of children with an anxiety disorder. J Family Psychol. (2008) 22:875–84. doi: 10.1037/a0012861

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Baker JK, Fenning RM, Crnic KA. Emotion socialization by mothers and fathers: coherence among behaviors and associations with parent attitudes and children’s social competence. Soc Dev. (2011) 20:412–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00585.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Havighurst SS, Harley AE, Kehoe C. Tuning in to teens: emotionally intelligent parenting program manual. 2nd Edition. Melbourne, Australia: University of Melbourne. (2019).

Google Scholar

49. Richmond S, Schwartz O, Johnson KA, Seal ML, Bray K, Deane C, et al. Exploratory factor analysis of observational parent-child interaction data. Assessment. (2020) 27:1758–76. doi: 10.1177/1073191118796557

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Penza-Clyve S, Zeman J. Initial validation of the emotion expression scale for children (EESC). J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2002) 31:540–7. doi: 10.1207/153744202320802205

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Caiado B, Canavarro MC, Moreira H. The bifactor structure of the emotion expression scale for children in a sample of school-aged portuguese children. Assessment. (2023) 30:1065–79. doi: 10.1177/10731911221082038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Bunford N, Dawson AE, Evans SW, Ray AR, Langberg JM, Owens JS, et al. The difficulties in emotion regulation scale-parent report: A psychometric investigation examining adolescents with and without ADHD. Assessment. (2020) 27:921–40. doi: 10.1177/1073191118792307

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (2004) 26:41–54. doi: 10.1023/B:Joba.0000007455.08539.94

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Osman A, Wong JL, Bagge CL, Freedenthal S, Gutierrez PM, Lozano G. The Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21 (DASS-21): further examination of dimensions, scale reliability, and correlates. J Clin Psychol. (2012) 68:1322–38. doi: 10.1002/jclp.21908

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Dorn LD, Susman EJ, Nottelmann ED, Inoff-Germain G, Chrousos GP. Perceptions of puberty: Adolescent, parent, and health care personnel. Dev Psychol. (1990) 26:322–9. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.26.2.322

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Marshall WA, Tanner JM. Variations in the pattern of pubertal changes in boys. Arch Dis Child. (1970) 45:13–23. doi: 10.1136/adc.45.239.13

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Ellis R, Fernandes A, Simmons JG, Mundy L, Patton G, Allen NB, et al. Relationships between adrenarcheal hormones, hippocampal volumes and depressive symptoms in children. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2019) 104:55–63. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.02.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. team R. RStudio: integrated development environment for R. Boston, MA: Posit Software (2023). P. RStudio.

Google Scholar

59. van Buuren S, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. mice: multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J Stat Software. (2011) 45:67. doi: 10.18637/jss.v045.i03

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Edwards JR. The study of congruence in organizational-behavior research - critique and a proposed alternative (Vol 58, pg 51). Organizational Behav Hum Decision Processes. (1994) 58:323–3. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1994.1029

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. De Los Reyes A, Kazdin AE. Informant discrepancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: a critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for further study. Psychol Bull. (2005) 131:483–509. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.483

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Aiken LS, West SG. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications, Inc (1991).

Google Scholar

63. Morris AS, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Myers SS, Robinson LR. The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc Dev. (2007) 16:361–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Pinquart M. Associations of parenting dimensions and styles with internalizing symptoms in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Marriage Family Rev. (2016) 53:613–40. doi: 10.1080/01494929.2016.1247761

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Yeo GH, Cheah CSL, Sim TN. A tale of two countries: Singaporean and Chinese parents’ emotion socialisation during childhood and the relation to adolescents’ emotion regulation. Int J Psychol. (2020) 55:163–72. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12576

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Ackard DM, Neumark-Sztainer D, Story M, Perry C. Parent-child connectedness and behavioral and emotional health among adolescents. Am J Prev Med. (2006) 30:59–66. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2005.09.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Cheung CS, Pomerantz EM, Wang MF, Qu Y. Controlling and autonomy-supportive parenting in the United States and China: beyond children’s reports. Child Dev. (2016) 87:1992–2007. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12567

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Cavendish W, Montague M, Enders C, Dietz S. Mothers’ and adolescents’ Perceptions of family environment and adolescent social-emotional functioning. J Child Family Stud. (2014) 23:52–66. doi: 10.1007/s10826-012-9685-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Angold A, Costello EJ, Worthman CM. Puberty and depression: the roles of age, pubertal status and pubertal timing. Psychol Med. (1998) 28:51–61. doi: 10.1017/s003329179700593x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Hourigan SE, Goodman KL, Southam-Gerow MA. Discrepancies in parents’ and children’s reports of child emotion regulation. J Exp Child Psychol. (2011) 110:198–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.03.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Bornstein MH, Putnick DL, Lansford JE, Pastorelli C, Skinner AT, Sorbring E, et al. Mother and father socially desirable responding in nine countries: Two kinds of agreement and relations to parenting self-reports. Int J Psychol. (2015) 50:174–85. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12084

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. De Los Reyes A, Ohannessian CM. Introduction to the special issue: discrepancies in adolescent-parent perceptions of the family and adolescent adjustment. J Youth Adolescence. (2016) 45:1957–72. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0533-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Welsh DP, Galliher RV, Powers SI. Divergent realities and perceived inequalities: Adolescents’, mothers’, and observers’ perceptions of family interactions and adolescent psychological functioning. J Adolesc Res. (1998) 13:377–402. doi: 10.1177/0743554898134002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Hazen E, Schlozman S, Beresin E. Adolescent psychological development: A review. Pediatr Rev. (2008) 29:161–8. doi: 10.1542/pir.29-5-161

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Kehoe CE, Havighurst SS, Harley AE. Tuning in to Teens: Investigating moderators of program effects and mechanisms of change of an emotion focused group parenting program. Dev Psychol. (2020) 56:623–37. doi: 10.1037/dev0000875

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Joormann J, Gotlib IH. Emotion regulation in depression: relation to cognitive inhibition. Cognit Emot. (2010) 24:281–98. doi: 10.1080/02699930903407948

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Alloy LB, Abramson LY. Depressive realism: Four theoretical perspectives. In: Cognitive processes in depression. New York, NY: The Guilford Press (1988). p. 223–65.

Google Scholar

78. Friedlmeier W, Corapci F, Cole PM. Emotion socialization in cross-cultural perspective. Soc Pers Psychol Compass. (2011) 5:410–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00362.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: maternal emotion socialization, emotion regulation, informant discrepancy, multi-measurement, early adolescence

Citation: Zhao J, Pozzi E, Lin SC, Kehoe CE, Havighurst SS and Whittle S (2025) Association between maternal emotion socialization and emotion regulation in early adolescents with elevated internalizing symptoms: insights from multi-informant discrepancies. Front. Psychiatry 16:1497007. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1497007

Received: 16 September 2024; Accepted: 03 February 2025;
Published: 05 March 2025.

Edited by:

Eduardo Fernández-Jiménez, Hospital Infantil La Paz, Spain

Reviewed by:

Stella Tsotsi, Oslo Metropolitan University, Norway
Jacob Holzman, University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus, United States

Copyright © 2025 Zhao, Pozzi, Lin, Kehoe, Havighurst and Whittle. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sarah Whittle, c3doaXR0bGVAdW5pbWVsYi5lZHUuYXU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset

94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more