Relationships between screen time and childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a Mendelian randomization study
- 1Department of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Kodaira, Japan
- 2Research Center for Child Mental Development, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Hamamatsu, Japan
- 3United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University, University of Fukui, Suita, Japan
A Commentary on
Relationships between screen time and childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a Mendelian randomization study
By Meng Z, Ao B, Wang W, Niu T, Chen Y, Ma X and Huang Y (2024) Front. Psychiatry 15:1441191. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1441191
1 Introduction
Screen time has been increasingly linked to several developmental issues in children, including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The rise of digital technology has significantly increased children’s exposure to screens, raising concerns about potential long-term neurodevelopmental effects. A recent study by Meng et al. aimed to investigate the causal relationship between screen time and ADHD using Mendelian randomization (MR), a powerful technique that attempts to infer causality using genetic data (1). However, before accepting their conclusions, it is essential to summaries the currently available evidence on this topic and compare it with the assumptions, methodology and generalizability of the results described in the study by Meng et al.
2 Screen time and ADHD
2.1 Observational studies
The early studies showed that youth with ADHD engaged in more screen time than youth without ADHD (2, 3). In adolescents with ADHD aged 13-17 years, Becker et al. reported that nighttime media use contributed to sleep problems and comorbid internalizing symptoms (4).
In 2016, Montagni et al. reported that there was a dose-dependent associations between screen time and self-perceived levels of attention problems and hyperactivity in French university students (5). Thoma et al. then extended the target to a community sample and reported that media time, sleep deviation and circadian rhythm were correlated with ADHD symptomatology (6). The Canadian Healthy Infant Longitudinal Development (CHILD) study reported that increased screen time in preschool was associated with worse inattention problems in the general population (7). However, none of them concluded that there was a causal link between screen time and the diagnosis of ADHD.
In order to further investigate the causal relationship between screen time and ADHD, longitudinal studies have been carried out. For example. Levelink et al. examined longitudinal associations between recreational screen time in early childhood (e.g. at ages 2, 4, and 6) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) at ages 8 to 10. They found that screen time in early childhood was not associated with ADHD (8). Furthermore, using a total of 2511 children from the Brazilian High-Risk Cohort for Psychiatry Disorders, Bado et al. found that higher screen use in children and adolescents may be a consequence of psychopathology rather than a cause. However, Soares et al. reported that ADHD symptoms at age 22 were positively associated with total screen time at ages 11, 15 and 18 (9). The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study concluded that it could not establish causality, and that increased screen time was unlikely to be directly harmful to 9- and 10-year-old children (10).
Taken together, a substantial body of evidence suggests an association between screen time and ADHD symptoms, however, no cross-sectional or longitudinal study has conclusively established causality.
2.2 Genetic studies
In 2022, we showed that the Polygenic Risk Score for ADHD (ADHD-PRS) was associated with increased screen time in children (11). Our study used a long-term observational cohort of 1258 children to monitor screen time at multiple points (from 18 to 40 months), and children’s screen time can be categorized into four distinct trajectories: low, medium, high, and increasing. The association between ADHD-PRS and increasing screen time was significant, suggesting that genetic risk for ADHD predisposes children to engage in more screen-based activities over time. Subsequently, Zhang also used ADHD-PRS in the data from the ABCD study and found that ADHD-PRS was associated with longer screen time (12). They also demonstrated that the association between screen time and attention problems was largely explained by genetic confounders such as ADHD-PRS. Furthermore, more recently, Yang et al. reported an association between longer screen time and reduced fractional anisotropy (FA) values in several white matter tracts in over 11,000 children aged 9-11 years, but they also showed that reduced FA mediated the association between ADHD-PRS and screen time (13). These findings suggest a unidirectional relationship between screen time and ADHD. Specifically, ADHD appears to predispose children to longer screen time rather than the reverse, since it is unlikely that long screen time affect ADHD-PRS which is calculated utilizing the individual genetic variations.
MR is a robust technique for inferring causality from summary GWAS data and, to our knowledge, the study by Meng et al. is the first MR study to examine screen time and ADHD. They did not find a causal relationship between ADHD and screen time, but between screen time and ADHD (1).
3 Discussion
What factors contributed to these inconsistent conclusions? One possible reason is the summary data used in the MR analysis. Meng et al. used GWAS summary data on screen time from the adult population. In fact, there are some literature showing that screen time in childhood is not consistently associated with screen time in adulthood, which is influenced by various factors such as lifestyle or partners’ attitudes towards media (14). Second, although Meng et al. used sensitivity analyses to check for pleiotropy which justifies an assumption to conduct MR analysis, the small number of instrumental variables (SNPs) used in some categories (e.g., only one SNP for mobile phone usage) weakens the strength of their conclusions. Therefore, readers should be aware that this study may not have used a sample size with adequate statistical power.
In conclusion, while the study by Meng et al. provides intriguing insights into the genetic underpinnings of screen time and ADHD, its findings should be interpreted with caution since the limitations of statistical power of Mendelian randomization and the reliance on adult screen time data may have weakened the strength of their conclusions. To date, three independent PRS studies have suggested that possible causal effect of ADHD on screen time, but these findings should also be replicated. Future studies using GWAS summary data on screen time in children may provide clearer conclusion on this topic.
Author contributions
NT: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. KT: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
Generative AI statement
The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Meng Z, Ao B, Wang W, Niu T, Chen Y, Ma X, et al. Relationships between screen time and childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a Mendelian randomization study. Front Psychiatry. (2024) 15:1441191. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1441191
2. Gorely T, Marshall SJ, Biddle SJ. Couch kids: correlates of television viewing among youth. Int J Behav Med. (2004) 11:152–63. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm1103_4
3. Gordon-Larsen P, McMurray RG, Popkin BM. Adolescent physical activity and inactivity vary by ethnicity: The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. J Pediatr. (1999) 135:301–6. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(99)70124-1
4. Becker SP, Lienesch JA. Nighttime media use in adolescents with ADHD: links to sleep problems and internalizing symptoms. Sleep Med. (2018) 51:171–8. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2018.06.021
5. Montagni I, Guichard E, Kurth T. Association of screen time with self-perceived attention problems and hyperactivity levels in French students: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open. (2016) 6:e009089. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009089
6. Thoma VK, Schulz-Zhecheva Y, Oser C, Fleischhaker C, Biscaldi M, Klein C, et al. Sleep quality, and ADHD symptoms in a community sample and a sample of ADHD patients aged 8 to 18 years. J Atten Disord. (2020) 24:576–89. doi: 10.1177/1087054718802014
7. Tamana SK, Ezeugwu V, Chikuma J, Lefebvre DL, Azad MB, Moraes TJ, et al. Screen-time is associated with inattention problems in preschoolers: Results from the CHILD birth cohort study. PloS One. (2019) 14:e0213995. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213995
8. Levelink B, van der Vlegel M, Mommers M, Gubbels J, Dompeling E, Feron FJM, et al. The longitudinal relationship between screen time, sleep and a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in childhood. J Atten Disord. (2021) 25:2003–13. doi: 10.1177/1087054720953897
9. Soares PSM, de Oliveira PD, Wehrmeister FC, Menezes AMB, Gonçalves H. Is screen time throughout adolescence related to ADHD? Findings from 1993 Pelotas (Brazil) birth cohort study. J Atten Disord. (2022) 26:331–9. doi: 10.1177/1087054721997555
10. Paulich KN, Ross JM, Lessem JM, Hewitt JK. Screen time and early adolescent mental health, academic, and social outcomes in 9- and 10- year old children: Utilizing the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development ℠ (ABCD) Study. PloS One. (2021) 16:e0256591. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256591
11. Takahashi N, Tsuchiya KJ, Okumura A, Harada T, Iwabuchi T, Rahman MS, et al. The association between screen time and genetic risks for neurodevelopmental disorders in children. Psychiatry Res. (2023) 327:115395. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2023.115395
12. Zhang Y, Choi KW, Delaney SW, Ge T, Pingault JB, Tiemeier H. Shared genetic risk in the association of screen time with psychiatric problems in children. JAMA Netw Open. (2023) 6:e2341502. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2023.41502
13. Yang A, Rolls ET, Dong G, Du J, Li Y, Feng J, et al. Longer screen time utilization is associated with the polygenic risk for Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with mediation by brain white matter microstructure. EBioMedicine. (2022) 80:104039. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2022.104039
Keywords: ADHD, screen time, polygenic risk score, Mendelian randomization, causality
Citation: Takahashi N and Tsuchiya KJ (2024) Commentary: Relationships between screen time and childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a Mendelian randomization study. Front. Psychiatry 15:1512149. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1512149
Received: 16 October 2024; Accepted: 25 November 2024;
Published: 24 December 2024.
Edited by:
Lambert Zixin Li, Stanford University, United StatesReviewed by:
Hanyang Shen, Stanford University, United StatesPeilin Yang, Barcelona Graduate School of Economics, Spain
Copyright © 2024 Takahashi and Tsuchiya. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Nagahide Takahashi, bmFnYWhpZGUudGFrYWhhc2hpQG5jbnAuZ28uanA=