- 1Department of Teaching and Research on Party Building, Guizhou Institute of Public Administration, Guiyang, China
- 2Research center of Humanities and Medicine, Zunyi Medical University, Zunyi, China
- 3Department of Education, Guizhou Provincial Government, Guiyang, China
Conduct disorder refers to a persistent and repetitive pattern of problematic behavior, and is usually accompanied by deficits in cognitive processing of emotional stimuli. Behavioral and physiological correlates of these deficits have been the subject of sufficiently many investigations. Meanwhile, several theories concerning conduct disorder have been developed. However, the validity of these theories to explain the emotional processing deficits among conduct-disordered individuals has not been tested. Here we summarized four relevant theories, including “social-cognitive theory and social-information-processing theory”, “violence inhibition model”, “optimal stimulation/arousal theory and stimulation/sensation seeking theory”, and “fearlessness theory”, and discussed their validity in predicting the emotional processing deficits among conduct disordered individuals. Future directions on the theories and evidence were proposed.
1 Introduction
1.1 Conduct disorder
Conduct disorder (CD) is defined as a persistent and repetitive pattern of problematic behavior that violates others’ rights or that violates age-appropriate social norms or rules, and is characterized by a pattern of severe antisocial and aggressive behavior (1). CD usually manifests in childhood or adolescence, and is usually associated with delinquent or criminal behaviors (2–4).
It is worth noting that there are two types of CD, the child/early-onset CD (CO-CD) and the adolescent-onset CD (AO-CD), and the mechanisms between them may be different. CO-CD is thought to arise from neurodevelopmental difficulties which play a causal role in the aetiology of their antisocial behavior. CO-CD usually shows verbal intelligence quotient (IQ) deficits. In contrast, AO-CD is thought to arise from the social imitation of deviant peers. Previous studies demonstrated that the gray matter volume (GMV) of the right temporal lobe was reduced among CO-CDs (5), while the cortical thickness in the paralimbic system was found attenuated among adolescents with AO-CD (6). Another study showed that the GMVs of the bilateral amygdala in both CO-CD and AO-CD were reduced while the GMV of the right insular was only reduced in the AO-CD group (7).
2 Emotional processing among CD individuals
It is important to study the mechanism behind CD, as a thorough understanding of these processes can contribute to the development of more effective treatments and interventions. This, in turn, can greatly reduce antisocial behaviors for individuals with CD. Furthermore, a deeper understanding of these mechanisms can also aid in the early detection and prevention of CD, thereby reducing the overall societal impact and burden associated with CD. Emotional processing has been considered a core function in social functioning. The deficits in emotional cognition may lead to antisocial behavior and thus be the underlying mechanism of CD.
2.1 Deficits in emotional recognition
Emotional recognition is shown impaired among individuals with CD (8). For all basic emotions of happiness, sadness, anger, fear, disgust, and surprise, and for both static and dynamic expressions, children and adolescents with CD were found impaired in emotional recognition (9–11). However, different studies may draw different conclusions on the deficits in specific emotions. For example, some researchers found that individuals with CD showed impairments in the recognition of negative emotions (12), such as anger, disgust, fear, and sadness, but not for happiness and surprise (13). Other researchers found that adolescents with CD had deficits in recognizing anger, surprise, fear, happiness, and sadness but not disgust (14). Among the emotions, the recognition of sadness and fear may be most usually and consistently found impaired among CDs. For example, the recognition of sad and fearful faces was found impaired among children with psychopathic tendencies involved in affective-interpersonal disturbance and impulsive behavior/conduct problems (15). Another study showed that conduct problems were inversely correlated with the recognition of sad and fearful expressions but not happy, angry, disgusted, or surprised expressions among children aged 11-14 years (16). However, with a classification task, it was shown that children with CD tended to incorrectly identify happiness and fear as angry (17), and adolescents with CD were more likely to classify neutral faces as fearful (11). The different findings among studies may result from the different tasks that were adopted. Some researchers adopted the Emotion Hexagon task to assess facial emotion recognition in which participants were required to view six morphed facial expressions continua (9, 14, 16, 18, 19). Some researchers adopted an alternative-forced-choice task in which participants were asked to discriminate facial emotions (10, 11, 13, 20). Some researchers adopted the Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy to assess the recognition of sadness, happiness, anger, and fear (15). Further studies are required to elucidate the difference between these tasks.
The type of CD is also an influential factor that largely affects the deficits of emotional processing among CD. As we described above, CO-CD arises from a neurodevelopmental deviation, while AO-CD is developed by social imitation from deviant peers. Therefore, individuals with CO-CD may show more severe deficits in emotional recognition. Research revealed that male adolescents with CO-CD showed impairments in recognizing angry, disgusted, and happy facial expressions, whereas those with AO-CD only showed deficits in recognizing fear (21). Besides the developing type, another way to differentiate the type of CD is based on the trait of callous-unemotional (CU). It was proposed that the impairment in emotion recognition among CD was related to CU traits (9). Research revealed that CD with high CU showed deficits in recognizing sad faces (18, 22), while CD with low CU showed deficits in recognizing fearful faces (23). Consistently, evidence illustrated that higher levels of CU traits were associated with better performance in recognizing fearful expressions (10, 22, 23). Neuroimaging evidence indicated that, compared with typically developing controls and CDs with low CU, CDs with high CU showed hyperactivity within the dorsolateral, dorsomedial, and ventromedial prefrontal regions during emotional recognition of angry and fearful faces (24). However, there was also evidence showing little influence of the CU trait on the recognition of emotions (25).
2.2 The neural substrates of emotional processing deficits
Cappadocia et al. (26) summarized that individuals with CD exhibit abnormal structures and functions, including reduced P300 amplitude, deactivation in the anterior cingulate cortex, attenuated activation in the left amygdala in response to negative stimuli, reduced volume of the right temporal lobe, lowered serotonin levels, and attenuated autonomic nervous system reactivity. Regarding research concerning facial emotions, CD individuals showed reduced activities in brain regions related to antisocial behavior and amygdala for angry faces when they passively viewed angry, sad, and neutral faces (27). Another study also found reduced activation in the amygdala in response to angry and sad (vs. neutral) faces among CD (28). Consistent with the behavioral evidence, the neural activity deficits of emotional processing are also modulated by the type of CD. First, the CO-CD and AO-CD subgroups showed different neural activities. It was found that, compared with controls and AO-CD, the CO-CD group exhibited reduced activation in the amygdala for sad (vs. neutral) faces (27). Second, CU may also modulate the neural activities for emotional processing. It was found that CD with high CU and low CU showed different response patterns in the amygdala for various emotional faces (29, 30). For example, amygdala responses to fear or distress stimuli decreased among CD with high CU, while they increased among CD with low CU (31). For emotional faces that were presented subliminally, CD with low CU also exhibited greater responses in the right amygdala to fearful faces than CD with high CU (30). Contrary to the reduced activities in the amygdala, CD with high CU showed hyperactivity in the dorsolateral, dorsomedial, and ventromedial prefrontal regions and increased activation in the posterior cingulate and precuneal cortices, compared with CD with low CU, when viewing angry and fearful faces (24). Taken together, these results demonstrated impaired functioning of the amygdala to negative stimuli among individuals with CD, especially those with CO-CD and high CU traits.
In summary, understanding the emotional deficits in individuals with CD holds significant implications. First, it may help develop targeted interventions to improve emotional processing in these individuals. By addressing these deficits early, it may reduce the risk of further antisocial behaviors and promote more adaptive social functioning. Furthermore, it provides insights into educational strategies that could help in the identification and support of high-risk youth, thereby preventing the emergence of more severe behavioral problems. By enhancing the emotional awareness of educators and guardians, these strategies can in turn lead to more effective interventions and support systems.
3 Theories concerning the emotional processing deficits
Numerous theories have been proposed to explain behaviors characterized by antisocial tendencies, including aggression. From a biological standpoint, there are the prefrontal cortex dysfunction hypothesis (32) and the optimal arousal theory (33, 34). When considering social factors, there are theories such as the general strain theory (35) and social learning theory (36). Personal factors encompass a range of theories, including cognition-related theories, such as the social information processing theory (37), social cognitive theory (38), and violence inhibition model (39, 40); affect-related theories, like the emotion dysregulation theory (41) and integrated emotion systems model (42); and personality-related theories, including the fearlessness theory (43), stimulation-seeking theory (44, 45) and others. Additionally, there are comprehensive theoretical frameworks, such as the general aggression model (46, 47) and the biopsychosocial model (48).
However, as we discussed above, emotional processing deficits may be a core factor contributing to the antisocial behaviors in individuals with CD and acquired attention from more and more researchers. Among the aforementioned theories, the social cognitive/social information processing theory, the violence inhibitory model, the optimal stimulation/arousal theory, the stimulation/sensation seeking theory, and the fearlessness theory are particularly well-suited for elucidating the underlying mechanisms. Therefore, we aim to discuss the connection of these four theories with the emotional processing of CD individuals in detail.
3.1 Social-cognitive theory and social-information-processing theory
The social-cognitive theory primarily includes two basic hypotheses: belief and belief-driven social information processing (38). Belief refers to an idea or proposition that is unrelated to actual objective truth but is accepted for being considered to be true (49). Everyone has developed interconnected belief networks, and this highly related cluster of beliefs is often called schema (50, 51). Schema is not developed based on truth and objectivity but is a subjective cognitive structure that is greatly influenced by one’s past knowledge and experience. For example, self-schema is cognitive generalizations about the self that are derived from early experience. Information in a schema can be used to perceive, describe, interpret, and predict complex society. The schemata about oneself and society will, to a large extent, automatically guide a person to understand the behavior of himself/herself and others (51). Schneider (52) proposed that social information processing is fundamentally top-down or schema-driven. To improve the processing efficiency of social information, people generally do not have sufficient time and resources to understand or interpret their own and others’ behaviors in a logical, thoughtful, and careful way. Instead, they rely on their schemata to automatically and unconsciously understand the ambiguous social environment.
According to the social-information-processing theory (37), an individual’s behavioral response to social stimuli could be viewed as the result of a series of steps for social information processing. The first step is encoding related aspects of social stimuli from sensory input, for example, selective attention to social cues (e.g., facial expressions) and the storage of cue information in short-term memory. The second step is mental representation, in which individuals keep a subjective interpretation of a stimulus, rather than the stimulus itself, in their memory. The third step is response accessing which could be one or more behavioral and emotional responses triggered by mental representation. For example, if one interprets the other’s angry expression as hostility or a threat to himself, he/she may respond in a hostile, aggressive way. However, not all accessing responses will be executed, and thus the fourth step is response evaluation and selection, which reflects the decision-making process. Reactions can be evaluated in terms of moral acceptability and expected outcomes. The final step is the enactment, which translates the selected response into actual action. This theory primarily focuses on the mental process which leads to maladapted behavioral responses to social stimuli (37). Sestir and Bartholow (53) proposed that the way humans perceive, process, store, and retrieve information shapes their aggression. In a word, social information processing links schemata to behaviors, while schemata link early experiences to future social information processing in certain situations. One’s adverse experience shaped the maladaptive schemata which in turn may have an adverse impact on his/her social information processing, and then this maladaptive processing may lead to problematic behaviors. A previous study revealed that the negative social cognitive bias played a mediating role in the relationship between exposure to community violence and aggression, which supported this theory (54).
In conclusion, according to the social-cognitive theory and the social-information-processing theory, the schema is the knowledge structure shaped by one’s unique experience and can automatically guide his/her social information processing which involves encoding, mental representation, response accessing, response selection, and enactment (37, 38, 55, 56). Previous studies revealed that aggressive or violent individuals were more likely to hold or endorse aggression-related schema (57–59), and that these individuals usually showed a pattern of biased processing, for example, the negative social-cognitive bias (54), the attention bias towards hostile social cues (60–62), selective recall for hostile social cues (63) and the hostile attribution bias (63, 64). The social-cognitive theory and the social-information-processing theory can be extended to describe and interpret CD. The logic is that, if a specific information-processing action is associated with a deviant behavior (e.g., assault or aggression), a general processing pattern (e.g., attentional bias to hostile cues) will be relevant to a general behavioral pattern (e.g., CD) (37). Because of exposure to childhood abuse or maltreatment (65–69) and exposure to aggressive/deviant models or peers (70–72), individuals with CD develop a maladapted or distorted schema, which leads them to preferentially encode hostile or threatening cues, to mentally interpret or represent social cues as threats, to easily access aggressive responses and finally to engage in aggressive behavior.
The two theories explain how maladaptive cognitive processing such as emotional processing impact behavioral outcomes. CD individuals exhibit difficulties in recognizing and interpreting emotional expressions, which can lead to misinterpretations of others’ intentions and subsequent aggressive reactions. These deficits in emotional recognition are believed to be a significant contributor to the development and maintenance of CD symptoms. However, more caution is needed when using social information processing theory to explain emotional processing deficits in CD individuals. Although social information processing deficits like hostile attribution bias are often thought to play a significant role in fostering antisocial and aggressive behaviors, it remains somewhat unclear how broader impairments in the processing of emotional faces might contribute to such problematic behaviors. For example, a recent study has indicated that male adolescent delinquents with CD exhibit deficits in attentional orientation to hostile (angry) and threatening (fearful) faces, rather than sad and happy faces (73), which seems to be inconsistent with the social cognitive theory and social information process theory. To better understand how deficits in emotional processing contribute to problematic behaviors, a negative interpretation bias toward facial emotions could be crucial. Individuals with CD were more likely to identify happiness and fear as angry (17), and to classify neutral faces as fearful (11). Some studies have revealed that emotional recognition training can significantly improve negative interpretation of emotional faces (e.g., recognize angry faces as happy ones), which in turn reduces hostility, state anger, aggression, and behavioral problems (74–76). Additionally, the impact of impairment in emotional processing on behaviors among CD individuals might be modulated by some personal traits such as CU traits (9, 10, 18, 22, 23). It is possible that although the cues of fear or sadness expressed by a victim might stop the misbehavior of individuals without CU traits, the same cues might has no effect on individuals with CU traits. Therefore, understanding the nuances of these emotional processing deficits is crucial for developing effective interventions to reduce antisocial and aggressive behaviors.
3.2 Violence inhibition model
The violence inhibition model comprises at least two stages, namely emotional perception (including empathy) and movement cessation, and typically the organism learns to regulate aggression by perceiving others’ distress (39, 40). When the distress during non-verbal communication (e.g., sad expression) is activated, humans have a cognitive mechanism that can initiate their withdrawal responses. This is a schema that can make individuals tend to withdraw from aggression and can mediate the relationship between the distressed cues and aggression. A previous study revealed that distressed cues (e.g., a sad face) can indeed terminate children’s aggression (77). According to this model, it can be assumed that individuals with CD have defects in emotional processing for distressing stimuli.
At the first stage of this model (emotional perception), the distressing stimuli lose their value because individuals with CD have difficulty in normally encoding affective stimuli. Evidence showed that adolescents with CD usually exhibited lower empathy and corresponding abnormal neural activities in emotion-related areas such as the amygdala and insula, indicating difficulty in emotionally arousing among them (8, 78–80). Direct measurements of the emotional responses and physical responses to affective stimuli also revealed the defect. For example, boys with CD reported lower levels of emotional responses to unpleasant stimuli (81), and showed reduced electrodermal responses to the distress cues (e.g. a crying face) compared with threatening cues (e.g. an angry face) (82). Consistent with these findings, it was shown that boys with conduct problems needed more stages before they successfully recognized sad expressions, and that they were more likely to recognize the fearful expression as another emotion, which indicated encoding defects for distressing emotions (83).
At the second stage (action stop), the model proposes that individuals with CD have defects in executive function related to violence inhibition (e.g. error monitoring and response inhibition). Previous studies revealed that CD was correlated with lower executive cognitive function capacity in female adolescents (84), as well as deficits in response inhibition (85), and individuals with CD had deficits in inhibitory control (13, 86). Evidence showed that the cingulate, insular, and prefrontal cortices were associated with and modulate aggression (87). Within this framework, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), as the rostral component of the fronto-parietal executive function network, plays a crucial role in regulating impulsivity and inhibiting aggression. Previous studies demonstrated that CD symptoms were associated with a reduced structure in the prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex (88). Furthermore, individuals with CD demonstrate significant differences in effective connectivity within the inhibition control network when compared to healthy controls (89). Taken together, the violence inhibition model predicts that both deficits in emotional perception and impulsive inhibition result in conduct problems.
Similarly, the I3 model also emphasizes that the three distinct processes of instigation, which encompass immediate environmental stimuli (e.g., provocation), along with impellance and inhibition, encompassing situational or dispositional qualities, interact and further produce aggression (90, 91). According to this theory, aggressive behavior is not simply a response to provocation (instigation) but is also influenced by the individual’s ability to inhibit such impulses (inhibition). By integrating this perspective, the violence inhibition model can be refined to account for the dynamic nature of aggressive responses.
In summary, the violence inhibition model divides the influence of emotional processing on behavioral outcomes into two distinct and separate stages, which provides a clear explanation of how emotional cognitive processing plays a role in behavioral outcomes. By breaking down the process into these two stages, the model offers a nuanced understanding of the relationship between emotional cognition and the subsequent behavioral responses that are shaped by it. However, the model has limitations when explaining the deficits in emotional processing, because the impairments may be confined specifically to emotional cognition in response to distressing stimuli, rather than being a general issue across all emotional processing domains.
3.3 Optimal stimulation/arousal theory and stimulation/sensation seeking theory
Quay (92) proposed that both the prolonged absence of stimulation and the prolonged presentation of persistent monotonic stimulation are unpleasant and can trigger greater motivation. According to the optimal stimulation/arousal theory, everyone has an optimal arousal level of stimulation, and a large deviation from this optimal level will lead to an aversive state, and then make the individual exhibit compensatory behaviors to recover the optimal arousal level by enhancing or reducing current arousal level (33, 34). Thus, sensory overload generally leads to physical or mental withdrawal and limits sensory input, while sensory deprivation generally leads to increased physical or mental activity.
Regarding individuals with CD, they are usually under-aroused, and thus exhibit the state of stimulation/sensation seeking. Previous studies revealed that children with CD showed reduced baseline or resting heart rates, skin conductance, and electrodermal activity, as well as lower task electrodermal activity (26, 93). Besides, studies also showed that male adolescents with aggressive CD showed attenuated respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), fewer skin conductance responses, and a longer pre-ejection period (PEP) that was an index of sympathetic (beta-adrenergic) activity at baseline (94), and that female adolescents with CD had lower morning plasma cortisol levels (95). Moreover, children with CD showed lower autonomic nervous system (ANS) and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis activity (93, 95–97). These physiological indicators were not only reduced among CD individuals, but also associated with CD symptoms. For example, cortisol stress reactivity and cortisol recovery could negatively predict aggression (98), and declined cortisol stress reactivity could predict CD symptoms (99). Taken together, it suggests that individuals with CD are under-aroused. However, it should also be noted that some evidence may not support this proposition (100).
According to the optimal stimulation/arousal theory and the stimulation/sensation seeking theory, it can be assumed that individuals with CD have a low level of physiological arousal less than the optimal arousal level, so they are more likely to exhibit hyperactivity, such as social rules violation or illegal behaviors, to increase their physiological arousal levels and make up for the aversive state (33, 34, 45, 92, 101). Individuals with CD have greater needs for external/behavioral sensory-seeking than internal/mental sensory-seeking, and they may be raised in environments that are difficult to meet their sensory needs in a socially acceptable way, which forces them to meet their needs in a way that violates social or legal norms. Some researchers found that higher heart rate reactivity was associated with conduct problems, for example, higher heart rate levels in response to frustration and provocation (97, 102). Other evidence showed that heart rates were correlated with aggression and rule-breaking, and sensation-seeking played a mediating role in the relationship between heart rates and rule-breaking in male adolescents (103, 104). These results support that antisocial and aggressive behaviors are stimulating for some individuals and are a way to satisfy their stimulation-seeking, implying that violence and aggression are byproducts of stimulation-seeking behaviors.
In the frame of the optimal stimulation/arousal theory and the stimulation/sensation seeking theory, individuals with CD are hard to be aroused and stimulated by emotional stimuli. A lot of studies demonstrated that when viewing emotional stimuli, individuals with CD reported attenuated skin conductance responses (81), reduced temperament skin conductance magnitudes (105), lower startle-elicited blinks (18, 106–108), and lower affect-related activities in the amygdala/hippocampal formation, parahippocampal gyrus, ventral striatum, and anterior and posterior cingulate gyri (109).
In contrast to the violent inhibition model, both the optimal stimulation/arousal theory and the stimulation/sensation-seeking theory indicate that impaired emotional cognition might not be confined solely to the affective valence. It is possible that emotional processing might be affected by the intensity of stimulation, implying that the level of arousal could play a significant role in how emotions are processed. Therefore, while these two theories provide some insights into the nature of emotional processing deficits, they offer limited explanations for emotional processing deficits among individuals with CD. This limitation arises because these theories do not adequately address why individuals with CD process different types of emotions in varying ways.
3.4 Fearlessness theory
According to the fearlessness theory, individuals need fear, to a certain extent, to inhibit antisocial and violent behaviors, and the lack of fear for social punishment in childhood may result in poor fear conditioning and inadequate development of conscience (43). Fearlessness in individuals with CD is found to be associated with lower sensitivity to punishment cues (110). As the sensitivity to punishment reflects the behavioral inhibition in the context of aversive outcomes (111), fearless individuals are insensitive to the negative outcomes (e.g. punishment) caused by their aggressive behaviors, which may lead to difficulty in learning to inhibit antisocial and aggressive behaviors. A recent study revealed that youths with CD showed deficits in learning to stimuli evoking punishments (9). Electrophysiological evidence suggested that male adolescents with CD exhibited decreased responses in a gambling task to the loss which implied punishment (112). Therefore, fearlessness may play a crucial role in conduct problems. Previous studies demonstrated that fearless boys were at a high risk of CD (113), and children with increasing conduct problems were characterized by increases in their levels of fearlessness (114). Longitudinal studies revealed that the level of fearlessness for fearful voices at age 2 could not only predict the initial level of conduct problems at age 2 but also predict the persistence of conduct problems between 2 and 8 years of age (115). Another study showed that the fearless temperament at age 2 could even predict conduct problems at age 13 (116).
Moreover, the fearlessness theory also suggests that a low level of arousal under moderate stress may reflect a low level of fear (43, 117), and thus could also indicate weak emotional processing among CD individuals, especially for anger and fear. For example, boys with CD showed reduced corrugator muscle activity, indicating a deficit in perceiving anger when exposed to dynamic angry expressions (118). In addition, they also showed decreased heart rate in response to fear and sadness, which was correlated with CD symptoms (119). Another study revealed that boys with conduct problems were more likely to identify fearful faces as another expression even when the fearful expressions were at full intensity (83). These results indicate that individuals with CD have problems processing threatening stimuli, which may reduce the arousal of fear induced by these stimuli. However, there are few direct examinations of the relationship between fearlessness and cognitive processing of emotional stimuli. One study revealed that fearlessness in individuals with CD was associated with lower sensitivity to aversive stimuli (punishment cues) (110). Future studies are required to further investigate this relationship.
The fearlessness theory posits that individuals with CD exhibit reduced fear responses to aversive stimuli, which may contribute to their increased likelihood of engaging in antisocial behaviors. However, although this theory provides a compelling explanation for certain behaviors in specific contexts, it does not fully account for the complexity of emotional processing deficits observed in these individuals. Similar to the violence inhibition model, the fearlessness theory encounters certain limitations when it comes to comprehensively explaining the emotional processing deficits in these individuals. The impairments might be uniquely restricted to emotional cognition in response to threatening or aversive stimuli. This suggests that although fearlessness may play a role, it is not the only factor influencing the emotional recognition impairments observed in CD. Therefore, although fearlessness may be a significant factor, it is essential to consider a broader range of influences to gain a comprehensive understanding of the emotional processing deficits in CD.
4 Prospects
Individuals with CD show significant deficits in processing emotional information. We summarized several theoretical perspectives which could predict and explain the emotional processing deficits, including “social-cognitive theory and social-information-processing theory”, “violence inhibition model”, “optimal stimulation/arousal theory and stimulation/sensation seeking theory”, and “fearlessness theory”. Each theory focuses on a distinct facet that sheds light on why conduct disorder (CD) and aggressive behaviors emerge and develop. From these perspectives, emotional processing is also influenced and impaired among CD individuals. Although these theories may successfully predict the deficits in emotional cognition observed in individuals with CD, the underlying logic and specific predictions can vary considerably from one theory to another. For example, according to the optimal stimulation/arousal theory and stimulation/sensation seeking theory, it is hypothesized that the impaired emotional cognition might not depend on the affective valence, but is a more generalized impairment. In contrast, the fearlessness theory posits that individuals with CD may exhibit specific deficits in cognitive processing related to negative emotions, particularly those that are threatening. Regarding the violence inhibition model, it suggests that the impairments may be restricted to emotional cognition for distressing stimuli, implying a more selective deficit. At last, the social-cognitive theory and the social-information-processing theory predict that individuals with CD will show cognitive biases toward schema-consistent emotion stimuli (e.g. angry expression for aggressive schema). For example, they might exhibit a trend to interpret ambiguous expressions as angry, which aligns with their aggressive schema. This indicates that their emotional processing is biased towards recognizing and responding to emotional cues that reinforce their pre-existing aggressive frameworks. In summary, while each theory offers valuable insights into the emotional processing deficits associated with CD and aggression, they differ in their explanations and predictions. These differences highlight the complexity of emotional cognition in CD and underscore the need for a multifaceted approach when studying and addressing these deficits. Further evidence is required to reconcile different theories and to develop a more unified theory of CD.
Additionally, the type of CD is a crucial factor that modulates the emotional processing among CD, as we discussed above. Further theories may pay more attention to explaining emotional recognition deficits among subtypes of CD. For example, evidence showed that CO-CD was impaired in recognition of angry, disgusted, and happy facial expressions whereas AO-CD was impaired in recognition of fear (21). Neuroimaging evidence suggested that CO-CD exhibited hypoactivity in the amygdala for sad faces, compared with AO-CD (27). The second way to subdivide CD is based on the CU trait. Some researchers found that CD with low CU showed deficits in recognizing fearful faces, while those with high CU showed deficits in recognizing sad faces (18, 22, 23). CU traits can also modulate the neural activity of emotion processing. Evidence showed that, activities in the amygdala for fear or distress stimuli decreased in CD with high CU, while they increased in CD with low CU (29–31). Another study revealed hyperactivity in prefrontal, posterior cingulate, and precuneal cortices among CD with high CU, compared with CD with low CU (24). As different types of CD may lead to different deficits in emotional processing, it would be a good test of validity for different theories. Nevertheless, more evidence should be collected to elucidate the effect of the type of CD on emotional processing more clearly.
Besides, more kinds of emotional cognition should be examined. Most studies till now concern the encoding processes of static emotional stimuli. First, other cognitive processes should also be examined. One important process might be emotional attention. It was revealed that adolescents with CD showed attentional avoidance and difficulty in attentional disengagement from facial expressions including angry, fearful, and happy faces (120). Another study found that adolescents with CD fixated less on the eyes of fearful and sad expressions (10). Other processes, such as memory and decision-making, require further investigations. Second, the deficits in dynamic emotional stimuli should be examined. It was found that, when viewing sad film clips, children with CD showed a larger increase in RSA than controls (121). However, when viewing an escalating conflict, male adolescents with aggressive CD showed less RSA reactivity than controls (94). There was also a study revealing that impaired emotional recognition in CD existed for both static and dynamic expressions (10). Taken together, a more general examination should be conducted on the emotional processing among CD to provide more evidence for the theories.
Finally, as the present review mainly focuses on emotional deficits and related theories among individuals with CD, other theories were not included. However, they should not be ignored or underestimated. For example, as impulsiveness is a typical characteristic of CD, the prefrontal dysfunction theory is critical in explaining the impulsiveness of CD. According to the prefrontal dysfunction theory, structural and functional deficits in the prefrontal lobe are associated with antisocial and aggressive behavior, because prefrontal dysfunction results in a lack of inhibitory control over antisocial and violent behaviors (32, 122). Interestingly, stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal lobe by tDCS (transcranial direct current stimulation) enhanced the activation in the prefrontal cortex and reduced aggressive intent (123). Therefore, prefrontal dysfunction theory is a successful theory concerning impulsiveness and aggression among CD. Further reviews may focus on other theories when discussing other deficits related to CD.
Author contributions
XL: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. HK: Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. TB: Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. ZP: Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4 ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2000).
2. Bowen KL, Morgan JE, Moore SC, van Goozen SH. Young offenders' Emotion recognition dysfunction across emotion intensities: explaining variation using psychopathic traits, conduct disorder and offense severity. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (2014) 36:60–73. doi: 10.1007/s10862-013-9368-z
3. Pardini DA, Fite PJ. Symptoms of conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and callous-unemotional traits as unique predictors of psychosocial maladjustment in boys: advancing an evidence base for DSM-V. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2010) 49:1134–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.07.010
4. Rosenblatt JA, Rosenblatt A, Biggs EE. Criminal behavior and emotional disorder: Comparing youth served by the mental health and juvenile justice systems. J Behav Health Serv Res. (2000) 27:227–37. doi: 10.1007/BF02287315
5. Kruesi MJP, Casanova MF, Mannheim G, Johnson-Bilder A. Reduced temporal lobe volume in early onset conduct disorder. Psychiatry research: Neuroimaging. (2004) 132:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2004.07.002
6. Jiang Y, Guo X, Zhang J, Gao J, Wang X, Situ W, et al. Abnormalities of cortical structures in adolescent-onset conduct disorder. Psychol Med. (2015) 45:3467–79. doi: 10.1017/s0033291715001361
7. Fairchild G, Passamonti L, Hurford G, Hagan CC, von dem Hagen EA, van Goozen SH, et al. Brain structure abnormalities in early-onset and adolescent-onset conduct disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2011) 168:624–33. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10081184
8. Cohen D, Strayer J. Empathy in conduct-disordered and comparison youth. Dev Psychol. (1996) 32:988–98. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.32.6.988
9. Kohls G, Baumann S, Gundlach M, Scharke W, Bernhard A, Martinelli A, et al. Investigating sex differences in emotion recognition, learning, and regulation among youths with conduct disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2020) 59:263–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.04.003
10. Martin-Key NA, Graf EW, Adams WJ, Fairchild G. Facial emotion recognition and eye movement behaviour in conduct disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2018) 59:247–57. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12795
11. Short RML, Sonuga-Barke EJS, Adams WJ, Fairchild G. Does comorbid anxiety counteract emotion recognition deficits in conduct disorder? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2016) 57:917–26. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12544
12. Bons D, van den Broek E, Scheepers F, Herpers P, Rommelse N, Buitelaaar JK. Motor, emotional, and cognitive empathy in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder and conduct disorder. J Abnormal Child Psychol. (2013) 41:425–43. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9689-5
13. Kleine Deters R, Naaijen J, Rosa M, Aggensteiner PM, Banaschewski T, Saam MC, et al. Executive functioning and emotion recognition in youth with oppositional defiant disorder and/or conduct disorder. World J Biol Psychiatry. (2020) 21:539–51. doi: 10.1080/15622975.2020.1747114
14. Sully K, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Fairchild G. The familial basis of facial emotion recognition deficits in adolescents with conduct disorder and their unaffected relatives. Psychol Med. (2015) 45:1965–75. doi: 10.1017/s0033291714003080
15. Stevens D, Charman T, Blair RJR. Recognition of emotion in facial expressions and vocal tones in children with psychopathic tendencies. J Genet Psychol. (2001) 162:201–11. doi: 10.1080/00221320109597961
16. Blair RJR, Coles M. Expression recognition and behavioural problems in early adolescence. Cogn Dev. (2000) 15:421–34. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00039-9
17. Cadesky EB, Mota VL, Schachar RJ. Beyond words: How do problem children with ADHD and/or conduct problems process nonverbal information about affect? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2000) 39:1160–7. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200009000-00016
18. Fairchild G, Stobbe Y, van Goozen SH, Calder AJ, Goodyer IM. Facial expression recognition, fear conditioning, and startle modulation in female subjects with conduct disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 68:272–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.019
19. Pajer K, Leininger L, Gardner W. Recognition of facial affect in girls with conduct disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2010) 175:244–51. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.06.003
20. Aspan N, Vida P, Gadoros J, Halasz J. Conduct symptoms and emotion recognition in adolescent boys with externalization problems. ScientificWorldJournal. (2013) 2013:826108. doi: 10.1155/2013/826108
21. Fairchild G, Van Goozen SHM, Calder AJ, Stollery SJ, Goodyer IM. Deficits in facial expression recognition in male adolescents with early-onset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2009) 50:627–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02020.x
22. Schwenck C, Gensthaler A, Romanos M, Freitag CM, Schneider W, Taurines R. Emotion recognition in girls with conduct problems. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2014) 23:13–22. doi: 10.1007/s00787-013-0416-8
23. Woodworth M, Waschbusch D. Emotional processing in children with conduct problems and callous/unemotional traits. Child: Care Health Dev. (2008) 34:234–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2007.00792.x
24. Aghajani M, Klapwijk ET, Andershed H, Fanti KA, van der Wee NJA, Vermeiren RRJM, et al. Neural processing of socioemotional content in conduct-disordered juvenile offenders with limited prosocial emotions. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2021) 105:110045. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2020.110045
25. Schwenck C, Mergenthaler J, Keller K, Zech J, Salehi S, Taurines R, et al. Empathy in children with autism and conduct disorder: group-specific profiles and developmental aspects. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2012) 53:651–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02499.x
26. Cappadocia MC, Desrocher M, Pepler D, Schroeder JH. Contextualizing the neurobiology of conduct disorder in an emotion dysregulation framework. Clin Psychol Rev. (2009) 29:506–18. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.001
27. Passamonti L, Fairchild G, Goodyer IM, Hurford G, Hagan CC, Rowe JB, et al. Neural abnormalities in early-onset and adolescence-onset conduct disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2010) 67:729–38. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.75
28. Ewbank MP, Passamonti L, Hagan CC, Goodyer IM, Calder AJ, Fairchild G. Psychopathic traits influence amygdala-anterior cingulate cortex connectivity during facial emotion processing. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci. (2018) 13:525–34. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy019
29. Jones AP, Laurens KR, Herba CM, Barker GJ, Viding E. Amygdala hypoactivity to fearful faces in boys with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. Am J Psychiatry. (2009) 166:95–102. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07071050
30. Viding E, Sebastian CL, Dadds MR, Lockwood PL, Cecil CA, De Brito SA, et al. Amygdala response to preattentive masked fear in children with conduct problems: the role of callous-unemotional traits. Am J Psychiatry. (2012) 169:1109–16. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12020191
31. Thornton LC, Penner EA, Nolan ZT, Adalio CJ, Sinclair S, Meffert H, et al. The processing of animacy information is disrupted as a function of callous-unemotional traits in youth with disruptive behavior disorders. NeuroImage Clin. (2017) 16:498–506. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.08.024
32. Raine A. Annotation: The role of prefrontal deficits, low autonomic arousal, and early health factors in the development of antisocial and aggressive behavior in children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2002) 43:417–34. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00034
33. Zentall SS. Optimal stimulation as theoretical basis of hyperactivity. Am J Orthopsychiatry. (1975) 45:549–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1975.tb01185.x
34. Zentall SS, Zentall TR. Optimal stimulation: A model of disordered activity and performance in normal and deviant children. psychol Bull. (1983) 94:446–71. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.94.3.446
35. Agnew R. Foundation for a general strain theory of crime and delinquency. Criminology. (1992) 30:47–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.1992.tb01093.x
36. Bandura A. Social learning theory of aggression. J Communication. (1978) 28:12–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1978.tb01621.x
37. Dodge KA. Social-cognitive mechanisms in the development of conduct disorder and depression. Annu Rev Psychology. (1993) 44:559–84. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.003015
38. Gannon TA. Social cognition in violent and sexual offending: an overview. Psychology Crime Law. (2009) 15:97–118. doi: 10.1080/10683160802190822
39. Blair RJR. A cognitive developmental approach to morality: Investigating the psychopath. Cognition. (1995) 57:1–29. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00676-P
40. Blair RJR. Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders, and psychopathy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2001) 71:727–31. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.71.6.727
41. Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol. (1998) 2:271–99. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
42. Blair RJR. Applying a cognitive neuroscience perspective to the disorder of psychopathy. Dev Psychopathol. (2005) 17:865–91. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050418
43. Raine A. The psychopathology of crime: Criminal behavior as a clinical disorder. San Diego, CA: Academic Press (1993). doi: 10.1016/B978-0-08-057148-5.50005-8
44. Mawson AR. Stimulation-induced behavioral inhibition: A new model for understanding physical violence. Integr Physiol Behav Sci. (1999) 34:177–97. doi: 10.1007/BF02688683
46. Anderson C, Carnagey N. Violent evil and the general aggression model. In: Miller A, editor. The Social Psychology ofGood and Evil. New York: Guilford Publications (2004). p. 168–92.
47. Anderson CA, Bushman BJ. Human Aggression. Annu Rev Psychol. (2002) 53:27–51. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231
48. Dodge KA, Pettit GS. A biopsychosocial model of the development of chronic conduct problems in adolescence. Dev Psychol. (2003) 39:349–71. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.39.2.349
49. Nisbett RE, Ross L. Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment. Hoboken, NJ: Prentice-Hall (1980). doi: 10.2307/2184495
50. Bless H, Fiedler K, Strack F. Social cognition: How individuals construct social reality. London: Psychology Press (2004).
52. Schneider DJ. Social cognition. Annu Rev Psychol. (1991) 42:527–61. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.42.020191.002523
53. Sestir MA, Bartholow BD. Theoretical explanations of aggression and violence. In: Aggressive offenders' cognition: Theory, research and practice. Chichester, MA: John Wiley & Sons Ltd (2007). p. 157–78. doi: 10.1002/9780470746295.ch8
54. Bradshaw CP, Rodgers CRR, Ghandour LA, Garbarino J. Social–cognitive mediators of the association between community violence exposure and aggressive behavior. School Psychol Q. (2009) 24:199–210. doi: 10.1037/a0017362
55. Crick NR, Dodge KA. A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment. psychol Bull. (1994) 115:74–101. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74
56. Dodge KA. A social information processing model of social competence in children. In: Perlmutter M, editor. Cognitive Perspectives on Children's Social and Behavioral Development. New York: Psychology Press (2020). p. 85–134. doi: 10.4324/9781315802343-7
57. Archer J, Haigh AM. Beliefs about aggression among male and female prisoners. Aggressive Behav. (1997) 23:405–15. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1997)23:6<405::AID-AB1>3.0.CO;2-F
58. Archer J, Haigh AM. Do beliefs about aggressive feelings and actions predict reported levels of aggression? Br J Soc Psychol. (1997) 36:83–105. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1997.tb01120.x
59. Polaschek DLL, Collie RM, Walkey FH. Criminal attitudes to violence: Development and preliminary validation of a scale for male prisoners. Aggressive Behav. (2004) 30:484–503. doi: 10.1002/ab.20081
60. Cima MJ, Vancleef LMG, Lobbestael J, Meesters CMG, Korebrits AM. Don't you dare look at me, or else: Negative and aggressive interpretation bias, callous unemotional traits and type of aggression. J Child Adolesc Behav. (2014) 2:1–9. doi: 10.4172/2375-4494.1000128
61. Gouze KR. Attention and social problem solving as correlates of aggression in preschool males. J Abnormal Child Psychol. (1987) 15:181–97. doi: 10.1007/BF00916348
62. Smith P, Waterman M. Processing bias for sexual material: the emotional stroop and sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: J Res Treat. (2004) 16:163–71. doi: 10.1177/107906320401600206
63. Dodge KA, Frame CL. Social cognitive biases and deficits in aggressive boys. Child Dev. (1982) 53:620–35. doi: 10.2307/1129373
64. Dodge KA, Newman JP. Biased decision-making processes in aggressive boys. J Abnormal Psychol. (1981) 90:375–9. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.90.4.375
65. Afifi TO, McMillan KA, Asmundson GJ, Pietrzak RH, Sareen J. An examination of the relation between conduct disorder, childhood and adulthood traumatic events, and posttraumatic stress disorder in a nationally representative sample. J Psychiatr Res. (2011) 45:1564–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.08.005
66. Dargis M, Newman J, Koenigs M. Clarifying the link between childhood abuse history and psychopathic traits in adult criminal offenders. Personality Disord: Theory Res Treatment. (2016) 7(3):221–8. doi: 10.1037/per0000147
67. Dodge KA, Feldman E. Issues in social cognition and sociometric status. In: Peer rejection in childhood. New York: Cambridge University Press (1990). p. 119–55.
68. Maniglio R. Significance, nature, and direction of the association between child sexual abuse and conduct disorder: A systematic review. Trauma Violence Abuse. (2015) 16:241–57. doi: 10.1177/1524838014526068
69. Young SE, Smolen A, Hewitt JK, Haberstick BC, Stallings MC, Corley RP, et al. Interaction between MAO-A genotype and maltreatment in the risk for conduct disorder: failure to confirm in adolescent patients. Am J Psychiatry. (2006) 163:1019–25. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.6.1019
70. Bandura A. Social learning analysis of aggression. In Ribes-Inesta E., Bandura A. (Eds.), Analysis of delinquency and aggression. (1973) Oxford, England: Lawrence Erlbaum..
71. Kendler KS, Jacobson K, Myers JM, Eaves LJ. A genetically informative developmental study of the relationship between conduct disorder and peer deviance in males. Psychol Med. (2008) 38:1001–11. doi: 10.1017/s0033291707001821
72. Miller GE, Prinz RJ. Enhancement of social learning family interventions for childhood conduct disorder. Psychol Bull. (1990) 108:291–307. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.108.2.291
73. Kou H, Luo W, Li X, Yang Y, Xiong M, Shao B, et al. Cognitive deficits for facial emotions among male adolescent delinquents with conduct disorder. Front Psychiatry. (2022) 13:937754. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.937754
74. Li F, Li X, Kou H. Emotional recognition training enhances attention to emotional stimuli among male juvenile delinquents. Psychol Res Behav Manage. (2023) 16:575–86. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S403512
75. Penton-Voak IS, Thomas J, Gage SH, McMurran M, McDonald S, Munafò MR. Increasing recognition of happiness in ambiguous facial expressions reduces anger and aggressive behavior. psychol Sci. (2013) 24:688–97. doi: 10.1177/0956797612459657
76. Wells AE, Hunnikin LM, Ash DP, van Goozen SHM. Improving emotion recognition is associated with subsequent mental health and well-being in children with severe behavioural problems. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2021) 30:1769–77. doi: 10.1007/s00787-020-01652-y
77. Camras LA. Facial expressions used by children in a conflict situation. Child Dev. (1977) 48:1431–5. doi: 10.2307/1128503
78. Lockwood PL, Sebastian CL, McCrory EJ, Hyde ZH, Gu X, De Brito SA, et al. Association of callous traits with reduced neural response to others' pain in children with conduct problems. Curr Biol. (2013) 23:901–5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.018
79. Schepman K, Taylor E, Collishaw S, Fombonne E. Face emotion processing in depressed children and adolescents with and without comorbid conduct disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2012) 40:583–93. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9587-2
80. Sebastian CL, McCrory EJ, Cecil CA, Lockwood PL, De Brito SA, Fontaine NM, et al. Neural responses to affective and cognitive theory of mind in children with conduct problems and varying levels of callous-unemotional traits. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2012) 69:814–22. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2070
81. Herpertz SC, Mueller B, Qunaibi M, Lichterfeld C, Konrad K, Herpertz-Dahlmann B. Response to emotional stimuli in boys with conduct disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2005) 162:1100–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1100
82. Blair RJR. Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with psychopathic tendencies. Pers Individ Dif. (1999) 27:135–45. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00231-1
83. Blair RJR, Colledge E, Murray L, Mitchell DG. A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2001) 29:491–8. doi: 10.1023/a:1012225108281
84. Giancola PR. Neuropsychological functioning and antisocial behavior–Implications for etiology and prevention. In: The science, treatment, and prevention of antisocial behaviors: Application to the criminal justice system. Kingston, NJ: Civic Research Institute (2000). p. 11–11-11-16.
85. Castellanos-Ryan N, Rubia K, Conrod PJ. Response inhibition and reward response bias mediate the predictive relationships between impulsivity and sensation seeking and common and unique variance in conduct disorder and substance misuse. Alcoholism: Clin Exp Res. (2011) 35:140–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01331.x
86. Bonham MD, Shanley DC, Waters AM, Elvin OM. Inhibitory control deficits in children with oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder compared to attention deficit/Hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol. (2021) 49:39–62. doi: 10.1007/s10802-020-00713-9
87. Potegal M, Nordman JC. Non-angry aggressive arousal and angriffsberietschaft: A narrative review of the phenomenology and physiology of proactive/offensive aggression motivation and escalation in people and other animals. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2023) 147:105110. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105110
88. Bayard F, Nymberg Thunell C, Abé C, Almeida R, Banaschewski T, Barker G, et al. Distinct brain structure and behavior related to ADHD and conduct disorder traits. Mol Psychiatry. (2020) 25:3020–33. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0202-6
89. Zhang J, Li B, Gao J, Shi H, Wang X, Jiang Y, et al. Impaired frontal-basal ganglia connectivity in male adolescents with conduct disorder. PloS One. (2015) 10:e0145011. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145011
90. Finkel EJ. Chapter One - The I3 Model: Metatheory, Theory, and Evidence. In: Olson JM, Zanna MP, editors. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 49 . San Diego, CA: Academic Press (2014). p. 1–104. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-800052-6.00001-9
91. Finkel EJ, Hall AN. The I3 Model: a metatheoretical framework for understanding aggression. Curr Opin Psychol. (2018) 19:125–30. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.013
92. Quay HC. Psychopathic personality as pathological stimulation-seeking. Am J Psychiatry. (1965) 122:180–3. doi: 10.1176/ajp.122.2.180
93. van Goozen SHM, Matthys W, Cohen-Kettenis PT, Buitelaar JK, van Engeland H. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and autonomic nervous system activity in disruptive children and matched controls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2000) 39:1438–45. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200011000-00019
94. Beauchaine TP, Katkin ES, Strassberg Z, Snarr J. Disinhibitory psychopathology in male adolescents: discriminating conduct disorder from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder through concurrent assessment of multiple autonomic states. J Abnorm Psychol. (2001) 110:610–24. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.110.4.610
95. Pajer K, Gardner W, Rubin RT, Perel J, Neal S. Decreased cortisol levels in adolescent girls with conduct disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2001) 58:297–302. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.3.297
96. Shoal GD, Giancola PR, Kirillova GP. Salivary cortisol, personality, and aggressive behavior in adolescent boys: a 5-year longitudinal study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2003) 42:1101–7. doi: 10.1097/01.Chi.0000070246.24125.6d
97. van Goozen SHM, Matthys W, Cohen-Kettenis PT, Gispen-de Wied C, Wiegant VM, Engeland HV. Salivary cortisol and cardiovascular activity during stress in oppositional-defiant disorder boys and normal controls. Biol Psychiatry. (1998) 43:531–9. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(97)00253-9
98. Schoorl J, van Rijn S, de Wied M, van Goozen SHM, Swaab H. Neurobiological stress responses predict aggression in boys with oppositional defiant disorder/conduct disorder: a 1-year follow-up intervention study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2017) 26:805–13. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-0950-x
99. Schoorl J, van Rijn S, de Wied M, van Goozen SHM, Swaab H. The role of anxiety in cortisol stress response and cortisol recovery in boys with oppositional defiant disorder/conduct disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2016) 73:217–23. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.08.007
100. Schoorl J, van Rijn S, de Wied M, van Goozen SHM, Swaab H. Variability in emotional/behavioral problems in boys with oppositional defiant disorder or conduct disorder: the role of arousal. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2016) 25:821–30. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0790-5
101. Raine A, Venables PH, Mednick SA. Low Resting Heart Rate at Age 3 Years Predisposes to Aggression at Age 11 Years: Evidence From the Mauritius Child Health Project. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (1997) 36:1457–64. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199710000-00029
102. Lorber MF. Psychophysiology of aggression, psychopathy, and conduct problems: A meta-analysis. psychol Bull. (2004) 130:531–52. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.531
103. Portnoy J, Raine A, Chen FR, Pardini D, Loeber R, Jennings JR. Heart rate and antisocial behavior: The mediating role of impulsive sensation seeking. Criminology: Interdiscip J. (2014) 52:292–311. doi: 10.1111/1745-9125.12038
104. Sijtsema JJ, Veenstra R, Lindenberg S, van Roon AM, Vernulst FC, Ormel J, et al. Mediation of sensation seeking and behavioral inhibition on the relationship between heart rate and antisocial behavior: The TRAILS study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2010) 49:493–502. doi: 10.1097/00004583-201005000-00010
105. Benning SD, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG. Psychopathy, startle blink modulation, and electrodermal reactivity in twin men. Psychophysiology. (2005) 42:753–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00353.x
106. Fairchild G, Van Goozen SHM, Stollery SJ, Goodyer IM. Fear conditioning and affective modulation of the startle reflex in male adolescents with early-onset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder and healthy control subjects. Biol Psychiatry. (2008) 63:279–85. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.019
107. Syngelaki EM, Fairchild G, Moore SC, Savage JC, van Goozen SHM. Affective startle potentiation in juvenile offenders: The role of conduct problems and psychopathic traits. Soc Neurosci. (2013) 8:112–21. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2012.712549
108. van Goozen SHM, Snoek H, Matthys W, van Rossum I, van Engeland H. Evidence of fearlessness in behaviourally disordered children: a study on startle reflex modulation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2004) 45:884–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00280.x
109. Kiehl KA, Smith AM, Hare RD, Mendrek A, Forster BB, Brink J, et al. Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biol Psychiatry. (2001) 50:677–84. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01222-7
110. Fanti KA, Panayiotou G, Lazarou C, Michael R, Georgiou G. The better of two evils? Evidence that children exhibiting continuous conduct problems high or low on callous-unemotional traits score on opposite directions on physiological and behavioral measures of fear. Dev Psychopathol. (2016) 28:185–98. doi: 10.1017/s0954579415000371
111. Torrubia R, Ávila C, Moltó J, Caseras X. The Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray's anxiety and impulsivity dimensions. Pers Individ Dif. (2001) 31:837–62. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00183-5
112. Gao Y, Chen H, Jia H, Ming Q, Yi J, Yao S. Dysfunctional feedback processing in adolescent males with conduct disorder. Int J Psychophysiol. (2016) 99:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.11.015
113. Côté S, Tremblay RE, Nagin DS, Zoccolillo M, Vitaro F. Childhood behavioral profiles leading to adolescent conduct disorder: risk trajectories for boys and girls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2002) 41:1086–94. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200209000-00009
114. Klingzell I, Fanti KA, Colins OF, Frogner L, Andershed A-K, Andershed H. Early childhood trajectories of conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits: the role of fearlessness and psychopathic personality dimensions. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. (2016) 47:236–47. doi: 10.1007/s10578-015-0560-0
115. Shaw DS, Gilliom M, Ingoldsby EM, Nagin DS. Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Dev Psychol. (2003) 39:189–200. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.189
116. Barker ED, Oliver BR, Viding E, Salekin RT, Maughan B. The impact of prenatal maternal risk, fearless temperament and early parenting on adolescent callous-unemotional traits: A 14-year longitudinal investigation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2011) 52:878–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02397.x
117. Raine A. The psychopathology of crime: Criminal behavior as a clinical disorder. Gulf Professional Publishing (1997).
118. de Wied M, van Boxtel A, Zaalberg R, Goudena PP, Matthys W. Facial EMG responses to dynamic emotional facial expressions in boys with disruptive behavior disorders. J Psychiatr Res. (2006) 40:112–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.08.003
119. Souroulla AV, Panteli M, Robinson JD, Panayiotou G. Valence, arousal or both? Shared emotional deficits associated with Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder and Oppositional/Defiant-Conduct Disorder symptoms in school-aged youth. Biol Psychol. (2019) 140:131–40. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.11.007
120. Short RML, Adams WJ, Garner M, Sonuga-Barke EJS, Fairchild G. Attentional biases to emotional faces in adolescents with conduct disorder, anxiety disorders, and comorbid conduct and anxiety disorders. J Exp Psychopathol. (2016) 7:466–83. doi: 10.5127/jep.053915
121. Oldenhof H, Jansen L, Ackermann K, Baker R, Batchelor M, Baumann S, et al. Psychophysiological responses to sadness in girls and boys with conduct disorder. J Psychopathology Clin Sci. (2020) 131:314–26. doi: 10.1037/abn0000524
122. Raine A. Antisocial behavior and psychophysiology: A biosocial perspective and a prefrontal dysfunction hypothesis. In: Handbook of antisocial behavior. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc (1997). p. 289–304.
Keywords: conduct disorder, emotional processing, theoretical perspectives, cognitive deficit, emotional cognition
Citation: Li X, Kou H, Bi T and Peng Z (2024) Deficits in emotional cognition among individuals with conduct disorder: theoretical perspectives. Front. Psychiatry 15:1507695. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1507695
Received: 08 October 2024; Accepted: 13 November 2024;
Published: 10 December 2024.
Edited by:
Qinghua He, Southwest University, ChinaReviewed by:
Michael Potegal, University of Minnesota Twin Cities, United StatesJie Du, Chongqing Technology and Business University, China
Copyright © 2024 Li, Kou, Bi and Peng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Zuoyang Peng, cGVuZ3p1b3lhbmdnekAxNjMuY29t