Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 19 December 2024
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Mind-body medicine and its impacts on psychological networks, quality of life, and health - Volume II View all 37 articles

Satiety-enhancing placebo intervention decreases selective attention to food cues

  • 1Institute of Medical Psychology, Faculty of Medicine, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Munich, Germany
  • 2Department for Pediatric Medicine, Schwabing Hospital, Munich, Germany
  • 3Division of Health Promotion, Coburg University of Applied Sciences and Arts, Coburg, Germany

Background: As placebo interventions could influence appetite and satiety in first studies, they are a promising tool for the future treatment of obesity. Furthermore, individuals with heightened body weight show increased selective attention for food cues. This study aimed to investigate whether placebo induced changes of appetite and satiety can affect attention allocation and to examine correlating factors.

Methods: In a double-blind design, 63 healthy participants were randomized into one of three groups: the enhanced appetite placebo group, the enhanced satiety placebo group, or the control group. Appetite and satiety were induced by administering a placebo capsule along with a group specific expectancy manipulation. One hour later, participants performed a visual probe task to measure attentional bias by comparing reaction times for different conditions. Correlations between reaction times and subjective hunger and satiety ratings, as well as current food craving and plasma ghrelin levels, were explored.

Results: The induction of attentional bias toward non-food stimuli was successful in women in the enhanced satiety placebo group but not in the enhanced appetite placebo group. Women of the enhanced satiety placebo group showed significantly higher reaction times for food cues compared to non-food cues. Across conditions, reaction times were associated with subjective hunger ratings and current food craving in women. No attentional bias was induced in men in either placebo group.

Conclusion: Placebo-induced satiety inhibited attention allocation toward food in healthy women, potentially mediated by reduced hunger and food craving. Placebo effects on satiety could thus be demonstrated on a highly complex cognitive process.

1 Introduction

Overweight and obesity are major cardiovascular risk factors and continue to pose a growing global health challenge. As of 2022, 43% of adults worldwide were classified as overweight, and 16% as obese (1). Although increased energy intake and reduced physical activity are primary contributors to elevated body weight (2), recent studies have also focused on the neurobiological and cognitive mechanisms regulating appetite and satiety (3, 4) in search of novel therapeutic strategies. Modifying attention to food cues has emerged as a promising approach for addressing dysregulated attentional processes related to appetite.

Attentional bias arises from various attention-regulation mechanisms that enable the brain to prioritize processing of relevant stimuli, helping meet immediate needs as efficiently as possible (5). Early research on attentional bias primarily focused on threat-related stimuli (6, 7), yet biases have since been observed for a range of stimuli types (8, 9). Food-related stimuli, often perceived positively, may inherently induce attentional bias (10), particularly when addressing an immediate need like hunger (8). For example, in studies using the dot probe task, hungry participants demonstrated heightened attentional bias toward food-related words (e.g., chocolate, honey) over neutral words, as evidenced by faster reaction times (11). Additionally, elevated attentional bias toward food cues has been observed in overweight and obese individuals compared to those of normal weight, suggesting a dysregulated attentional process in obesity similar to that seen in individuals with substance use disorders (12, 13). Functional MRI studies further support this, showing a positive correlation between body mass index (BMI) and activation in brain regions associated with attention when exposed to food cues (14).

Appetite and satiety are closely tied to mental states such as craving (15) and expectations. For instance, studies show that participants preferred Coke more when it was branded (16), and perceived taste ratings for cheese and yogurt were lower when labeled as fat-reduced (17). Expectations also influence physiological appetite regulation, including the release of the appetite-stimulating hormone ghrelin. In a study by Crum and colleagues, participants were given an identical milkshake described as either “high-calorie, indulgent” or “low-calorie, sensible,” and blood ghrelin levels were measured before and after consumption. Participants consuming the “indulgent” milkshake displayed a significantly steeper decline in ghrelin levels, suggesting a stronger sense of satiety compared to the “sensible” milkshake (18). Positive expectations, a major component of the placebo effect, could therefore be harnessed to modulate appetite and satiety. Recent research further shows that beliefs about a hunger-altering placebo intervention influence medial prefrontal cortex activation and even impact later food choices (19). Additional evidence comes from studies demonstrating that when the expected satiety from a previous meal is higher, calorie intake at the next meal is lower (20), underscoring the powerful effect of expectations on eating behavior. Finally, a recent systematic review provides initial evidence suggesting that placebo interventions can promote weight loss in adults (21).

In this study, we integrated insights from placebo and attention research to examine whether placebo-induced changes in appetite and satiety can influence attentional bias toward food cues. Within a larger trial investigating placebo effects on appetite, satiety, and their objective markers, participants were administered a placebo capsule with instructions that it would either stimulate appetite or increase satiety (22). Later, a subgroup of these participants completed a visual probe task, one of the most established paradigms for measuring attentional bias (23). We hypothesized that placebo-induced appetite would heighten attention to food cues, evidenced by faster reaction times for food stimuli relative to neutral stimuli, while placebo-induced satiety would reduce attention to food cues. Additionally, we explored both behavioral and physiological factors that might moderate attentional bias in our analyses.

2 Material and methods

2.1 Participants

Healthy participants aged 18-40 years with normal body weight (BMI 19-25kg/m2) were included in the main study, within which this substudy was embedded. Exclusion criteria comprised pregnancy or breastfeeding, smoking, alcohol or drug abuse, food allergies, regularly intake of medication (except contraceptives), acute or chronic disease, history of psychiatric disease, surgery within the last four weeks prior to participation, elevated fasting blood glucose levels (>100mg/dl), and clinically relevant anxiety or depression scores [score >7 in at least one subscale of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) (24)]. Recruitment was conducted through university mailing lists and flyers. All participants provided written informed consent and received 45€ as compensation.

2.2 Study design

This study was conducted at the Institute of Medical Psychology at Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Germany, and was nested within a larger double-blinded randomized controlled trial investigating the neurobiological mechanisms of placebo interventions on appetite and satiety [for details, see (22)]. In the main study, 90 participants were randomly allocated to one of three groups stratified by sex: enhanced appetite placebo, enhanced satiety placebo, or control. For ethical reasons and to ensure double-blinding, six additional participants were randomly allocated to the enhanced appetite verum and enhanced satiety verum groups (Figure 1). Since this substudy started later than the main study, only 67 out of the 96 study participants from the main study were included. The original study protocol and the amendment describing this substudy were approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Germany (approval number 650-15).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Study Design. The main study was designed as a randomized, double-blind, controlled trial, in which 96 participants were randomized into one of five groups (control, appetite placebo, appetite verum, satiety placebo, satiety verum). A total of 29 measurements had already been conducted before the start of this substudy. Verum groups were included only for double-blinding and were not analyzed further. Randomization was maintained, which led to slightly different group sizes within the substudy.

2.3 Randomization and blinding

As detailed in (22), sequentially numbered and sealed envelopes were prepared by a person not directly involved in the experiment, using a computer-generated randomization list. Each envelope contained a capsule along with information about the type of intervention (either appetite-enhancing, satiety-enhancing, or control). To ensure double-blinding, neither the participant nor the experimenter knew whether the capsule in the appetite- or satiety-enhancing intervention contained a placebo or an active ingredient.

2.4 Experimental procedure

The experimental procedure of the main experiment is described in detail in Hoffmann and colleagues (22). In brief, each participant underwent a single experimental session starting at 8 a.m. after fasting for 10 to 12 hours. Finger blood samples to measure blood glucose levels were taken using a BG Star device (Sanofi-Aventis, Hannover, Germany). Blood samples to assess ghrelin levels were repeatedly drawn from a peripheral intravenous catheter flushed with saline. After placing electrodes to monitor the electrocardiogram and the electrogastrogram, participants completed the “Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale” [HADS (24)] and the “Food Craving Questionnaire - Trait” [FCQ-T (25, 26)], rated their current levels of hunger and satiety on 100 mm visual analogue scales (VAS), and assessed their current food craving using the “Food Craving Questionnaire – State” [FCQ-S (25)]. Thereafter, the first ghrelin blood sample was taken. The experimenter then opened the sealed envelope, performed the verbal expectancy manipulation according to group allocation, and the participant swallowed the provided capsule with 100 ml of mineral water. After each of two consecutive 30 min resting periods, hunger and satiety ratings were assessed, and ghrelin blood samples were collected. Participants were then asked to complete the FCQ-S (25) again. Following a brief instruction on the visual probe task (VPT) by the experimenter, participants performed the VPT while the experimenter left the room to minimize distraction. After completing the VPT, participants in the appetite- and satiety-enhancing intervention groups were asked to guess their allocation to either the verum or placebo group.

2.5 Interventions

Lactose (Heirler Cenovis GmbH, Radolfzell, Germany) was used as the inert ingredient for the control group and placebo groups, while an alginate complex (CM3 Alginat Kapseln, Easyway GmbH, Monheim, Germany) served as the satiety-enhancing active ingredient, and a bitter herb extract (Appetit-Anreger, Zirkulin Naturheilmittel GmbH, Bremen) was used as the appetite-enhancing active ingredient. In the appetite-enhancing and satiety-enhancing intervention groups, participants were informed that they would receive a capsule containing a real treatment or a placebo. Participants in the control group were told they would receive a capsule with an inert substance [for details, see (22)].

2.6 Visual probe task

The VPT was designed using “Visual Studio, Framework.net 4.5.2” in C# and performed on a fast PC. Participants were seated 500 mm from a 400 mm screen. One hundred pictures were paired, with five pairs used for a brief instruction. The remaining 45 pairs included either two pictures of neutral objects (15 pairs) or one picture of appetizing food paired with one neutral object (30 pairs). All pictures were sourced from the “food.pics database” (27) and screened for similar complexion, object size, brightness, and contrast within each pair. Additionally, food pictures were included only if they had a calorie density between 250-500 kcal/100g and total calories greater than 250 kcal per picture, along with high craving and palatability scores in the database’s internal ratings. Participants rated the 30 food types depicted in the selected pictures for personal craving prior to the experimental session day. Only the 15 highest-rated food picture pairs were used to create individualized VPTs for each participant, enhancing the internal reliability of the task by selecting the most attractive stimuli (28).

A total of 120 reaction times (RTs) under three different conditions were measured. Each trial began with a fixation cross displayed for 500 ms in the center of the screen. Two hundred and fifty milliseconds after its disappearance, two pictures were displayed for 100 ms each, positioned 60 mm to the left and right of the center, respectively. Immediately after the pictures disappeared, a dot appeared at the former position of one of the pictures. Participants were instructed to detect the dot as quickly as possible and then press a button (“A” for left or “L” for right) to confirm detection. The time between the appearance of the dot and the button press was recorded as RT. After a variable interval of 300 ms to 2000 ms, the next trial started with the display of the fixation cross (29, 30).

The three conditions were defined as congruent (30 trials per participant), incongruent (30 trials per participant), and baseline (60 trials per participant). Trials featuring two neutral objects were classified as baseline, measuring the participant’s individual ordinary RT. If one picture was a neutral object and the other a food object, the trial was considered congruent if the dot appeared at the location of the food picture and incongruent if it appeared at the location of the neutral picture (Figure 2).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Conditions of the visual probe task. Pictures were displayed for 100 ms before disappearing, then dot appears. (A) Condition congruent: One neutral and one food picture. Dot appears behind food picture. (B) Condition incongruent: One neutral and one food picture. Dot appears behind neutral picture. (C) Condition baseline: Two neutral pictures. Dot appears behind one of neutral pictures.

2.7 Outcome parameters

2.7.1 Reaction times of visual probe task

As primary outcome parameters, the mean RTs and standard deviations (SD) for each condition (Mbaseline, Mcongruent, Mincongruent) were calculated from the 120 measured RTs per participant. Before calculation, outliers were excluded following a previously established three-stage process similar to other outlier exclusion procedures in studies using the VPT (29, 31). First, all trials with errors (e.g., pressing the wrong button) or trials with RTs <100 ms or >2000 ms were removed, as they clearly do not reflect the true RTs. Second, all trials differing by more than 3.5 SD from the participant’s condition-specific mean RT were excluded. Third, all trials differing by more than 2.5 SD from the condition-specific mean RT of the entire sample were excluded, as they might reflect atypical situations (e.g., lack of understanding of the test, poor concentration).

In the VPT, lower mean RT for the congruent condition (Mcongruent) compared to the baseline condition (Mbaseline) indicates a higher orienting toward food stimuli, while a higher mean RT for the incongruent condition (Mincongruent) compared to Mbaseline indicates delayed disengagement from food stimuli. Conversely, a higher mean RT for the congruent condition (Mcongruent) compared to the baseline condition (Mbaseline) indicates delayed disengagement from non-food stimuli, while a lower mean RT for the incongruent condition (Mincongruent) compared to Mbaseline indicates enhanced orienting toward non-food stimuli.

2.7.2 Behavioral parameters

A 100 mm VAS was used for repeated measurements of subjective hunger and satiety ratings, ranging from “not at all hungry/full” to “extremely hungry/full”. Additionally, a dichotomous scale was administered to determine whether participants believed they had received a placebo or an active treatment.

2.7.3 Questionnaires

The Food Craving Questionnaire – State (FCQ-S; 25) and the short version of the Food Craving Questionnaire – Trait (FCQ-T; 26) were employed to assess general food craving and to measure current food craving at multiple time points. Both the state and trait measures of food craving have been shown to enhance selective attention to food cues (15). Furthermore, the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) was utilized to screen for elevated levels of anxiety and depression, following the suggested cut-off values [(24); see above]. Additionally, participants were asked to provide their body weight and height for the calculation of the BMI.

2.7.4 Plasma ghrelin

To measure plasma ghrelin levels, blood samples were taken at three different time points into commercially available EDTA tubes (2.7ml) prepared with 54 µl of 4mM 4-(2-aminoethyl)benzenesulfonyl fluoride hydrochloride (AEBSF) (32). The samples were immediately stored on ice after collection until they were centrifuged within 60 minutes for 10 minutes at 3,000g and 4°C. Two subsamples of 500 µl per blood sample were transferred to Eppendorf tubes prepared with 100 µl of 1 mM HCl and gently mixed before being stored at -70°C until final analyses. Plasma ghrelin levels (pg/ml) were measured in duplicate, following the protocol, using the Human Ghrelin (total) ELISA Kit (Catalogue number EZGRT-89K, Merck Millipore, Darmstadt, Germany).

2.8 Statistical analyses

Before analysis, all data were assessed for normality using skewness, kurtosis, and the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. All continuous outcome parameters met the normality assumption. To account for individual differences in RTs, a mixed analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was employed to explore the RTs of the VPT. In this analysis, Mbaseline served as a covariate, while condition-specific mean RTs (“condition”) were treated as a within-subjects factor. The factors “group” (enhanced appetite placebo, enhanced satiety placebo, control) and “sex” were included as between-subjects factors. To directly compare Mcongruent and Mincongruent with Mbaseline, two-tailed t-tests were used. Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons were applied where appropriate to control for the increased risk of Type I errors. Dichotomous parameters were analyzed using the chi-square test.

Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated to assess possible relationships between subjective states of hunger, satiety, and food craving, individual RTs, ghrelin levels, and general food craving. Participants from both placebo groups (enhanced appetite placebo and enhanced satiety placebo) and the control group were included, as an association between appetite regulation and RTs was expected in the entire sample. To account for participants’ individual ordinary RTs, difference scores (Δ) were calculated and entered into the correlational analyses as follows: for the congruent condition (ΔMcongruent = McongruentMbaseline) and for the incongruent condition (ΔMincongruent = MincongruentMbaseline). To account for baseline differences in hunger, satiety, and food craving ratings as well as ghrelin levels, Δ scores were computed and entered into the correlational analyses as follows: for hunger (ΔHunger (VAS) = VAShunger before VPT – VAShunger baseline), for satiety (ΔSatiety (VAS) = VASsatiety before VPT – VASsatiety baseline), for current food craving (ΔFood craving (FCQ-S) = FCQ-Sbefore VPT – FCQ-Sbaseline), and for ghrelin levels (ΔGhrelin = GhrelinbeforeVPT – Ghrelinbaseline).

All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (Version 25), and a significance level of p ≤ 0.05 was assumed.

3 Results

3.1 Participants

Out of 240 individuals who responded to the recruitment efforts for the main study, 113 were screened for eligibility. Seventeen participants were excluded before the experimental session (3 did not meet inclusion criteria, 12 did not provide informed consent, and one did not show up). Additionally, one participant was retrospectively excluded due to elevated fasting blood glucose levels and was replaced with a new participant. In total, 96 participants were included and completed the experimental session. Twenty-nine of these were tested before commencing this substudy. Four of the remaining participants received a verum treatment and were therefore excluded from further analyses. Consequently, data from 63 participants (31 men, 32 women) were included in the following analyses (Figure 1).

The study groups were comparable at baseline in terms of demographic, behavioral, and humoral parameters, as well as general food craving scores, and anxiety and depression scores (Table 1). The mean age of participants was 23.6 years (SD = 2.9), and the mean BMI was 21.6 (SD = 1.8).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Group characteristics at baseline.

3.2 Reaction times in visual probe task

In total, 4.3% of the data were removed during the prescribed outlier exclusion process (2.1% in the first step, 0.6% in the second step, and 1.6% in the third step, the latter leading to exclusion of the VPT data of one participant in the enhanced appetite placebo group). The mixed ANCOVA revealed a significant three-way interaction (Fcondition x group x sex(2, 55) = 4.19, p = 0.020) (Table 2). Bonferroni-corrected two-way ANCOVAs, conducted separately for the placebo groups, showed a significant interaction between “condition” and “sex” in the enhanced satiety placebo group (Fcondition x sex(1, 18) = 6.14, p = 0.046), but not in the enhanced appetite placebo group (Fcondition x sex(1, 16) = 2.27, p = 0.304). Further Bonferroni-adjusted post hoc tests for the enhanced satiety placebo group revealed Mcongruent was significantly higher than Mincongruent in women (Fcondition(1,8) = 8.1, p = 0.044) (Figure 3), while this was not the case for men (Fcondition(1,9) = 0.004, p = 1) (Figure 4). To investigate whether this effect in women could be attributed to delayed disengagement from or enhanced orienting toward the food cues, two separate Bonferroni-corrected t-tests comparing Mcongruent and Mincongruent with Mbaseline in female participants from the enhanced satiety placebo group were performed. Neither test was significant (Mcongruent: t(9) = -1.236, p = 0.496, Mincongruent: t(9) = 0.687, p = 1).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means of Mcongruent and Mincongruent (controlled for Mbaseline) by group and sex.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Reaction times in women. Baseline-corrected reaction times (ms) in women according to condition and group (estimated means ± SD). Significant difference of reaction time to congruent and incongruent condition in the group placebo-satiety. *p<0.05.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Reaction times in men. Baseline-corrected reaction times (ms) in men according to condition and group (estimated means ± SD). No significant difference.

3.3 Correlations between reaction times and hunger, satiety, and food craving ratings

Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated separately for men and women. In women, significant correlations emerged between ΔMincongruent and ΔHunger ratings (r = 0.356, p = 0.046) as well as ΔFood craving (r = 0.389, p = 0.028), but not with ΔSatiety ratings (r = -0.334, p = 0.062). In contrast, ΔMcongruent, was not related to ΔHunger ratings, ΔSatiety ratings, or ΔFood craving in women. All analyses were repeated for men, and no significant correlations were found (Table 3).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Pearson correlation coefficients between ΔMcongruent and ΔMincongruent (controlled for Mbaseline) and behavioral ratings (ΔHunger, ΔSatiety, ΔFood craving, controlled for baseline levels), humoral measurements (ΔGhrelin, controlled for baseline levels), and general food craving.

3.4 Correlations between reaction times and ghrelin levels

Neither ΔMcongruent nor ΔMincongruent correlated with ΔGhrelin in either sex (Table 3).

3.5 Correlations between reaction times and general food craving

Neither ΔMcongruent nor ΔMincongruent correlated with general food craving (FCQ-T) in either sex (Table 3).

3.6 Treatment guesses

In the satiety placebo group, nine out of 21 participants (42.9%) believed they received an active treatment, while only one out of 20 participants (5%) in the appetite placebo group thought the same. A significant group difference was observed (χ2 = 7.96, p = 0.005). In total, 4 women (20%) and 6 men (29%) in the placebo groups guessed they had received an active ingredient. There was no significant difference between sexes (χ2 = 0.41, p = 0.523).

4 Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate attentional bias toward food cues following placebo-induced appetite and satiety in healthy men and women. The results partially confirm our hypothesis that placebo-induced appetite and satiety would affect selective attention to food stimuli. Notably, we demonstrated for the first time that women receiving the placebo intervention to enhance satiety exhibited longer reaction times for food cues compared to neutral stimuli, indicating reduced attention to food cues after expectancy manipulation. Conversely, the placebo intervention to enhance appetite did not affect attention allocation to food cues neither in women nor in men.

Our findings align with several studies demonstrating decreased selective attention to food cues following satiety induction. For example, a study observed a decreased percentage of fixations and reduced viewing time of food pictures in an eye movement paradigm among women with weight concerns after satiety was induced using a placebo pill paired with a verbal suggestion (33). Another study using the VPT revealed an attentional bias toward two different food types in a hungry state. After consumption of one food type until sated, a significant decrease of selective attention to that food was detected, while no such decrease was observed for the uneaten food (34). These studies, in conjunction with our findings, suggest that selective attention adjusts rapidly and even indicate that attention is drawn away from stimuli addressing satisfied needs.

Interestingly, despite successful satiety induction in both sexes (22), no attentional bias could be detected in men from the enhanced satiety placebo group. Treatment guesses also did not differ between men and women, refuting the idea that men’s potentially higher skepticism accounts for this discrepancy. Recent studies suggest that sex significantly influences the relationship between eating behavior and visual attention to food cues; for instance, emotional eating correlates with visual attention only in women (35). Additionally, women tend to focus more on low-calorie, low-fat foods (36, 37) but are more likely to consume sweet snacks when experiencing stress (38), whereas men generally pay more attention to both sweet and savory high-calorie foods (37). In this study, the predominance of high-calorie food images could partly explain the observed gender difference. While placebo-induced satiety may reduce selective attention to high-calorie foods in women, it may not counteract men’s naturally higher attention toward such foods.

The sex-specific pattern in attentional bias, despite similar reductions in satiety for both sexes, is also consistent with findings from placebo research. While men and women reported comparable placebo effects on visceral pain and nausea, their neurobiological correlates differed (39, 40). Specifically, the insular cortex showed varying activation patterns, with placebo effects in men involving neural down-regulation of the insular cortex and related interoceptive experiences, such as pain or nausea. Conversely, placebo effects in women were more closely associated with prefrontal cortex activity (40). According to a recent functional MRI study, placebo-induced satiety in women may result from altered perceptual attentional filtering, where the perceived tastiness of food becomes less relevant under decreased hunger conditions (19).

Contrary to our expectations and prior studies (11, 29), we did not observe an attentional bias for food cues in the enhanced appetite placebo group. The most likely explanation for this finding is that the appetite-enhancing placebo intervention did not successfully induce increased hunger in our study (22). Notably, significantly more participants believed they had received an active treatment in the satiety-enhancing intervention group, suggesting that the placebo intervention was more credible in this group than in the appetite-enhancing intervention group.

In our female participants, decreased selective attention to food cues was correlated with changes in hunger ratings and current food craving. A recent meta-analysis similarly provided evidence for a positive correlation between selective attention to food cues and both current food craving and subjective hunger (41). Notably, general food craving did not correlate with attentional bias for food cues, neither in previous research (42) nor in our study. In addition, the correlation of hunger and food cravings with Mincongruent but not Mcongruent suggests that the altered attentional bias may result from enhanced orienting toward non-food stimuli rather than delayed disengagement from non-food stimuli.

In this study, we used a cue exposure duration of 100 ms, as prior studies on food attentional bias, especially when using the VPT, indicated this duration as beneficial (7, 29). Very short exposure durations (<250 ms) serve as subliminal exposures that appear to engage early attentional processes such as enhanced orienting. Support for this interpretation comes from an eye-tracking study showing that, in a fed state, normal-weight individuals tend to direct their initial gaze to the non-food picture, indicating an initial orienting response to the non-food stimulus (31). Furthermore, Potthoff and colleagues found a higher percentage of fixation on non-food images after placebo-induced satiety, pointing in a similar direction (33).

Contrary to our expectations, no correlation between the appetite-enhancing gut hormone ghrelin and attentional bias was detected. Notably, ghrelin levels remained unchanged in the enhanced satiety placebo group (22). These findings support the conclusion that placebo-induced satiation is not mediated by ghrelin. Rather, verbally induced expectations may lead to a top-down modulation of satiety that operates independently of humoral mechanisms. Further evidence for this interpretation comes from a recent study showing that, although expectations of increased satiety reduced calorie intake in subsequent meals and throughout the day, no effect on ghrelin levels was observed (20). However, further research should examine whether other peptides involved in appetite regulation, such as cholecystokinin, glucagon-like peptide 1, and peptide YY, may play a role in expectation-enhanced satiety (43).

Several limitations of this study should be noted. First, because we analyzed men and women separately, each group size was limited to 10 to 12 participants. Future studies may benefit from reducing the number of intervention groups or increasing the sample size. Second, this study examined only healthy, normal-weight participants. Including overweight or obese individuals would allow for investigation into whether placebo interventions can alter the dysregulated attentional processes associated with obesity (12). Third, we used expectancy manipulation, rather than conditioning, to induce placebo effects on satiety and appetite. Conditioning, which is known to engage unconscious processes and produce longer-lasting placebo responses (44, 45), could be advantageous for addressing long-term outcomes such as body weight.

We propose three key areas for further research. First, the potential of deregulating visual attention as a therapeutic tool should be explored for individuals with overweight and obesity. Ideally, this research would integrate multimodal strategies that combine approaches to achieve rapid and sustainable weight loss, ultimately reducing cardiovascular risk. Second, future longitudinal studies could use conditioning to generate longer-lasting placebo effects. Notably, a recent study using an expectancy-based placebo application demonstrated a decrease in appetite only during the first two days of a seven-day follow-up period (46). Third, the neurobiological mechanisms underlying placebo-induced satiety and the subsequent reduction in selective attention to food cues, as well as potential sex-specific differences, warrant further investigation. In this context, functional MRI could be a promising tool, especially for examining the involvement of the brain’s reward system, which plays a crucial role in food-related behaviors and can override physiological appetite control (47).

In conclusion, the results of the present study demonstrate that placebo-induced satiety inhibits attention allocation toward food cues in healthy women, presumably mediated by decreased subjective hunger and food craving. Attention allocation, as a largely unconscious process, verifies the placebo effect on satiety also at an objective level. Our findings align with growing evidence that placebo interventions, particularly those utilizing expectancy effects, can significantly influence food craving and related behaviors. Rodríguez-Martín and colleagues (48) demonstrated that expectancy-based placebo interventions were effective in controlling food craving and intrusive food-related thoughts over time. This suggests that cognitive factors, such as the belief in a treatment’s efficacy, may modulate attention to and desire for food independently of physiological hunger signals. In our study, similar top-down processes likely contributed to the observed placebo effects on attentional bias and satiety in women. These findings support the potential of placebo-based cognitive interventions not only in normal-weight individuals but also as a feasible adjunct for managing food craving in overweight and obese populations, offering a promising avenue for further research on placebo-induced modulation of eating behavior.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by ethics committee of the Medical Faculty, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Germany. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

ML: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft. VH: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. KM: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. KM received financial support by the Theophrastus Foundation (Germany), the Schweizer-Arau Foundation (Germany), and the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (ME-3675/1-1).

Acknowledgments

The content of the study has been previously reported as part of the first author's thesis (49), available here: https://edoc.ub.uni-muenchen.de/30394/7/Lanz_Marina.pdf.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. World Health Organisation. Obesity and overweight (2024). Available online at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight (accessed June 12, 2024).

Google Scholar

2. Koliaki C, Dalamaga M, Liatis S. Update on the obesity epidemic: after the sudden rise, is the upward trajectory beginning to flatten? Curr Obes Rep. (2023) 12:514–27. doi: 10.1007/s13679-023-00527-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Stoeckel LE, Birch LL, Heatherton T, Mann T, Hunter C, Czajkowski S, et al. Psychological and neural contributions to appetite self-regulation. Obesity. (2017) 25:S17–25. doi: 10.1002/oby.v25.S1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Althubeati S, Avery A, Tench CR, Lobo DN, Salter A, Eldeghaidy S. Mapping brain activity of gut-brain signaling to appetite and satiety in healthy adults: A systematic review and functional neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2022) 136:104603. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104603

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Vuilleumier P. How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cognit Sci. (2005) 9:585–94. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. MacLeod C, Mathews A, Tata P. Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J Abnormal Psychol. (1986) 95:15. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.95.1.15

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IMH. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull. (2007) 133:1–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Pool E, Brosch T, Delplanque S, Sander D. Attentional bias for positive emotional stimuli: a meta-analytic investigation. psychol Bulletin. (2016) 142:79. doi: 10.1037/bul0000026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Field M, Cox WM. Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2008) 97:1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Leland D, Pineda J. Effects of food-related stimuli on visual spatial attention in fasting and nonfasting normal subjects: Behavior and electrophysiology. Clin neurophysiology. (2006) 117:67–84. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.09.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Mogg K, Bradley BP, Hyare H, Lee S. Selective attention to food-related stimuli in hunger: are attentional biases specific to emotional and psychopathological states, or are they also found in normal drive states? Behav Res Ther. (1998) 36:227–37. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00062-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Hendrikse JJ, Cachia RL, Kothe EJ, McPhie S, Skouteris H, Hayden MJ. Attentional biases for food cues in overweight and individuals with obesity: a systematic review of the literature. Obes Rev. (2015) 16:424–32. doi: 10.1111/obr.2015.16.issue-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Field M, Werthmann J, Franken I, Hofmann W, Hogarth L, Roefs A. The role of attentional bias in obesity and addiction. Am Psychol Assoc. (2016) 35(8):767–80. doi: 10.1037/hea0000405

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Yokum S, Ng J, Stice E. Attentional bias to food images associated with elevated weight and future weight gain: an fMRI study. Obesity. (2011) 19:1775–83. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.168

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Kemps E, Tiggemann M. Attentional bias for craving-related (chocolate) food cues. Exp Clin psychopharmacology. (2009) 17:425. doi: 10.1037/a0017796

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. McClure SM, Li J, Tomlin D, Cypert KS, Montague LM, Montague PR. Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks. Neuron. (2004) 44:379–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Wardle J, Solomons W. Naughty but nice: A laboratory study of health information and food preferences in a community sample. Health Psychol. (1994) 13:180. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.13.2.180

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Crum AJ, Corbin WR, Brownell KD, Salovey P. Mind over milkshakes: mindsets, not just nutrients, determine ghrelin response. Health Psychol. (2011) 30:424. doi: 10.1037/a0023467

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Khalid I, Rodrigues B, Dreyfus H, Frileux S, Meissner K, Fossati P, et al. Mapping expectancy-based appetitive placebo effects onto the brain in women. Nat Commun. (2024) 15:248. doi: 10.1038/s41467-023-44569-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Brown SD, Duncan J, Crabtree D, Powell D, Hudson M, Allan JL. We are what we (think we) eat: The effect of expected satiety on subsequent calorie consumption. Appetite. (2020) 152:104717. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104717

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Żegleń M, Kryst Ł, Bąbel P. Diet, gym, supplements, or maybe it is all in your mind? A systematic review and meta-analysis of studies on placebo and nocebo effects in weight loss in adults. Obes Rev. (2024) 25:e13660. doi: 10.1111/obr.13660

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hoffmann V, Lanz M, Mackert J, Muller T, Tschop M, Meissner K. Effects of placebo interventions on subjective and objective markers of appetite-A randomized controlled trial. Front Psychiatry. (2018) 9:706. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00706

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Yiend J. The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cogn emotion. (2010) 24:3–47. doi: 10.1080/02699930903205698

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (1983) 67:361–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Cepeda-Benito A, Gleaves DH, Williams TL, Erath SA. The development and validation of the state and trait food-cravings questionnaires. Behav Ther. (2000) 31:151–73. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(00)80009-X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Meule A, Hermann T, Kubler A. A short version of the Food Cravings Questionnaire-Trait: the FCQ-T-reduced. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:190. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00190

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Blechert J, Meule A, Busch NA, Ohla K. Food-pics: an image database for experimental research on eating and appetite. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:617. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00617

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Christiansen P, Mansfield R, Duckworth J, Field M, Jones A. Internal reliability of the alcohol-related visual probe task is increased by utilising personalised stimuli and eye-tracking. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2015) 155:170–4. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.07.672

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Tapper K, Pothos EM, Lawrence AD. Feast your eyes: hunger and trait reward drive predict attentional bias for food cues. Emotion. (2010) 10:949–54. doi: 10.1037/a0020305

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Bradley BP, Mogg K, Wright T, Field M. Attentional bias in drug dependence: vigilance for cigarette-related cues in smokers. Psychol Addict Behav. (2003) 17:66–72. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.17.1.66

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Castellanos EH, Charboneau E, Dietrich MS, Park S, Bradley BP, Mogg K, et al. Obese adults have visual attention bias for food cue images: evidence for altered reward system function. Int J Obes. (2009) 33:1063–73. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.138

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Liu J, Prudom CE, Nass R, Pezzoli SS, Oliveri MC, Johnson ML, et al. Novel ghrelin assays provide evidence for independent regulation of ghrelin acylation and secretion in healthy young men. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. (2008) 93:1980–7. doi: 10.1210/jc.2007-2235

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Potthoff J, Jurinec N, Schienle A. Placebo effects on visual food cue reactivity: an eye-tracking investigation. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:525. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00525

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. di Pellegrino G, Magarelli S, Mengarelli F. Food pleasantness affects visual selective attention. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). (2011) 64:560–71. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.504031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Hummel G, Ehret J, Zerweck I, Winter SS, Stroebele-Benschop N. How eating behavior, food stimuli and gender may affect visual attention–An eye tracking study. Eating behaviors. (2018) 31:60–7. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2018.08.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Banović M, Chrysochou P, Grunert KG, Rosa PJ, Gamito P. The effect of fat content on visual attention and choice of red meat and differences across gender. Food Qual Preference. (2016) 52:42–51. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.03.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Hummel G, Zerweck I, Ehret J, Winter SS, Stroebele-Benschop N. The influence of the arrangement of different food images on participants’ attention: An experimental eye-tracking study. Food Qual Preference. (2017) 62:111–9. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.07.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Grogan SC, Bell R, Conner M. Eating sweet snacks: gender differences in attitudes and behaviour. Appetite. (1997) 28:19–31. doi: 10.1006/appe.1996.0067

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Theysohn N, Schmid J, Icenhour A, Mewes C, Forsting M, Gizewski E, et al. Are there sex differences in placebo analgesia during visceral pain processing? A fMRI study in healthy subjects. Neurogastroenterol Motility. (2014) 26:1743–53. doi: 10.1111/nmo.2014.26.issue-12

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Haile A, Watts M, Aichner S, Stahlberg F, Hoffmann V, Tschoep MH, et al. Central correlates of placebo effects in nausea differ between men and women. Brain behavior. (2022) 12:e2685. doi: 10.1002/brb3.v12.8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Hardman CA, Jones A, Burton S, Duckworth JJ, McGale LS, Mead BR, et al. Food-related attentional bias and its associations with appetitive motivation and body weight: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Appetite. (2021) 157:104986. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104986

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Ruddock H, Field M, Jones A, Hardman C. State and trait influences on attentional bias to food-cues: The role of hunger, expectancy, and self-perceived food addiction. Appetite. (2018) 131:139–47. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.08.038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Yu JH, Kim M-S. Molecular mechanisms of appetite regulation. Diabetes Metab J. (2012) 36:391. doi: 10.4093/dmj.2012.36.6.391

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Fedele L, Marchini M, Acaia B, Garagiola U, Tiengo M. Dynamics and significance of placebo response in primary dysmenorrhea. Pain. (1989) 36:43–7. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(89)90110-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Meissner K. Wie wirkt ein Plazebo? internist prax. (2014) 54:579–88.

Google Scholar

46. Unger I, Schienle A. Taking placebos as needed to reduce appetite: A randomized controlled trial with ecological momentary assessment. Behav Sci. (2023) 13:207. doi: 10.3390/bs13030207

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Amin T, Mercer JG. Hunger and satiety mechanisms and their potential exploitation in the regulation of food intake. Curr Obes Rep. (2016) 5:106–12. doi: 10.1007/s13679-015-0184-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Rodríguez-Martín BC, Frías-Hernández LM, O’Farril-Jiménez JC, Guillén-Verano ÁM, Pastorrecio-González L, Chinea-Monzón Y, et al. Placebo and food cravings control: results from two randomized controlled trials. Longhua Chin Med. (2020) 3. doi: 10.21037/lcm.2020.01.01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Lanz M. Verschiebung der selektiven Aufmerksamkeit durch erwartungsinduzierten Hunger und erwartungsinduzierte Sättigung-eine randomisierte kontrollierte Studie. Munich, Germany: Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich (2022).

Google Scholar

Keywords: placebo effect, expectation, attentional bias, selective attention, appetite, satiety, food craving, visual probe task

Citation: Lanz M, Hoffmann V and Meissner K (2024) Satiety-enhancing placebo intervention decreases selective attention to food cues. Front. Psychiatry 15:1472532. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1472532

Received: 29 July 2024; Accepted: 06 November 2024;
Published: 19 December 2024.

Edited by:

Georg Seifert, Charité University Medicine Berlin, Germany

Reviewed by:

Boris C. Rodríguez-Martín, International University of La Rioja, Spain
Dongdong Ni, The University of Queensland, Australia
Anna Rudzińska, Jagiellonian University Medical College, Poland

Copyright © 2024 Lanz, Hoffmann and Meissner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marina Lanz, bWxhbnpAdC1vbmxpbmUuZGU=; Karin Meissner, a2FyaW4ubWVpc3NuZXJAaHMtY29idXJnLmRl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.