- 1Department of Functional Sciences, Discipline of Public Health, Center for Translational Research and Systems Medicine, “Victor Babes” University of Medicine and Pharmacy, Timisoara, Romania
- 2Department of Neuroscience, Discipline of Psychiatry, Center for Cognitive Research in Neuropsychiatric Pathology (NeuroPsy-Cog), “Victor Babes” University of Medicine and Pharmacy, Timisoara, Romania
- 3Psychiatry Compartment, “Dr. Victor Popescu” Emergency Military Clinical Hospital, Timisoara, Romania
Introduction: This study explores whether high alexithymia values correlate with low levels of empathy, while also trying to identify potential connections with social media addiction.
Methods: We hypothesized that alexithymia mediates the relationship between social media addiction and empathy levels in a sample of undergraduate students. The study population consisted of 649 medical students in the 4th/5th/6th University year, recruited between March and May 2021. For this assessment, we employed three psychometric instruments: the Toronto Empathy Questionnaire (TEQ), the Social Media Addiction Scale-Student Form (SMAS-SF), and the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20). A pathway analysis investigated alexithymia as a mediator between social media addiction and the degree of empathy in medical undergraduates. Sobel’s test and the Baron and Kenny approach were used for testing mediation.
Results: The TEQ total mean score was 48.76 ± 5.65, while the TAS-20 total mean score was 47.71 ± 11.49. Further analysis of the TAS-20 scale scores showed that 21.42% of students had possible alexithymia, while 14.02% had clear alexithymia. The SMAS-SF total mean score was 73.20 ± 14.59. None of the students reported levels consistent with major social media addiction. The mediated effect of the TAS-20 is about 1.3 times larger than the direct effect of the SMAS-SF on TEQ.
Discussion: We found a significant negative correlation between empathy and alexithymia in medical students. Alexithymia was a mediator between social media addiction and empathy. Therefore, we recommend further efforts to identify potential levels of alexithymia in medical students, in order to successfully develop tailored interventions aimed at increasing their emotional awareness.
1 Introduction
Alexithymia represents an inability to describe one’s emotional states. Several studies point out that alexithymia disrupts the ability to identify feelings of others (1). From an etymological point of view, the word comes from Greek: a = lack, lexis = word, and thymos = mood or emotion. Thus, it can be translated as the inability to read and express emotions. The term was coined during the 1970s by two psycho-therapists (Peter E. Sifneos and John C. Nemiah), looking to summarize symptoms they had noticed in their patients suffering from psychosomatic illnesses (2–4). More recently, there has been a growing scientific interest in the way alexithymia affects interpersonal relationships, with some authors hypothesizing that alexithymic individuals have difficulty interacting with others, since alexithymia correlates with emotional skills-related problems, such as difficulties in building and maintaining interpersonal relationships, smaller social networks, and reduced social skills (5, 6).
Since alexithymia is frequently linked to depressive and anxious states of mind, it is likely that people with alexithymia have trouble controlling their negative emotions (1, 7). Alexithymia along with high impulsivity, has been correlated with problematic internet use, consequently raising the theory that people who struggle to identify and communicate their emotions are more inclined to use online video games to escape negative feelings (8). Social media has increased over the past years and has been proven to be used as a maladaptative coping mechanism (9). The use of emotion- and avoidance-oriented coping strategies is favorably correlated with alexithymia, as assessed by the TAS-20 in teenage samples, and negatively correlated with task-oriented coping strategies (10). Di Blasi et al. view the emotional challenges associated with impulsivity and alexithymic features as specific elements that constitute an emotion dysregulation process (11). In alexithymic individuals, restricted and intolerant attitudes toward their own short-comings and limitations, as well as a failure to recognize themselves as a part of the larger human condition, go alongside a constricted awareness of emotions in self and others (12). Therefore, maladaptive coping mechanisms, such as drinking and negative emotional states, like despair, anxiety, and stress can be frequently linked to alexithymia (13).
The prevalence of alexithymia in the general population is estimated to be around 10% (14–16). Some researchers established that this deficiency exists in both healthy and unhealthy individuals (17), characterized by difficulties in identifying, analyzing, and ex-pressing emotions, as well as involving certain restrictions, in terms of externally oriented thinking and imagination. A 5-year follow-up study in the general population has indicated that alexithymia can essentially be considered a stable personality trait (18). There are different theories regarding the emergence of alexithymia, with some studies showing that childhood trauma is a substantial contributing element (19). Some authors suggest that childhood trauma, including emotional abuse and severe emotional and physical neglect, can predict the emergence of alexithymia in later life (20).
According to the research examining the relationship between alexithymia, social media use, and smartphone addiction, there was a strong correlation between alexithymia and the severity of smartphone use (21).
The ability to identify and control one’s own emotions as well as those of others is known as emotional intelligence (EI). On the other hand, empathy is the capacity to comprehend the feelings of others, and alexithymia is the inability to feel and communicate emotions verbally. Emotional intelligence is a crucial prerequisite for success in the medical field (22).
Empathy has a multidimensional character, comprising cognitive and emotional dimensions. It includes the ability to perceive the perspective of others, correctly identify their subjective reality, and have appropriate affective responses that follow the perception of the emotional states of those around us (23).
Empathy has two components: affective, which comprises the individuals’ capacity to feel what others feel; cognitive, which is the ability to identify, interpret and understand the mental states of others, which involves perspective-taking (24, 25). Empathy helps create and maintain these social connections, allowing people to understand, share and respond accordingly to other people emotional states (26).
In healthcare, the professional-patient relationship is primarily based on empathy, as this can help build trust and improve communication, thus creating a safe environment to explore existing possibilities and make the best medical decisions (27, 28). Alternatively, a healthcare professional showing high levels of empathy has been shown to help reduce patients’ stress levels, as well as their anxiety and depression, as well as improve their prognosis (29, 30).
As medical professionals on the frontline of dealing and acting immediately and without fail for the most vulnerable, doctors must be highly specialized. On the path to-ward developing their medical career, medical students must spend significantly more time acquiring the required professional knowledge. Research suggests that this particular area of expertise involves significantly higher academic pressure than other disciplines (31). Keeping this in mind, it becomes somewhat inevitable to logically infer that, as a direct cause of these inherent and long-term pressures, medical students are at a high risk of developing mental health issues, such as burnout, anxiety or depression (32), as alexithymia is a recognized risk factor for these mental health difficulties, particularly in health science students (33). These facts indicate that alexithymia may negatively impact the health professional-patient relationship, starting early on and notable in medical students’ personal and professional lives (34).
In recent years, socializing via the Internet has become an increasingly integral part of young adults’ lives. Social networking sites are online communication tools that allow users to create a public or private profile to interact with others. Although they have helped people connect in new and innovative ways, several researchers have pointed out that excessive use of social networks has negative health consequences, including structural and functional changes in brain regions involved in emotional processing, attention, decision-making, and cognitive control.
Pertaining to our subject at hand, there are differing views regarding the correlation between social media use and empathy. Various studies have noted that empathy is negatively correlated to social media addiction (35), as, according to Dailey (36), empathetic individuals are less likely to develop a social media addiction.
However, certain studies have suggested that this is an indirect connection (37, 38), some authors have demonstrated the opposite (39, 40), while others have highlighted mostly a neutral relationship between the two (41).
This study aimed to explore if high values of alexithymia in medical students correlate with low values of empathy, and to identify any potential connections with social media addiction. We hypothesized that alexithymia mediates the relationship between social media addiction and the degree of empathy in undergraduate medical students, all of which are elements that may influence future medical careers, since these factors influence how we relate to those around us.
2 Materials and methods
The study population consisted of 649 medical students in their 4th, 5th, or 6th year of studies, recruited between March and May 2021. The sample is part of one designed for a survey with larger scope, aiming to identify the relationship between the use of social networking and individual Theory of Mind (ToM), empathy and alexithymia levels in undergraduate students.
2.1 Socio-demographic features of participants
The mean age of participating students was 23.45 ± 1.30 years. Of all the participants, 79.66% were female, 38.83% were fourth‐year students, 26.35% were fifth‐year students, and 34.82% were sixth‐year students. The socio-demographic characteristics of the sample are shown in Table 1
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the “Victor Babes” University of Medicine and Pharmacy of Timisoara, Romania (protocol No. 15/20.03.2020).
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
2.2 Instruments
We used several instruments for this assessment. The Toronto Empathy Questionnaire (TEQ) was priorly validated in Romanian (42) and measures the empathy level as a uni-dimensional instrument, consisting of 16 items, with a 5-point Likert scale-type answer: never, rarely, sometimes, often, always, and with scores ranging from 0 to 4. Total scores range from a minimum of 16 to 64 points. The validation study for TEQ in Romanian (42) reported a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.727 and an ICC of 0.776. The validation of TEQ was conducted using the same sample as in the current study.
The Social Media Addiction Scale-Student Form (SMAS-SF) was developed by Sahin et al. (43) and was previously validated in Romanian (44). The questionnaire consists of 29 items with a 5-point Likert scale, from “totally disagree” (1 point), “disagree” (2 points), “neither agree nor disagree” (3 points), “agree” (4 points), “totally agree” (5 points). The total score ranges from a minimum of 29 to a maximum of 145. The total score can be interpreted as: no addiction (≤ 58 points), mild addiction (59-87 points), moderate addiction (88-116 points), and severe addiction (≥ 117 points). The validation of the SMAS-SF in Romanian (44) demonstrated a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.817 and an ICC of 0.829 and used the same sample as in the current study.
The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) was developed by Bagby et al. (45) and consists of 20 items measuring the difficulty in identifying and describing emotions. Alexithymia, as measured by this instrument, is characterized by three factors. The first, entitled “difficulties in identifying feelings” (DIF), and the second factor, entitled “difficulties in describing feelings” (DDF), refer to emotional awareness and expression. They might, therefore, be considered as “affect-related”. The third factor, entitled “externally-oriented thinking” (EOT), refers to a specific tendency to deal with simple themes and to avoid affective thinking. Possible answers are quantified on a 5-point Likert scale, from “strongly disagree” (1 point), “disagree” (2 points), “neither agree nor disagree” (3 points), “agree” (4 points), “strongly agree” (5 points). We employed the recently validated Romanian version (46) for this study. Total scores ranged from 20 points, as a minimum, to 100 points as a maximum. Scores were classified as non-alexithymia (≤51 points), possible alexithymia (52‐60 points), and alexithymia (≥61 points) (47). The validation study in Romanian (Morariu, 2013) reported a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.83. For the TAS-20, the Cronbach’s alpha in the current sample was 0.749.
Besides the specific questionnaires mentioned above, the survey also included demographic questions, such as gender, age, year of study, and final average grade in the previous academic year.
The survey was hosted on a platform and could be accessed using a Google Play application (android and iOS) or a desktop version (https://timsonet.ro). Students could access the survey using a series of alphanumeric codes, randomly generated to assure anonymity.
The final database was imported to the Stata program version 16.1 (StataCorp, College Station, Texas, USA). The categorical variables are represented as absolute and relative frequencies, and continuous variables are presented as mean and standard deviation (SD). A p-value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. Since the data were not normally distributed, we used the Mann-Whitney test to test for differences between males and females for total scores. The degree of correlation between different questionnaires was test-ed with the Pearson point product correlation. Using structural equation modeling, we created a pathway analysis investigating alexithymia as a mediator between social media addiction and the degree of empathy in medical undergraduates.
The mediation model was analyzed using sgmediation2 command in Stata. For the best test of mediation effect, the bootstrapping procedure to measure indirect effect was carried out and 95% confidence intervals were estimated. The number of bootstrap samples was 5000.
3 Results
3.1 The participants’ scales’ mean scores
The study’s findings showed that the TEQ total mean score was 48.76 ± 5.65, with scores ranging from 18 to 63. The TAS total mean score was 47.71 ± 11.49, with scores ranging from 23 to 83 (Table 2). Further analysis of the TAS scale scores showed that 21.42% of the students had possible alexithymia, and 14.02% had clear alexithymia. The SMAS-SF total mean score was 73.20 ± 14.59, with scores ranging from 31 to 115. A detailed analysis of the SMAS-SF scale scores indicated that as much as 67.18% of the students had a mild addiction, and 15.72% had a moderate addiction. None of the students presented levels consistent with major social media addiction. The use of the SMAS-SF scale allowed the study group to be divided. Those who fell into the no addiction group were considered to be social media users.
Table 2. The participants’ Toronto Empathy Questionnaire, Toronto Alexithymia Scale, and SMAS-SF scores (N = 649).
3.2 Distribution of the scale mean scores according to gender
The students’ TEQ total mean score was higher in females (P<0.001). The difference between mean scores for the TAS total was statistically insignificant between males and females. However, the DIF and EOT factors recorded statistically significant differences between the genders. The SMAS-SF total mean score was also higher in females (P=0.001). All data are presented in Table 3.
3.3 Correlations
The DDF and EOT were negatively correlated with the TEQ; the DIF presented a much lower significant negative correlation with the TEQ. The DIF and DDF scores were positively correlated with the SMAS-SF scores, and there was no significant correlation between the TEQ and the SMAS-SF. All correlations are presented in Table 4.
Table 4. Pearson-moment product correlations between alexithymia, empathy, and social media addiction scores.
3.4 Path analysis model
Following the correlation analyses results, we performed mediation analyses to further examine the association between social media addiction, alexithymia and the degree of empathy in undergraduate medical students.
In Model 1, social media addiction was not significantly associated with empathy (path c)(β= -0.0068, P =0.652). In Model 2, social media addiction had a significant relationship with alexithymia (path a)(β=0.254, P < 0.001). In Model 3, both social media addiction and alexithymia were included in the mediation model and showed a significant relationship with empathy. Simultaneously, the standardized regression coefficient (β) for social media addiction decreased from -0.007 to 0.027. Moreover, the results of the non-parametric bootstrapping method confirmed the significance of the indirect effect of social media addiction through alexithymia (95% bootstrap CI=-0.047, -0.021). A bootstrapped 95% confidence interval (CI) confirmed that the indirect effect of social media addiction had an impact of -0.034 which was produced by alexithymia as a mediator on empathy (Table 5). These findings corroborate our hypothesis that alexithymia may play a mediator role in the association between social media addiction and empathy. Figure 1 illustrates the mediation model, along with standardized path coefficients.
Figure 1. Path diagram showing alexithymia as a mediator between social media addiction and the degree of empathy in undergraduate medical students. Numbers on the single-headed arrows represent standardized regression coefficients (Beta).
4 Discussions
In our daily life, and to function optimally as medical professionals, we employ instinct and intuition, alongside reasoning and logical deductions. In this complex inter-play, empathy plays an essential role, found at the core of the therapeutic relationship, and the essence of the doctor-patient relationship. Being able to place oneself in someone else’s place can be challenging, especially when the other is suffering. However, instrumental, as it is the only way to more precisely try to understand what that the other is going through.
The present study examined the relationships between alexithymia, social media addiction, and empathy using validated self-administered questionnaires for medical students. People with alexithymia experience difficulties understanding their feelings, as well as those of others, which could result in limitations in the empathic abilities of alexithymia individuals. On the other hand, in the workplace, they also experience difficulties in being able to socialize with colleagues, as they seem inexpressive, aloof, which, in turn, leads to a high probability of becoming unemployed (48).
There are several studies that suggest alexithymia plays an important role in the etiology of addictive behaviors (49–51). Social media is what facilitates interpersonal relationships, which can lead to addiction in people with a high level of alexithymia.
Both empathy and alexithymia are necessary qualities for someone looking to pursue a future medical career. When considering the question of the reasoning behind that, the most likely answer is that empathy allows the building of relationships, and facilitates understanding what patients think, thus making it easier for healthcare workers to respond appropriately. Empathy allows us to manage what we feel, even when faced with stressful situations, without being overwhelmed (52). Moreover, empathy is what allows us to offer unconditional and disinterested help to another.
In our study, the total TEQ score was 48.76 ± 5.65, which is slightly higher than those reported in the literature. Differences between women and men were also significant, favoring the former. A recent study carried out in 57 countries, in 2022, reached the same conclusions, namely that women, regardless of their age and country of origin, scored higher than men in the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test, used on a broad scale, to measure the degree of “cognitive empathy” (53).
In this study, the prevalence of medical students with alexithymia was of 14.01%, slightly higher than that reported in the general population (13, 53), but lower than those reported in other student populations (54, 55). For someone with alexithymia, understanding one’s own emotional issues is problematic, which is even more difficult when dealing with someone else. An alexithymic health professional will struggle in their interactions, ultimately negatively impact the therapeutic alliance and treatment they offer others (56). Therefore, we suggest that medical students and health professionals in general should be familiar with the concept of alexithymia and its significance in their own personal and professional lives (57, 58).
Among other results, we also found a negative, and significant correlation between the TEQ Scale and the mean TAS-20 scores. Current research supports a cerebral connection between alexithymia and empathy, with the connection established at the level of the insular cortex, where the processing of internal affective states is carried out. The anterior cingulate cortex is associated with emotion processing and social rewards, and a reduced activity in this cerebral area may be associated with alexithymia (59). In this view, those with alexithymic traits will tend to have less empathy. Consequently, young people undergoing healthcare education should determine their own capacity for empathy, and those who struggle in this area should receive specific psycho-education on empathy (60).
In our study, we chose to position the personality trait as a mediator rather than a predictor due to specific theoretical considerations. While personality traits are generally considered stable and develop early in life, recent research suggests that they can also be influenced and shaped by environmental factors and behaviors acquired later in life, such as social media use. For instance, there is evidence that frequent engagement in certain behaviors can reinforce or even modify certain personality traits over time, suggesting a bidirectional relationship (61, 62). Valdespino et al. (63) have explored the temporal relationship between alexithymia and empathy, demonstrating that alexithymia serves as a precursor to empathy abnormalities. Therefore, in our theoretical model, social media use is posited as an influential factor that could shape personality traits, thereby justifying its role as a predictor.
Our path analysis model showed that alexithymia is an essential mediator between social media addiction and empathy. Empathy implies one’s capacity to adopt and understand another’s experiences and emotions. Secondary to their difficulty in identifying and describing emotions, people with alexithymia might find it strenuous to imagine and perceive the emotional experiences of others. In terms of virtual communication, social media rarely offers non-verbal clues, such as facial expressions, body language and tone of voice, essential elements on which communication skills are built and how we, as a species, have learned to understand emotional states in others. Also, social media usually offers curated, unrealistic portrayals of life, as well as acts of aggression, which some-times surmount current constrains and policies, subsequently being responsible for traumatic effects on their consumers. Thus, this confirms studies showing that maltreatment and trauma may be a contributing factor in alexithymia (64–66).
For people with alexithymia, who struggle on a daily-basis to accurately interpret said clues, the interposition of social media raises their inaccurate interpretation of other people’s emotions, and lowers their empathy. Increasing the overall time spent on social media might impair empathic responses and reduce the quality of social interactions. According to certain clinical studies, people suffering from social media addiction also have lower empathy levels (67, 68), adding to the well-established theory that empathy correlates with social relationships (69).
Although the present study shows that alexithymia can mediate the relationship between social media use and empathy, it is not the major determining factor, since there are specific individual coping mechanisms, and characteristic personality traits, which can also influence the dynamics of this relationship.
The present research has several limitations, the main one being that it was con-ducted in a single location, on a population with shared cultural traits. Although alexithymia is a mediator of empathy, only about 11% of the variability in empathy is explained by this model. Since the SRMR was zero, other factors should be considered in the future. The study was performed on medical students, so conclusions cannot be extended to the general population. However, the number of participants in our study was large enough to consider the results as reliable. For future studies, objective measures and investigator ratings should be added to the assessment of the connections between alexithymia and empathy.
A significant limitation of this study is the use of cross-sectional data for mediation analysis. While we identified a potential mediating role of alexithymia in the relationship between social media addiction and empathy, it is important to acknowledge that cross-sectional designs limit our ability to infer causality or the temporal order of these relationships. Longitudinal data would be necessary to confirm these mediation effects over time and to establish a clearer causal pathway.
5 Conclusions
According to our best knowledge, this is the first paper that analyzes alexithymia as a mediating factor between social media addiction and empathy. Our results indicated a significant negative correlation between empathy and alexithymia in medical students. Alexithymia was a mediator between social media addiction and empathy. Therefore, we recommend timely and specific efforts to identify levels of alexithymia in more medical students, which can lead to the design of tailored interventions aimed at increasing emotional awareness and aptitudes.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Ethics statement
Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.
Author contributions
SU: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft. A-CB: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. CS: Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. IR: Investigation, Writing – original draft. AB: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. IP: Investigation, Writing – original draft. CG-O: Investigation, Writing – original draft.
Funding
The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was supported by an internal grant of Victor Babes University of Medicine and Pharmacy Timisoara, contract number 3EXP/1219/30.01.2020.
Acknowledgments
We would like to acknowledge Victor Babes University of Medicine and Pharmacy Timisoara for their support in covering the costs of publication for this research paper.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Lyvers M, Kohlsdorf S. M, Edwards MS, Thorberg FA. Alexithymia and mood: recognition of emotion in self and others. Am J Psychol 1 April. (2017) 130:83–92. doi: 10.5406/amerjpsyc.130.1.0083
2. Apfel RJ, Sifneos PE. Alexithymia: concept and measurement. Psychother Psychosom. (1979) 32:180–90. doi: 10.1159/000287386
3. Nemiah JC, Sifneos PE. Psychosomatic illness: a problem in communication. Psychother Psychosom. (1970) 18:154–60. doi: 10.1159/000286074
4. Sifneos PE. The prevalence of 'alexithymic' characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom. (1973) 22:255–62. doi: 10.1159/000286529
5. Timoney LR, Holder MD. Correlates of alexithymia. In: Timoney LR, Holder MD, editors. Emotional Processing Deficits and Happiness. Dordrecht Heidelberg New York London: Springer (2013). p. 41–60.
6. Luminet O, Nielson KA, Ridout N. Cognitive-emotional processing in alexithymia: an integrative review. Cognit Emot. May. (2021) 35:449–87. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2021.1908231
7. Foran HM, O’Leary KD. The role of relationships in understanding the alexithymia–depression link. Eur J Pers. (2013) 27:470–80. doi: 10.1002/per.1887
8. Maganuco NR, Costanzo A, Midolo LR, Santoro G, Schimmenti A. Impulsivity and alexithymia in virtual worlds: A study on players of world of warcraft. Clin Neuropsychiatry. (2019) 16:127–34.
9. Ahmed S, Dixon MJ. Instagram, Depression, and Dark Flow – Using Social Media as a Maladaptive Coping Mechanism. New York: Elsevier (2023). doi: 10.2139/ssrn.4391734.
10. Talebi Joybari M. Depression and interpersonal problems in adolescents: their relationship with alexithymia and coping styles. Iran J Psychiatry Behav Sci. (2014) 8:38–45.
11. Blasi MD, Giardina A, Giordano C, Coco GL, Tosto C, Billieux J, et al. Problematic video game use as an emotional coping strategy: Evidence from a sample of MMORPG gamers. J Behav Addict. (2019) 8:25–34. doi: 10.1556/2006.8.2019.02
12. Preece DA, Becerra R, Robinson K, Allan A, Boyes M, Chen W, et al. What is alexithymia? Using factor analysis to establish its latent structure and rela-tionship with fantasizing and emotional reactivity. J Pers. (2020) 88:1162–76. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12563
13. Lyvers M, Randhawa A, Thorberg FA. Self-compassion in relation to alexithymia, empathy, and negative mood in young adults. Mindfulness. (2020) 11:1655–65. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01379-6
14. Schroeders U, Kubera F, Gnambs T. The structure of the Toronto alexithymia scale (TAS-20): A meta-analytic confirmato-ry factor analysis. Assessment. Dec. (2022) 29:1806–23. doi: 10.1177/10731911211033894
15. Franz M, Popp K, Schaefer R, Sitte W, Schneider C, Hardt J, et al. Alexithymia in the German general population. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epi-demiology. (2008) 43:54–62. doi: 10.1007/s00127-007-0265-1
16. Mattila AK, Kronholm E, Jula A, Salminen JK, Koivisto AM, Mielonen RL, et al. Alexithymia and somatization in general population. Psychosomatic Med. (2008) 70:716–22. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31816ffc39
17. Lane RD, Sechrest L, Riedel R. Sociodemographic correlates of alexithymia. Compr Psychiatry. (1998) 39:377–85. doi: 10.1016/S0010-440X(98)90051-7
18. Salminen JK, Saarijärvi S, Toikka T, Kauhanen J, Aärelä E. Alexithymia behaves as a personality trait over a 5-year period in Finnish general population. J Psychosom Res. (2006) 61:275–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.01.014
19. Güleç MY, Altintaş M, İnanç L, Bezgin CH, Koca EK, Güleç H. Effects of childhood trauma on somatization in major depres-sive disorder: The role of alexithymia. J Affect Disord. (2013) 146:137–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.06.033
20. Zlotnick C, Mattia JI, Zimmerman M. The relationship between posttraumatic stress disorder, childhood trauma and alexi-thymia in an outpatient sample. J Traumatic Stress. (2001) 14:177–88. doi: 10.1023/A:1007899918410
21. Gündoğmuş İ, Aydın MS, Algül A. The relationship of smartphone addiction and alexithymia. Psychiatry Investig. (2021) 18:841–9. doi: 10.30773/pi.2021.0072
22. Di Lorenzo R, Venturelli G, Spiga G, Ferri P. Emotional intelligence, empathy and alexithymia: a cross-sectional survey on emotional competence in a group of nursing students. Acta BioMed. (2019) 90:32–43. doi: 10.23750/abm.v90i4-S.8273
23. Jackson PL, Meltzoff AN, Decety J. How do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy. Neuroimage. (2005) 24:771–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.006
24. Decety J, Jackson PL. A social-neuroscience perspective on empathy. Curr Dir psychol Science. (2006) 15:54–8. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00406.x
26. Decety J, Jackson PL. The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav Cognit Neurosci Rev. (2004) 3:71–100. doi: 10.1177/1534582304267187
27. Ong LML, de Haes JCJM, Hoos AM, Lammes FB. Doctor-patient communication: A review of the literature. Soc Sci Med. (1995) 40:903–18. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)00155-M
28. Stewart M, Brown JB, Boon H, Galajda J, Meredith L, Sangster M. Evidence on patient-doctor communication. Cancer Prev Control. (1999) 3:25–30.
29. Lorié Á, Reinero DA, Phillips M, Zhang L, Riess H. Culture and nonverbal expressions of empathy in clinical settings: A systematic review. Patient Educ Counseling. (2017) 100:411–24. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2016.09.018
30. Hemmerdinger JM, Stoddart SDR, Lilford RJ. A systematic review of tests of empathy in medicine. BMC Med Education. (2007) 7:24. doi: 10.1186/1472-6920-7-24
31. Bond AR, Mason HF, Lemaster CM, Shaw SE, Mullin CS, Holick EA, et al. Embodied health: the effects of a mind-body course for medical students. Med Educ Online. (2013) 18:1–8. doi: 10.3402/meo.v18i0.20699
32. Popa-Velea O, Diaconescu L, Mihăilescu A, Jidveian Popescu M, Macarie G. Burnout and its relationships with alexithymia, stress, and social support among Romanian medical students: A cross-sectional study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2017) 14. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14060560
33. Zhang Y, Zhao Y, Mao S, Li G, Yuan Y. Investigation of health anxiety and its related factors in nursing students. Neuro-psychiatr Dis Treat. (2014) 10:1223–34. doi: 10.2147/ndt.S61568
34. Zhu Y, Luo T, Liu J, Qu B. Influencing factors of alexithymia in Chinese medical students: a cross-sectional study. BMC Med Educ. (2017) 17:66. doi: 10.1186/s12909-017-0901-8
35. Dalvi-Esfahani M, Niknafs A, Alaedini Z, Barati Ahmadabadi H, Kuss DJ, Ramayah T. Social Media Addiction and Empathy: Moderating impact of personality traits among high school students. Telematics Informatics. (2021) 57:101516. doi: 10.1016/j.tele.2020.101516
36. Dailey SL, Howard K, Roming SMP, Ceballos N, Grimes T. A biopsychosocial approach to understanding social media addiction. Hum Behav Emerging Technologies. (2020) 2:158–67. doi: 10.1002/hbe2.182
37. Alloway T, Runac R, Qureshi M, Kemp G. Is facebook linked to selfishness? Investigating the relationships among social media use, empathy, and narcissism. Soc Networking. (2014) 03:150–8. doi: 10.4236/sn.2014.33020
38. Carrier LM, Spradlin A, Bunce JP, Rosen LD. Virtual empathy: Positive and negative impacts of going online upon empathy in young adults. Comput Hum Behav. (2015) 52:39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.05.026
39. Errasti J, Amigo I, Villadangos M. Emotional uses of facebook and twitter: its relation with empathy, narcissism, and self-esteem in adolescence. Psychol Rep. (2017) 120:997–1018. doi: 10.1177/0033294117713496
40. Vossen HGM, Valkenburg PM. Do social media foster or curtail adolescents’ empathy? A longitudinal study. Comput Hum Behavior. (2016) 63:118–24. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.040
41. Sharma A, Miner AS, Atkins DC, Althoff T. A computational approach to understanding empathy expressed in text-based mental health support. arXiv pre-print server. (2020). doi: 10.18653/v1/2020.emnlp-main
42. Ursoniu S, Serban CL, Giurgi-Oncu C, Rivis IA, Bucur A, Bredicean AC, et al. Validation of the Romanian version of the toronto empathy questionnaire (TEQ) among undergraduate medical students. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182412871
43. Sahin C. Social media addiction scale - student form: the reliability and validity study. Turkish Online J Educ Technol. (2018) 17:169–82.
44. Ursoniu S, Serban CL, Giurgi-Oncu C, Rivis IA, Bucur A, Papava I, et al. Validation of the Romanian version of the social media addiction scale-student form (SMAS-SF) among undergraduate medical students. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2022) 18:1195–205. doi: 10.2147/ndt.S368476
45. Bagby RM, Parker JDA, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia scale—I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosomatic Res. (1994) 38:23–32. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1
46. Morariu RA, Ayearst LE, Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. Development and validation of a Romanian adaptation of the 20-item to-ronto alexithymia scale (TAS-20-RO). Rev Romana Psihiatrie. (2013) 15:155–9.
47. Bagby RM, Parker JDA, Taylor GJ. Twenty-five years with the 20-item Toronto alexithymia scale. J Psychosom Res. (2020) 131:109940. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2020.109940
48. Schumacker RE, Lomax RG. Path models. In: Schumacker RE, Lomax RG, editors. A Beginner's Guide to Structural Equation Modeling: Fourth Edition, 4th ed. New York: Routledge (2015). p. 69–84.
49. Pituch KA, Stevens JP. Structural equation modeling. In: Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences, 6th ed. New York: Routledge (2016). p. 639–726.
50. Arrabales Moreno Raúl. Evaluación y Tratamiento de la Alexitimia con Herramientas de Inteligencia Artificial (2019). Univer-sidad Internacional de la Rioja, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud. Available online at: https://www.conscious-robots.com/papers/TFM_MPGS_Arrabales_vWeb.pdf (Accessed 15 march 2024).
51. Mahapatra A, Sharma P. Association of Internet addiction and alexithymia - A scoping review. Addict Behav Jun. (2018) 81:175–82. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.02.004
52. Marchetti D, Verrocchio MC, Porcelli P. Gambling problems and alexithymia: A systematic review. Brain Sci. (2019) 9. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9080191
53. Terzioğlu MA, Uğurlu TT. Social media addiction in medical faculty students; the relationship with dissociation, social anxiety, and alexithymia. Pamukkale Med J. (2023) 16:580–92. doi: 10.31362/patd.1321281
54. Marzilli E, Cerniglia L, Cimino S, Tambelli R. Internet Addiction among Young Adult University Students during the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Role of Peritraumatic Distress, Attachment, and Alexithymia. Int J Environ-mental Res Public Health. (2022) 19:15582. doi: 10.3390/ijerph192315582
55. Greenberg DM, Warrier V, Abu-Akel A, Allison C, Gajos KZ, Reinecke K, et al. Sex and age differences in "theory of mind" across 57 countries using the Eng-lish version of the "Reading the Mind in the Eyes" Test. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2023) 120:e2022385119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2022385119
56. McGillivray L, Becerra R, Harms C. Prevalence and demographic correlates of alexithymia: A comparison between aus-tralian psychiatric and community samples. J Clin Psychol. (2017) 73:76–87. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22314
57. Aljaffer MA, Almadani AH, Alghamdi SA, Alabdulkarim IM, Albabtain MA, Altameem RM, et al. Prevalence and associated factors of alexithymia among medical students: A cross-sectional study from Saudi Arabia. Neurosci (Riyadh). (2022) 27:257–62. doi: 10.17712/nsj.2022.4.20220049
58. Hamaideh SH. Alexithymia among Jordanian university students: Its prevalence and correlates with depression, anxiety, stress, and demographics. Perspect Psychiatr Care. (2018) 54:274–80. doi: 10.1111/ppc.12234
59. Finset A. Emotional intelligence, alexithymia, and the doctor-patient relationship. In: Koh KB, editor. Somatization and Psy-chosomatic Symptoms. New York: Springer (2013). p. 91–8.
60. Faramarzi M, Khafri S. Role of alexithymia, anxiety, and depression in predicting self-efficacy in academic students. Sci-entificWorldJournal. (2017) 2017:5798372. doi: 10.1155/2017/5798372
61. Wood MA, Bukowski WM, Lis E. The digital self: how social media serves as a setting that shapes youth’s emotional experiences. Adolesc Res Rev. (2016) 1:163–73. doi: 10.1007/s40894-015-0014-8
62. Appel M, Marker C, Gnambs T. Are social media ruining our lives? A review of meta-analytic evidence. Rev Gen Psychol. (2020) 24:60–74. doi: 10.1177/1089268019880891
63. Valdespino A, Antezana L, Ghane M, Richey JA. Alexithymia as a transdiagnostic precursor to empathy abnormalities: the functional role of the insula. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:2234. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02234
64. Tolmunen T, Heliste M, Lehto SM, Hintikka J, Honkalampi K, Kauhanen J. Stability of alexithymia in the general popula-tion: an 11-year follow-up. Compr Psychiatry. (2011) 52:536–41. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.09.007
65. Aslan G, Bakan AB, Yildiz M. An investigation of the relationship between alexithymia and empathy tendency in university students receiving health education. Perspect Psychiatr Care. (2021) 57:709–16. doi: 10.1111/ppc.12602
66. Ditzer J, Wong EY, Modi RN, Behnke M, Gross JJ, Talmon A. Child maltreatment and alexithymia: A meta-analytic review. psychol Bull. (2023) 149:311–29. doi: 10.1037/bul0000391
67. Lachmann B, Sindermann C, Sariyska RY, Luo R, Melchers M, Becker B, et al. The role of empathy and life satisfaction in internet and smartphone use disorder. Original Res Front Psychol. (2018) 9:398. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00398
68. Jiao C, Wang T, Peng X, Cui F. Impaired empathy processing in individuals with internet addiction disorder: an event-related potential study. Original Res Front Hum Neurosci. (2017) 11:498. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00498
Keywords: social media addiction, medical students, empathy, smartphone addiction, alexithymia
Citation: Ursoniu S, Bredicean A-C, Serban CL, Rivis I, Bucur A, Papava I and Giurgi-Oncu C (2024) The interconnection between social media addiction, alexithymia and empathy in medical students. Front. Psychiatry 15:1467246. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1467246
Received: 19 July 2024; Accepted: 28 August 2024;
Published: 19 September 2024.
Edited by:
Chul-Hyun Cho, Korea University, Republic of KoreaReviewed by:
Anastasio Tini, Marche Polytechnic University, ItalyGeorgia Panayiotou, University of Cyprus, Cyprus
Copyright © 2024 Ursoniu, Bredicean, Serban, Rivis, Bucur, Papava and Giurgi-Oncu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Adina Bucur, bucur.adina@umft.ro
†ORCID: Sorin Ursoniu, orcid.org/0000-0001-9215-5151
Ana-Cristina Bredicean, orcid.org/0000-0003-0205-5497
Costela Lacrimioara Serban, orcid.org/0000-0003-4648-025X
Ioana Rivis, orcid.org/0000-0003-4453-1818
Adina Bucur, orcid.org/0000-0003-1606-4319
Ion Papava, orcid.org/0000-0003-0963-8222
Catalina Giurgi-Oncu, orcid.org/0000-0001-8962-0000