Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 26 November 2024
Sec. Psychopathology

Longitudinal predictors for internalizing and externalizing symptomatology at age 4: KUNO-Kids cohort study

Irina Jarvers*Irina Jarvers1*Stephanie KandspergerStephanie Kandsperger1Angelika EckerAngelika Ecker1Susanne Brandstetter,Susanne Brandstetter2,3Michael Kabesch,Michael Kabesch2,3Angela KningerAngela Köninger4Michael Melter,Michael Melter2,3Sebastian KerzelSebastian Kerzel2Jochen KittelJochen Kittel2Christian Apfelbacher,Christian Apfelbacher3,5Romuald BrunnerRomuald Brunner1 on behalf of KUNO-Kids study group
  • 1Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
  • 2University Children’s Hospital Regensburg (KUNO) at the Hospital St. Hedwig of the Order of St. John , University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
  • 3Research and Development, Wissenschafts- und Entwicklungs-Campus Regensburg (WECARE), Hospital St. Hedwig of the Order of St. John, Regensburg, Germany
  • 4Department of Gynecology and Obstetrics, Hospital St. Hedwig of the Order of St. John, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
  • 5Institute of Social Medicine and Health Systems Research, Otto von Guericke University Magdeburg, Magdeburg, Germany

Introduction: Numerous early-life risk factors are thought to significantly contribute to the development of psychological problems in toddlerhood. However, these factors have seldom been investigated concomitantly and longitudinally, and few studies include both mothers and fathers. This study examines the longitudinal impact of early environmental, parental, and child-specific risk factors on children’s internalizing and externalizing symptomatology at age 4.

Methods: Families were recruited from a perinatal center at birth and completed self-report questionnaires at birth, 4 weeks postpartum, 6 months postpartum, and annually thereafter. The final population-based sample consisted of n = 560 mothers (and fathers) who gave birth after June 2015, with children who turned 4 years old before March 31, 2021. The primary outcomes, children’s internalizing and externalizing symptomatology at age 4, were measured using the Strengthsand Difficulties Questionnaire. Linear mixed effect models were used to estimate growth curves for predictors between 4 weeks and 4 years postpartum, which were subsequently entered into multivariable linear regressions to predict internalizing and externalizing symptomatology at age 4.

Results: The study identified several key risk factors: environmental (lack of social support, lower parental education, male sex), parental (poor parental mental health, increased parenting stress, parental sleep difficulties) and child-specific (children’s low physical health, children’s reduced sleep quality, temperament).

Discussion: The findings underscore that most identified risk factors are related to children’s temperament, mental and physical health of parents, their experienced stress, and families’ social support networks. These insights highlight the importance of targeted interventions focusing on improving parental mental health, reducing stress, and enhancing social support to mitigate early-life psychological problems in children.

1 Introduction

Mental health disorders are among the leading causes of morbidity worldwide, with a global prevalence of 10.7% for any disorder (1). These issues often begin in childhood and adolescence (2), and can predict mental health disorders in adulthood (35). Mental health problems prior to diagnosed disorders, including internalizing (directed towards the self, e.g., anxiety, depression) and externalizing (directed towards the outside, e.g., aggression, hyperactivity) symptomatology, in early childhood can significantly contribute to mental health problems 14 and 16 years later (6, 7). This emphasizes the importance of early screenings, interventions, and the identification of risk factors contributing to internalizing and externalizing symptomatology in preschool children.

The dual-factor model of mental health posits that mental health is characterized by two separate but related dimensions: the presence of positive mental health and the absence of mental illness (8). This model emphasizes that individuals can experience a spectrum of well-being, even while suffering from mental health disorders. Consequently, when examining factors influencing mental health, it is essential to consider both those that increase the likelihood of developing mental health issues and those that offer protective benefits. In their systematic review, Carneiro et al. (9) identified three categories of risk factors for internalizing and externalizing symptoms in children aged 3 to 6 years: environmental, parental, and child-specific factors. Environmental factors include low socio-economic status (SES) and low parental education, both of which are linked to additional stressors like financial struggles (9). Parental factors encompass substance abuse and mental health issues, which can hinder a parent’s ability to attend to their child’s needs (10). Child-specific factors included male sex and temperament traits such as inhibitory control (1114). Boys are often noted to have reduced deliberate control of attention compared to girls, making male sex a relevant risk factor (15). Additionally, temperament traits can influence behavior, leading to difficulties in inhibiting actions, reduced sociability, or increased shyness in children (16, 17). However, while these risk factors have been examined individually across various samples, the complex interplay among them remains poorly understood. It is crucial to also consider protective factors—such as supportive social networks—that can mitigate the negative impacts of these risk factors. By exploring both sides of the equation, it is possible to develop a more holistic understanding of child mental health.

In addition to meta-analyses, individual studies have highlighted additional risk factors, including parenting stress (18), parental physical health (19), and reduced quality of maternal bonding (20). Parenting stress and physical health problems can diminish the resources available for caring for children (18, 19), similar to the effects of mental health problems. Maternal bonding is considered a critical first step for fostering healthy attachment, as attachment problems are commonly linked to increased rates of psychopathology in children (21, 22). Lastly, social support is considered vital for parents, especially those with high SES and lower risk (23). It is believed to enhance the overall environment for parents, enabling them to seek help during periods of increased parental stress or mental health challenges (24, 25). However, most of these investigations have been conducted cross-sectionally and considered only a few known risk-factors. A comprehensive longitudinal study that examines these factors simultaneously would provide valuable insights.

In addition to postnatal risk factors, prenatal factors such as prenatal stress (26), toxicant exposure, and maternal infections (27) have been implicated. These prenatal factors are hypothesized to contribute to changes in brain development, which can subsequently impact children’s mental health and behavioral problems (27, 28). While the evidence linking these prenatal factors to child development is expanding, further research is essential to elucidate the specific mechanisms involved and how these influences interact with postnatal factors to shape mental health trajectories. It is particularly important to examine whether these prenatal influences continue to have predictive value as children age.

Sleep quality is another risk factor affecting both parents and children. Associations have been identified between sleep difficulties and mental health problems in children (29) and between sleep difficulties in mothers and parenting stress (30, 31). Additionally, extensive crying before the age of 1 due to abdominal pain, defined as colic, has shown mixed results regarding its impact on externalizing symptoms (32). Despite these associations, there is a notable research gap regarding the mechanisms through which sleep quality influences child development and whether it remains an influencing factor when considering associated risk factors, for example parental stress.

While numerous risk factors influencing children’s mental health have been examined in isolation, it is crucial to study them in conjunction to understand their potential interactions and collinearity. For instance, the interplay between prenatal and postnatal factors, including maternal sleep quality, stress, and environmental influences, can significantly shape developmental trajectories. Additionally, protective factors, including supportive parenting practices, strong social networks, and individual resilience traits, may mitigate the negative impacts of risk factors (33). Understanding how these protective elements interact with risk factors can provide a more comprehensive view of child mental health. The dynamic nature of these influences underscores the need for longitudinal studies, which are essential for exploring these complex relationships over time. Such studies can capture how risk and protective factors interact and change as children develop, revealing how they collectively affect mental health outcomes.

Moreover, the existing literature predominantly focuses on maternal influences, raising questions about the inclusion of paternal factors in research on children’s mental health. Recent meta-analyses suggest that father-specific aspects, such as attachment, also play a significant role in predicting children’s internalizing and externalizing symptoms (34, 35). This highlights an important gap in current research: the need to integrate paternal perspectives and experiences, alongside maternal influences, to fully understand the dynamics of child development and mental health. Addressing this gap will enhance our understanding of the multifaceted nature of these risk factors and inform more effective interventions.

To address this research gap, the present study utilized data from the KUNO-Kids health study (36), a German birth cohort, to investigate a range of environmental, parental (both maternal and paternal), and child-specific risk and protective factors for internalizing and externalizing problems at age 4. By adopting this comprehensive approach, the study aims to shed light on the intricate interactions among these factors, ultimately contributing to a deeper understanding of their collective impact on child mental health.

2 Methods

2.1 Study setting

The present study was conducted within the scope of the KUNO-Kids birth cohort study (36), which began recruitment in June 2015. The KUNO-Kids birth cohort study has three main objectives: (a) to enhance understanding of child health using novel omics technologies and a systems medicine approach, (b) to identify new modifiable factors affecting child health and opportunities for prevention, and (c) to provide a platform for evaluating the feasibility and effectiveness of targeted interventions. After childbirth, mothers were informed about the study’s objectives and procedures. Upon providing written, informed consent, they were included in the baseline assessment and subsequently contacted at 4 weeks (T1), 6 months (T2), and annually postpartum (T3-T6). The initial assessment after childbirth included an interview and a self-report questionnaire. All subsequent assessments were self-report postal questionnaires inquiring about the mother, the father, and the child (see Figure 1 for an overview).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Overview of measures over time. * PHQ and GAD-7 were not filled out at T5. T2 was only filled out by mothers. PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; FSozU, Social Support Questionnaire; GAD-7, Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale; PHQ, Patient Health Questionnaire; EBI, Parenting Stress Index; CSHQ, Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire; SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; Physical Health, general health assessment; Colics, assessment of colic at 4 and 6 months of age; EAS, Emotionality-Activity-Sociability Temperament Inventory.

2.2 Ethical considerations

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2013. All procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Regensburg (file numbers: 14-101-0347, 19-1646-101).

2.3 Participants

Participants were mothers who gave birth at St. Hedwig hospital in Eastern Bavaria (Regensburg, Germany) and their families. To be eligible for participation, mothers had to be at least 18 years old and possess adequate German comprehension to provide informed consent. Only one child per family was included in the study. A total of 1,528 families consented to participate between June 2015 and March 2021. At 4 years postpartum, measures for internalizing and externalizing symptomatology, as well as child temperament were introduced, resulting in a final sample of 560 families (36.6%). Attrition bias due to loss to follow-up is common in longitudinal studies and can lead to over- or underestimation of effects, especially with differential loss. In the KUNO-Kids study, a substantial percentage of families were lost between childbirth and the 1-year follow-up (36). Over time, the study increasingly overrepresents well-educated families, leading to greater selection bias. While this bias may not affect all exposure-outcome associations, it is particularly relevant where differential loss to follow-up occurs.

2.4 Measures

Figure 1 provides an overview of the concepts and respective measures utilized in this study. The primary outcome measure was children’s externalizing and internalizing symptomatology at age 4.

2.4.1 Environmental factors

Several environmental factors were assessed, including parental education, at-risk pregnancies (yes/no), antibiotics during pregnancy (yes/no) and premature birth (yes/no). Education was assessed via three categories, <10 years of education (scored as 1), 10-12 years of education (scored as 2) and a-levels and higher (scored as 3). Additional environmental risk factors, such as prenatal infections (n = 2), were excluded due to insufficient case numbers.

Mothers also completed the short form of the Fragebogen zur Sozialen Unterstützung (F-SozU) (37), a German measure of perceived social support. The F-SozU consists of 14 items, with scores ranging from 14 to 70, where higher values indicate a stronger social support network. Psychometric properties of the F-SozU were shown to be satisfactory in general population and clinical samples (38). In the current sample the F-SozU showed a reliability of Cronbach’s α = .96.

2.4.2 Parental factors

Parental overall health was assessed via a single question asking parents to rate their health on a visual analogue scale (VAS) from 0 (worst possible health state) to 100 (best possible health state).

Parental sleep difficulties were assessed using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) (39), a sleep report questionnaire measuring sleep quality over the past month. The PSQI score ranges from 0 to 21, with values below 6 indicating good sleep quality. Excellent psychometric properties have been established for both, samples in the Unites States (40), as well as Germany (41). The PSQI was administered starting at T3. In the current sample the PSQI showed a reliability of Cronbach’s α = .79.

Anxious and depressive symptomatology was measured using the Generalized Anxiety Disorder 7 (GAD-7) (42) and the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ) (43). The GAD-7 consists of 7 items with values between 0-4 corresponding to no or low, 5-9 to mild, 10-14 to moderate and 15-21 to severe anxiety symptoms. Psychometric properties including measurement invariance of the GAD-7 were confirmed to be excellent in a large German general population sample (44).The PHQ-9 consists of 9 items, with scores of 0-4 indicating none or minimal depression, 5-9 indicating mild depression, 10-14 indicating moderate depression, 15-19 indicating moderately severe depression, and 20-27 indicating severe depression. Reliability and validity of the German version of the PHQ-9 has been confirmed to be excellent in a large general population sample (45). Both questionnaires were administered at T1, T3, T4 and T6 for both parents and additionally at T2 for mothers. In the current sample the GAD-7 showed a reliability of Cronbach’s α = .86 and the PHQ-9 a reliability of Cronbach’s α = .91.

Parenting stress was assessed using the Eltern-Belastungs-Inventar (EBI) (46), the German version of the Parenting Stress Index (PSI) (47). Administered scales for both mothers and fathers included personal burden (PB), perceived lack of parental competence (PC), and attachment problems (AT). Internal consistency of the EBI was reported to be.95 and its construct validity could be confirmed (46). The questionnaire scores range from 4 to 20 points, with higher scores indicating greater parental stress. In the current sample the EBI showed a reliability of Cronbach’s α = .90.

2.4.3 Child-specific factors

Children’s sleep difficulties were assessed using the German version of the Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ) (48), starting at T3. The CSHQ has 45 items, with 33 contributing to a total score. The clinical cut-off is set at 41, with scores above this threshold indicating clinically relevant sleep problems (49). Psychometric properties of the German version have been confirmed in community, as well as clinical samples with sleep disorders (48). In the current sample the CSHQ showed a reliability of Cronbach’s α = .85.

Internalizing and externalizing symptomatology were assessed using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire 4-17 (SDQ) (50), which can compute both internalizing and externalizing scales. Scores can be categorized as slightly above average (80th-90th centile), high (90th-95th centile) and very high (<95th centile) (51). According to cut-offs introduced by Goodman et al. (51), scores of 7 are considered at-risk, and scores of 9 are critical. The psychometric properties of the German parent-report version of the SDQ have been reported as good (52). In our current sample, the SDQ demonstrated a total scale reliability of Cronbach’s α = .63, with reliabilities of Cronbach’s α = .67 for the internalizing scale and Cronbach’s α = .79 for the externalizing scale.

Child temperament was assessed using the German version of the Emotionality-Activity-Sociability Temperament Inventory (EAS) (53). The EAS includes 20 items across four scales: emotionality, activity, shyness, and sociability. Each item is scored from 1 to 5, with higher scores indicating greater agreement with the corresponding temperament dimension. The psychometric properties of the German EAS were previously validated in a sample of twins aged 2 to 14 years (54). In our current sample, the EAS demonstrated a total scale reliability of Cronbach’s α = .58, with individual scale reliabilities of α = .77 for emotionality, α = .70 for activity, α = .80 for shyness, and α = .61 for sociability. The SDQ and EAS were only administered at T6, with the SDQ serving as the outcome measure. Additionally, a variable differentiated between children who never showed colic and those who exhibited colic at 4 weeks and/or 6 months of age.

2.5 Statistical analysis

Data preparation and computation of growth curves were performed using the R statistical package, version 4.2.3 (55). Descriptive statistics, multiple imputation, and multivariable regression models were computed using SPSS 28 (56). The sample size was determined based on completed SDQ reports at T6 when children turned 4 years old. Missing data were present in 73.3% of variables, 20.9% of cases, and in 2.0% of values overall, primarily due to attrition. Little’s Missing Completely At Random test (MCAR) was conducted across all variables and time points and was non-significant (χ2 = 1004.56, p = .620). Multiple imputation under fully conditional specification was used to create 20 imputed data sets, with Rubin’s rules applied for pooling and default settings used. For sensitivity analysis, a complete-case analysis was performed and compared with the imputed data. Missing data for mothers’ education, children’s sex, and colic were not imputed, leading to reduced sample sizes in later regressions (n = 549 for internalizing, n = 536 for externalizing).

Following recommendations by Chen et al. (57), a two-stage mixed effects model was used to assess longitudinal risk factors for internalizing and externalizing symptomatology at the age of 4. Given that our dependent variable was assessed only once, while most predictors were longitudinal, traditional linear mixed-effects modeling was not applicable. This two-staged mixed model effectively integrates the strengths of growth curve analysis and linear regression, allowing us to use growth curves as predictors and evaluate both the slope and intercept of individual variables over time, which is crucial for understanding their impact on symptomatology. Its capacity to handle longitudinal data, accommodate individual differences, manage missing data, and reduce collinearity ensures a thorough and efficient identification of significant risk and protective factors while maintaining model parsimony.

First, longitudinally time-varying variables were modeled as a function of time using random slopes and intercepts, similar to the growth curve model approach proposed by Welten et al. (58). Growth curves were computed for the following variables: mothers’ sleep difficulties (PSQI), fathers’ sleep difficulties (PSQI), mothers’ and fathers’ overall health, mothers’ social support (F-SozU), mothers’ and fathers’ depression and anxiety (PHQ-9, GAD-7), mothers’ and fathers’ parenting stress (EBI), children’s physical health, and children’s sleep difficulties (CSHQ). Individual values for the slope (development over time) and intercept (intensity) for each variable were extracted and used as predictor variables.

Second, the resulting subject-specific best linear unbiased predictor (BLUP) estimates were used as predictors in two linear regression models (internalizing score, externalizing score), in addition to predictors assessed once. The intercept represents the general value of scores over time, while the slope corresponds to changes in scores over time. This method effectively handles collinearity, accommodates missing data, and reduces the number of variables in the linear regression model. To ensure parsimony, regression models were recomputed to include only significant predictors from the second step (i.e., reduced regression models). For all models VIF values were below the critical threshold of 5 (59), with the largest VIF value being 3.9. The false discovery rate (FDR) was applied to correct for multiple comparisons in reduced regression models, with the significance level set at p <.05.

3 Results

3.1 Sample characteristics

The total sample consisted of 560 parents who completed the outcome measure SDQ for their 4-year-old children. Sample characteristics are depicted in Table 1 and Figure 2 depicts the non-imputed sample’s questionnaire scores over time.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean questionnaire scores over time for mothers, fathers, and child. (A) Average sleep difficulties score (PSQI) across time for mothers and fathers, (B) Children’s average sleep difficulties score (CSHQ) across time. (C) Average physical health scores of mothers, fathers, and children across time. (D) Average social support reported by the mother across time. (E) Average anxious symptomatology (GAD-7) of mothers and fathers across time. (F) Average depressive symptomatology (PHQ) of mothers and fathers across time. (G) Average personal burden (EBI PB) reported by mothers and fathers across time. (H) Average lack of parental competence (EBI PC) reported by mothers and fathers. (I) Average attachment problems (EBI AT) reported by mothers and fathers. PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; FSozU, Social Support Questionnaire; GAD-7, Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale; PHQ, Patient Health Questionnaire; EBI, Parenting Stress Index; CSHQ, Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire; Physical Health, general health assessment.

For internalizing symptomatology on the SDQ at age 4, 12.1% of children were slightly above average (80th-90th percentile), 6.0% were high (90th-95th percentile), and 2.3% were very high (>95th percentile). For externalizing symptomatology, 13.8% of children were slightly above average, 5.3% were high, and 4.4% were very high. Regarding clinical cut-offs, a total of 5.1% of children were at-risk and 1.5% were critical for internalizing symptomatology, while 41.7% were at-risk and 23.8% were critical for externalizing symptomatology. Regarding comparisons with previously reported German norms for the SDQ, approximately 83.1% of children received a total score below 12, aligning with the reported percentage of 80.0 – 85.0% who show non-critical values (60). A total of 9.0% showed at-risk scores compared to norm percentages of 5.0 – 8.0% (60). Finally, 7.8% showed critically high scores, which aligns with the reported 7.0 – 10.0% (60).

3.2 Linear regressions predicting internalizing and externalizing symptomatology

Overall, two linear regression models were computed with the SDQ internalizing and externalizing scores as dependent variables (see Tables 2, 3). Reduced regression models are depicted in Table 4. Longitudinal predictors included the intercept and slope of growth curves computed in the first step, along with children’s temperament. Mothers’ education and prenatal factors, such as at-risk pregnancies, premature birth and use of antibiotics during pregnancy, were added as predictors to both models. Sex and colic were added as predictors for the externalizing score, based on prior work (18, 32).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of complete linear regression model predicting children’s internalizing symptomatology at age 4.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of complete linear regression model predicting children’s externalizing symptomatology age 4.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of reduced linear regression models predicting children’s internalizing and externalizing problems at the age of 4.

The model predicting the SDQ internalizing score was significant (F (42,525) = 10.08, p <.001) and explained 46.7% of the variance which corresponds to Cohen’s f = 0.94, indicating a large effect (61). Significant predictors included child temperament (EAS emotionality, shyness, sociability), social support over time (FSozU slope), mother’s and father’s anxious symptomatology (GAD-7 intercept), mother’s depressive symptomatology (PHQ intercept), mother’s perceived personal burden (EBI PB intercept), mother’s perceived lack of parental competence (EBI PB intercept), father’s depressive symptomatology over time (PHQ slope), and the child’s physical health over time (Health slope). In the reduced linear regression model, all predictors remained significant (see Table 4). The regression model on the complete-case dataset showed the same significant predictors.

The model predicting the SDQ externalizing score was significant (F (44,512) = 7.57, p <.001) and explained 41.6% of the variance, corresponding to Cohen’s f = 0.85, indicating a large effect. Significant predictors included child temperament (EAS emotionality, activity, shyness), mother’s sleep difficulties (PSQI intercept), mother’s perceived lack of parental competence over time (EBI PC slope), mother’s general attachment (EBI AT intercept), mother’s education, father’s depressive symptomatology over time (PHQ slope), father’s perceived lack of parental competence over time (EBI PC slope), the child’s overall sleep difficulties (CSHQ intercept), sex of the child and preterm birth. In the reduced linear regression model, mother’s perceived lack of parental competence over time (EBI PC slope) and preterm birth did not remain significant (see Table 4). The regression model on the complete-case dataset showed the same significant predictors.

4 Discussion

This study aimed to identify both general (sex, parental education, child temperament, prenatal factors) and longitudinal (sleep, social support, parental mental health, and stress) parental, environmental and child-specific risk and protective factors between the age of 4 weeks and 4 years for internalizing and externalizing symptomatology at the age of 4 within the KUNO-Kids cohort. Analyzing the prevalence of internalizing and externalizing symptomatology among 4-year-old children, we found that one-fifth exhibited above-average internalizing symptoms, while almost one-fourth displayed above-average externalizing symptoms according to the SDQ. This is in line with previously reported frequencies for internalizing and externalizing problems in German preschoolers (60).

Several risk and protective factors emerged that contribute to the early manifestation of these behaviors. For internalizing symptoms, maternal depression, fathers’ worsening depression and anxiety, mothers’ perceived lack of parental competence, and child emotionality and shyness were significant risk factors. In contrast, protective influences included increasing social support, maternal anxiety and perceived burden, improvements in children’s physical health, and child sociability. These results suggest that while certain aspects of parental mental health and child temperament may increase the likelihood of internalizing behaviors, social and emotional support networks, as well as improvements in child health, can mitigate these risks.

In terms of externalizing symptomatology, risk factors were largely related to maternal sleep difficulties, fathers’ increasing depression, perceived attachment problems, and child-related factors such as sleep difficulties, high emotionality, and activity, with male sex also posing a higher risk. Protective factors included higher maternal education, fathers’ perceived lack of parental competence, and child shyness. These findings suggest that externalizing behaviors are shaped not only by parental and child health but also by broader familial dynamics, indicating potential areas for intervention.

In the following, the identified risk and protective factors will be discussed in relation to findings from previous studies.

4.1 Environmental factors

The predictive impact of parental education on children’s externalizing symptomatology has been underscored in previous research, revealing that mothers with lower education levels are more susceptible to additional stressors such as unemployment, financial strain, or substandard housing conditions (9). In our study, we found that mothers’ education served as a significant negative predictor for externalizing symptomatology, though not for internalizing symptomatology. This discrepancy may be attributed to the generally high educational attainment within our sample and the nuanced presentation of internalizing symptoms in children (62). Additionally, externalizing symptomatology tends to be disruptive and may prompt the need for quick interventions, which can vary based on educational background.

Previous research has highlighted the positive influence of social support, particularly among mothers with high socioeconomic status and low-risk profiles (23). Given that our sample predominantly consisted of families with higher levels of education, the observed association between increased social support over time and reduced internalizing symptomatology in children at age 4 is plausible. However, it is important to acknowledge that while social support may mitigate internalizing symptoms within this demographic, its effectiveness may differ in a more diverse sample.

Overall, mothers’ education and perceived social support were protective factors for predicting children’s internalizing and externalizing symptomatology. The findings suggest that children of parents with lower educational status may benefit from targeted interventions that address the additional stressors these families face, such as unemployment and financial strain. Enhancing social support networks early on through parent-child centers, parent-child groups, and community programs could help parents connect with others in similar situations, fostering a supportive environment. These efforts may mitigate both externalizing and internalizing symptoms in children by providing parents with resources and peer support, reducing stress and promoting positive parenting practices.

4.2 Parental factors

Parental mental health, in particular maternal depression and anxiety, has frequently been identified as a crucial contributor to internalizing and externalizing symptomatology in children (10). Our study found that maternal depression and worsening paternal depression over time were significantly associated with increased internalizing symptoms in children. Interestingly, maternal anxiety was negatively related to children’s internalizing symptomatology, suggesting that anxious mothers reported fewer internalizing symptoms in their children compared to less anxious mothers. This could indicate a protective aspect of maternal anxiety, where heightened vigilance and attention to the child might play a role, or it might reflect a reporting bias.

Worsening paternal depression over time emerged as a consistent predictor for both internalizing and externalizing symptomatology in our sample. Previous research has similarly identified the impact of paternal depressive symptoms on both internalizing and externalizing problems in children (63, 64). Notably, it was the worsening of paternal depression, rather than elevated depression scores per se, that had a significant predictive effect. This worsening of depressive symptoms can disrupt the family dynamic, adding stress and burden to the other parent and potentially affecting the child’s emotional and behavioral development.

Another critical parental risk factor is parenting stress (9, 18), assessed through parents’ perceived personal burden, perceived lack of parental competence, and perceived attachment problems. In our study, mothers’ perceived attachment problems significantly predicted increased externalizing symptomatology in children, while mothers’ personal burden was a negative (protective) predictor of internalizing symptomatology. Personal burden reflects mothers’ attitudes towards their parental role and the degree to which they find it burdensome (65). An increased perception of the role as burdensome may lead to less attention toward the child and greater difficulty in recognizing internalizing symptoms (62).

Contrary to findings by Dagan et al. (35) and Deneault et al. (34), fathers’ attachment was not a significant predictor of either internalizing or externalizing symptomatology. Dagan et al. (35) argue that externalizing symptoms are more likely when attachment with both parents is disorganized. Given that mothers are more often the primary caregivers in Germany (66), it is possible that mothers’ perceived attachment problems had a stronger impact in our sample. Alternatively, fathers may have reported fewer attachment problems, or mothers’ reports of symptomatology may have been biased by their perceived attachment issues.

Regarding parental competence, mothers’ perceived lack of parental competence was a significant positive (risk) predictor of internalizing symptomatology, while fathers’ perceived lack of parental competence was a significant negative (protective) predictor of externalizing symptomatology. Mothers may feel incompetent in their role due to the often subtle and hard-to-detect nature of internalizing symptoms (62). Conversely, the relationship between fathers’ perceived parental competence and children’s externalizing behavior might be bidirectional. Children exhibiting externalizing behaviors may lead fathers to attribute the problem to the child’s behavior rather than their own competence. This relationship might also be influenced by the child’s temperament (13).

Finally, the impact of parental sleep difficulties was examined. Our study found that mothers’ sleep difficulties were significantly associated with increased externalizing symptomatology in their children. Sleep deprivation is known to lead to heightened exhaustion and overwhelm (30), which can impair mothers’ ability to effectively manage and respond to their children’s behavior. This reduced capacity to structure and guide behavior may contribute to more frequent and intense behavioral escalations in children.

Overall, parental mental health, parental stress, and maternal sleep difficulties played a crucial role in predicting children’s internalizing and externalizing symptomatology. The findings emphasize the critical need for parents, especially those experiencing maternal depression and worsening paternal depression, to access mental health care to support their emotional well-being and, in turn, their children’s development. Early screening for mental health difficulties among parents could help identify those who may need additional support, ensuring timely interventions. Promoting awareness of governmental aid programs can further empower parents by informing them of available resources, potentially reducing parental stress and alleviating fears of failure in their parenting roles. Additionally, addressing parenting stress and perceived parental competence through sleep support programs and community resources could foster a more nurturing environment for children, ultimately mitigating both internalizing and externalizing symptoms.

4.3 Child-specific factors

The most common child-specific factor associated with externalizing symptomatology is biological sex (9). Consistent with previous research, our study found that being male was significantly associated with increased externalizing symptomatology at age 4. One hypothesis for this association is that young boys may exhibit reduced deliberate control of attention (15), which can contribute to higher levels of disruptive and aggressive behaviors.

Additionally, children’s physical health from 4 weeks to 4 years of age was a significant negative (protective) predictor for internalizing symptomatology at the age of 4. While most studies highlight the predictive relationship between mental health problems in childhood and physical health issues in adolescence (67, 68), the bidirectional nature of this relationship has not been extensively examined. In adults, the impact of physical symptoms, such as bodily pain and gastrointestinal problems, on mental health is well-documented (69). It is plausible that physical symptoms similarly affect the mental well-being of young children.

Contrary to the findings of DeSantis et al. (32), our study did not confirm the presence of colic in the first year of life as a risk factor for externalizing symptomatology in 4-year-olds. While colic was a significant predictor in the main model, it did not remain significant in the more parsimonious, reduced model. This discrepancy suggests that colic might be a byproduct of other underlying difficulties and does not emerge as significant when multiple risk factors are considered concurrently. This finding highlights the importance of examining a broad range of factors to understand the complex interplay of influences on child development.

In addition to parental sleep difficulties, children’s overall sleep problems were significantly associated with externalizing symptomatology. Previous research has established links between childhood sleep problems and externalizing behaviors in adolescence (70). Our study reinforces these findings by highlighting the importance of sleep quality in early childhood, even when accounting for a wide range of concurrent factors. This underscores the critical need for early intervention and support to address sleep issues as a preventive measure against the development of externalizing symptomatology.

A significant child-specific factor explaining much of the variance in our study was child temperament. Child emotionality and shyness were positive (risk) predictors of internalizing symptomatology, while child sociability was a negative (protective) predictor. Additionally, child emotionality and activity were positive (risk) predictors, and child shyness was a negative (protective) predictor of externalizing symptomatology. Child emotionality consistently constituted a risk for both types of symptomatology in our sample, aligning with previous longitudinal studies from infancy/preschool to adolescence (16, 17). The dimension emotionality refers to children that frequently feel tension accompanied by intense autonomous arousal that evolves into feelings and expressions of fear and anger (71). This is also closely related to the concept of emotional reactivity, which is also known as a risk factor for emotion regulation difficulties (72), and subsequently mental health problems (73). Children with heightened emotionality may lack the skills to regulate these emotions, putting them at increased risk for mental health problems. Interestingly, child shyness had differential predictive power, with increased shyness relating to higher internalizing symptoms but lower externalizing symptoms. Shyness is commonly associated with internalizing traits (74) and a tendency to internalize thoughts and feelings (75), which may explain its relation to internalizing while inversely relating to externalizing behaviors. Additionally, child activity has been previously identified as a risk factor for externalizing problems (17). However, this relationship may be bidirectional, as hyperactive children might exhibit more movement, leading to higher reports of their activity level. Furthermore, child sociability was identified as a negative (protective) predictor of internalizing symptomatology, suggesting that children who are more sociable are less likely to exhibit internalizing symptoms. This finding aligns with numerous studies that highlight the positive impact of sociability on children’s mental health (76, 77). Considering that temperament is regarded as stable (78), it may be important to investigate which aspects of sociability, shyness and activity can be changed through interventions.

Overall, children’s temperament, sleep difficulties and physical health can be summarized as relevant child-specific factors for internalizing and externalizing symptomatology at the age of 4. The findings suggest the need for tailored interventions for boys, who are more prone to externalizing symptoms, emphasizing programs that enhance self-regulation, attention control, and behavioral management strategies. Prioritizing physical health through regular medical check-ups and prompt treatment of any health issues can play a crucial role in preventing emotional difficulties. Additionally, addressing sleep quality is essential, as sleep problems are linked to increased behavioral issues, highlighting the importance of educating parents on sleep hygiene. Fostering sociability and emotional regulation in children—especially those exhibiting high emotionality or shyness—can also help reduce the risk of internalizing symptoms. By implementing these targeted strategies, we can create a supportive environment that promotes positive mental health outcomes for children at risk, ultimately enhancing their overall development.

4.4 Strengths and limitations

The strengths of the present study include its longitudinal cohort design and relatively large sample size, which allowed for the comprehensive examination of risk and protective factors over an extended period. Additionally, the use of a two-model approach preserved subjective variance over time, reduced collinearity, and improved the handling of missing data. Additionally, our study stands out for its inclusion of data from both parents, enabling the identification of distinct paternal and maternal risk factors.

Limitations of the study include its reliance on self-reported assessments and the exclusive use of mothers’ reports to assess children’s internalizing and externalizing symptomatology at age 4. This may have led to an underrepresentation of the father’s perspective, potentially overlooking important aspects of the child’s behaviour. Future longitudinal studies would benefit from incorporating reports from multiple observers to obtain a more comprehensive understanding of children’s symptoms.

Additionally, the high attrition rate in the sample poses a limitation, resulting in a disproportionately educated sample and potentially limiting the generalizability of the findings to less educated populations. Finally, the absence of direct child variables, such as objective measures of development, is another limitation, as it precludes the control for confounding factors. Future research should include such measures to provide a more nuanced understanding of the factors influencing child behaviour.

5 Conclusion

Overall, our study identified several key risk and protective factors associated with children’s internalizing and externalizing symptomatology, encompassing environmental, parental, and child-specific domains. These factors include a lack of social support and lower parental education as environmental risks, reduced parental mental health and increased parenting stress as parental risks, and temperament, poor physical health, and sleep difficulties as child-specific risks. These findings underscore the critical importance of family support systems and early interventions targeting parental stress and mental well-being. Moreover, they highlight the complexity of addressing rather stable temperament traits such as children’s sociability and shyness. Moving forward, it is imperative for future research to investigate the effectiveness of parental interventions aimed at bolstering social support networks and addressing stress, and to assess their subsequent impact on children’s mental health outcomes.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee of the University of Regensburg (file numbers: 14-101-0347, 19-1646-101). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

IJ: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SKa: Conceptualization, Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. AE: Conceptualization, Investigation, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. SB: Data curation, Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. MK: Funding acquisition, Investigation, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. AK: Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. MM: Funding acquisition, Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. SKe: Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. JK: Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing. CA: Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. RB: Conceptualization, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing.

The members of the KUNO-Kids study group

Andreas Ambrosch (Institute of Laboratory Medicine, Microbiology and Hygiene, Barmherzige Brüder Hospital, Regensburg, Germany), Petra A. Arndt (ZNL Transfercenter of Neuroscience and Learning, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany), Andrea Baessler (Department of Internal Medicine II, Regensburg University Medical Center, Regensburg, Germany), Mark Berneburg (Department of Dermatology, University Medical Centre Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany), Stephan Böse-O’Reilly (University Children’s Hospital Regensburg (KUNO), Hospital St. Hedwig of the Order of St. John, Regensburg, Germany), Romuald Brunner (Clinic of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Bezirksklinikum Regensburg (medbo), Regensburg, Germany), Sara Fill Malfertheiner (Clinic of Obstetrics and Gynecology St. Hedwig, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany), André Franke (Institute of Clinical Molecular Biology, Christian-Albrechts-University of Kiel, Kiel, Germany), Sebastian Häusler (Clinic of Obstetrics and Gynecology St. Hedwig, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany), Iris Heid (Department of Genetic Epidemiology, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany), Stefanie Heinze (Bavarian Health and Food Safety Authority (LGL) Munich, Germany), Wolfgang Högler (Department of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, Johannes Kepler University Linz, Linz, Austria), Sebastian Kerzel (Department of Pediatric Pneumology and Allergy, University Children’s Hospital Regensburg (KUNO), Hospital St. Hedwig of the Order of St. John, Regensburg, Germany), Michael Koller (Center for Clinical Studies, University Hospital Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany), Michael Leitzmann (Department of Epidemiology and Preventive Medicine, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany), Áine Lennon (Department of Conservative Dentistry and Periodontology, University Hospital Regensburg, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany), David Rothfuß (City of Regensburg, Coordinating Center for Early Interventions, Regensburg, Germany), Wolfgang Rösch (Department of Pediatric Urology, University Medical Center, Regensburg, Germany), Bianca Schaub (Pediatric Allergology, Department of Pediatrics, Dr. von Hauner Children’s Hospital, University Hospital, LMU Munich, Munich, Germany), Stephan Weidinger (Department of Dermatology, Venereology and Allergy, University Hospital Schleswig-Holstein, Campus Kiel, Kiel, Germany) and Sven Wellmann (Department of Neonatology, University Children’s Hospital Regensburg (KUNO), Hospital St. Hedwig of the Order of St. John, Regensburg, Germany).

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Financial support was provided by the University Children’s Hospital Regensburg (KUNO) and the Hospital St Hedwig of the Order of St John.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Dattani S, Ritchie H, Roser M. Mental Health. Our World in Data (2021). Available online at: https://ourworldindata.org/mental-health (Accessed October 22, 2024).

Google Scholar

2. Polanczyk GV, Salum GA, Sugaya LS, Caye A, Rohde LA. Annual research review: A meta-analysis of the worldwide prevalence of mental disorders in children and adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2015) 56:345–65. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.2015.56.issue-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Aalto-Setälä T, Marttunen M, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Poikolainen K, Lönnqvist J. Depressive symptoms in adolescence as predictors of early adulthood depressive disorders and maladjustment. AJP. (2002) 159:1235–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1235

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Narusyte J, Ropponen A, Alexanderson K, Svedberg P. Internalizing and externalizing problems in childhood and adolescence as predictors of work incapacity in young adulthood. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2017) 52:1159–68. doi: 10.1007/s00127-017-1409-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Hofstra MB, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC. Child and adolescent problems predict DSM-IV disorders in adulthood: a 14-year follow-up of a Dutch epidemiological sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2002) 41:182–9. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200202000-00012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Arslan İB, Lucassen N, van Lier PAC, de Haan AD, Prinzie P. Early childhood internalizing problems, externalizing problems and their co-occurrence and (mal)adaptive functioning in emerging adulthood: a 16-year follow-up study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2021) 56:193–206. doi: 10.1007/s00127-020-01959-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Hofstra MB, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC. Continuity and change of psychopathology from childhood into adulthood: A 14-year follow-up study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2000) 39:850–8. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200007000-00013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Greenspoon PJ, Saklofske DH. Toward an integration of subjective well-being and psychopathology. Soc Indic Res. (2001) 54:81–108. doi: 10.1023/A:1007219227883

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Carneiro A, Dias P, Soares I. Risk factors for internalizing and externalizing problems in the preschool years: systematic literature review based on the child behavior checklist 1½–5. J Child Fam Stud. (2016) 25:2941–53. doi: 10.1007/s10826-016-0456-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Edwards RC, Hans SL. Infant risk factors associated with internalizing, externalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems in young children. Dev Psychol. (2015) 51:489–99. doi: 10.1037/a0038800

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Ryan SM, Ollendick TH. The interaction between child behavioral inhibition and parenting behaviors: effects on internalizing and externalizing symptomology. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. (2018) 21:320–39. doi: 10.1007/s10567-018-0254-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Gilbert KE, Whalen DJ, Tillman R, Barch DM, Luby JL, Jackson JJ. Observed personality in preschool: associations with current and longitudinal symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2019) 47:1875–88. doi: 10.1007/s10802-019-00567-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Forbes MK, Rapee RM, Camberis AL, McMahon CA. Unique associations between childhood temperament characteristics and subsequent psychopathology symptom trajectories from childhood to early adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2017) 45:1221–33. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0236-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Wichstrøm L, Penelo E, Rensvik Viddal K, de la Osa N, Ezpeleta L. Explaining the relationship between temperament and symptoms of psychiatric disorders from preschool to middle childhood: hybrid fixed and random effects models of Norwegian and Spanish children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2018) 59:285–95. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.2018.59.issue-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Else-Quest NM, Hyde JS, Goldsmith HH, Van Hulle CA. Gender differences in temperament: A meta-analysis. psychol Bulletin. (2006) 132:33–72. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.33

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Phillips EM, Brock RL, James TD, Nelson JM, Espy KA, Nelson TD. Empirical support for a dual process model of the p-factor: Interaction effects between preschool executive control and preschool negative emotionality on general psychopathology. J Psychopathol Clin Science. (2022) 131:817–29. doi: 10.1037/abn0000777

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Kostyrka-Allchorne K, Wass SV, Sonuga-Barke EJS. Research Review: Do parent ratings of infant negative emotionality and self-regulation predict psychopathology in childhood and adolescence? A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective longitudinal studies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2020) 61:401–16. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13144

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Tsotsi S, Broekman BFP, Shek LP, Tan KH, Chong YS, Chen H, et al. Maternal parenting stress, child exuberance, and preschoolers’ Behavior problems. Child Dev. (2019) 90:136–46. doi: 10.1111/cdev.2019.90.issue-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kinnunen L, Nordström T, Niemelä M, Räsänen S, Whittle S, Miettunen J. Parental physical illnesses and their association with subsequent externalizing and internalizing symptoms in children. J Child Fam Stud. (2021) 30:2677–89. doi: 10.1007/s10826-021-02079-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Fuchs A, Möhler E, Reck C, Resch F, Kaess M. The early mother-to-child bond and its unique prospective contribution to child behavior evaluated by mothers and teachers. Psychopathology. (2016) 49:211–6. doi: 10.1159/000445439

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Groh AM, Roisman GI, van Ijzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Fearon RP. The significance of insecure and disorganized attachment for children’s internalizing symptoms: a meta-analytic study. Child Dev. (2012) 83:591–610. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01711.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Madigan S, Atkinson L, Laurin K, Benoit D. Attachment and internalizing behavior in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Dev Psychol. (2013) 49:672–89. doi: 10.1037/a0028793

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Arikan G, Kumru A, Korkut B, Ilhan AO. Examining toddlers’ Problem behaviors: the role of SES, parenting stress, perceived support and negative intentionality. J Child Fam Stud. (2019) 28:3467–78. doi: 10.1007/s10826-019-01529-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Park GA, Lee ON. The moderating effect of social support on parental stress and depression in mothers of children with disabilities. Occup Ther Int. (2022) 2022:5162954. doi: 10.1155/2022/5162954

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Gilligan M, Suitor JJ, Rurka M, Silverstein M. Multigenerational social support in the face of the COVID-19 pandemic. J Family Theory Review. (2020) 12:431–47. doi: 10.1111/jftr.12397

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Huizink AC, Rooij SRde. Prenatal stress and models explaining risk for psychopathology revisited: Generic vulnerability and divergent pathways. Dev Psychopathology. (2018) 30:1041–62. doi: 10.1017/S0954579418000354

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Tien J, Lewis GD, Liu J. Prenatal risk factors for internalizing and externalizing problems in childhood. World J Pediatr. (2020) 16:341–55. doi: 10.1007/s12519-019-00319-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Van den Bergh BRH, van den Heuvel MI, Lahti M, Braeken M, de Rooij SR, Entringer S, et al. Prenatal developmental origins of behavior and mental health: The influence of maternal stress in pregnancy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 117:26–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Short MA, Bartel K, Carskadon MA. Chapter 32 - Sleep and mental health in children and adolescents. In: Grandner MA, editor. Sleep and health. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Academic Press (2019). p. 435–45. Available at: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128153734000320.

Google Scholar

30. Meltzer LJ, Mindell JA. Relationship between child sleep disturbances and maternal sleep, mood, and parenting stress: A pilot study. J Family Psychol. (2007) 21:67–73. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.67

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. McCann D. Sleep deprivation is an additional stress for parents staying in hospital. J Specialists Pediatr Nursing. (2008) 13:111–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6155.2008.00142.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. DeSantis A, Coster W, Bigsby R, Lester B. Colic and fussing in infancy, and sensory processing at 3 to 8 years of age. Infant Ment Health J. (2004) 25:522–39. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20025

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Meng X, Fleury MJ, Xiang YT, Li M, D’Arcy C. Resilience and protective factors among people with a history of child maltreatment: a systematic review. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2018) 53:453–75. doi: 10.1007/s00127-018-1485-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Deneault AA, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Groh AM, Fearon PRM, Madigan S. Child-father attachment in early childhood and behavior problems: A meta-analysis. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. (2021) 2021:43–66. doi: 10.1002/cad.v2021.180

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Dagan O, Schuengel C, Verhage ML, van IJzendoorn MH, Sagi-Schwartz A, Madigan S, et al. Configurations of mother-child and father-child attachment as predictors of internalizing and externalizing behavioral problems: An individual participant data (IPD) meta-analysis. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. (2021) 2021:67–94. doi: 10.1002/cad.v2021.180

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Brandstetter S, Toncheva AA, Niggel J, Wolff C, Gran S, Seelbach-Göbel B, et al. KUNO-Kids birth cohort study: rationale, design, and cohort description. Mol Cell Pediatr. (2019) 6:1. doi: 10.1186/s40348-018-0088-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Fydrich T, Sommer G, Tydecks S, Brähler E. Fragebogen zur sozialen Unterstützung (F-SozU): Normierung der Kurzform (K-14). Z für Medizinische Psychologie. (2009) 18:43–8.

Google Scholar

38. Sommer G, Fydrich T. Entwicklung und Überprüfung eines Fragebogens zur sozialen Unterstützung (F-SOZU). Diagnostica. (1991) 37:160–78.

Google Scholar

39. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. (1989) 28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Carpenter JS, Andrykowski MA. Psychometric evaluation of the pittsburgh sleep quality index. J Psychosomatic Res. (1998) 45:5–13. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(97)00298-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Backhaus J, Junghanns K, Broocks A, Riemann D, Hohagen F. Test–retest reliability and validity of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index in primary insomnia. J Psychosomatic Res. (2002) 53:737–40. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00330-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JBW, Löwe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Internal Med. (2006) 166:1092–7. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Löwe B, Spitzer RL, Zipfel S, Herzog W. PHQ-D: Gesundheitsfragebogen für Patienten; Manual Komplettversion und Kurzform. New York, NY: Pfizer (2002).

Google Scholar

44. Hinz A, Klein AM, Brähler E, Glaesmer H, Luck T, Riedel-Heller SG, et al. Psychometric evaluation of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Screener GAD-7, based on a large German general population sample. J Affect Disord. (2017) 210:338–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.12.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Kocalevent RD, Hinz A, Brähler E. Standardization of the depression screener Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) in the general population. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. (2013) 35:551–5. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2013.04.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Tröster H. Eltern-Belastungs-Inventar: EBI; deutsche Version des Parenting Stress Index (PSI) von RR Abidin. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. (2011).

Google Scholar

47. Abidin R, Flens JR, Austin WG. The Parenting Stress Index. In: Forensic uses of clinical assessment instruments. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, Mahwah, NJ, US (2006). p. 297–328.

Google Scholar

48. Schlarb AA, Schwerdtle B, Hautzinger M. Validation and psychometric properties of the German version of the Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ-DE). Somnologie. (2010) 14:260–6. doi: 10.1007/s11818-010-0495-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Owens JA, Spirito A, McGuinn M. The Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ): psychometric properties of a survey instrument for school-aged children. Sleep. (2000) 23:1043–51. doi: 10.1093/sleep/23.8.1d

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Van Roy B, Veenstra M, Clench-Aas J. Construct validity of the five-factor Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) in pre-, early, and late adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2008) 49:1304–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01942.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Goodman A, Lamping DL, Ploubidis GB. When to use broader internalising and externalising subscales instead of the hypothesised five subscales on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): data from British parents, teachers and children. J Abnormal Child Psychol. (2010) 38:1179–91. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9434-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Rothenberger A, Becker A, Erhart M, Wille N, Ravens-Sieberer U, the BELLA study group. Psychometric properties of the parent strengths and difficulties questionnaire in the general population of German children and adolescents: results of the BELLA study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2008) 17:99–105. doi: 10.1007/s00787-008-1011-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Spinath FM. Psychometric properties and behavioral-genetic results with the German Emotionality-Activity-Sociability Temperament Inventory (EAS, Buss & Plomin, 1984). Z Fur Differentielle Und Diagnostische Psychologie. (2000) 21:65–75. doi: 10.1024//0170-1789.21.1.65

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Spinath FM. Temperamentsmerkmale bei kindern. Z für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie. (2000) 21:65–75. doi: 10.1024//0170-1789.21.1.65

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. R Core Team. R: A language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2022). Available at: http://www.R-project.org/.

Google Scholar

56. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 29.0.2.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp (2023). Available at: https://www.ibm.com/products/spss-statistics.

Google Scholar

57. Chen YH, Ferguson KK, Meeker JD, McElrath TF, Mukherjee B. Statistical methods for modeling repeated measures of maternal environmental exposure biomarkers during pregnancy in association with preterm birth. Environ Health. (2015) 14:9. doi: 10.1186/1476-069X-14-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Welten M, de Kroon MLA, Renders CM, Steyerberg EW, Raat H, Twisk JWR, et al. Repeatedly measured predictors: a comparison of methods for prediction modeling. Diagn Prognostic Res. (2018) 2:5. doi: 10.1186/s41512-018-0024-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Kock N, Lynn G. Lateral collinearity and misleading results in variance-based SEM: an illustration and recommendations. J Assoc Inf Syst. (2012) 13. doi: 10.17705/1jais.00302

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Klasen H, Woerner W, Rothenberger A, Goodman R. Die deutsche Fassung des Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ-Deu) - Übersicht und Bewertung erster Validierungs- und Normierungsbefunde (2003). Available online at: https://psycharchives.org/en/item/86f7e970-a515-4eb6-84e2-e942bda4d051 (Accessed October 22, 2024).

Google Scholar

61. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum (1988).

Google Scholar

62. Carpenter AL, Puliafico AC, Kurtz SMS, Pincus DB, Comer JS. Extending parent–child interaction therapy for early childhood internalizing problems: new advances for an overlooked population. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. (2014) 17:340–56. doi: 10.1007/s10567-014-0172-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Low J, Bishop A, Pilkington PD. The longitudinal effects of paternal perinatal depression on internalizing symptoms and externalizing behavior of their children: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ment Health Prev. (2022) 26:200230. doi: 10.1016/j.mhp.2022.200230

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Cheung K, Theule J. Paternal depression and child externalizing behaviors: A meta-analysis. J Family Psychol. (2018) 33(1):98–108. doi: 10.1037/fam0000473

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Irlbauer-Müller V, Eichler A A, Donhauser J, Poehlmann N E, Stemmler M, Moll G H, et al. Das eltern-belastungs-inventar (EBI). Diagnostica. (2018) 64:37–48. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924/a000191

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Steinbach A, Schulz F. Stability and change in german parents’ Childcare patterns across two decades. Soc Politics: Int Stud Gender State Society. (2022) 29:428–45. doi: 10.1093/sp/jxab017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Jamnik MR, DiLalla LF. Health outcomes associated with internalizing problems in early childhood and adolescence. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:60. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00060

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Ruchkin V, Schwab-Stone M. A longitudinal study of somatic complaints in urban adolescents: the role of internalizing psychopathology and somatic anxiety. J Youth Adolescence. (2014) 43:834–45. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-9966-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Simon GE, VonKorff M, Piccinelli M, Fullerton C, Ormel J. An international study of the relation between somatic symptoms and depression. New Engl J Med. (1999) 341:1329–35. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199910283411801

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Quach JL, Nguyen CD, Williams KE, Sciberras E. Bidirectional associations between child sleep problems and internalizing and externalizing difficulties from preschool to early adolescence. JAMA Pediatr. (2018) 172:e174363. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2017.4363

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Buss AH, Plomin R. The EAS Approach to Temperament. In: The Study of Temperament. Hove, East Sussex, United Kingdom: Psychology Press (1986).

Google Scholar

72. Glenn CR, Blumenthal TD, Klonsky ED, Hajcak G. Emotional reactivity in nonsuicidal self-injury: Divergence between self-report and startle measures. Int J Psychophysiology. (2011) 80:166–70. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.02.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Menefee DS, Ledoux T, Johnston CA. The importance of emotional regulation in mental health. Am J Lifestyle Med. (2022) 16:28–31. doi: 10.1177/15598276211049771

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Eggum ND, Eisenberg N, Reiser M, Spinrad TL, Valiente C, Sallquist J, et al. Relations over Time among Children’s Shyness, Emotionality, and Internalizing Problems. Soc Dev. (2012) 21:109–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00618.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Jung S, Poole KL, Schmidt LA. Children’s shyness, emotional intelligence, and internalizing behaviors. Pers Individ Differences. (2023) 211:112242. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2023.112242

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Kim YH. My Child Likes to be with People: Sociability Trajectories from age 2 to age 4 and Behavior Problems at First Grade. Early Educ Dev. (2023) 34:1026–39. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2022.2089814

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Sette S, Zava F, Baumgartner E, Laghi F, Coplan RJ. Examining links between social withdrawal subtypes and internalizing problems among Italian primary school children. Eur J Dev Psychol. (2023) 20:268–86. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2022.2086118

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Kopala-Sibley DC, Olino T, Durbin E, Dyson MW, Klein DN. The stability of temperament from early childhood to early adolescence: A multi-method, multi-informant examination. Eur J Pers. (2018) 32:128–45. doi: 10.1002/per.2151

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: internalizing, externalizing, risk factors, protective factors, mental health, preschool children, longitudinal

Citation: Jarvers I, Kandsperger S, Ecker A, Brandstetter S, Kabesch M, Köninger A, Melter M, Kerzel S, Kittel J, Apfelbacher C and Brunner R (2024) Longitudinal predictors for internalizing and externalizing symptomatology at age 4: KUNO-Kids cohort study. Front. Psychiatry 15:1449108. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1449108

Received: 06 August 2024; Accepted: 08 November 2024;
Published: 26 November 2024.

Edited by:

Yanfeng Xu, University of South Carolina, United States

Reviewed by:

Yao Wang, University of Texas at Arlington, United States
Sadia Jehan, University of South Carolina, United States

Copyright © 2024 Jarvers, Kandsperger, Ecker, Brandstetter, Kabesch, Köninger, Melter, Kerzel, Kittel, Apfelbacher and Brunner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Irina Jarvers, aXJpbmEuamFydmVyc0B1a3IuZGU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.