Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 23 October 2024
Sec. Autism

Perceived quality of parent-child interaction in parents of autistic children: relationship with parental education level

Feiying WangFeiying Wang1Wenchong DuWenchong Du2Lai-Sang Iao*Lai-Sang Iao2*
  • 1Child Health Section, Affiliated Maternity and Child Health Care Hospital of Nantong University, Jiangsu, China
  • 2NTU Psychology, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, United Kingdom

Introduction: Most autism research was conducted in Western, Educated, Industrialised, Rich and Democratic (WEIRD) countries. This study is the first to examine factors that were associated with perceived quality of parent-child interaction in non-WEIRD parents of autistic children.

Methods: Ninety-one Chinese parents of autistic children (82 females, 9 males) completed an online survey which involved two sections. The first section included demographics questions about the parent and the family, including age, gender, educational level, and household income. It also assessed parents’ perceived quality of parent-child interaction and their autistic traits. The second section included demographics questions about their autistic child, including age and gender, and assessed autistic traits and behavioural problems.

Results: Parent’s education level was associated with and the only predictor of their perceived quality of parent-child interaction. The higher educational level in parents the higher quality of parent-child interaction was perceived by the parents.

Discussion: These findings underscored the significance of parents’ education level as a unique predictor of perceived parent-child interaction quality among Chinese parents of autistic children, providing implications to corresponding education and social policies in China and other non-WEIRD countries.

Introduction

Autism is clinically characterised by difficulties in social interaction and communication, alongside repetitive behaviours and restricted interests (1). Children diagnosed with autism may also exhibit additional behavioural problems such as hyperactivity and anxiety (e.g., 2, 3), which have been shown to negatively impact the quality of parent-child interaction (e.g., 4, 5). Research indicates that these interactions are not only pivotal for the child’s development (e.g., 6, 7) but also significantly affects parents’ mental health (e.g., 8, 9). Understanding the factors that contribute to the quality of this interaction in families with autistic children is of global importance.

Lower-quality interaction with parents was found in autistic children compared to typically developing children and children with other developmental conditions (10, 11). From as young as 10 months, infants at elevated likelihood for autism showed fewer initiations of interaction with their parents (12), and by 11 months, those later diagnosed with autism showed decreased social engagement in free play with their parents (13). Non-social autistic traits exhibited by infants at elevated likelihood for autism who were later diagnosed with autism, such as attention disengagement, may also negatively influence the quality of parent-child interaction (14, 15). Additionally, it was found that the level of autistic traits and behavioural problems demonstrated in autistic children aged 2 to 12 years was correlated with lower quality of parent-child interaction (4, 5, 16). However, it is critical to acknowledge that parent-child interaction is a reciprocal process that involves the responsive behaviours of both the child and parent within the context of their social ecology (17). Consequently, the quality of this interaction in families with autistic children cannot be attributed solely to the child characteristics including autistic traits and behavioural problems. A comprehensive assessment must also account for parent characteristics and the family’s circumstances, all of which operate within and are shaped by a broader ecological framework. This ecological framework includes attitudes and ideologies of a culture (6).

In infants with an increased likelihood of an autism diagnosis, their parents have been observed to demonstrate greater directiveness and less sensitivity in their responses, potentially influencing the dynamics of early interactions (18, 19). Parental interaction styles, including increased directiveness, have been variably beneficial, particularly in promoting higher engagement and participation in play among children with certain developmental challenges (20). These styles reflect adaptations to the child’s diagnosis, age, and language level, suggesting a nuanced landscape of parenting behaviours that are responsive to the unique developmental trajectories of autistic children. Furthermore, parents of autistic children may encounter their own social-communicative obstacles and display subclinical autistic traits (21, 22). Such subclinical autistic traits, often referred to as Broad Autism Phenotype (BAP), which includes aloofness, rigidity, and pragmatic language difficulties (23), can complicate the caregiving experience. For instance, mothers of autistic children with higher BAP scores reported more parenting challenges (24). This finding may be explained by the relationships between specific BAP traits and certain parenting practices; for instance, pragmatic language impairments can hinder effective communication with the child, aloofness has been associated with a reduction in positive behaviours like praise, and high levels of rigidity can lead to a tendency to dismiss the child’s emotions (25). However, Parr et al. (26) reported no significant difference in mother-child interaction quality based on the presence of BAP traits, though mothers with more pronounced BAP traits exhibited less enhancement in interaction quality following a parent-mediated intervention. As such, the relationship between BAP traits and perceived quality of parent-child interaction in parents of autistic children remains uncertain.

Parent-child interaction quality is also known to be influenced by another parent characteristics namely educational attainment and a family factor namely household income in the context of both typical development and developmental disabilities (e.g., 2729). For instance, De Falco et al. (28) showed that mothers with higher education level were more likely to be able to provide a supportive framework for parent-child interaction while appropriately scaffolding child play. However, the associations between parent-child interaction quality, parental education level and household income have rarely been examined in the context of autism. To our knowledge, there has only been one study that reported no associations between such variables in a small sample of autistic children and their parents (30). A larger sample of autistic children and their parents may be needed to clarify whether parental education level and household income were associated with parent-child interaction quality. There is also a need to consider the ecological framework that may have shaped the research findings.

Most autism research was conducted in Western, Educated, Industrialised, Rich and Democratic (WEIRD) countries. People in these countries typically acknowledge the neurological underpinnings of autism (e.g., 31, 32) whereas people in non-WEIRD countries attribute autism to inadequate parenting practices (33, 34) or preternatural reasons such as being possessed by evil spirits (35, 36). Their beliefs on the nature of autism and the stigma associated with it affect their judgments of an autistic child, parental behaviour and other aspects involving the child and the parent, e.g., parent-child interaction and family stress (37). In non-WEIRD countries such as China where people attribute autism to inadequate parenting practices (33, 34), parents of autistic children may perceive themselves playing a significant role in their interaction with their autistic child. They face significant pressure to alter their interactions with their children, as documented by Chen (38). Moreover, Chinese parents often adopt the role of their child’s educator, and their effectiveness in this role, as well as their understanding of autism, may be influenced by their education level (39). The limited availability and high costs of special educational and social services in China compound the challenges faced by these parents, especially those with lower household income who may need to quit their jobs to care for and educate their autistic child (4042). High family financial burdens could impede parents’ interaction with their autistic children. Consequently, factors such as the parent’s education and household income may influence the perceived quality of interaction with autistic children in Chinese families, contrasting with Ruble et al.’s findings (30). Without an examination of these possible relationships in Chinese populations, Chinese practitioners may adopt the research findings obtained from WEIRD countries and assume that addressing child characteristics may support perceived quality of parent-child interaction in Chinese parents of autistic children. Despite the high cost of services, their effectiveness on supporting perceived quality of parent-child interaction in Chinese parents of autistic children could be low. There is thus a need to investigate the factors that contribute to the perceived quality of parent-child interaction in Chinese parents with autistic children.

This study sought to identify the unique and combined factors associated with the perceived quality of parent-child interaction among Chinese parents of autistic children. While previous literature suggests links between this perceived quality and child-specific traits such as autism and behavioural problems (4, 5, 16), the associations with parents’ autistic traits and education level as well as household income remain unclear. As the first study accounting for child, parent and family characteristics simultaneously in understanding perceived quality of parent-child interaction in parents of autistic children, this study was exploratory. The insights gained from the current study aim to inform the development of targeted interventions and public policies to improve parent-child interaction in autism.

Methods

Participants

Ninety-three Chinese parents of autistic child aged 5 to 15 years were recruited for an online survey from Rudong Hanling Children’s Rehabilitation Center and Qidong Qianfan Children’s Rehabilitation Center in Nantong, China. Each participant confirmed their status as the main caregiver of at least one autistic child and provided informed consent before commencing the survey. Of these participants, 2 were excluded due to incomplete responses, resulting in a final sample of 91 parents (82 females, 9 males; see Table 1 for detailed sample characteristics). Participants did not receive incentive for their participation in the survey. Ethical approval was obtained from the university ethics committee before recruitment commencing.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean, standard deviation and range of all variables.

Measures and procedure

The online survey was cross-sectional and involved two sections. The first section included demographics questions about the parents and the families, including age, gender, education level, and household income. It also assessed parents’ perceived quality of parent-child interaction using one subscale of the Parenting Stress Index-Short Form (PSI-SF; 43, 44) and explored parents’ autistic traits using the Broad Autism Phenotype Questionnaire (BAPQ; 45). The second section collected demographics information about their autistic child, including age and gender, and evaluated autistic traits using the short Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ-10; 46) and behavioural problems using the Child Behaviour Checklist (higher score indicated more problems; 47). The completion of the survey was estimated to take around 20 minutes.

The Parenting Stress Index-Short Form (PSI-SF) is frequently used for assessing parental stress in those caring for autistic and non-autistic children (e.g., 44, 48). This questionnaire includes 36 items divided into three subscales: Parental Distress (PD), Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction (PCDI), and Difficult Child (DC). A modified 15-item version, developed specifically for Chinese parents, demonstrated strong psychometric properties and was highly correlated with the original PSI-SF (44). The PCDI subscale of this version was therefore used in this study to assesses the perceived quality of Chinese parent-child interaction. The subscale consists of 5 items using a 5-point Likert scale (5= strongly agree to 1= strongly disagree), and a higher mean score of the 5 items indicates lower quality. Sample items included “Most times I feel that my child likes me and wants to be close to me” and “My child rarely does things for me that make me feel good”. The Chinese version of the PSI-SF has good internal consistency (α ≥ 0.80) and concurrent validity with the Multidimensional Social Support Scale (r = -0.34; 49).

The BAPQ is used to evaluate subclinical traits of autism, especially in parents of autistic children (50, 51). This questionnaire is composed of three subscales, namely aloof, rigidity, and pragmatic language with a total of 36 items. Participants are required to rate each item on a 6-point Likert scale (1= very rarely to 6= very often). The sample items include “I like being around other people”, “I am comfortable with unexpected changes in plan” and “People have to talk me into trying something new”. A higher mean score of the 36 items on the questionnaire indicates higher level of autistic traits. The Chinese version of the BAPQ showed good reliability and validity (α = 0.78; 52).

The Chinese translation of the short ten-item version of the Autism Spectrum Quotient for children (46) is a parent-report questionnaire that assesses autistic traits in children. It consists of 10 descriptive statements about preferences and habits. The sample items in the questionnaire include “S/he usually concentrates more on the whole picture, rather than the small details” and “S/he finds it easy to go back and forth between different activities”. For each item, participants select one of four response options: “definitely agree,” “slightly agree,” “slightly disagree,” or “definitely disagree”. However, the four options are grouped and scored as either 0 or 1. A higher total score of the 10 items indicates the presence of more autistic traits. The Chinese version of the AQ-10 for children demonstrated satisfactory internal consistency (α = 0.79–0.84) and concurrent validity with the Social Responsiveness Scale (r = 0.79–0.81; 53).

The Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) is a widely used parent-completed assessment for rating behavioural, emotional, and social problems in children and adolescents (47). The CBCL consists of 113 questions. Each question is scored using a 3-point Likert scale (0 = absent, 1= occurs sometimes and 2= occurs often). It provides summed scores for three summary scales that include: internalising, externalising, and total problems (maximum achievable score = 226). It is widely used for clinical assessment in China and its reliability in the Chinese population was found to be 0.76 for internalising, 0.81 for externalising, and 0.83 for total problems (5456). Multiple studies have also confirmed its validity (57, 58).

Results

Descriptive statistics were calculated and presented in Table 1. The mean AQ-10 score and the mean BAPQ score were slightly above the cut-off of 6 and 3.15 suggested in Allison et al. (46) and Hurley et al. (45) respectively. Figure 1 shows the distribution of the AQ-10, BAPQ and PCDI scores. The majority of the parents did not have a graduate degree and had relatively low household income. Parents’ ratings on perceived quality of parent-child interaction and child behavioral problems were comparable to previous studies (e.g., 48, 59).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Distribution of the scores of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient-10 (AQ-10), the Broad Autism Phenotype Questionnaire (BAPQ) and the Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction (PCDI).

Correlations of key measures are presented in Table 2. It was found that higher parent education level was associated with lower PCDI score, lower BAPQ score, higher household income, and higher AQ-10 score. Lower BAPQ was also associated with lower CBCL score. Multiple regression analysis was then conducted to investigate the extent to which the PCDI score could be predicted by key measures and other demographics (i.e., age and gender of both parent and child) given their potential influence on parent-child interaction (e.g., adolescents tend to spend less time with parents than younger children; 60).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Pearson correlations among key measures.

There was independence of observations as indicated by a Durbin-Watson statistic of 2 (61). VIF was less than 10 and Tolerance was greater than .1 for all variables indicating an absence of multicollinearity (62). The Breush-Pagan test revealed no evidence of heteroscedasticity. Residuals were normally distributed based on a Normal Q-Q plot, A total of 10% of variance of the PCDI score was explained by the regression model (Multiple R2 = .19, Adjusted R2 = .10; see Table 3). The overall association between the predictors and the PCDI score was significant, F(9, 81) = 2.12, p <.05. Table 3 showed that the PCDI score was uniquely predicted by parent’s education level while all other predictors were controlled for.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Multiple regression analysis of the contribution of all predictors to parent-child dysfunctional interaction.

Discussion

This study is the first to identify factors that were associated with perceived quality of parent-child interaction in Chinese parents of autistic children. Results of the bivariate correlation analysis and the multiple regression analyses were consistent, which indicated that parent’s education level was associated with and the only significant predictor of their perceived quality of parent-child interaction. Higher education levels in parents were significantly linked to improved interaction quality, as evidenced by lower scores on the Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction scale. This insight provides implications to intervention strategies for Chinese parents of autistic children and public policies on improving overall education levels in China and other non-WEIRD countries.

Our findings are consistent with those reported in the literature of typical development and developmental disability that parent’s education level was positively associated with parent-child interaction quality (e.g., 27, 63). However, our findings diverge from those of Ruble et al.’s (30), who found no association between parent demographics and parent-child interaction quality in a limited sample of autistic children and parents. This inconsistency may be attributed to sample differences between the two studies. With a larger and more diverse sample, our study was better positioned to detect a significant relationship between perceived parent-child interaction quality and parental education level. Parents’ gender also showed a potential of predicting their perceived quality of parent-child interaction (p = .05). However, this finding needs to be interpreted with caution given that the sample was largely female (90%). As such, our results demonstrated that among Chinese parents of autistic children, perceived parent-child interaction quality was solely linked to parental education level, rather than any child-specific characteristics such as autistic traits or behavioural problems, despite the inherent bidirectional nature of parent-child interactions. This absence of significant associations between perceived quality of parent-child interaction and child autistic traits and behavioural problems contradicts prior literature (e.g., 4, 16). However, our findings reveal a positive association between parental education level and child autistic traits, suggesting that parents with higher education levels were more likely to report a greater prevalence of autistic traits in their autistic child. Meanwhile, these parents also reported higher perceived quality of parent-child interaction. These findings suggested that Chinese parents with higher education levels may possess enhanced competencies in interacting with their autistic children while acknowledging their child’s autistic traits. Previous research has similarly indicated that mothers with higher education levels in WEIRD countries tended to engage in a more mutually affective interactions with their children (27) and demonstrated better awareness of their children’s autistic traits and obtained autism diagnoses for their children (e.g., 64, 65). Therefore, it is plausible that parental education level has positive effects on autism awareness and perceived quality of parent-child interaction among WEIRD and non-WEIRD parents.

Another explanation could relate to the Chinese culture norms and perceptions regarding autism. As noted previously, Chinese parents often perceive themselves as primary educators of their children, with their level of education potentially affecting their ability to fulfil this role. This is particularly significant given the scarcity and expense of services available for autistic children in China (e.g., 40, 42). Meanwhile, prevalent societal beliefs in China attribute autism to inadequate parenting practices (33, 34) - a stigma that may be internalised by parents irrespective of their educational background. For example, participants in Su et al. (34) expressed self-doubt, with one suggesting, “I suspected it might be due to my way of teaching,” while another despite being well-educated, indicated, “You need to look to parents if the child is not taught well.” As such, Chinese parents may perceive themselves, rather than their autistic children, as primarily responsible for the quality of parent-child interaction. This perspective could explain the observed relationship between perceived parent-child interaction quality and parent’s education level in our study, wherein child characteristics did not demonstrate any significant associations. In contrast, WEIRD parents typically acknowledge the neurological underpinnings of autism (e.g., 31, 32), potentially leading them to attribute perceived interaction quality to child characteristics such as child autistic traits and behavioural problems, as reported in studies like Hobson et al. (4), rather than parent demographics as suggested in Ruble et al. (30). Nevertheless, further cross-cultural investigations are necessary to confirm such differences between WEIRD and non-WEIRD contexts.

The current study also found that perceived quality of parent-child interaction was not associated with household income despite that parents with higher education levels tended to report higher household income. Given that household income is the total income of all household members, it may not have a significant contribution to the main caregivers’ perceived parent-child interaction quality as the main caregivers’ education level may have. On the other hand, higher household income may not guarantee access to the limited and costly services available in China, particularly in the specific city from which our sample was recruited. As this study represents the inaugural exploration of factors associated with perceived quality of parent-child interaction in Chinese parents of autistic children, further investigations across diverse regions of China are needed. However, the absence of association between perceived parent-child interaction quality and household income observed in this study is consistent with Ruble et al.’s findings (30) obtained from a WEIRD sample. No association was also observed between perceived quality of parent-child interaction and parent’s autistic traits in the current study. This is consistent with Parr et al.’s (26) initial finding that there were no significant differences in mother-child interaction scores between mothers with high versus low BAP traits. Although parent’s autistic traits may associate with parenting practices and challenges (24, 25), they may not have an influence on perceived quality of parent-child interaction in both WEIRD and non-WEIRD parents of autistic children.

Nevertheless, there are other parental factors that may impact on parent-child interaction but were not considered in the current study. For example, mothers with panic disorder/agoraphobia were less sensitive during mother-child interaction than mothers without current mental disorders (66). There may also be a cross-partner effects of parental mental health on parent-child interaction. For example, mother’s parenting stress level was negatively associated with father’s warmth toward their autistic child 12 months later (67). As such, parental mental health could be a contributing factor to parents’ perceived quality of parent-child interaction, particularly in parents of autistic children who are vulnerable to negative psychological outcomes (e.g., 68, 69). Furthermore, Yu et al. (70) suggested that Chinese citizens were more likely to hold stigma beliefs towards autism compared to United States citizens. In this sense, Chinese parents of autistic children may frequently encounter stigmatised experiences, potentially impacting on their mental health and perceived quality of parent-child interaction. These links among affiliate stigma, parental mental health, and perceived quality of parent-child interaction may or may not be specific to the Chinese social context. Further research would be required to test for this possibility.

To conclude, our findings underscored the significance of parental education level as a unique predictor of perceived parent-child interaction quality among Chinese parents of autistic children. This association may be explained by the possibility that parents with higher education levels may possess enhanced competencies in interacting with their autistic children. It may also be explained by parents’ role as educators within the Chinese cultural context, coupled with prevailing societal attitudes towards autism. While there is a growing trend towards improved education levels and awareness of autism in China, disparities persist in access to essential information, resources, and services, particularly in certain regions, which can impact the perceived quality of parent-child interaction. Future studies should aim to test how these findings may be generalised across China and other non-WEIRD countries. It is also important to note that this study focused on parents’ perceived quality of parent-child interaction rather than objective measures of parent-child interaction quality. Future studies could therefore incorporate detailed observational measures to rigorously assess the quality of interactions between parent and child as well as among family members. It may also be essential to measure these interactions under a neurodivergent rather than neurotypical lens given the double empathy hypothesis, which suggests that neurotypical people have just as much difficulty in understanding neurodivergent people as vice versa (71). Future studies could also adopt longitudinal designs to elucidate the causal dynamics between key variables and parent-child interaction quality. Nevertheless, our findings provide implications to intervention strategies for Chinese parents of autistic children, focusing on developing their competencies in interacting with their autistic children despite their education level rather than addressing their child’s autistic traits and behavioural characteristics. Our findings also provide implications to corresponding education and social policies with a particular focus on improving overall education level and promoting public understanding of autism in China and other non-WEIRD countries.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The study involving humans was approved by Nottingham Trent University’s Schools of Business, Law and Social Sciences Research Ethics Committee. The study was conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

FW: Writing – original draft. WD: Writing – review & editing. L-SI: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was partly supported by the research funding awarded to L-SI from the Department of Psychology at Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom, and research funding awarded to FW (Nantong Municipal Science and Technology Bureau Key Technology Research Project (ID: MSZ2022032), Nantong Municipal Health Commission Surface-Level Directive Scientific Research Project (ID: MS2022075), and Nantong Municipal Social Livelihood Science and Technology Plan (Guidance) Project (ID: MSZ2023011)).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. (2022). doi: 10.1176/appi.book.9780890425787

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Hastings SE, Hastings RP, Swales MA, Hughes JC. Emotional and behavioural problems of children with autism spectrum disorder attending mainstream schools. Int J Dev Disabil. (2022) 68(5):633–40.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

3. Totsika V, Hastings RP, Emerson E, Lancaster GA, Berridge DM. A population-based investigation of behavioural and emotional problems and maternal mental health: Associations with autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2011) 52:91–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02295.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hobson JA, Tarver L, Beurkens N, Peter Hobson R. The relation between severity of Autism and parent-child interaction: A study in the context of relationship development intervention. J Abnormal Child Psychol. (2016) 44:745–55. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0067-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Mello C, Rivard M, Morin D, Patel S, Morin M. Symptom severity, internalized and externalized behavioral and emotional problems: Links with parenting stress in mothers of children recently diagnosed with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2022) 52:2400–13. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05131-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Bronfenbrenner U. The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press (1979).

Google Scholar

7. McCall RB, Groark CJ, Hawk BN, Julian MM, Merz EC, Rosas JM, et al. Early parent-child interaction and children’s development: Lessons from the St. Petersburg-USA Orphanage Intervention Research Project. Clin Child Family Psychol Rev. (2019) 22:208–24. doi: 10.1007/s10567-018-0270-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Abidin RR. Parenting Stress Index, Third Edition: Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc (1995).

Google Scholar

9. Frank SJ, Roubal KC, Breitzer GM, Godin JL. Separating the effects of child problems and parent-child interactions on parent strain. J Child Family Stud. (2017) 26:248–61. doi: 10.1007/s10826-016-0560-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Temelturk RD, Yurumez E, Cıkılı Uytun M, Oztop DB. Parent-child interaction, parental attachment styles and parental alexithymia levels of children with ASD. Res Dev Disabil. (2021) 112:103922–2. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2021.103922

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Del Rosario C, Nixon E, Quigley J, Whitehouse AJO, Maybery MT. Parent-child interaction and developmental outcomes in children with typical and elevated likelihood of autism. Infant Behav Dev. (2023) 71:101830. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101830

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Pijl MKJ, Bontinck C, Rommelse NNJ, Begum Ali J, Cauvet E, Niedzwiecka A, et al. Parent-child interaction during the first year of life in infants at elevated likelihood of autism spectrum disorder. Infant Behav Dev. (2021) 62:101521. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101521

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Campbell SB, Leezenbaum NB, Mahoney AS, Day TN, Schmidt EN. Social engagement with parents in 11-month-old siblings at high and low genetic risk for autism spectrum disorder. Autism. (2015) 19:915–24. doi: 10.1177/1362361314555146

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Landry R, Bryson SE. Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2004) 45:1115–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00304.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Canu D, van der Paelt S, Canal-Bedia R, Posada M, Vanvuchelen M, Roeyers H. Early non-social behavioural indicators of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in siblings at elevated likelihood for ASD: A systematic review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2021) 30:497–538. doi: 10.1007/s00787-020-01487-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Huang C-Y, Yen H-C, Tseng M-H, Tung L-C, Chen Y-D, Chen K-L. Impacts of autistic behaviors, emotional and behavioral problems on parenting stress in parents of children with Autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2014) 44:1383–90. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-2000-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Bozicevic L, Lucas C, Magai DN, Ooi Y, Maliwichi L, Sharp H, et al. Evaluating parent-child interactions in low-and middle-income countries: a systematic review of tools and methods. J Reprod Infant Psychol. (2024), 1–36. doi: 10.1080/02646838.2024.2321615

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Meirsschaut M, Warreyn P, Roeyers H. What is the impact of autism on mother-child interactions within families with a child with autism spectrum disorder? Autism Res. (2011) 4:358–67. doi: 10.1002/aur.217

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Wan MW, Green J, Elsabbagh M, Johnson M, Charman T, Plummer F, et al. Quality of interaction between at-risk infants and parent at 12-15 months is associated with 3-year autism outcome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2012) 54:763–71. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Bottema-Beutel K, Kim SY. A systematic literature review of autism research on caregiver talk. Autism Res. (2021) 14:432–49. doi: 10.1002/aur.2461

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Bora E, Aydın A, Saraç T, Kadak MT, Köse S. Heterogeneity of subclinical autistic traits among parents of children with autism spectrum disorder: Identifying the broader autism phenotype with a data-driven method. Autism Res. (2017) 10:321–6. doi: 10.1002/aur.1661

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Rubenstein E, Chawla D. Broader Autism Phenotype in parents of children with Autism: A systematic review of percentage estimates. J Child Family Stud. (2018) 27:1705–20. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-1026-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Sucksmith E, Roth I, Hoekstra RA. Autistic traits below the clinical threshold: Re-examining the Broader Autism Phenotype in the 21st century. Neuropsychol Rev. (2011) 21:360–89. doi: 10.1007/s11065-011-9183-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kulasinghe K, Whittingham K, Mitchell AE. Mental health, broad autism phenotype and psychological inflexibility in mothers of young children with autism spectrum disorder in Australia: A cross-sectional survey. Autism. (2021) 25:1187–202. doi: 10.1177/1362361320984625

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. DeLucia EA, Semones O, Stanton K, McDonnell CG. Advancing understanding of autism within families: Parent broader autism phenotype traits differentially relate to parenting behavior. J Child Family Stud. (2022) 31:753–63. doi: 10.1007/s10826-021-02022-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Parr JR, Gray L, Wigham S, McConachie H, Couteur AL. Measuring the relationship between the parental Broader Autism Phenotype, parent-child interaction, and children’s progress following parent mediated intervention. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2015) 20:24–30. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2015.07.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Potharst ES, Schuengel C, Last BF, van Wassenaer AG, Kok JH, Houtzager BA, et al. Difference in mother-child interaction between preterm- and term-born preschoolers with and without disabilities. Acta Paedia. (2012) 101:597–603. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2012.02599.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. De Falco S, Emer A, Martini L, Rigo P, Pruner S, Venuti P. Predictors of mother-child interaction quality and child attachment security in at-risk families. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:898. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00898

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Ho LLK, Li WHC, Cheung AT, Luo Y, Xia W, Chung JOK. Impact of poverty on parent-child relationships, parental stress, and parenting practices. Front Public Health. (2022) 10:849408. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.849408

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Ruble L, McDuffie A, King AS, & Lorenz D. Parent responsiveness and social interaction behaviors of young children with autism. Topics Early Childhood Special Educ. (2008) 28:158–70. doi: 10.1177/0271121408323009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Anbar NN, Dardennes RM, Prado-Netto A, Kaye K, Contejean Y. Treatment choices in autism spectrum disorder: The role of parental illness perceptions. Res Dev Disabil. (2010) 31:817–28. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.02.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Dardennes RM, Al Anbar NN, Prado-Netto A, Kaye K, Contejean Y, Al Anbar NN. Treating the cause of illness rather than the symptoms: Parental causal beliefs and treatment choices in autism spectrum disorder. Res Dev Disabil. (2011) 32:1137–46. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.01.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Wang Z, Shi Y. Current social investigation of cognitive on autistic children. J Nanchang Normal Univ. (2015) 36:75–80. doi: 10.1080/1034912X.2019.1687857

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Su S, Paynter J, Gilmore L. Chinese parents’ understanding of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Int J Disabil Develop Educ. (2021) 68:414–26. doi: 10.1080/1034912X.2019.1687857

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Bakare MO, Agomoh AO, Ebigbo PO, Eaton J, Okonkwo KO, Onwukwe JU, et al. Etiological explanation, treatability and preventability of childhood autism: a survey of Nigerian healthcare workers’ opinion. Ann Gen Psychiatry. (2009) 8:1–8. doi: 10.1186/1744-859X-8-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Gona JK, Newton CR, Rimba K, Mapenzi R, Kihara M, Van de Vijver FJ, et al. Parents’ and professionals’ perceptions on causes and treatment options for autism spectrum disorders (ASD) in a multicultural context on the Kenyan coast. PloS One. (2015) 10:e0132729. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132729

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Dyches TT, Wilder LK, Sudweeks RR, Obiakor FE, Algozzine B. Multicultural issues in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2004) 34:211–22. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000022611.80478.73

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Chen JY. Chinese parents’ perception of autism spectrum disorders: An exploration of the influence of culture. Waltham, Massachusetts: Brandeis University (2014).

Google Scholar

39. Xu Y, Farver JA, Zhang Z, Zeng Q, Yu L, Cai B. Mainland Chinese parenting styles and parent-child interaction. Int J Behav Dev. (2005) 29:524–31. doi: 10.1080/01650250500147121

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Huang AX, Jia M, Wheeler JJ. Children with autism in the People’s Republic of China: Diagnosis, legal issues, and educational services. J Autism Dev Disord. (2013) 43:1991–2001. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1722-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Sun X, Allison C, Auyeung B, Matthews FE, Baron-Cohen S, Brayne C. What is available for case identification in autism research in mainland China? Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2013) 7:579–90. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2012.11.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Ou JJ, Shi LJ, Xun GL, Chen C, Wu RR, Luo XR, et al. Employment and financial burden of families with preschool children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders in urban China: Results from a descriptive study. BMC Psychiatry. (2015) 15:3. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-0382-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Abidin RR. Parenting stress index-short form. Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology Press (1990).

Google Scholar

44. Luo J, Wang M-C, Gao Y, Zeng H, Yang W, Chen W, et al. Refining the parenting stress index–short form (PSI-SF) in Chinese parents. Assessment. (2021) 28:551–66. doi: 10.1177/1073191119847757

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Hurley RSE, Losh M, Parlier M, Reznick JS, Piven J. The broad autism phenotype questionnaire. J Autism Dev Disord. (2007) 37:1679–90. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0299-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Allison C, Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S. Toward brief “red flags” for autism screening: The short autism spectrum quotient and the short quantitative checklist in 1,000 cases and 3,000 controls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2012) 51:202–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.11.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families (2001).

Google Scholar

48. Huang XQ, Zhang H, Chen S. Neuropsychiatric symptoms, parenting stress and social support in Chinese mothers of children with autism spectrum disorder. Curr Med Sci. (2019) 39:291–7. doi: 10.1007/s11596-019-2033-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Wang Y, Song J, Chen J, Zhang Y, Wan Q, Huang Z. Examining the psychometric properties of the simplified Parenting Stress Index-Short Form with Chinese parents of children with cerebral palsy. Soc Behav Personal: Int J. (2021) 49:1–10. doi: 10.2224/sbp.9857

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Nayar K, Sealock JM, Maltman N, Bush L, Cook EH, Davis LK, et al. Elevated polygenic burden for Autism Spectrum Disorder is associated with the broad autism phenotype in mothers of individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Biol Psychiatry (1969). (2021) 89:476–85. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.08.029

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Yousefvand S, Dadgar H, Mohammadi MR, Maroufizadeh S, Yousefvand S. Broader Autism Phenotype and communication skills in parents of children with Autism. J Modern Rehabil (Online). (2022) 16:338–46. doi: 10.18502/jmr.v16i4.10761

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Lin J, Fu X, Dai S, Zeng Y, Yang R, Liu R, et al. The Broad Autism Phenotype Questionnaire (BAPQ): Strengths, weaknesses and future improvements in Chinese version. Asian J Psychiatry. (2021) 60:102655–5. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2021.102655

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Sun F, Dai M, Lin L, Sun X, Murray AL, Auyeung B, et al. Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of autism spectrum quotient-children’s version: A sex-specific analysis. Autism Res. (2019) 12:303–15. doi: 10.1002/aur.2053

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Leung PWL, Kwong SL, Tang CP, Ho TP, Hung SF, Lee CC, et al. Test-retest reliability and criterion validity of the Chinese version of CBCL, TRF, and YSR. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2006) 47:970–3. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01570.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Tepper P, Liu X, Guo C, Zhai J, Liu T, Li C. Depressive symptoms in Chinese children and adolescents: Parent, teacher, and self-reports. J Affect Disord. (2008) 111:291–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.03.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Li F, Tang Y, Li F, Fang S, Liu X, Tao M, et al. Psychological distress in parents of children with autism spectrum disorder: A cross-sectional study based on 683 mother-father dyads. J Pediatr Nurs. (2022) 65:e49–55. doi: 10.1016/j.pedn.2022.02.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Weine AM, Phillips JS, Achenbach TM. Behavioral and emotional problems among Chinese and American children: Parent and teacher reports for ages 6 to 13. J Abnormal Child Psychol. (1995) 23:619–39. doi: 10.1007/BF01447666

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Liu X, Kurita H, Guo C, Miyake Y, Ze J, Cao H. Prevalence and risk factors of behavioral and emotional problems among Chinese children aged 6 through 11 years. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (1999) 38:708–15. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199906000-00018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Ooi YP, Rescorla L, Ang RP, Woo B, Fung DSS. Identification of autism spectrum disorders using the child behavior checklist in Singapore. J Autism Dev Disord. (2011) 41:1147–56. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1015-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Milkie MA, Mattingly MJ, Nomaguchi KM, Bianchi SM, Robinson JP. The time squeeze: Parental statuses and feelings about time with children. J Marriage Family. (2004) 66:739–61. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-2445.2004.00050.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Field A. Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. 4th ed. Los Angeles: SAGE Publications Ltd (2013).

Google Scholar

62. Hair JF, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE. Multivariate data analysis. 7th ed. Harlow, England: Pearson (2014).

Google Scholar

63. Green CM, Berkule SB, Dreyer BP, Fierman AH, Huberman HS, Klass PE, et al. Maternal literacy and associations between education and the cognitive home environment in low-income families. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2009) 163:832–7. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.136

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Durkin MS, Maenner MJ, Meaney FJ, Levy SE, di Guiseppi C, Nicholas JS, et al. Socioeconomic inequality in the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from a U.S. cross-sectional study. PloS One. (2010) 5:e11551. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011551

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. King MD, Bearman PS. Socioeconomic status and the increased prevalence of autism in California. Am Sociol Rev. (2011) 76:320–46. doi: 10.1177/0003122411399389

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Schneider S, Houweling JE, Gommlich-Schneider S, Klein C, Nündel B, Wolke D. Effect of maternal panic disorder on mother-child interaction and relation to child anxiety and child self-efficacy. Arch Women’s Ment Health. (2009) 12:251–9. doi: 10.1007/s00737-009-0072-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Hickey EJ, Hartley SL, Papp L. Psychological well-being and parent-child relationship quality in relation to child autism: An actor-partner modeling approach. Family Process. (2020) 59:636–50. doi: 10.1111/famp.12432

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Mugno D, Ruta L, D’Arrigo VG, Mazzone L. Impairment of quality of life in parents of children and adolescents with pervasive developmental disorder. Health Qual Life Outcomes. (2007) 5:1–9. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-5-22

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Hou YM, Stewart L, Iao LS, Wu CC. Parenting stress and depressive symptoms in Taiwanese mothers of young children with autism spectrum disorder: Association with children’s behavioural problems. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil. (2018) 31:1113–21. doi: 10.1111/jar.12471

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Yu L, Stronach S, Harrison AJ. Public knowledge and stigma of autism spectrum disorder: Comparing China with the United States. Autism. (2020) 24(6):1531–1545. doi: 10.1177/136236131990083

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Milton DE. On the ontological status of autism: The ‘double empathy problem’. Disabil Soc. (2012) 27:883–7. doi: 10.1080/09687599.2012.710008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism, parent-child interaction quality, parental education level, Chinese, non-WEIRD countries

Citation: Wang F, Du W and Iao L-S (2024) Perceived quality of parent-child interaction in parents of autistic children: relationship with parental education level. Front. Psychiatry 15:1433823. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1433823

Received: 17 May 2024; Accepted: 30 September 2024;
Published: 23 October 2024.

Edited by:

Antonio Narzisi, Stella Maris Foundation (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Olivia Boorom, University of Kansas, United States
Arianna Bentenuto, University of Trento, Italy

Copyright © 2024 Wang, Du and Iao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lai-Sang Iao, lai-sang.iao@ntu.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.