Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 24 May 2024
Sec. Psychopathology

Cognitive and emotional empathy in acute and remitted anorexia nervosa: a systematic review

  • 1School of Psychology, Deakin University, Geelong, VIC, Australia
  • 2Institute for Health and Sport, Victoria University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia

Background: Impairments in empathy are well established in anorexia nervosa (AN). It is unclear, however, whether these deficits only occur in the acute phases of AN due to neurocognitive impacts of starvation (often referred to as context-dependent, or state-like), or if deficits remain once remission has been achieved (trait-like). This debate is commonly referred to as the ‘state vs trait’ debate.

Objective: This systematic review aims to summarise existing literature regarding empathy in AN, and to investigate whether empathy deficits in AN are state- or trait-based.

Method: A total of 1014 articles were identified, and seven articles remained after the screening process. These seven articles, comparing empathy across three groups (acute AN, remission of AN, and non-clinical controls), were evaluated and summarised in accordance with PRISMA guidelines. Articles were required to have included all three groups and report on either cognitive empathy and/or emotional empathy.

Results: The majority of studies were of satisfactory quality. The results identified were inconsistent, with few articles lending some support to the ‘state’ hypothesis and others producing nonsignificant results.

Conclusions: There is minimal literature comparing empathy in acute and remission phases of AN. While there were some inconsistencies in included articles, some data indicate that there may be slight improvements to emotional and cognitive empathy following recovery of AN. Further research is needed to better enrich knowledge regarding the role of state vs trait with regard to neurocognitive difficulties experienced by individuals with AN.

Systematic review registration: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?RecordID=335669, identifier CRD42022335669.

1 Introduction

Recent research has identified various neurocognitive impairments in individuals with anorexia nervosa (AN). From a social cognitive perspective, reduced empathy has been reported in AN samples (14), similar to effects observed in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (57). Indeed, high levels of autistic traits, including socially relevant characteristics, have been noted to correlate with eating disorder psychopathology in clinical and non-clinical adolescent and adult samples (810). Notably, these empathy deficits have been shown to be restricted to AN specifically, and are not observed in other eating disorders such as bulimia nervosa (11).

Despite a growing body of evidence indicative of reduced empathic ability among AN samples, conflicting findings and inconsistencies are commonplace in the literature. Individuals with AN have difficulty identifying and recognizing the emotions of others (12, 13), which includes deciphering others’ intentions, thoughts, beliefs, and emotions (4). This skill is commonly termed cognitive empathy, and is also referred to as “mentalising” ability or “theory of mind”. Cognitive empathy is the capacity to understand and attribute emotional states to others by interpreting behaviour and intention, and is a crucial skill for developing and maintaining meaningful relationships (14, 15). By contrast, emotional empathy goes beyond inferring emotions and involves actual changes to an individual’s own emotional state, congruent with the emotions of the individual they are observing (16), and is also considered vital in establishing high quality interpersonal relationships (15). In this regard, Gaggero et al. (17) provide evidence to suggest that individuals with AN are able to appropriately adapt their facial expression in response to observing another individual in a distressing situation (17). Together, these findings are seemingly in line with the findings a review and meta-analysis which provides evidence supporting the notion of impaired cognitive empathy in AN, but also suggest that affective empathy might be relatively intact in among this population (18). In considering, however, that women with AN experience increased levels of alexithymia, or ability to recognize and label emotional states (19), the functional benefits of this “intact” emotional empathy is unclear. Other factors, such as participant age (18, 20) and methodological factors such as inconsistency between tasks and specific mentalising sub-domains measured (21) might account for some of the inconsistency observed in the literature.

Individuals with AN have been shown to have smaller social support groups (22), demonstrate diminished responsiveness and sensitivity to social cues (23, 24), and can be more negative in their interpretation of social situations (25), particularly when feeling ostracized (26). Research investigating the presence of social deficits in AN and illness outcomes found lower effectiveness of treatment and longer AN duration when social impairments were present (2730). As these social and interpersonal impairments affect engagement in relationships, there is likely a secondary impact on intervention (31), as the typical approach of individual psychotherapies for AN often relies on quality therapeutic alliances (32). Indeed, McIntosh et al. (33) report interpersonal psychotherapy approaches to be least effective in treating AN. Interestingly, improvements in alexithymia symptoms has been observed to be related to improvements in eating disorder psychopathology (34), which highlights the importance of socio-cognitive awareness in the treatment of AN.

The consensus among researchers is that the presence of interpersonal difficulties in AN negatively impacts treatment outcomes (24, 35, 36). Given a large component of success in AN treatment is dependent on therapeutic relationship quality (37, 38), deficits in socialisation and possible poor relationship quality may hinder the therapeutic relationship and, by extension, the effectiveness of treatment.

While empathy impairments have been shown to be associated with AN (18, 39), they are not a diagnostic feature of the condition (40). Rather, such social difficulties are more typically associated with neurodevelopmental conditions such as ASD (40), clinically significant traits of which are highly prevalent in AN populations (41). Indeed, impairments in empathy similarly occur between autistic individuals and those with AN (12, 13, 42), though there is research to suggest greater difficulty among autistic cohorts (13). It is unclear, however, why individuals with AN display neurocognitive traits akin to ASD. A prominent theory explaining the presence of autistic features in AN speculates that the physiological and cognitive deficits caused by starvation can produce ASD-like symptoms, including empathy difficulties (20, 43, 44). This explanation is supported by research comparing acute versus recovered patients, which has identified that autistic symptomology is more profound in acute AN when starvation is present (also referred to as the ‘state-like’ hypothesis) (4, 45, 46). Conversely, it has also been suggested that autistic symptomology in AN is more stable or trait-based, where, irrespective of starvation and malnutrition effects, individuals in acute and recovered phases of AN both demonstrate cognitive and emotional empathy challenges (also referred to as the ‘trait-like’ hypothesis) (22, 24, 47, 48). Moreover, literature observing autistic symptomology prior to AN onset (49) also supports this “trait-like” hypothesis. Overall, a consensus remains unclear and these mixed findings, commonly referred to as the ‘state versus trait debate’, indicate a need for further investigation that directly compares the presence of ASD symptomology in acute AN and recovered AN to determine which hypothesis is supported or if elements of both ‘state’ and ‘trait’ may explain the presence of autistic traits in AN.

Given these gaps in the literature, this review aimed to summarise existing knowledge of empathy in AN to investigate the impact that starvation status may have on the presence of cognitive and emotional empathy deficits in AN, which will inform the ‘state vs trait’ debate. Such insights should have direct relevance for recommendations for treatment strategies.

2 Method

The review process was conducted in accordance with the PRISMA guidelines (50) and registered with PROSPERO (ID: CRD42022335669). We acknowledge here a slight deviation from the registered protocol. In the planning phases of this manuscript, it was intended that a systematic review and meta-analysis be conducted. As detailed below, however, only a small number of studies with varying outcome variables met inclusion criteria and were included for review. As such, only a systematic review is presented and there have been some changes to the team composition.

2.1 Search strategy

An initial literature search was conducted on the 12th of August 2022 using the Medline, PsychINFO, and Scopus databases, which cover literature from psychological and other allied health disciplines. Search terms were (a) “Anorexia Nervosa” OR “Eating Disorder” OR “Anore*” and (b) “Mentalisation” OR “Mentali#ing” OR “Theory of Mind” OR “Mental flexib*” OR “Perspective Taking” OR “emotion* awar*” OR “emotion* perception” OR “emotion* detection” OR “emotion* identification” OR “emotion* recognition” OR “Cognitive Empathy” OR “Empathy” OR “Empathy Quotient” OR “Empath*” OR “EQ” OR “compassion*” OR “Emotion* Contagion.” Key words were selected based on the variables of interest for review, the author’s expertise, and by screening key words often presented in relevant literature. Searches in each database were limited to English language and peer-reviewed articles. This search was repeated on the 19th of August 2023. One reviewer (IEG) completed searchers and compiled outcomes into the RAYAAN article screening platform (51). One additional paper that was not identified by the initial search strategy but fit the above specified criteria was included by the researchers. To check that any additional papers had not similarly been missed by the initial search, an expanded search was conducted in December 2023 using the following additional search terms: “Social Cognition” OR “Affective Cognition” OR “Reading the Minds in the Eyes” OR “The Films Expressions Task” OR “The Multifaceted Empathy Test” OR “The Frith-Happé Animations.” No additional papers were identified from this search.

2.1.1 Eligibility criteria

Articles were considered for inclusion in the review if they met the following criteria:

1. Published in English.

2. Peer-reviewed.

3. Included three groups: an AN acute group, an AN remission whose weight had been restored, and a non-clinical comparison group (Healthy Control [HC] group). AN groups were required to confirm via self-report that they had received a diagnosis of AN according to either the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders or the International Classification of Diseases diagnostic criteria.

4. Reported on at least one of the outcomes of interest: cognitive empathy or emotional empathy. No restrictions were made on the types of outcome measures studies used to assess these outcomes.

No restrictions were placed on date of publication or the age of the sample.

2.2 Screening

As depicted in Figure 1, the initial search yielded 1014 articles. 403 duplicates were removed, and two reviewers (IEG and MK) then completed an independent and blind title and abstract screening of all 638 articles. Any conflicts from this screening were resolved through mutual reassessment of the articles. 6.4% of the 638 articles presented a conflict, and, after resolution, 96 articles remained to be screened as full reports. From this pool of articles, 6 met inclusion criteria and were thus included in the present review. No additional articles were found on the repeated search in August or December, 2023. However, one additional article was found by the researchers outside of the formal search process. As such, 7 articles are included in the review.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 PRISMA flowchart describing identification of literature. Adapted from Page et al. (50).

2.3 Data extraction

The following details were extracted from each included article: first author, year of publication, overall and subgroup sample size, age, body mass index (BMI), biological sex percentages of the sample, and outcome measures used. All data were manually extracted from each included article.

2.4 Quality assessment

The quality of included studies was assessed using the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) (52), which evaluates studies in relation to three categories: participant selection (representativeness, size, non-respondents), comparison between groups in the study (ascertainment of screening, comparability of subjects), and outcome assessment (assessment, statistical test). There are four criteria for sample selection, two criteria for comparison, and three criteria for outcome assessment. The NOS uses a star-based system where each study can obtain one star for each assessed criterion in the selection and outcome criterion and a maximum of two stars for the comparability criterion, thereby achieving a maximum of nine stars overall. Studies are then determined to be either unsatisfactory (0-4 stars), satisfactory (5-6 stars), good (7-8 stars), or very good (9-10 stars) based on their star rating.

3 Results

3.1 Description of studies

A summary of these data is provided in Table 1. Of the seven included studies the average age range was between 18-30. Half of the studies had an all-female sample; however, all studies’ samples were above 90% female. AN diagnosis was determined using the DSM-5 in three studies (5557), using the DSM-IV-TR in the three studies (14, 54, 58), and using the ICD-10 in one study (53). All seven articles employed quantitative data analysis approaches, but each investigates different outcome measures to assess empathy.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Summary of studies.

3.2 Quality assessment

As seen in Table 2, only two studies received a ‘good’ rating. All other studies were ‘satisfactory’, and one study was ‘unsatisfactory’. All studies had representative samples (employed random sampling), used validated measures to confirm AN diagnosis and selected appropriate statistical tests to analyse data. The majority of studies had sound response rates from participants with few dropouts. However, only two studies explicitly investigated and accounted for potential confounds such as gender and comorbidities, e.g. anxiety, depression and ASD, and only one study included a justification for their sample size by providing a power analysis. All studies failed to either report or utilise blinded assessment.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Newcastle-Ottawa Scale Quality Assessment: Star Totals for Each Domain.

3.3 Emotional empathy

Two articles investigated emotional empathy (56, 58). While the results of both studies indicated superior performance on emotional empathy tasks in control participants, the small effect only reached statistical significance in one of these studies (56) Both studies also indicated that the AN remission group performed better than the AN acute group but worse than the non-clinical comparison group, though the effects were small and not significant. Additionally, both articles observed a significant positive correlation between BMI and emotional empathy, further supporting that emotional empathy improves as weight increases. Moreover, 58 also noted a significant difference between the AN acute group and the non-clinical comparison group. As both studies reported worse performance by the AN groups, this outcome confirms emotional empathy deficits in AN observed in previous literature (14).

3.4 Cognitive empathy

One study found that the AN remission group performed significantly better than the AN acute group with a large effect size, and performed comparably to the health controls group when recognising negative emotional states (14). Additionally, there was no significant difference between groups for positive valance, but the data indicated that the AN acute group performed (non-significantly) the worst, followed by the AN remission and then health controls group, again with small effects. There was, however, a small effect size involving the AN remission group, whereby this group demonstrated superior performance to the acute AN group, and inferior performance to the healthy controls group (14). Once again, for neutral states, the same pattern of performance was observed where the AN acute group performed worst, albeit this was not statistically significant, followed by the AN remission and then the healthy controls group, which was again not statistically significant. Although not significant, four (14, 28, 55, 56) of the six articles investigating cognitive empathy observed that the healthy controls group demonstrated the highest accuracy in emotion recognition followed by AN remission group and then the AN acute group.

Moreover, one study (56) reported a small but significant positive correlation between BMI and cognitive empathy. Furthermore, one study discerned that the AN acute group performed significantly worse than the non-clinical comparison group, with a large effect size, once again confirming cognitive empathy deficits in AN in line with previous literature (14).

In addition, one study identified that both the remission and acute AN groups performed significantly worse than the non-clinical comparison group to a small degree, suggesting that even individuals with AN who have achieved weight restoration still may show impairments in theory of mind (54). Similarly, one study (53) found that the AN acute group performed better than the AN remission and HC groups on one outcome measure. However, these differences were not observed on any of the three other outcome measures. By contrast, Leslie et al. (57), did not observe any groups differences or trends in cognitive empathy outcomes.

4 Discussion

The primary aim of this review was to summarise the available literature investigating empathy in AN and to investigate the impact that starvation might have on the presence of cognitive and emotional deficits in AN. Given that, potentially domain-specific (18), reduced empathic ability is commonly reported among AN samples (18, 39), which likely impacts other areas of functioning such as socialisation (8, 9), the present review examined research investigating elements of both emotional and cognitive empathy. It is critical that researchers and clinicians better understand empathy deficits in AN populations and how they may impact socialisation and relationships in order to reduce any impacts on therapeutic relationships and subsequent intervention effectiveness.

From the small body of literature available, it can be concluded that while impairments in empathy may improve once remission of AN has been achieved, the effects are small and further research utilising more comprehensive assessment and larger sample sizes comparing the acute and remission phases of AN is critical. Nevertheless, the reviewed literature could be considered somewhat consistent with previous research highlighting starvation-induced psychological impairments can produce cognitive and socialisation difficulties, which might lend some insight as to why autistic trait are so commonly observed in AN populations (20, 43, 44, 59). However, as most studies reported nonsignificant results, conclusions that can be drawn are limited in this instance. While some patterns in the data indicated that the remission groups performed better than acute AN groups and worse than nonclinical control groups, only two of the seven studies reported observed this pattern at a statistically significant level. A possible explanation for this inconsistency is the difference in outcome measures selected in each included article. Almost all studies utilised different measures, which may assess different empathy domains and also vary in their sensitivity to capturing empathy deficits. Additionally, previous literature has typically compared participants in the acute stage of AN to control groups with no psychiatric history and consistently observed significant deficits in empathy in AN in this regard (39, 42, 60). Thus, the inclusion of remission groups is a comparatively new area of interest and, as such, the literature is scarce and lacks consistency.

Additionally, one study (57) did not support the state debate and, instead, identified comparable performance across all three groups. Unexpectedly, no differences were observed between the acute AN and nonclinical comparison groups on cognitive empathy skills, which is inconsistent with previous literature (2, 53, 61). This outlying study utilised the Frith-Happé animations which, unlike the other performance-based measures, presents animated triangles as opposed to faces to assess theory of mind. It is possible the differentiation in this presentation of the measure could explain why the findings were not consistent with previous literature.

4.1 Limitations and direction for future research

There are several limitations within this systematic review that are important to consider when interpreting findings. Firstly, only a small number of studies were obtained, particularly for those assessing emotional empathy, and used various measures. Studies reviewed were often underpowered, which was highlighted by the number of patterns in the data that did not reach significance.The diversity in measures used for each study presents an additional confound. One study (58) gathered self-report data, requiring participants to reflect on past experiences, while all other studies utilised performance-based measures to capture empathy levels at present. This made comparing results and drawing conclusions difficult and highlights the need for additional research in this area.

Additionally, the samples within each included study overwhelmingly represented female participants. While a diagnosis of AN is more common among biological females compared to males (40) and thus sample demographics are generally aligned with clinical expectations, AN does still occur in males and there is, therefore, a gap in our understanding of the male profile of AN and the consequent neurocognitive implications. As a result, cognitive and emotional empathy could not be sufficiently examined in males. Indeed, a possible area of exploration for future research could be to examine possible differences in empathy as a factor of biological sex and also of gender to determine if there are discrepancies. Indeed, there is a higher prevalence of AN among gender diverse populations (62) and is therefore important to consider in addition to biological sex. Any differences are likely beneficial to consider in treatment to ensure intervention effectiveness as empathy deficits may impact upon the therapeutic relationship.

Finally, a factor not explored yet in the literature is the time spent in recovery. Those in remission from AN whose weight has been clinically restored may demonstrate increasing improvements in their empathy skills as they continue to recover. Further suggestions for future research, therefore, relate to the comparison of empathy in acute AN and remission across the life span, between biological sexes and among gender diverse populations, and the longitudinal recovery of AN and how empathetic ability changes across this time span. Additionally, future research should consider investigating other areas of neurocognition which may be affected be starvation such as executive functioning and memory.

4.2 Summary and conclusion

While it is generally noted in the literature that people with AN present with some degree of potentially domain specific (18) reduced empathic ability, the limited literature considered in this present review provides incentive for deeper investigation of the role of starvation on neurocognitive performance, particularly in the socio-cognitive domain. However, understanding the underpinnings of why this impairment is present may be crucial in informing treatment decision making and altering ‘gold- standard’ intervention for a condition that is proven difficult to treat with long-term efficacy. The current patterns in the data are inconsistent, which could be attributable to the varying methodological and sample characteristics of each study. Additionally, studies predominantly reported non-significant outcomes. Our findings, therefore, suggest that while emotional empathy and cognitive empathy may improve throughout recovery, further investigation utilising comprehensive measures of empathy is necessary. It is anticipated that these findings will assist clinicians treating AN to account for these empathetic deficits when building a therapeutic relationship with clients from this population, setting client goals, and delivering interventions to enhance treatment outcomes.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

IG: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. PE: Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. MF: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. MK: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Deakin University provided funds to support the publication of this manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Odent M. Autism and anorexia nervosa: Two facets of the same disease? Med Hypotheses. (2010) 75:79–81. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2010.01.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Dell'Osso L, Carpita B, Gesi C, Cremone IM, Corsi M, Massimetti E, et al. Subthreshold autism spectrum disorder in patients with eating disorders. Compr Psychiatry. (2018) 81:66–72. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Nalbant K, Kalaycı BM, Akdemir D, Akgül S, Kanbur N. Emotion regulation, emotion recognition, and empathy in adolescents with anorexia nervosa. Eating Weight Disorders-Studies Anorexia Bulimia Obes. (2019) 24:825–34. doi: 10.1007/s40519-019-00768-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Vuillier L, Carter Z, Teixeira AR, Moseley RL. Alexithymia may explain the relationship between autistic traits and eating disorder psychopathology. Mol Autism. (2020) 11:1–19. doi: 10.1186/s13229-020-00364-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Smith H, Milne E. Reduced change blindness suggests enhanced attention to detail in individuals with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Disciplines. (2009) 50:300–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01957.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Kimhi Y. Theory of mind abilities and deficits in autism spectrum disorders. Topics Lang Disord. (2014) 34:329–43. doi: 10.1097/TLD.0000000000000033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Demetriou EA, DeMayo MM, Guastella AJ. Executive function in autism spectrum disorder: history, theoretical models, empirical findings, and potential as an endophenotype. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:753. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Coombs E, Brosnan M, Bryant-Waugh R, Skevington SM. An investigation into the relationship between eating disorder psychopathology and autistic symptomatology in a non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol. (2011) 50:326–38. doi: 10.1348/014466510X524408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Rhind C, Bonfioli E, Hibbs R, Goddard E, Macdonald P, Gowers S, et al. An examination of autism spectrum traits in adolescents with anorexia nervosa and their parents. Mol Autism. (2014) 5:37–59. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-5-56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Fithall K, Gray IE, Linardon J, Phillipou A, Donaldson PH, Albein-Urios N, et al. Exploring the role of autistic traits and eating disorder psychopathology on mentalising ability in the general population [Article]. BMC Psychol. (2023) 11:269. doi: 10.1186/s40359-023-01306-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Preti A, Siddi S, Marzola E, Abbate Daga G. Affective cognition in eating disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the performance on the “reading the mind in the eyes” test. Eating Weight Disorders-Studies Anorexia Bulimia Obes. (2022) 27:2291–307. doi: 10.1007/s40519-022-01393-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Postorino V, Scahill L, De Peppo L, Fatta LM, Zanna V, Castiglioni MC, et al. Investigation of autism spectrum disorder and autistic traits in an adolescent sample with anorexia nervosa. J Autism Dev Disord. (2017) 47:1051–61. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-3023-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Leppanen J, Sedgewick F, Treasure J, Tchanturia K. Differences in the Theory of Mind profiles of patients with anorexia nervosa and individuals on the autism spectrum: A meta-analytic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 90:146–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Oldershaw A, Hambrook D, Tchanturia K, Treasure J, Schmidt U. Emotional theory of mind and emotional awareness in recovered anorexia nervosa patients. Psychosomatic Med. (2010) 72(1):73–9. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181c6c7ca

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Jones CRG, Simonoff E, Baird G, Pickles A, Marsden AJS, Tregay J, et al. The association between theory of mind, executive function, and the symptoms of autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2018) 11:95–109. doi: 10.1002/aur.1873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Bekkali S, Youssef GJ, Donaldson PH, Albein-Urios N, Hyde C, Enticott PG. Is the putative mirror neuron system associated with empathy? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev. (2021) 31:14–57. doi: 10.1007/s11065-020-09452-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Gaggero G, Luminet O, Vermeulen N, De Timary P, Nandrino JL, Goffinet S, et al. A multidimensional examination of affective and cognitive empathy in anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review (2023) 31:505–19. doi: 10.1002/erv.2980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Kerr-Gaffney J, Harrison A, Tchanturia K. Cognitive and affective empathy in eating disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:102. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Mason TB, Lesser EL, Dolgon-Krutolow AR, Wonderlich SA, Smith KE. An updated transdiagnostic review of social cognition and eating disorder psychopathology. J Psychiatr Res. (2021) 143:602–27. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Baron-Cohen S, Jaffa T, Davies S, Auyeung B, Allison C, Wheelwright S. Do girls with anorexia nervosa have elevated autistic traits? Mol Autism. (2013) 4:1–8. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-4-24

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Tauro JL, Wearne TA, Belevski B, Filipčíková M, Francis HM. Social cognition in female adults with Anorexia Nervosa: A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2022) 132:197–210. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.11.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Oldershaw A, DeJong H, Hambrook D, Schmidt U. Social attribution in anorexia nervosa. Eur Eating Disord Rev. (2018) 26:197–206. doi: 10.1002/erv.2588

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Dalmaso M, Castelli L, Scatturin P, Carli L, Todisco P, Palomba D, et al. Altered social attention in anorexia nervosa during real social interaction. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:1–8. doi: 10.1038/srep23311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kalayci BM, Nalbant K, Akdemir D. Autistic traits and social responsiveness: The relationship between autistic traits and comorbid psychiatric symptoms in adolescents with anorexia nervosa. Nöropsikiyatri Arsivi. (2021) 58:283–8. doi: 10.29399/npa.27175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Sedgewick F, Leppanen J, Goh F, Hayward H, Happé F, Tchanturia K. ). Similarities and differences in theory of mind responses of patients with anorexia nervosa with and without autistic features. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:318. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Meneguzzo P, Collantoni E, Bonello E, Busetto P, Tenconi E, Favaro A. The predictive value of the early maladaptive schemas in social situations in anorexia nervosa. Eur Eating Disord Rev. (2020) 28:318–31. doi: 10.1002/erv.2724

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Carter JC, Kelly AC, Norwood SJ. Interpersonal problems in anorexia nervosa: Social inhibition as defining and detrimental. Pers Individ Dif. (2012) 53:169–74. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.02.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Harrison A, Mountford VA, Tchanturia K. Social anhedonia and work and social functioning in the acute and recovered phases of eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 218:187–94. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Ambwani S, Berenson KR, Simms L, Li A, Corfield F, Treasure J. Seeing things differently: An experimental investigation of social cognition and interpersonal behavior in anorexia nervosa. Int J eating Disord. (2016) 49:499–506. doi: 10.1002/eat.22498

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Blackburn B, O’Connor J, Parsons H. Becoming needless: A psychoanalytically informed qualitative study exploring the interpersonal and intrapsychic experiences of longstanding anorexia nervosa. Int J Appl Psychoanalytic Stud. (2021) 18:428–42. doi: 10.1002/aps.1679

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Zucker NL, Losh M, Bulik CM, LaBar KS, Piven J, Pelphrey KA. Anorexia nervosa and autism spectrum disorders: Guided investigation of social cognitive endophenotypes. psychol Bull. (2007) 133:976–1006. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.6.976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Eating Disorders Victoria. Resources (2014). Available online at: https://www.eatingdisorders.org.au/eating-disorders-a-z/eating-disorder-facts-and-resources/.

Google Scholar

33. McIntosh VVW, Jordan J, Carter FA, Luty SE, McKenzie JM, Bulik CM, et al. Three psychotherapies for anorexia nervosa: A randomized, controlled trial. Am J Psychiatry. (2005) 162:741–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.4.741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Meneguzzo P, Garolla A, Bonello E, Todisco P. Alexithymia, dissociation and emotional regulation in eating disorders: Evidence of improvement through specialized inpatient treatment. Clin Psychol Psychother. (2022) 29:718–24. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Kinnaird E, Tchanturia K. Looking beneath the surface: Distinguishing between common features in autism and anorexia nervosa. J Behav Cognit Ther. (2021) 31:3–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jbct.2020.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Saure E, Laasonen M, Raevuori A. Anorexia nervosa and comorbid autism spectrum disorders. Current Opin Psychiat. (2021) 34:569–75. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000742

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Graves TA, Tabri N, Thompson-Brenner H, Franko DL, Eddy KT, Bourion-Bedes S, et al. A meta-analysis of the relation between therapeutic alliance and treatment outcome in eating disorders. Int J of Eating Disorders (2017) 50:323–40. doi: 10.1002/eat.22672

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Sly R, Morgan JF, Mountford VA, Lacey JH. Predicting premature termination of hospitalised treatment for anorexia nervosa: The roles of therapeutic alliance, motivation, and behaviour change. Eating Behaviors (2013) 14:119–23. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Konstantakopoulos G, Ioannidi N, Patrikelis P, Gonidakis F. Empathy, mentalizing, and cognitive functioning in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Soc Neurosci. (2020) 15:477–88. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2020.1760131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5-TR ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2022). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425787

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Brown C, Stokes M. Intersection of eating disorders and the female profile of autism. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clinics North Americia. (2020) 29:409–17. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2019.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Courty A, Maria AS, Lalanne C, Ringuenet D, Vindreau C, Chevallier C, et al. Levels of autistic traits in anorexia nervosa: A comparative psychometric study. BMC Psychiatry. (2013) 13:222. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-13-222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Treasure J. Coherence and other autistic spectrum traits and eating disorders: building from mechanism to treatment. The Birgit Olsson lecture. Nordic J Psychiatry. (2013) 67:38–42. doi: 10.3109/08039488.2012.674554

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Bora E, Köse S. Meta-analysis of theory of mind in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: A specific impairment of cognitive perspective taking in anorexia nervosa? Int J Eating Disord. (2016) 49:739–40. doi: 10.1002/eat.22572

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Westwood H, Eisler I, Mandy W, Leppanen J, Treasure J, Tchanturia K. Using the autism-spectrum quotient to measure autistic traits in anorexia nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Autism Dev Disord. (2016) 46:964–77. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2641-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Dinkler L, Taylor MJ, Råstam M, Hadjikhani N, Bulik CM, Lichtenstein P, et al. Anorexia nervosa and autism: A prospective twin cohort study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2021) 62:316–26. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Hollesen A, Clausen L, & Rokkedal K. Multiple family therapy for adolescents with anorexia nervosa: a pilot study of eating disorder symptoms and interpersonal functioning. J Family Ther. (2013) 35:53–67. doi: 10.1111/1467-6427.12000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Doris E, Westwood H, Mandy W, Tchanturia K. A qualitative study of friendship in patients with Anorexia Nervosa and possible Autism Spectrum Disorder. Psychology. (2014) 5:1338–49. doi: 10.4236/psych.2014.511144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Brede J, Babb C, Jones C, Elliott M, Zanker C, Tchanturia K, et al. “For me, the anorexia is just a symptom, and the cause is the autism”: Investigating restrictive eating disorders in autistic women. J Autism Dev Disord. (2020) 50:4280–96. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04479-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. (2021) 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Ouzzani M, Hammady H, Fedorowicz Z, Elmagarmid A. Rayyan—a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Systematic Rev. (2016) 5:1–10. doi: 10.1186/s13643-016-0384-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Wells GA, Shea B, O’Connell D, Peterson J, Weich V, Losos M, et al. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale 9NOS) for assessing the quality of non-randmised studies in meta-analysis (2019). Available online at: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_.

Google Scholar

53. Bentz M, Jepsen JRM, Pedersen T, Bulik CM, Pedersen L, Pagsberg AK, et al. Impairment of social function in young females with recent-onset anorexia nervosa and recovered individuals. J Adolesc Health. (2017) 60:23–32. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Harrison A, Tchanturia K, Treasure J. Attentional bias, emotion recognition, and emotion regulation in anorexia: state or trait? Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 68:755–61. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Kerr-Gaffney J, Mason L, Jones E, Hayward H, Ahmad J, Harrison A, et al. Emotion recognition abilities in adults with anorexia nervosa are associated with autistic traits. J Clin Med. (2020) 9:1057. doi: 10.3390/jcm9041057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Kerr-Gaffney J, Harrison A, Tchanturia K. Autism spectrum disorder traits are associated with empathic abilities in adults with anorexia nervosa. J Affect Disorders. (2020) 266:273–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.01.169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Leslie M, Halls D, Leppanen J, Sedgewick F, Smith K, Hayward H, et al. Neural correlates of theory of mind are preserved in young women with anorexia nervosa. Front Psychol. (2020) 2348. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.568073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Morris R, Bramham J, Smith E, Tchanturia K. Empathy and social functioning in anorexia nervosa before and after recovery. Cogn neuropsychiatry. (2014) 19:47–57. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2013.794723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Keys A, Brozek J, Henschel A, Mickelsen O, Taylor HL. The Biology of Human Starvation Volume II. Minneapolis, Minnesota: University of Minnesota Press (1950). doi: 10.5749/j.ctv9b2tqv

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Oldershaw A, Treasure J, Hambrook D, Tchanturia K, Schmidt U. Is anorexia nervosa a version of autism spectrum disorders? Euro Eating Disord Rev. (2011) 19:462–74. doi: 10.1002/erv.1069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Pruccoli J, Solari A, Terenzi L, Malaspina E, Angotti M, Pignataro V, et al. Autism spectrum disorder and anorexia nervosa: An Italian prospective study. Ital J Pediatr. (2021) 47:1–6. doi: 10.1186/s13052-021-01006-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Harrop EN, Hecht HK, Harner V, Call J, Holloway BT. “How do I exist in this body… That’s outside of the norm?” Trans and nonbinary experiences of conformity, coping, and connection in atypical anorexia. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2023) 20:1156. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20021156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: anorexia nervosa, mentalising, empathy, acute, remission, state-trait

Citation: Gray IE, Enticott PG, Fuller-Tyszkiewicz M and Kirkovski M (2024) Cognitive and emotional empathy in acute and remitted anorexia nervosa: a systematic review. Front. Psychiatry 15:1385185. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1385185

Received: 12 February 2024; Accepted: 08 May 2024;
Published: 24 May 2024.

Edited by:

Giulia Ballarotto, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Reviewed by:

Jess Kerr-Gaffney, King’s College London, United Kingdom
Paolo Meneguzzo, University of Padua, Italy
Massimo Pasquini, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2024 Gray, Enticott, Fuller-Tyszkiewicz and Kirkovski. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Indigo E. Gray, ierez@deakin.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.