Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 11 July 2024
Sec. Anxiety and Stress Disorders

Heart rate response to cognitive load as a marker of depression and increased anxiety

  • 1School of Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
  • 2Faculty of Social Sciences, Laboratory for Cognitive Research, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
  • 3Faculty of Biology and Biotechnology, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
  • 4Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
  • 5Laboratory of Human Higher Nervous Activity, Institute of Higher Nervous Activity and Neurophysiology of the Russian Academy of Sciences, Moscow, Russia

Introduction: Understanding the interplay between cardiovascular parameters, cognitive stress induced by increasing load, and mental well-being is vital for the development of integrated health strategies today. By monitoring physiological signals like electrocardiogram (ECG) and photoplethysmogram (PPG) in real time, researchers can discover how cognitive tasks influence both cardiovascular and mental health. Cardiac biomarkers resulting from cognitive strain act as indicators of autonomic nervous system function, potentially reflecting conditions related to heart and mental health, including depression and anxiety. The purpose of this study is to investigate how cognitive load affects ECG and PPG measurements and whether these can signal early cardiovascular changes during depression and anxiety disorders.

Methods: Ninety participants aged 18 to 45 years, ranging from symptom-free individuals to those with diverse psychological conditions, were assessed using psychological questionnaires and anamnesis. ECG and PPG monitoring were conducted as volunteers engaged in a cognitive 1-back task consisting of two separate blocks, each with six progressively challenging levels. The participants’ responses were analyzed to correlate physiological and psychological data with cognitive stressors and outcomes.

Results: The study confirmed a notable interdependence between anxiety and depression, and cardiovascular responses. Task accuracy decreased with increased task difficulty. A strong relationship between PPG-measured heart rate and markers of depression and trait anxiety was observed. Increasing task difficulty corresponded to an increase in heart rate, linked with elevated levels of depression and trait anxiety. A strong relationship between ECG-measured heart rate and anxiety attacks was observed. Increasing task difficulty corresponded to an increase in heart rate, linked with elevated levels of anxiety attacks, although this association decreased under more challenging conditions.

Discussion: The findings underscore the predictive importance of ECG and PPG heart rate parameters in mental health assessment, particularly depression and anxiety under cognitive stress induced by increasing load. We discuss mechanisms of sympathetic activation explaining these differences. Our research outcomes have implications for clinical assessments and wearable device algorithms for more precise, personalized mental health diagnostics.

1 Introduction

The escalating global health burden of cardiovascular and mental health disorders, highlighted by the World Health Organization (1), indicates the need for advanced preventative strategies and interventions. Cognitive stress, also known as mental stress (2, 3), refers to the ways in which stress factors such as increased cognitive load affect mental processes. Cognitive stress is a crucial factor that links physiological and psychological well-being to mental health conditions such as depression and anxiety. It affects cognition and emotional processing (3, 4). The complex interplay between heart activity, stress, and cognitive function has been examined (5, 6) establishing connections between cognitive decline, vascular health, and mental well-being (7). Heart rate variability (HRV) has emerged as a focal point, particularly in its correlation with depression and anxiety (6, 812). Studies have consistently shown that HRV, which reflects the autonomic nervous system’s ability to respond to various psychological and physiological stimuli, is closely linked with mental health conditions and cognitive load (5, 13). Additionally, heart rate mean (HR) as a biomarker for mental disorders has been determined (1315).

Application of photoplethysmography (PPG), a non-invasive method for measuring blood volume changes in the microvascular bed of tissue, for assessing heart rate offers a different lens through which to view the heart’s response to stress. Recent studies highlight the importance of peripheral vasoconstriction, governed by the sympathetic nervous system, as an indicator of mental effort and stress levels (1619). There is a significant physiological connection between PPG amplitude variations and stress responses, as well as cognitive load (2022). PPG is also used in the context of anxiety and depression assessment (23). This measurement provides critical insights into the neurophysiological mechanisms underpinning stress, anxiety, and depression (24, 25).

Electrocardiogram (ECG) provides cardiac indicators that enhance our understanding of stress-related physiological responses. ECG monitors heart rate and contractility, predominantly influenced by adrenergic receptors response (26, 27). This cardiac response is reflected in the vascular activity, measurable through photoplethysmography. PPG measures peripheral vascular tone, primarily affected by noradrenergic receptors (28), as well as adrenergic receptors (29, 30). These differences could provide critical insights into the neurophysiological mechanisms underpinning stress, anxiety, and depression (24, 25). Understanding mental health disorders can be aided by concurrently examining cardiac and vascular responses (31). The integration of ECG with PPG is advancing in AI-driven personalized diagnostics and wearable technologies (3234).

Our study addresses the reactivity of ECG and PPG to stress by examining physiological and cognitive responses to modulated cognitive load in a larger sample size of participants with a range of mental conditions. Psychological assessments were conducted for all participants. In the pilot study, we observed a gradual increase in the sympathetic activity of the autonomic nervous system, correlating with respiratory and cardiovascular responses under cognitive load (35) and linking patterns of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) with questionnaire responses (36).We hypothesized that ECG and PPG HRV parameters would demonstrate the most pronounced association with traits of depression and anxiety, predicated on the assumption that the complexity level of tasks. This enhances our understanding of the mechanisms underlying anxiety and depression.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

A cohort of 95 individuals, initially recruited between the ages of 19 and 48 years (average age = 25 ± 7.8; 61 females), participated in the study. After removing individuals below 18 or above 45 years due to technical issues in their data recordings, 90 subjects (average age = 25 ± 7.6; 57 females) were included in the final analysis. Participants exhibiting traits of mental disorders were recruited through referrals from practitioners, while individuals with normal mental traits were invited via social networks and the faculty’s public website. Uniform assessments, consisting of questionnaires and medical histories, were conducted for all participants following a standardized protocol that ensured confidentiality and data anonymization. Participants were classified based on questionnaire responses, self-reported data, and preliminary diagnoses, all evaluated by an expert using normative parameters, facilitating a precise distinction between normative and non-normative traits.

2.2 Questionnaires and psychological assessment

The participants underwent a detailed anamnestic review to report their experiences with anxiety attacks and depressive episodes, using the following encoding systems. Anxiety attacks were assessed using a binary scale, where ‘0’ indicated no anxiety attacks during the individual’s lifespan and ‘1’ indicated the presence of any anxiety attacks at any point in the past five years. This scale was chosen due to the self-reported nature of the information and the necessity of a two-parameter approach for mental health professionals. Episodes of depression were evaluated on a three-point scale. A score of ‘0’ was given if there were no episodes of depression throughout the individual’s life. A ‘0.5’ was assigned if episodes of depression were self-reported without the involvement of mental health professionals. Likewise, a score of ‘1’ was used when episodes of depression were both self-reported and confirmed by a mental health practitioner. This approach allowed for precise differentiation between self-reported anxiety and various levels of depression. Key psychological assessments included the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) (3740) and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, Trait Version (STAI-T) by Spielberger (41, 42), to quantify depressive symptoms and differentiate between state and trait anxiety. Our study analyzes self-reported questionnaire data to comprehensively analyze traits of anxiety and depression. We included both participants referred by mental health practitioners and those recruited independently, assessing their mental states using validated inventories. Employing a triangulation approach, we aimed to differentiate symptoms in our assessment. In subsequent text, we will mention traits of depression (BDI), traits of anxiety (STAI), as well as self-reported depressive episodes and self-reported anxious episodes. If both depression-related evaluations (BDI-II and self-reported episodes) align, we will label it as ‘depression’. Similarly, if anxiety-related evaluations (STAI-T and self-reported episodes) align, we will label it as ‘anxiety’.

2.3 Stimuli and procedure

For inducing cognitive stress, we employed the Cognitive 1-back Colour Matching Task (CMT) (43). This approach was selected to balance the challenge across participants while avoiding cultural and educational bias, making it particularly suitable for diverse study populations. This task was tested in preliminary studies, which confirmed its effectiveness at inducing cognitive load without exceeding participant capabilities. Furthermore, the level of task difficulty can be adjusted by increasing the number of objects that participants must remember, transitioning to a 2-back or 3-back design. In our study, we regulated difficulty by altering the object count. This approach ensures a controlled increment in cognitive load, facilitating a more precise evaluation of cognitive stress induced by memory tasks as tested in previous research (35, 44).

Participants were instructed to view images of colored balls and compare each with the preceding one to identify color matches. Key 1 was designated for matches, and key 2 for mismatches, with a primary emphasis on color rather than position. The task consists of two parts, each comprising six escalating difficulty levels. Each level includes 17 trials, totaling 102 samples per block (204 samples for 2 blocks). Among these, 49 trials required a ‘2’ response for non-matching samples, and 41 necessitated a ‘1’ response for matching samples. It commences with a simple task of recognizing a single-color change in an image with balloons and progressively escalates to identifying up to six color changes. Each image was displayed for one second, with a 30-second rest provided after each block. Task completion for each level took about a minute, and a block took approximately seven minutes, contingent on participants’ response speed. The entire experiment lasted no more than 15 minutes per participant, with limited time for answering. Participants received visual feedback after each trial (Figure 1). Immediate feedback was provided after each response, indicating correctness, incorrectness, or responses exceeding three seconds. After completing each level, participants evaluated their performance.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Cognitive Task Design and Feedback Structure. (A) Task Progression: Each block displays six balloon bundles, representing a gradient in difficulty from left (simpler configurations) to right (complex configurations). (B) Feedback Component: Two paired balloon bundle sets are presented. Adjacent feedback icons (straight-mouth for “same” and curved-mouth for “different”) provide responses post a 3-second interval. This design assesses participants’ ability to discern and recall balloon color patterns, with immediate feedback facilitating performance tracking.

We assessed how participants managed cognitive stress by correlating their physiological responses with their accuracy in providing correct answers. Participants’ rewards included a base amount for participation, supplemented by a performance-based bonus calculated according to the number of correct answers, following the methodology employed in the pilot study (35). The decision time (the time from the appearance of the second image with balloons to pressing the key to answer) for each block and level was analyzed separately. Stimuli sequence and data for the analysis of behavioral responses were obtained using Eye-Link Experiment Builder 2.3.1 (Mississauga, Ontario, Canada: SR Research Ltd., 2020) synchronized with ECG and PPG recording.

2.4 Recording and signal processing

ECG and PPG data were continuously recorded to assess autonomic nervous system reactions using a rheograph-polyanalyzer RGPA-6/12, with a sampling rate of 250 Hz and a filtration range of 0.5–75 Hz. ECG measurements utilized sensors strategically placed on the right and left wrists, as well as the right ankle. Simultaneously, PPG data registration employed sensors positioned on the distal phalanx of the middle finger on the left hand. The initial data processing was conducted using Neurokit2 (45), specialized software designed for such tasks. It featured a custom processing function tailored specifically for ECG and PPG signal types, ensuring comprehensive signal purification and subsequent peak detection.

For the ECG dataset, a fifth-order Butterworth high-pass filter with a cutoff frequency of 0.5 Hz was applied, in combination with a 50 Hz power line filter. The QRS complexes in the ECG signal were identified based on their gradient steepness, with R-peaks recognized as local maxima (45, 46). When processing Photoplethysmography (PPG) data, a third-order Butterworth filter with a passband ranging from 0.5 to 8 Hertz was employed. The processing methodology described in Method IV (47) was applied to PPG signals for data purification and peak detection. This approach, used for detecting systolic peaks in acceleration photoplethysmograms (APG) signals, involves a three-stage algorithm: pre-processing with bandpass filtering and squaring the signal, feature extraction using moving averages to identify potential systolic peaks, and classification through dynamic thresholding for accurate peak determination. This method, integrated into the NeuroKit2 toolkit, effectively removed extraneous noise and artifacts from the PPG signal, ensuring cleaner and more reliable data for analysis.

Mean cardiac parameters, including HRV time and frequency domains, were calculated for each block level from both ECG and PPG signals, including Heart Rate, High Frequency (HF), Low Frequency (LF), MeanNN (the average of all normal-to-normal intervals), Root Mean Square of Successive Differences (RMSSD), and Standard Deviation of NN intervals (SDNN), thereby providing a comprehensive analysis of autonomic nervous system activity. Following the initial processing with Neurokit2, an issue of over-detection of R-peaks in the ECG dataset became apparent, suggesting the presence of extraneous signals and peaks. The BioPsyKit toolkit, with artifact detection capabilities (48), was utilized to filter out extraneous peaks employing the rooted algorithm (49).

2.5 Statistical analysis

To test the hypotheses, we first computed the mean heart rate measures and response accuracy for each individual at different task difficulty levels. Subsequently, we employed the task difficulty level, ranging from 1 to 6 colors, as a categorical variable in our analysis. This approach was crucial to ensure that we did not overlook potential non-linear relationships and interactions with task difficulty.

2.5.1 Association of heart rate indicators with depression and anxiety

We set out to determine which HRV indicator has the strongest association with anxiety and depression. For each heart rate variable and each anxiety/depression variable, we applied a linear mixed effects model using the nlme package in R (50). The model was specified as follows: heart rate ~ anxiety/depression * difficulty + (~1 | ID), where the dependent variable was heart rate, and the fixed effects included the anxiety/depression score and task difficulty. We included a random intercept for participant code (ID). The random intercept was consistently utilized throughout this analysis to account for individual differences in the target variable, reducing the impact of individual variability. Model comparison was conducted using the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), a metric for assessing information loss in a model (51). To ensure fair comparisons via AIC, we standardized the target variables to a common scale using the z-scaling method.

2.5.2 Relationship between depression, anxiety, and heart rate response to task difficulty

The next aim was to determine how HRV is related to task difficulty, anxiety, and depression. Subsequently, for each depression (BDI depression, self-reported episodes of depression) and anxiety (STAI trait anxiety, self-reported anxiety attacks) measure, we examined the 4 models that exhibited the best performance according to AIC. To assess the model coefficients, we employed Wald t-statistics with a significance threshold of p <.01. Additionally, these models were replicated with the addition of gender as a predictor (see Supplementary Materials for results).

2.5.3 Relationship between heart rate response to task difficulty and task-solving accuracy

Furthermore, for heart rate indicators displaying the most robust relationships with depression and anxiety, we developed linear mixed effects models with the following formulation: heart rate ~ accuracy * difficulty + (~1 | ID). We evaluated the coefficients of these models using Wald t-statistics, maintaining a significance threshold of p < 0.01.

3 Results

3.1 Basic behavioral statistics

The Pearson correlation test revealed a significant association between trait anxiety (STAI; mean = 44.7 ± 8.9) and trait depression (BDI; mean = 9.3 ± 7.1; r = 0.6, p < 0.001). Two-way ANOVA tests indicated that significant relationships between depression episodes and anxiety attacks with STAI trait anxiety (F (2, 84) = 3.95, p = 0.02; F (1, 84) = 11.84, p < 0.001; respectively), as well as with BDI depression (F (2, 84) = 19.04, p < 0.001; F (1, 84) = 27.38, p < 0.001; respectively). However, depression episodes were not significantly associated with anxiety attacks (χ² = 5.21, p = 0.07). Accuracy, measured as the percentage of correct answers (86.7% ± 5.1%), decreased as task difficulty increased (r = -0.75, p < 0.001). Accuracy was not related to anxiety (r = -0.06, p = 0.56), depression (r = 0.06, p = 0.56), depression episodes and anxiety attacks (F = 0.23, p = 0.79; F = 0.15, p = 0.7; relatively). Descriptive statistics for response accuracy, HRV and psychological indicators can be found in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Analysis results, delineating physiological and psychological variable values categorized by groups.

3.2 Heart rate variability indicators and depression and anxiety traits

Initially, we identified the HRV indicator most strongly associated with anxiety and depression (refer to Subsection 2.5.1 in Methods). Models incorporating trait anxiety (STAI), trait depression (BDI), and episodes of depression as predictors indicated that the PPG mean heart rate had the lowest information loss (AIC = 134, AIC = 133.7, AIC = 132.3, respectively). For models involving anxiety attacks, the ECG mean heart rate model showed the lowest information loss (AIC = 141). AICs of all models can be found in Table 2. Thus, the analysis revealed a statistically significant association between mean heart rate and depression and anxiety indicators, which was more pronounced than with other HRV parameters. Furthermore, the examination of PPG-derived mean heart rate data indicated significant statistical associations with parameters such as personal anxiety, self-reported depression, and diagnosed depression. Concurrently, ECG-measured mean heart rate dynamics demonstrated a more pronounced association with episodes of anxiety attacks.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) of 48 models predicting heart rate parameters through anxiety/depression and task difficulty.

3.3 Mean heart rate, task difficulty and depression and anxiety traits

Subsequently, we explored how selected HRV indicators correlate with task difficulty, anxiety, and depression (refer to Subsection 2.5.2 in Methods). For the examination of trait anxiety (STAI), trait depression (BDI), and self-reported depression episodes, we analyzed models predicting PPG heart rate. For the analysis of self-reported anxiety attacks, we utilized models predicting ECG heart rate mean (as discussed in Section 3.2).

The mean heart rate demonstrated a positive association with trait anxiety (STAI) in the challenging task levels (3, 4, 5, 6 colors of balloons) when compared to the easiest task level (interaction between task difficulty and trait anxiety (STAI): t = 3.09, p < 0.01; t = 2.16, p = 0.03; t = 3.13, p < 0.01; t = 2.44, p = 0.02; respectively; refer to Figure 2). Additionally, the analyzed model unveiled a main effect indicating a negative association between task difficulty (3 and 5 colors) and the mean heart rate when contrasted with the easiest level (t = -2.36, p = 0.02; t = -2.66, p = 0.008; respectively). The mean heart rate also exhibited a positive association with episodes of anxiety attacks in the middle difficulty task level (4 colors of balloons) when compared to the easiest task level (interaction between task difficulty and anxiety attacks: t = 2.61, p < 0.01) (Figure 2).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Dependency between depression/anxiety and heart rate with increasing task difficulty relative to the easiest task condition. Difference (Δ) - difference in heart rate between the easiest difficulty level (1 color) and other difficulty levels. Top panels: results for depression (A) and anxiety (B) assessed with questionnaires (BDI, STAI-T). The more severe the depression/anxiety scores, the greater the increase in heart rate during difficult conditions. Low panels: results for self-reported depression (C) and anxiety (D) episodes in relation to healthy subjects. Self-reported depression episodes (self-diagnosed, psychiatrist-diagnosed) enhance the effect of heart rate increase during difficult conditions. Self-reported anxiety episodes enhance the effect of heart rate increase during the medium difficulty condition (4 colors). * p-value < 0.05, ** p-value < 0.01, *** p-value < 0.001

Furthermore, the mean heart rate displayed a positive association with BDI ratings of depression in the demanding task levels (3, 4, 5, 6 colors of balloons) relative to the easiest task level (interaction between task difficulty and trait depression: t = 3.61, p < 0.001; t = 3.21, p < 0.01; t = 3.03, p < 0.01; t = 3.36, p < 0.001; respectively) as illustrated in Figure 2.

Additionally, the mean heart rate exhibited a positive relationship with self-reported episodes of depression in the challenging task conditions (2, 3, 4, 6 colors of balloons) in comparison to the easiest condition (t = 2.45, p = 0.02; t = 3.27, p < 0.01; t = 3.44, p < 0.001; t = 2.31, p = 0.02; respectively; Figure 2). A similar effect was observed with self-reports of a formal psychiatrist diagnosis of depression (5 colors: t = 2.02, p = 0.04; 6 colors: t = 2.08, p = 0.04).

3.4 Post-hoc analysis: heart rate, task difficulty and task accuracy

The analysis unveiled a connection between the mean heart rate and accuracy. Specifically, the mean ECG heart rate exhibited a positive association with accuracy (main effect: t = 3.23, p = 0.001), although this effect diminished in the most challenging condition involving 6 colors (interaction between task difficulty and accuracy: t = -2.03, p = 0.04). Additionally, the analyzed model indicated a main effect of a positive relationship between task difficulty (4 and 5 colors of balloons) and the mean heart rate, in comparison to the easiest level (t = 2.25, p = 0.03; t = 2.5, p = 0.01; respectively).

Similarly, PPG heart rate displayed a positive association with task-solving accuracy (t = 4, p < 0.001) (Figure 3). However, this relationship significantly diminished in more challenging conditions (interaction between task difficulty and accuracy: t = -2.34, p = 0.01 (3 colors difficulty); t = -2.85, p < 0.01 (4 colors of balloons); t = -2.64, p < 0.01 (5 colors of balloons); t = -2.7, p < 0.01 (6 colors of balloons). The analyzed model also revealed a main effect indicating a positive relationship between task difficulty (3, 4, 5, 6 colors of balloons) and PPG heart rate, when compared to the easiest level (t = 2.57, p = 0.01; t = 3.21, p < 0.01; t = 3.07, p < 0.01; t = 3.21, p < 0.01; respectively).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Relationship between task-solving accuracy and mean heart hate derived from PPG data. The higher the heart rate, the greater the accuracy.

4 Discussion

The key findings revealed a pronounced association between the average heart rate, as measured through photoplethysmography (PPG), and indicators of depression and trait anxiety, more significant than the associations observed with heart rate variability (HRV) parameters derived from electrocardiography (ECG). Furthermore, the data demonstrated that mean heart rate was substantially influenced by task complexity among individuals presenting a spectrum of mental health conditions. This association was particularly pronounced in relation to trait anxiety (STAI) and trait depression (BDI), as well as self-reported depression episodes at elevated levels of task difficulty. It is also crucial to acknowledge that filtering ECG noise can be challenging, which may impact the limitations and results of both ECG and PPG signal preprocessing methods.

It is important to consider what the mean heart rate is measured through the detection of R waves, while PPG-obtained mean heart rate measurement relies on identifying the maxima of the pulse wave. Despite both methods targeting the same physiological indicator, they differ in their signal filtering processes. This distinction might elucidate the observed variance in ‘sensitivity’ within our models. The results demonstrate a substantial positive association between heart rate responses and depression, as well as trait anxiety, markers during challenging tasks, signifying a distinct pattern in individuals with anxiety attacks. Furthermore, post hoc analyses suggested a diminishing positive relation between mean heart rate and task accuracy in more complex tasks.

4.1 PPG heart rate mean and ECG heart rate mean and depression and anxiety traits

In examining the relationship between heart rate metrics and mental health conditions our research contributes new data to existing discussions. While the focus has traditionally been on HRV (52, 53) as a marker of the parasympathetic system’s adaptability (54, 55), our findings reveal a stronger predictive relationship with the mean heart rate (HR). Distinct from prevalent methods, our study measured HR and HRV during task performance, particularly under conditions of high workload. This contrasts with most existing research on HRV and its relationship to anxiety and depression, which predominantly focuses on resting or recovery phases, omitting the context of task execution (15). Our data suggest that HR stress response in the sympathetic system is more sensitive to conditions of anxiety and depression during challenging tasks than HRV indicators.

Furthermore, it was found that for anxiety and depression as measured by questionnaires, and for self-reported depression episodes, the strongest relation was observed with HR measured by PPG. Thus, despite ECG being considered a more reliable method for measuring HRV (56), and the relative scarcity of research into PPG signals in relation to depression and anxiety, our results suggest that PPG is an effective tool for gauging depression and trait anxiety through HR measurement. Concurrently, the highest connection for self-reported anxiety episodes (anxiety attacks) was observed with HR measured by ECG.

The study highlighted that PPG, a marker for cardiac and blood vessel activity reflecting peripheral vascular tone influenced by noradrenergic receptors, is closely linked with mental health issues, emphasizing the possible significance of noradrenaline’s role in sympathetic activation related to the investigated states and traits. These outcomes are aligned with previous studies (18, 24, 25, 29, 57, 58).The distinct cardiovascular receptor activities in the heart and vessels might explain the differential sensitivity of ECG and PPG in measuring anxiety and depression, despite the heart’s beating wave spreading from the heart to the vessels. β1-Adrenergic receptors, predominantly adrenergic (30, 59) in the heart, which increase heart rate and contractility in stress responses (60), are aligned with ECG measurements during self-reported anxiety attacks. However, PPG’s sensitivity to changes in peripheral blood flow is influenced by α1 receptors, predominantly noradrenergic (61, 62), which are responsible for vasoconstriction and thus their influence is a crucial factor in PPG measurements. These sympathetic vascular changes have a higher association in our study with anxiety and depression symptoms as assessed by questionnaires and self-reported depression episodes. Despite the presence of other factors in vessels, such as β2 adrenoreceptors, NO (nitric oxide), EDRF (endothelial derived relaxing factor), and others (63, 64), and vagal sympathetic regulation M2 in heart (65), these receptor mechanisms dominate their study, in connection with physiological research that can provide additional insights about ANS and CNS in response to stress.

4.2 Mean heart rate, task difficulty and depression and anxiety traits

It was found that mean heart rate responses to 1-back working memory task difficulty are related to levels of anxiety and depression, supporting our research hypothesis. Individuals with high depression scores on BDI and high trait anxiety as assessed by the STAI, as well as those with self-reported depression episodes (both self-diagnosed and psychiatrist-diagnosed), exhibited increased HR during challenging task levels. Additionally, the increased mean heart rate was observed during moderately difficult tasks in individuals with self-reported anxiety attacks. However, results for anxiety attacks were not replicated when gender was added to the model (refer to Supplementary Materials).

In previous studies, heart rate (HR) responses to stress, depression, and panic attacks have been identified as key areas of investigation (14, 66). Furthermore, it is well-established that tasks demanding executive functioning, which were employed in our study to induce psychological strain, correlate with heightened cardiac indicators, corroborating findings from prior research (31, 67). Within our study, we observed distinct patterns of HR fluctuations associated with cognitive load in individuals experiencing anxiety and depression. It may be inferred that heightened anxiety and depression (both clinical and non-clinical) are linked to an intensified stress response during the execution of difficult working memory tasks. However, contrasting views exist, suggesting that depression is associated with a reduced HR response in depressed individuals to stressful situations (13, 15). Therefore, HR increases in our study might reflect not merely a reaction to stress recovery, but an adaptive response to increasing task difficulty (68). This perspective is supported by studies indicating heightened nervous activity during working memory tasks, particularly among individuals with anxiety traits (44, 6972).

In our research Trait anxiety (STAI) and trait depression (BDI), along with self-reported episodes of depression, appear to similarly influence the HR response to task difficulty: an increase in HR response is observed in both depression and trait anxiety (Figure 2). This observation suggests that our study paradigm was more likely to detect a common physiological effect for trait anxiety (STAI), trait depression (BDI), and self-reported episodes of depression without effectively distinguishing between them. In contrast, only self-reported anxiety attacks demonstrated a notably different effect. HR response to task difficulties was heightened in subjects with reported anxiety attacks, but this was specifically evident at the average level of difficulty (Figure 2). In addition, the results showed that, unlike other psychological characteristics, only anxiety attacks showed a better association with ECG (Section 4.2). It is possible that anxiety attacks are associated with distinct β-adrenergic dysregulation (73, 74) and psychophysiological mechanisms, compared to those seen with trait anxiety (STAI), trait depression (BDI), and self-reported episodes of depression. This could be a relevant theme for future research. However, it should be noted that we did not statistically compare the direct effects of depression and anxiety, but rather how they influence cardiac responses.

4.3 Heart rate response patterns and task accuracy

In the research, we observed a positive relationship between HR and accuracy across various levels of task difficulty, with a more pronounced correlation in simpler tasks. This suggests that HR may reflect an adaptive stress response, where increased mental effort leads to improved performance, particularly in less complex tasks. However, this finding partly contrasts with previous studies. HR could be indicative of high cognitive load (55), especially in stressful conditions outside of the laboratory. Similarly, Solhjoo (13) observed that while increased cognitive load positively correlated with HRV, mean HR and single-item cognitive load measures exhibited a negative correlation with clinical reasoning performance. This highlights the potential of physiological monitoring as a tool for identifying individuals experiencing high cognitive load and at risk of underperforming in complex reasoning tasks, both with and without mental issues (15). We suggest that the heart rate monitoring could impact diverse perspectives in mental health assessment by integrating biochemical correlations (59), advanced algorithms, personalized Artificial Intelligence approaches with behavioral data, and cardiac biomarkers to provide reliable tools for mental health practitioners.

4.4 Limitations

This study, while offering insights and potential for future research, is constrained by its sample size. The variability in individual physiological and psychological responses can significantly impact HR patterns, underlining the need for larger and more diverse participant groups in subsequent studies. While this approach strengthened the clarity of our findings within an adult cohort, it necessitates further research across broader age ranges to ensure the universality and applicability of the results. Another limitation is the reliance on self-reported data, which necessitates more detailed research and refinement. Collaborations with experts and the integration of advanced technologies like artificial intelligence and machine learning could enhance data accuracy and interpretation. Additionally, methodological challenges in handling the noise typical of PPG signals are noteworthy. Preprocessing these signals, while crucial for data clarity, may inadvertently result in the loss of nuanced physiological information. Given known gender differences in the prevalence of anxiety and depression, our results might reflect indirect gender effects rather than direct correlations with anxiety and depression levels. Although we recalibrated our models to include gender as a predictor (refer to Supplementary Materials), confirming the significance of certain interactions irrespective of gender, the potential confounding impact of gender requires further investigation (75, 76).

5 Conclusion

In summary, this study explores the relationship between heart rate metrics and mental health, specifically focusing on depression and anxiety traits under cognitive stress. Our findings enhance the understanding of physiological markers, emphasizing the importance of HR in addition to HRV, and comparing measurement techniques such as PPG and ECG. The research outcomes underscore the superior predictive capability of heart rate, particularly metrics derived from PPG, in association with depression and anxiety during cognitive tasks. These findings advance our understanding of heart rate metrics as dependable indicators in mental health evaluations under cognitive stress. Heart rate metrics enhance diagnostic precision, supporting precision therapies and personalized mental health support. Crucially, integrating these findings into clinical diagnostic guidelines and algorithms for wearable technology and devices could significantly improve real-time, continuous monitoring of patient well-being.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Institutional Review Board of the Institute of the Higher Nervous Activity and Neurophysiology of the Russian Academy of Sciences (protocol #1, 25 February 2021). The IHNA Ethics Committee was established on October 21, 2014, by the Order of the IHNA Director No. 77/a. The Committee ensures adherence to modern scientific ethics standards and includes leading scientists in various neurobiology fields https://www.ihna.ru/en/employees/employee-info/ethics. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

EA: Data curation, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. NZ: Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. IP: Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. OM: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research follows from Strategic Project “Success and Self-Sustainability of the Individual in a Changing World”, which is part of Higher School of Economics’ development program under the “Priority 2030” academic leadership initiative. The “Priority 2030” initiative is run by Russia’s Ministry of Science and Higher Education as part of the National Project “Science and Universities”.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge Dr. Galina Portnova for inspiring us to conduct this study. We thank Olga Kashevarova for technical assistance, Krystsina Liaukovich and Danila Tolstobrov for help in collecting data. We are especially grateful to our volunteers who participated in the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1355846/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Freeman M. The World Mental Health Report: transforming mental health for all. World Psychiatry. (2022) 21:391–2. doi: 10.1002/wps.21018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Hussain J, Sundaraj K, Subramaniam ID. Cognitive stress changes the attributes of the three heads of the triceps brachii during muscle fatigue. PloS One. (2020) 15:e0228089. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0228089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Godoy LD, Rossignoli MT, Delfino-Pereira P, Garcia-Cairasco N, De Lima Umeoka EH. A comprehensive overview on stress neurobiology: basic concepts and clinical implications. Front Behav Neurosci. (2018) 12:127. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. De Kloet ER, Joëls M, Holsboer F. Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2005) 6:463–75. doi: 10.1038/nrn1683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Thayer JF, Hansen AL, Saus-Rose E, Johnsen BH. Heart rate variability, prefrontal neural function, and cognitive performance: the neurovisceral integration perspective on self-regulation, adaptation, and health. Ann Behav Med. (2009) 37:141–53. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9101-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Hu MX, Milaneschi Y, Lamers F, Nolte IM, Snieder H, Dolan CV, et al. The association of depression and anxiety with cardiac autonomic activity: The role of confounding effects of antidepressants. Depress Anxiety. (2019) 36:1163–72. doi: 10.1002/da.22966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol Rev. (2007) 87:873–904. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Hartmann R, Schmidt FM, Sander C, Hegerl U. Heart rate variability as indicator of clinical state in depression. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 9:735. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Cheng Y, Su M, Liu C, Huang Y, Huang W. Heart rate variability in patients with anxiety disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2022) 76:292–302. doi: 10.1111/pcn.13356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Chalmers JA, Quintana DS, Abbott MJA, Kemp AH. Anxiety disorders are associated with reduced heart rate variability: A meta-analysis. Front Psychiatry. (2014) 5:80/abstract. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00080/abstract

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Kemp AH, Quintana DS, Felmingham KL, Matthews S, Jelinek HF. Depression, comorbid anxiety disorders, and heart rate variability in physically healthy, unmedicated patients: implications for cardiovascular risk. PloS One. (2012) 7:e30777. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Shinba T. Characteristic profiles of heart rate variability in depression and anxiety. In: Asadpour V, Karakuş S, editors. Biomedical engineering IntechOpen, London, United Kingdom (2022). Available at: https://www.intechopen.com/chapters/81068.

Google Scholar

13. Solhjoo S, Haigney MC, McBee E, Van Merrienboer JJG, Schuwirth L, Artino AR, et al. Heart rate and heart rate variability correlate with clinical reasoning performance and self-reported measures of cognitive load. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:14668. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-50280-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Pittig A, Arch JJ, Lam CWR, Craske MG. Heart rate and heart rate variability in panic, social anxiety, obsessive–compulsive, and generalized anxiety disorders at baseline and in response to relaxation and hyperventilation. Int J Psychophysiol. (2013) 87:19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Schiweck C, Piette D, Berckmans D, Claes S, Vrieze E. Heart rate and high frequency heart rate variability during stress as biomarker for clinical depression. A systematic review. Psychol Med. (2019) 49:200–11. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718001988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Celka P, Charlton PH, Farukh B, Chowienczyk P, Alastruey J. Influence of mental stress on the pulse wave features of photoplethysmograms. Healthc Technol Lett. (2020) 7:7–12. doi: 10.1049/htl.2019.0001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Iani C, Gopher D, Lavie P. Effects of task difficulty and invested mental effort on peripheral vasoconstriction. Psychophysiology. (2004) 41:789–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00200.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Luo L, Xiao L, Miao D, Luo X. The relationship between mental stress induced changes in cortisol levels and vascular responses quantified by waveform analysis: investigating stress-dependent indices of vascular changes. In: 2012 international conference on biomedical engineering and biotechnology. IEEE, Macau, Macao (2012). p. 929–33. Available at: http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/document/6245274/.

Google Scholar

19. Kontaxis S, Gil E, Marozas V, Lazaro J, Garcia E, Posadas-de Miguel M, et al. Photoplethysmographic waveform analysis for autonomic reactivity assessment in depression. IEEE Trans BioMed Eng. (2021) 68:1273–81. doi: 10.1109/TBME.10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Gasparini F, Grossi A, Bandini S. A deep learning approach to recognize cognitive load using PPG signals. In: Proceedings of the 14th PErvasive technologies related to assistive environments conference. ACM, Corfu Greece (2021). p. 489–95. doi: 10.1145/3453892.3461625

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Greene S, Thapliyal H, Caban-Holt A. A Survey of Affective Computing for Stress Detection: Evaluating technologies in stress detection for better health. IEEE Consum Electron Mag. (2016) 5:44–56. doi: 10.1109/MCE.2016.2590178

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Xuan Q, Wu J, Shen J, Ji X, Lyu Y, Zhang Y. Assessing cognitive load in adolescent and adult students using photoplethysmogram morphometrics. Cognit Neurodyn. (2020) 14:709–21. doi: 10.1007/s11571-020-09617-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Lyzwinski LN, Elgendi M, Menon C. The use of photoplethysmography in the assessment of mental health: scoping review. JMIR Ment Health. (2023) 10:e40163. doi: 10.2196/40163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Montoya A, Bruins R, Katzman M, Blier P. The noradrenergic paradox: implications in the management of depression and anxiety. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2016) 541:557. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S91311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Morilak DA, Barrera G, Echevarria DJ, Garcia AS, Hernandez A, Ma S, et al. Role of brain norepinephrine in the behavioral response to stress. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 29:1214–24. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Goldstein DS. Adrenal responses to stress. Cell Mol Neurobiol. (2010) 30:1433–40. doi: 10.1007/s10571-010-9606-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Kvetňanský R, Pacák K, Fukuhara K, Viskupič E, Hiremagalur B, Nankova B, et al. Sympathoadrenal system in stress: interaction with the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical systema. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (1995) 771:131–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb44676.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Bremner JD, Krystal JH, Southwick SM, Charney DS. Noradrenergic mechanisms in stress and anxiety: I. preclinical studies. Synapse. (1996) 23:28–38. doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1098-2396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Liu Z, Barnes SA, Sokolnicki LA, Snyder EM, Johnson BD, Turner ST, et al. Beta-2 adrenergic receptor polymorphisms and the forearm blood flow response to mental stress. Clin Auton Res. (2006) 16:105–12. doi: 10.1007/s10286-006-0329-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Lucia CD, Eguchi A, Koch WJ. New insights in cardiac β-adrenergic signaling during heart failure and aging. Front Pharmacol. (2018) 9:904. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2018.00904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ernst H, Scherpf M, Pannasch S, Helmert JR, Malberg H, Schmidt M. Assessment of the human response to acute mental stress–An overview and a multimodal study. PloS One. (2023) 18:e0294069. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0294069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Benchekroun M, Chevallier B, Beaouiss H, Istrate D, Zalc V, Khalil M, et al. Comparison of Stress Detection through ECG and PPG signals using a Random Forest-based Algorithm. In: 2022 44th annual international conference of the IEEE engineering in medicine & Biology society (EMBC). IEEE, Glasgow, Scotland, United Kingdom (2022). p. 3150–3. Available at: https://ieeexplore.ieee.org/document/9870984/.

Google Scholar

33. Li X, Hu C, Meng A, Guo Y, Chen Y, Dang R. Heart rate variability and heart rate monitoring of nurses using PPG and ECG signals during working condition: A pilot study. Health Sci Rep. (2022) 5:e477. doi: 10.1002/hsr2.477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Healey JA, Picard RW. Detecting stress during real-world driving tasks using physiological sensors. IEEE Trans Intell Transp Syst. (2005) 6:156–66. doi: 10.1109/TITS.2005.848368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Portnova GV, Liaukovich KM, Vasilieva LN, Alshanskaia EI. Autonomic and behavioral indicators on increased cognitive loading in healthy volunteers. Neurosci Behav Physiol. (2023) 53:92–102. doi: 10.1007/s11055-023-01394-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Alshanskaia E, Martynova O. Eye tracking and autonomic nervous system responses. A machine learning approach to understanding cognitive stress. In: 2023 fifth international conference neurotechnologies and neurointerfaces (CNN). IEEE, Kaliningrad, Russian Federation (2023). p. 8–11.

Google Scholar

37. Beck AT. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1961) 4:561. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Beck AT, Rial WY, Rickels K. Short form of depression inventory: cross-validation. Psychol Rep. (1974) 34:1184–6. doi: 10.1177/003329417403403s01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown G. Beck depression inventory–II. The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX, USA (2011). doi: 10.1037/t00742–000.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Wideman TH, Sullivan MJL, Inada S, McIntyre D, Kumagai M, Yahagi N, et al. Beck depression inventory (BDI). In: Gellman MD, Turner JR, editors. Encyclopedia of behavioral medicine. Springer New York, New York, NY (2013). p. 178–9. doi: 10.1007/978–1-4419–1005-9_441

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press (1983).

Google Scholar

42. Spielberger CD, Reheiser EC. Assessment of emotions: anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity. Appl Psychol Health Well-Being. (2009) 1:271–302. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01017.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Arsalidou M, Pascual-Leone J, Johnson J. Misleading cues improve developmental assessment of working memory capacity: The color matching tasks. Cognit Dev. (2010) 25:262–77. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Proskurnina EV, Liaukovich KM, Bychkovskaya LS, Mikheev IV, Alshanskaia EI, Proskurnin MA, et al. Salivary antioxidant capacity and magnesium in generalized anxiety disorder. Metabolites. (2023) 13:73. doi: 10.3390/metabo13010073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Makowski D, Pham T, Lau ZJ, Brammer JC, Lespinasse F, Pham H, et al. NeuroKit2: A Python toolbox for neurophysiological signal processing. Behav Res Methods. (2021) 53:1689–96. doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01516-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Brammer J. biopeaks: a graphical user interface for feature extraction from heart- and breathing biosignals. J Open Source Software. (2020) 5:2621. doi: 10.21105/joss.02621

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Elgendi M, Norton I, Brearley M, Abbott D, Schuurmans D. Systolic peak detection in acceleration photoplethysmograms measured from emergency responders in tropical conditions. PloS One. (2013) 8:e76585. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Richer R, Küderle A, Ullrich M, Rohleder N, Eskofier B. BioPsyKit: A Python package for the analysis of biopsychological data. J Open Source Software. (2021) 6:3702. doi: 10.21105/joss.03702

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Berntson GG, Quigley KS, Jang JF, Boysen ST. An approach to artifact identification: application to heart period data. Psychophysiology. (1990) 27:586–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb01982.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D, Heisterkamp S, Van Willigen B. nlme: linear and nonlinear mixed effects models (2018). Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/nlme/index.html.

Google Scholar

51. Akaike H. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans Autom Control. (1974) 19:716–23. doi: 10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Hughes AM, Hancock GM, Marlow SL, Stowers K, Salas E. Cardiac measures of cognitive workload: A meta-analysis. Hum Factors J Hum Factors Ergon Soc. (2019) 61:393–414. doi: 10.1177/0018720819830553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Koch C, Wilhelm M, Salzmann S, Rief W, Euteneuer F. A meta-analysis of heart rate variability in major depression. Psychol Med. (2019) 49:1948–57. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719001351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Kim HG, Cheon EJ, Bai DS, Lee YH, Koo BH. Stress and heart rate variability: A meta-analysis and review of the literature. Psychiatry Investig. (2018) 15:235–45. doi: 10.30773/pi.2017.08.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Charles RL, Nixon J. Measuring mental workload using physiological measures: A systematic review. Appl Ergon. (2019) 74:221–32. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2018.08.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Schäfer A, Vagedes J. How accurate is pulse rate variability as an estimate of heart rate variability? Int J Cardiol. (2013) 166:15–29. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2012.03.119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Goddard AW, Ball SG, Martinez J, Robinson MJ, Yang CR, Russell JM, et al. Current perspectives of the roles of the central norepinephrine system in anxiety and depression. Depress Anxiety. (2010) 27:339–50. doi: 10.1002/da.v27:4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Rudner XL, Berkowitz DE, Booth JV, Funk BL, Cozart KL, D’Amico EB, et al. Subtype specific regulation of human vascular α 1 -adrenergic receptors by vessel bed and age. Circulation. (1999) 100:2336–43. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.100.23.2336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Woo AYH, Xiao RP. β-Adrenergic receptor subtype signaling in heart: From bench to bedside. Acta Pharmacol Sin. (2012) 33:335–41. doi: 10.1038/aps.2011.201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Santos IN, Spadari-Bratfisch RC. Stress and cardiac beta adrenoceptors. Stress. (2006) 9:69–84. doi: 10.1080/10253890600771858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Perez DM. α1-adrenergic receptors in neurotransmission, synaptic plasticity, and cognition. Front Pharmacol. (2020) 11:581098. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2020.581098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Reid JL. Alpha-adrenergic receptors and blood pressure control. Am J Cardiol. (1986) 57:E6–12. doi: 10.1016/0002-9149(86)90716-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Iaccarino G, Cipolletta E, Fiorillo A, Annecchiarico M, Ciccarelli M, Cimini V, et al. β 2 -adrenergic receptor gene delivery to the endothelium corrects impaired adrenergic vasorelaxation in hypertension. Circulation. (2002) 106:349–55. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000022690.55143.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Ignarro LJ, Buga GM, Wood KS, Byrns RE, Chaudhuri G. Endothelium-derived relaxing factor produced and released from artery and vein is nitric oxide. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (1987) 84:9265–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.84.24.9265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Myslivecek J, Trojan S. Regulation of adrenoceptors and muscarinic receptors in the heart. Gen Physiol Biophys. (2003) 22:3–14.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

66. Siddi S, Bailon R, Giné-Vázquez I, Matcham F, Lamers F, Kontaxis S, et al. The usability of daytime and night-time heart rate dynamics as digital biomarkers of depression severity. Psychol Med. (2023) 53:3249–60. doi: 10.1017/S0033291723001034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Delaney JPA, Brodie DA. Effects of short-term psychological stress on the time and frequency domains of heart-rate variability. Percept Mot Skills. (2000) 91:515–24. doi: 10.2466/pms.2000.91.2.515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Richter M, Friedrich A, Gendolla GHE. Task difficulty effects on cardiac activity. Psychophysiology. (2008) 45:869–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00688.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Ford TC, Simpson T, McPhee G, Stough C, Downey LA. Trait and state anxiety is marked by increased working memory-related parietal BOLD signal. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2018) 278:92–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Gawda B, Szepietowska E. Trait anxiety modulates brain activity during performance of verbal fluency tasks. Front Behav Neurosci. (2016) 10. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Jaiswal S, Tsai SY, Juan CH, Liang WK, Muggleton NG. Better cognitive performance is associated with the combination of high trait mindfulness and low trait anxiety. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:627. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Stout DM, Shackman AJ, Pedersen WS, Miskovich TA, Larson CL. Neural circuitry governing anxious individuals’ mis-allocation of working memory to threat. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:8742. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-08443-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Zhu H, Liu Z, Zhou Y, Yin X, Xu B, Ma L, et al. Lack of β2-AR increases anxiety-like behaviors and rewarding properties of cocaine. Front Behav Neurosci. (2017) 11:49/full. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00049/full

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Nesse RM. Adrenergic function in patients with panic anxiety. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1984) 41:771. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1984.01790190045005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Pigott TA. Gender differences in the epidemiology and treatment of anxiety disorders. J Clin Psychiatry. (1999) 60 Suppl 18:4–15.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

76. Albert PR. Why is depression more prevalent in women? J Psychiatry Neurosci. (2015) 40:219–21. doi: 10.1503/jpn.150205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive stress, electrocardiogram, photoplethysmogram, heart rate, HRV, depression, anxiety

Citation: Alshanskaia EI, Zhozhikashvili NA, Polikanova IS and Martynova OV (2024) Heart rate response to cognitive load as a marker of depression and increased anxiety. Front. Psychiatry 15:1355846. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1355846

Received: 14 December 2023; Accepted: 26 June 2024;
Published: 11 July 2024.

Edited by:

Tamas Kozicz, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, United States

Reviewed by:

Andrea Webb, Draper Laboratory, United States
Liang Cheng, Sichuan Sports College, China

Copyright © 2024 Alshanskaia, Zhozhikashvili, Polikanova and Martynova. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Evgeniia I. Alshanskaia, eisokolova@hse.ru

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.