Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 05 March 2024
Sec. Intellectual Disabilities
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychometrics in Psychiatry: Intellectual Disabilities View all articles

Validation of factor structure of the neurodevelopmental parent report for outcome monitoring in down syndrome: confirmatory factor analysis

  • 1Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
  • 2Department of Neurology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
  • 3Harvard Medical School, Harvard University, Boston, MA, United States
  • 4Biostatistics and Research Design Center, Institutional Centers for Clinical and Translational Research, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, MA, United States

Introduction: The Neurodevelopmental Parent Report for Outcome Monitoring (ND-PROM), initially developed to monitor developmental and behavioral functions in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), assesses symptoms across a wide range of domains relevant in Down syndrome (DS).

Methods: Psychometric properties of ND-PROM were assessed in 385 individuals with DS and 52 with a combined diagnosis of DS and ASD (DS+ASD), whose caregivers completed the ND-PROM questionnaire for a clinical visit in a specialized Down syndrome program at a tertiary pediatric hospital. Confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to evaluate the internal structure validity of the ND-PROM. Measurement invariance was assessed, with a comparison group of 246 individuals with ASD, and latent mean differences between the DS and ASD-only groups, as well as the combined DS+ASD groups, were assessed.

Results: Findings support the existence of the 12 clinically-derived factors in the DS population: Expressive Language, Receptive Language, Adaptive skills/Toileting, Social Emotional Understanding, Social Interaction, Independent Play, Sensory Processes, Challenging Behaviors, Impulse/ADHD, and Mental Health. Differences in response patterns of development and behaviors were observed between those with DS and those with ASD, including those with DS having higher abilities in nonverbal communication, social emotional understanding, and social interaction, and fewer restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests, impulsivity or ADHD symptoms, and mental health concerns compared to those with ASD. Individuals in the DS+ASD group had more difficulties with expressive and receptive language, nonverbal and social communication, social interaction, independent play, and adaptive skills than either the DS-only group or the ASD-only groups.

Discussion: The ND-PROM has a desirable factor structure and is a valid and clinically useful tool that captures a range of distinct and independent areas of developmental and behavioral functioning in DS, for individuals with and without an ASD diagnosis.

1 Introduction

Down syndrome (DS), caused by the presence of all or part of an extra chromosome 21, occurs in about 1/700 births (1). DS is the most common genetic cause of intellectual disability (ID), though cognitive, language, and adaptive abilities vary greatly (27). DS is associated with a high prevalence of co-occurring neurodevelopmental, behavioral, and mental health conditions that can greatly impact overall functioning (8). Co-occurring ASD is particularly prevalent, occurring in up to 39% (911), and is typically associated with lower cognitive and language abilities, and higher rates of behavior problems (12). There is also an increasingly recognized phenomenon of unexplained regression in DS, now known as Down Syndrome Regression Disorder, which is associated with loss of skills and onset of autistic-like behaviors or catatonia (1315). Thus, clinical care for individuals with DS requires clinicians to monitor developmental and behavioral progress across multiple domains, and to identify and manage any unexpected changes in behavior or deviations in development.

In order to enable efficient, patient-centered care, a standardized approach is needed that tracks a wide range of potential symptoms and identifies those at a heightened risk for co-occurring neurodevelopmental or mental health conditions. Parent-reported measures have been used in DS, including the Aberrant Behavior Checklist (12), Child Behavior Checklist (16), Social Responsiveness Scale (17), the Social Communication Questionnaire (18), and the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Disorders (19). However, many tools have not been validated in people with DS or are used only in specific age groups or to target particular symptom clusters. Evaluation of the breadth of symptoms and concerns that children and adolescents with DS may experience therefore necessitates the use of multiple scales. Thus, there remains a great need for a single tool that can be used to obtain information on developmental and behavioral domains applicable to people with DS, across a wide range of ages, developmental stages, and functional levels, that can ultimately be used to monitor clinically-relevant symptoms and skills.

The Neurodevelopmental Parent Report for Outcome Monitoring (ND-PROM; previously published as the Autism Spectrum Disorder Parent Report for Outcome Monitoring, ASD-PROM) is a freely-available tool initially developed to clinically monitor caregiver report of developmental and behavioral functions in children with ASD (20). The ND-PROM contains 128 Likert-scale items that cover a wide range of developmental skills and behaviors relevant to children with neurodevelopmental disabilities. Prior work using the ND-PROM in individuals ages 2-20 years old with ASD demonstrated clinical utility, test-re-test reliability, and good convergent validity with the Vineland-II (20). Further development of the ND-PROM involved delineation of 93 of the individual items into 12 clinical domains, which were subsequently supported using confirmatory factory analysis in the ASD population: Expressive Language, Receptive Language, Nonverbal Communication, Social Communication, Social Interaction, Independent Play, Adaptive Skills, Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors, Sensory Processes, Challenging/Aggressive Behaviors, Impulse/ADHD, and Mental Health (21). Items related to sleep, possible emergence of epilepsy, and drug or alcohol use from the original survey were not included in the factors. This suggested that the ND-PROM has good potential for independently assessing these key functional domains and identifying domains of relative strengths and weaknesses (e.g. restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests, or impulsivity or ADHD symptoms), which can identify targeted areas for intervention (21).

Given the applicability of these skill and behavioral areas in the clinical management and treatment of Down syndrome, the Boston Children’s Hospital Down Syndrome Program began implementation of the ND-PROM as part of standard clinical care to gather developmental and behavioral information from parents and caregivers about their children with DS prior to clinic visits. While previous confirmatory factor analysis in a population of children with ASD confirmed 12 separate clinical domains for which the questions were best represented (21), it is not clear that the same skills, symptoms and behaviors track similarly in a population of children with Down syndrome, which is a different neurodevelopmental condition with different neurocognitive and behavioral profiles. Therefore, the current study aims to assess psychometric properties of the ND-PROM in Down syndrome.

This paper describes the internal structure validity of the ND-PROM in a large clinical population of children and adolescents with DS, some of whom also have ASD (DS+ASD). Using Confirmatory Factor Analysis, we will examine factor structure and internal consistency of the measure in a new clinical population, determine the factor structure, confirm the measurement invariance of the ND-PROM, and assess latent mean differences among groups.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants and procedures

Participants included 385 individuals with DS and no diagnosis of ASD, along with 52 individuals with a dual diagnosis of DS and ASD (DS+ASD) who were seen in a specialized Down Syndrome Program in a tertiary pediatric hospital from 2017-2021. Patients were assessed and followed by specialty providers who regularly evaluate for ASD as part of their clinical practice using Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, 5th Edition (DSM-5) criteria (22). A comparison group consisting of 246 patients with a diagnosis of ASD and not DS were used to test measurement invariance, and to assess latent mean differences among DS, ASD, and DS+ASD groups. Details about this group, including diagnostic determination and data collection methodology, is available in Levin et al. (20). Caregivers completed the ND-PROM as part of standardized clinical procedures prior to clinical visits, to streamline clinical history taking and developmental and behavioral monitoring as a quality improvement initiative in the programs. Participants completed the ND-PROM using either a web-based system in which parents received automated prescheduled emails with secure links to complete the questionnaire online, or by completing a PDF or paper copy of the ND-PROM. The study was approved by the Boston Children’s Hospital Institutional Review Board.

2.2 Data analyses

All analyses were conducted using Mplus 8.9 (23). The level of significance was set to 5% for a two-tailed test.

2.2.1 Internal consistency

In order to measure internal consistency, which allows us to examine how reliably the ND-PROM is able to address the constructs it is meant to measure, two coefficients were engaged: the popular alpha of Cronbach and McDonald’s omega (2426), which is appropriate for non-tau equivalent instruments, where each factor is not assumed to have equal item-latent variable relations.

2.2.2 Confirmatory factor analysis to examine internal factor structure

Internal factor structure was examined using the Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) framework using the weighted least squares estimate using mean and variance adjustments for the expected non-normality of ordinal indicators. A 12-factor CFA model for ordered categorical indicators using the Weighted Least Squares Mean and Variance Adjusted Estimator (WLSMV) was estimated assuming the presence of 12 distinct, correlated dimensions. This covariance modeling approach estimates discrepancies between the population variance-covariance matrix and the sample-based matrix. Model fit is assessed using both absolute and relative criteria of exact fit, close fit, or not so close fit (27). Exact fit is based on the assumption that there is no discrepancy between hypothesized and estimated variance-covariance matrices, Σ(θ). It represents an extremely strict set of evaluative criteria in the measurement of real-life phenomena, as minimal discrepancies would result in large chi-square values, particularly in the presence of excessive power. Thus, evaluation of the magnitude of the chi-square values should be the last resort in evaluating model fit. The same logic applies also to evaluating residuals with RMSEA values equal to zero. Using values of the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) between 0.05 and 0.08 are suggestive of acceptable but still “not exact” fit.

In addition to relying on the RMSEA, a series of descriptive fit indices (28), relative or incremental (e.g., comparative fit index (CFI); Tucker-Lewis index (TLI)) were employed based on the discrepancy function, adjusting for model complexity (i.e., number of estimated parameters and degrees of freedom). A large number of simulation studies examining their strengths and weaknesses (29) have favored the CFI and TLI as being relatively unaffected by sample sizes and model complexity (30) and were thus, used in the present study. Evaluative criteria of proper model fit usually involve values greater than 0.900 or more recently greater than 0.950 on the descriptive fit indices (31), RMSEA values between 0.05 and 0.08 (i.e., between close and not-so-close fit), and non-significant chi-square values (a strict omnibus criterion).

2.2.3 Measurement invariance

Among the available restrictive models to test for measurement invariance, typically three levels of restriction are utilized (32). These are termed configural, metric, and scalar, and contain the necessary restrictions to conduct test of significance at the latent means level, presuming they are all satisfied. Consequently, across the two populations of DS and ASD, the three levels were examined with the configural model testing the equivalence of the factor model’s simple structure, the metric model imposing the equivalence of factor loadings linking the items to the latent construct, and last, the scalar or “strong invariance” model testing, in addition to the factor loadings, the equivalence of the intercept terms (or thresholds in categorical indicators). Tests of significance by use of difference chi-square tests for nested models are constructed to test whether the additional constraints are justified.

In cases where the classic protocol of strong measurement invariance was not met, we deferred to the alignment procedure developed by Muthen & Asparouhov (33). This methodology utilizes the configural model of no invariance and identifies the largest number of invariant parameters by allowing factor means and variances to vary freely across groups. The model utilizes a simplicity function to identify as many approximately invariant parameters as possible with few non-invariant parameters, thus attaining the goal of validly comparing latent means and variances between groups. To conclude the presence of measurement invariance, the number of significant between group parameters needs to be minimal or less than 25%. If measurement invariance is achieved, a latent means comparison can be conducted between the DS, ASD, and combined DS+ASD groups.

2.2.4 Effect size indicator

When latent means were contrasted, latent Cohen’s d effect size statistic was utilized which presents results in the standard deviation metric. Conventions for effect size are small (SD=0.2), medium (SD=0.5) and large (SD=0.8) (34). Meaningful differences are considered as those in excess of a 0.5 standard deviation.

3 Results

3.1 Sociodemographics

There were 204 (53%) males and 181 (47%) females in the DS group, 205 (83.3%) males and 41(16.7%) females in the ASD group, and 39 males (73.6%) and 14 females (26.4%) in the DS+ASD group (Table 1), with the chi-square test of the differences in proportions being significant [χ2(1)=62.828, p<0.001]. Females were significantly less represented in the ASD group and the DS+ASD group compared to the DS group alone. Mean age in the DS group was 9.22 years (SD=4.97), in the ASD group 9.03 years (SD=3.96) and in the DS+ASD group 9.66 years (SD=4.87) pointing to non-significant between groups differences [F(2, 681)=0.432, p=0.650].

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of study participants.

3.2 Construct validity and internal consistency reliability of ND-PROM in DS

Table 2 contains information about Cronbach’s alpha and McDonald’s omega across domains and groups. Estimates in alpha ranged between 0.578 and 0.925 in the DS group, between 0.632 and 0.932 in the ASD group, and between 0.538 and 0.924 in the DS+ASD group. The CFA model posited 12 latent factors as with the original ND-PROM in the ASD population (Levin 2022 vs SDBP Abstract). Global fit as judged by the chi-square test was significant, likely reflecting excessive levels of power. Use of descriptive fit indices and residuals pointed to acceptable model fit [CFI=0.90; TLI=0.90, RMSEA=0.04, Chi.square/DF=1.68, SRMR=0.10], indicating appropriateness of using this same 12-factor model in the DS cohort. These results followed Bartlett’s correction for sample size (see Supplementary Materials for R-function developed for that purpose).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Internal consistency reliability of ND-PROM constructs across DS and ASD groups.

3.3 Measurement invariance across DS, ASD, and DS+ASD groups

Table 3 displays results from the “exact” measurement invariance protocol when testing the 12 factor simple structure across the three groups. As expected, the chi-square statistical tests were significant for all three models, configural/metric/scalar with increased numbers of constraints. However, of interest were the comparisons between (a) configural and metric models, and (b) metric and scalar models. As shown in Table 3, neither the equivalence of factor loadings nor the equivalence of thresholds were supported when contrasting the DS and ASD samples. Consequently, the alignment procedure outlined above, was implemented to target partial measurement invariance so that tests of latent means would be possible.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Measurement invariance across DS, ASD, and DS+ASD groups using an exact-fit protocol.

Following alignment (33), results indicated that all but two of the factor loadings and all but eight of the intercepts were equivalent between the DS, ASD, and DS+ASD groups. Thus, the model was able to converge on a simplicity function where estimates of factor loadings and intercepts were largely invariant between groups. Table 4 displays factor loadings and intercepts between groups and the decision of equivalence based on alignment. The amount of significant and non-invariant parameters was equal to 5.4%, much less than the 25% guideline and close to the nominal level of significance on the number of significant tests due to chance.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 Standardized factor loadings from a confirmatory factor analysis model for the 12 latent variables of the ND-PROM across DS, ASD and DS+ASD groups.

3.4 Latent mean differences across DS, ASD, and DS+ASD groups

Table 5 and Figure 1 display latent means and between group comparisons using both inferential statistical criteria and effect size indicators. As shown in Table 5, there were significant differences between the DS and ASD groups on nonverbal communication and social interaction, with the DS group having significantly higher means (nonverbal communication difference=1.021 SD; social interaction difference=0.933 SD). Higher means in these areas are indicative of higher skill levels. Similarly significantly lower scores in the ASD were observed in RRBs (-1.078 SD), sensory processes (-0.881 SD), challenging behaviors (-0.614 SD), impulse/ADHD (-1.376 SD), and mental health (-1.050 SD). Last, there were significant differences between the DS and ASD groups in adaptive skills/toileting (-0.458 SD); with the DS group having significantly lower mean levels compared to the ASD group.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5 Latent mean differences between DS, ASD, and DS+ASD groups using standardized point estimates.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Latent mean differences between DS, ASD and DS+ASD groups on ND-PROM factors. Mean differences for each of the 12 factors of the Neurodevelopmental Parent Report for Outcome Montioring (ND-PROM) survey are shown for three groups: Down syndrome (DS; n=385), autism spectrum disorder (ASD; n=246), and a dual diagnosis of DS and ASD (DS+ASD; n=53).

When contrasting the DS to the DS+ASD groups, results indicated significantly higher functioning for the DS group in expressive language (1.558 SD), receptive language (1.262 SD), nonverbal communication (2.216 SD), social communication (1.489 SD), social interaction (2.079 SD), independent play (1.390 SD), and adaptive skills (1.103 SD). A significantly lower functioning of the combined group was observed in restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests (-1.064 SD), sensory processes (-1.672 SD), challenging behaviors (-0.699 SD), and impulse/ADHD (-0.841 SD). There were no differences between the DS and combined DS+ASD groups for the mental health domain (-0.128 SD).

Comparisons between the ASD and the DS+ASD groups indicated that the ASD group was significantly higher in expressive language (1.579 SD), receptive language (1.261 SD), nonverbal communication (0.904 SD), social communication (1.032 SD), social interaction (0.909 SD), independent play (1.275 SD), and adaptive skills (1.593 SD), all areas where higher means indicate higher skill or functioning levels. However, the ASD group also had a higher mean for impulse/ADHD (0.535 SD). Significantly lower functioning was observed for the combined group on sensory processes (-0.620 SD).

4 Discussion

The present study evaluated the ND-PROM as a clinical monitoring tool to assess skills and behaviors in the DS population. The study evaluates internal structure validity of the ND-PROM with measurement invariance across DS, ASD, and DS+ASD samples. Our findings support the existence of the 12 clinically derived factors in the DS population: Expressive Language, Receptive Language, Nonverbal Communication, Social Emotional Understanding, Social Interaction, Independent Play, Adaptive skills/Toileting, Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors and Interests, Sensory Processes, Challenging Behaviors, Impulse/ADHD, and Mental Health. This 12-factor model was previously confirmed through confirmatory factor analysis in the ASD population, and here we have shown that the ND-PROM tool works equally well and can capture the range of distinct and independent areas of developmental and behavioral functioning present in those with DS. Assessment of measurement invariance across DS, ASD, and DS+ASD groups showed that the ND-PROM was able to psychometrically distinguish between these three groups, indicating the specificity of the tool in elucidating different patterns of development and behaviors between children with DS, ASD, and DS+ASD.

Assessment of latent mean differences between DS and ASD revealed differences in patterns of development, and skills, and behaviors in those with DS compared to those with ASD. Expressive and receptive language, independent play skills, sensory processes, and challenging behaviors were similar in the sample populations of ASD and DS in the study. However, those with DS were found to have higher abilities in nonverbal communication, social emotional understanding, social interaction, and fewer reported restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests, impulsivity or ADHD symptoms, and mental health concerns compared to children with ASD in this study. These findings are largely in line with prior reports describing profiles for children with DS. Children with DS have been described to have relative strengths in nonverbal communication and social skills (2, 7, 35, 36), whereas these are defining core areas of impairment for children with ASD (22). Adaptive skills have previously been reported to be higher for children with DS compared to those with ASD (37, 38), however in this study focused on toileting skills, this pattern was not found. Additionally, while challenging behaviors and mental health concerns, as well as restricted and repetitive behaviors, and impulse control/ADHD symptoms are commonly reported in DS, they may occur less than in other populations of children with ID (4, 39).

Compared to the DS-only group, the DS+ASD group shows areas of concern in language and communication domains, as well as in social interaction, independent play, and adaptive skills. The combined group also had more issues with restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests, sensory processing, and challenging behaviors. This is consistent with previous research showing vulnerabilities in children and adolecents with co-occurring DS and ASD (12, 40, 41). When comparing to the ASD-only group, the DS+ASD group had decreased functioning in communication and language, including social communication and social interaction, as well as adaptive skills. This differs from previous research which showed fewer issues with social interaction in a combined DS+ASD group compared to an ASD-only group (42). Interestingly, the ASD-only group had higher levels of impulse/ADHD symptomatology reported.

We recognize several limitations of the current work. The study population included primarily White, non-Hispanic respondents, thus may not be representative. However, the ND-PROM is now available in additional languages, thus future analyses can include a more diverse population. Though previously parents reported that use of the ND-PROM had a positive impact on their child’s care (20), the length of the ND-PROM may be a limitation for its widespread application, especially in primary care clinics or other, non-tertiary care environments. In this study, the DS and ASD cohorts were not matched by cognitive level, thus more pronounced group differences may have been observed if the DS group were directly compared to a group with ASD and Intellectual Disability. Additionally, this study did not assess the stability of responses across repeated tests or the sensitivity of the measure to change over time, and therefore it is not clear how clinically meaningful changes in function might be represented on the ND-PROM. Additionally, though convergent validity was previously assessed in the ASD propulation (20), it was not repeated in the DS population as a part of this study. Finally, the ND-PROM responses were collected as part of clinical care visits to a specialty clinic within a tertiary pediatric care center. Therefore, patients included in both the DS and ASD samples might be more severely impacted than their peers in the general population, hence their desire to seek out specailized care. However, this scale was designed to facilitate clinical visits in the medical setting and this study has shown that it is useable within that setting. A last cautionary note relates to the relatively low internal consistency reliability estimates for some of the ND-PROM scales. In these instances we suggest caution about using the scores from individual domains for diagnostic and classification purposes. Instead future studies may consider revising the content of some of these scales so that levels of internal consistency reliability will increase.

Future directions will explore the use of the ND-PROM longitudinally to assess the ability to capture change over time, which will have implications when assessing response to intervention. Additionally, as the ND-PROM is now available in additional languages, future studies will include a more diverse population. Subsequent studies may also include an examination of a Computerized Adaptive Testing (CAT) framework using the computerized version of the ND-PROM, such that larger item pools can be created and an adaptive algorithm can be implemented to assess competency in each domain through defining a minimum tolerated error of measurement. CAT methodologies engage approximately 10-15% of the total number of items and thus, the gains in time, efficiency, with no sacrifice to the validity of the measure would enable increased use and scalability of the ND-PROM.

The ND-PROM is a clinically useful tool for assessing children and adolescents with DS, ASD, and DS+ASD, which captures a range of distinct and independent areas of developmental and behavioral functioning between and among these three groups.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because the data contain protected Personal Health Information. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to NB, nicole.baumer@childrens.harvard.edu.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Boston Children's Hospital Institutional Review Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The ethics committee/institutional review board waived the requirement of written informed consent for participation from the participants or the participants' legal guardians/next of kin because a waiver was issued; information included retrospectively analyzed data that was collected for clinical purposes.

Author contributions

NB: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. KP: Conceptualization, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. BZ: Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. GS: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful for the contributions of ND-PROM clinical workgroup.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1293937/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Data and Statistics on Down Syndrome. CDC (2023). Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/birthdefects/downsyndrome/data.html.

Google Scholar

2. Lott IT, Dierssen M. Cognitive deficits and associated neurological complications in individuals with Down’s syndrome. Lancet Neurol. (2010) 9:623–33. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70112-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Abbeduto L, Warren SF, Conners FA. Language development in Down syndrome: From the prelinguistic period to the acquisition of literacy. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. (2007) 13:247–61. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Chapman RS, Hesketh LJ. Behavioral phenotype of individuals with Down syndrome. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. (2000) 6:84–95. doi: 10.1002/1098-2779(2000)6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Dressler A, Perelli V, Feucht M, Bargagna S. Adaptive behaviour in Down syndrome: a cross-sectional study from childhood to adulthood. Wien Klin Wochenschr. (2010) 122:673–80. doi: 10.1007/s00508-010-1504-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Martin GE, Klusek J, Estigarribia B, Roberts JE. Language characteristics of individuals with down syndrome. Top Lang Disord. (2009) 29:112–32. doi: 10.1097/TLD.0b013e3181a71fe1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Næss KAB, Nygaard E, Ostad J, Dolva AS, Lyster SAH. The profile of social functioning in children with Down syndrome. Disabil Rehabil. (2017) 39:1320–31. doi: 10.1080/09638288.2016.1194901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Bull MJ. Down syndrome. N Engl J Med. (2020) 382:2344–52. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1706537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Spinazzi NA, Santoro JD, Pawlowski K, Anzueto G, Howe YJ, Patel LR, et al. Co-occurring conditions in children with Down syndrome and autism: a retrospective study. J Neurodev Disord. (2023) 15:9. doi: 10.1186/s11689-023-09478-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Oxelgren UW, Myrelid Å, Annerén G, Ekstam B, Göransson C, Holmbom A, et al. Prevalence of autism and attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder in Down syndrome: a population-based study. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2017) 59:276–83. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Richards C, Jones C, Groves L, Moss J, Oliver C. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder phenomenology in genetic disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. (2015) 2:909–16. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00376-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Capone GT, Grados MA, Kaufmann WE, Bernad-Ripoll S, Jewell A. Down syndrome and comorbid autism-spectrum disorder: characterization using the aberrant behavior checklist. Am J Med Genet A. (2005) 134:373–80. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.30622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Santoro JD, Filipink RA, Baumer NT, Bulova PD, Handen BL. Down syndrome regression disorder: updates and therapeutic advances. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2023) 36:96–103. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Rivelli A, Fitzpatrick V, Chaudhari S, Chicoine L, Jia G, Rzhetsky A, et al. Prevalence of mental health conditions among 6078 individuals with down syndrome in the United States. J Patient Cent Res Rev. (2022) 9:58–63. doi: 10.17294/2330-0698.1875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Rosso M, Fremion E, Santoro SL, Oreskovic NM, Chitnis T, Skotko BG, et al. Down syndrome disintegrative disorder: A clinical regression syndrome of increasing importance. Pediatrics. (2020) 145:e20192939. doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-2939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Esbensen AJ, Hoffman EK, Shaffer R, Chen E, Patel L, Jacola L. Reliability of parent report measures of behaviour in children with Down syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. (2018) 62:785–97. doi: 10.1111/jir.12533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Channell MM. The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS-2) in school-age children with Down syndrome at low risk for autism spectrum disorder. Autism Dev Lang Impair. (2020) 5:2396941520962406. doi: 10.1177/2396941520962406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Magyar CI, Pandolfi V, Dill CA. An initial evaluation of the Social Communication Questionnaire for the assessment of autism spectrum disorders in children with Down syndrome. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2012) 33:134–45. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e318240d3d9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Sánchez-Teruel D, Robles-Bello MA. Preliminary study on psychometric properties of an anxiety scale in down syndrome with anxiety symptoms. Int J Psychol Res (Medellin). (2020) 13:50–61. doi: 10.21500/20112084.4493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Levin AR, Baumer N, Amaral J, Sargado S, Pawlowski K, Chiujdea M, et al. Autism spectrum disorder parent report for outcome monitoring: A preliminary report of development and clinical utility. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2021) 42(4):272–82. doi: 10.1037/t84123-000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Baumer NT, Pawlowski K, Amaral J, Zhang B, Sideridis G, Levin AR. Clinically derived 12-factor structure and confirmatory factor analysis of the neurodevelopmental parent report for outcome monitoring (ND-PROM). (2023) 14:1243467. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1243467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. American Psychiatric Association, American Psychiatric Association DSM-5 Task Force. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Muthen LK, Muthen BO. Mplus: Statistical Analysis with Latent Variables: User’s Guide (Version 8). Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen (2017).

Google Scholar

24. McDonald RP. The theoretical foundations of principal factor analysis, canonical factor analysis, and alpha factor analysis. Br J Math Stat Psychol. (1970) 23:1–21. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.1970.tb00432.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Raykov T. Scale reliability, cronbach’s coefficient alpha, and violations of essential tau-equivalence with fixed congeneric components. Multivariate Behav Res. (1997) 32:329–53. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3204_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Raykov T. A method for obtaining standard errors and confidence intervals of composite reliability for congeneric items. Appl psychol Measurement. (1998) 22:369–74. doi: 10.1177/014662169802200406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. MacCallum RC, Hong S. Power analysis in covariance structure modeling using GFI and AGFI. Multivariate Behav Res. (1997) 32:193–210. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3202_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Brown TA. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research, Second Edition. New York, NY: Guilford Publications (2015). p. 482.

Google Scholar

29. Marsh HW, Balla JR, McDonald RP. Goodness-of-fit indexes in confirmatory factor analysis: The effect of sample size. psychol Bull. (1988) 103:391–410. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.103.3.391

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull. (1990) 107:238–46. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.107.2.238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hu L, Bentler PM. Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. psychol Methods. (1998) 3:424–53. doi: 10.1037//1082-989X.3.4.424

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kim ES, Yoon M. Testing measurement invariance: A comparison of multiple-group categorical CFA and IRT. Struct Equation Modeling: A Multidiscip J. (2011) 18:212–28. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2011.557337

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Muthén B, Asparouhov T. IRT studies of many groups: the alignment method. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:978. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis. Curr Dir psychol Sci. (1992) 1:98–101. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10768783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Fidler D, Most D, Booth-LaForce C, Kelly JF. Emerging social strengths in young children with down syndrome. Infants Young Children. (2008) 21:207–20. doi: 10.1097/01.IYC.0000324550.39446.1f

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Fidler DJ. The emergence of a syndrome-specific personality profile in young children with Down syndrome. Downs Syndr Res Pract. (2006) 10:53–60. doi: 10.3104/research

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Loveland KA, Kelley ML. Development of adaptive behavior in adolescents and young adults with autism and Down syndrome. Am J Ment Retard. (1988) 93:84–92.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

38. Rodrigue JR, Morgan SB, Geffken GR. A comparative evaluation of adaptive behavior in children and adolescents with autism, Down syndrome, and normal development. J Autism Dev Disord. (1991) 21:187–96. doi: 10.1007/BF02284759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Dykens EM. Psychiatric and behavioral disorders in persons with Down syndrome. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. (2007) 13:272–8. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Hamner T, Hepburn S, Zhang F, Fidler D, Robinson Rosenberg C, Robins DL, et al. Cognitive profiles and autism symptoms in comorbid down syndrome and autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2020) 41:172–9. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Molloy CA, Murray DS, Kinsman A, Castillo H, Mitchell T, Hickey FJ, et al. Differences in the clinical presentation of Trisomy 21 with and without autism. J Intellect Disabil Res. (2009) 53:143–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2008.01138.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Warner G, Howlin P, Salomone E, Moss J, Charman T. Profiles of children with Down syndrome who meet screening criteria for autism spectrum disorder (ASD): a comparison with children diagnosed with ASD attending specialist schools. J Intellect Disabil Res. (2017) 61:75–82. doi: 10.1111/jir.12344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: down syndrome, autism spectrum disorder, parent reported outcome measure, ASD-PROM, ND-PROM, confirmatory factor analysis, developmental questionnaire

Citation: Baumer NT, Pawlowski KG, Zhang B and Sideridis G (2024) Validation of factor structure of the neurodevelopmental parent report for outcome monitoring in down syndrome: confirmatory factor analysis. Front. Psychiatry 15:1293937. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1293937

Received: 13 September 2023; Accepted: 15 February 2024;
Published: 05 March 2024.

Edited by:

Ghaleb Hamad Alnahdi, Prince Sattam Bin Abdulaziz University, Saudi Arabia

Reviewed by:

Dimitrios Stamovlasis, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece
Angela You Gwaltney, RTI International, United States

Copyright © 2024 Baumer, Pawlowski, Zhang and Sideridis. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nicole T. Baumer, Nicole.Baumer@childrens.harvard.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.